Skip to main content
iScience logoLink to iScience
. 2023 Feb 28;26(4):106295. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2023.106295

A model of decentralized vision in the sea urchin Diadema africanum

Tianshu Li 1,2,3, John Kirwan 4,5, Maria Ina Arnone 4, Dan-Eric Nilsson 5, Giancarlo La Camera 1,2,3,6,
PMCID: PMC10025101  PMID: 36950121

Summary

Sea urchins can detect light and move in relation to luminous stimuli despite lacking eyes. They presumably detect light through photoreceptor cells distributed on their body surface. However, there is currently no mechanistic explanation of how these animals can process light to detect visual stimuli and produce oriented movement. Here, we present a model of decentralized vision in echinoderms that includes all known processing stages, from photoreceptor cells to radial nerve neurons to neurons contained in the oral nerve ring encircling the mouth of the animals. In the model, light stimuli captured by photoreceptor cells produce neural activity in the radial nerve neurons. In turn, neural activity in the radial nerves is integrated in the oral nerve ring to produce a profile of neural activity reaching spatially across several ambulacra. This neural activity is readout to produce a model of movement. The model captures previously published data on the behavior of sea urchin Diadema africanum probed with a variety of physical stimuli. The specific pattern of neural connections used in the model makes testable predictions on the properties of single neurons and aggregate neural behavior in Diadema africanum and other echinoderms, offering a potential understanding of the mechanism of visual orientation in these animals.

Subject areas: Marine organism, Behavioral neuroscience, Sensory neuroscience, Systems biology

Graphical abstract

graphic file with name fx1.jpg

Highlights

  • Some sea urchins and other echinoderms can detect visual stimuli despite lacking eyes

  • We model vision via neural processing of photoreceptor inputs from the body surface

  • The model accounts for the orienting behavior of sea urchin Diadema africanum

  • The model makes predictions on the physiology of the nervous system of echinoderms


Marine organism; Behavioral neuroscience; Sensory neuroscience; Systems biology

Introduction

Sea urchins are a large clade of echinoderms found at all depths in the ocean with diverse feeding and behavioral habits. Despite lacking eyes, these marine animals can visually resolve objects and move toward them, as well as point their spines toward looming visual stimuli.1,2,3,4,5,6,7 The long-spined sea urchin Diadema africanum,8 a night-active, herbivorous diadematoid of the eastern Atlantic which inhabits shallow seas and displays relocation behavior irrespective of the time of day, moves toward dark objects with a spatial resolution of 29°–69°, i.e., 0.01–0.03 cycles per degree.7 Such behavior is normally associated with centralized brain processing visual information from discrete eyes, yet sea urchins lack both. Therefore, sea urchins possess a unique visual system which has not been studied in terms of its information processing. Here, we set out to provide a mechanistic model of directional, low-resolution vision in sea urchins building on our knowledge of their general neural anatomy and the behavior observed in D. africanum. By providing such a model, we also aim to address a growing interest in the study of neural processing mechanisms in decentralized nervous systems (see e.g. Ref. 6,7,9,10,11,12). We especially aim to complement recent studies on visual resolution and locomotion in echinoderms with a quantitative framework of how sensory input integration and visual detection can be achieved by their decentralized systems.

In adult sea urchins, resolving vision may be mediated by photoreceptor cells (PRCs) situated in the tube feet of the animal. The tube feet emerge from five vertical grooves in the animal endoskeleton called “ambulacra”, located with pentaradial symmetry on the body surface. The tube feet are involved in such tasks as locomotion, positioning, cleaning, and feeding,13 but each tube foot also has PRCs.14,15,16 The anatomical and molecular description of PRCs in sea urchins come mostly from studies in the species Strongylocentrotus purpuratus, where it has been found that PRCs along the tube feet are shielded such that they only detect light from a restricted angle, approximately orthogonal to the body surface.4,5,16,17 Due to this property, the PRCs on the tube feet can together provide coarse spatial information as if the entire animal was a compound eye.4,16 However, sea urchins have no centralized brain which could process light information coming from the PRCs; rather, their internal nervous system comprises five radial nerves (RNs) and one oral nerve ring (ONR). The ONR is a commissure surrounding the mouth and interconnecting the RNs, and therefore is ideally poised to be responsible for both sensory integration and motor coordination, as previously suggested (see e.g. Ref. 3 and our discussion).

In this work, we build a theoretical model of the decentralized visual system of these echinoids based on previously published data on the behavior of D. africanum in response to light stimuli.7 In the model presented here, light information from PRCs is processed in the RNs and then relayed to ONR neurons, whose activity is readout to produce visually guided behavior. We apply our model to explain the behavior of D. africanum in the presence of isoluminant visual stimuli with a central dark “target” flanked by lighter regions.7 D. africanum also displays a spine-pointing response to looming circular fields subtending 13°–25°;7 here, however, we focus on modeling the movement toward stimuli that are isoluminant to the background as this allows us to discriminate spatial vision from phototaxis. In the experiments, the animals were initially placed at the center of a circular arena, with stimuli placed on its outer wall; the animals would then move toward the wall of the arena, and their behavior was analyzed in search for patterns of directional motion. The model presented here makes testable predictions on the behavior in this setup for a large class of visual stimuli, in particular on the three stimuli tested in the experiments of Kirwan et al.7 For these stimuli, the distribution of final positions predicted by the model matches, to a remarkable degree, the distribution of final positions reached by the animals.

To our knowledge, this is the first model of decentralized vision and visually guided behavior in sea urchins. The model combines all known neural processing stages, from PRCs to RN neurons to ONR neurons, including a probabilistic readout mechanism of the activity of ONR neurons that is responsible for visually guided movement. The specific pattern of neural connections used in the model also makes testable predictions on the properties of single neurons in each processing stage, such as their cell type (inhibitory vs. excitatory), their connectivity structure, and their response properties. All of these properties can in principle be measured in experiment and may lead to an understanding of the mechanism of visually guided orientation in echinoderms.

Results

Model of decentralized vision in sea urchins

General anatomical features

We constructed a model of vision in Diadema africanum by integrating three main components: PRCs, RNs, and ONR. PRCs and RNs were located along each of the five ambulacra; the PRCs were located outside the test, along tube feet,16 while the neurons were located inside the test along the latitudinal direction (along the θ angle in Figure 1A). For convenience, we grouped the PRCs in separate groups according to their target neurons in the RNs, and we did the same for the RN neurons (Figure 1B). The numbers of groups of PRCs and RN neurons in ambulacrum k were NPRCk and NRNk, respectively (see Table 1 for model parameter definitions and values). Similarly, we divided the ONR neurons in two subgroups, excitatory and inhibitory, and each subgroup was further divided into NONR groups based on the patterns of connectivity (details below). Since sea urchins have 5-fold symmetry, the center position of ambulacrum k was set to be φk=72(k1)°. Since the stimuli were vertically homogeneous (see section “model prediction of object taxis in Diadema africanum”), we simplified the 3D structure of the test to a horizontal slice (along the longitudinal angle φ, see Figure 1A).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic illustration of the model nervous system

(A) Schematic geometry of the animal. Left: 3D geometry. θ: latitudinal angle; φ: longitudinal angle. Right: transverse view of the animal cut above the ONR. The orange arcs indicate the distributions of PRCs on each ambulacrum (δ: half-width of the distribution). The green arcs indicate RN cells. The blue circle indicates eONR cells.

(B) Cartoon of the network structure used in the model (only 2 ambulacra shown at the top, represented by purple rectangles). Red segments terminating in a circle indicate inhibitory connections, blue arrows indicate excitatory connections. From top to bottom, orange circles indicate groups of PRCs, green circles indicate groups of RN neurons, red circles indicate groups of iONR neurons, and blue circles indicate groups of eONR neurons. The rightmost sketch illustrates how the action of RN cells onto eONR cells results into an effective inhibition of the latter.

Table 1.

Description and numerical values of the model parameters

Symbol Description Value
NPRCk Number of groups of PRCs on the kth ambulacrum 100
NRNk Number of groups of RNs on the kth ambulacrum 100
NONR Number of groups of eONR neurons (same for iONR neurons) 500
Δρ Acceptance angle of PRCs (width at half max of angular sensitivity function) 30°
δ Half-width of the distribution of PRCs locations on each ambulacrum 15°
ri,maxk,PRC Maximal activity of the ith PRC on ambulacrum k 1
ri,maxk,RN Maximal activity of the ith RN on ambulacrum k 1
ri,maxONR Maximal activity of the ith ONR 1
aRR Strength of the lateral connection weights in the RNs 0.25
aOO Strength of the lateral connection weights in the ONR 0.25
β Steepness of RN’s (ONR’s) sigmoidal response function 3 (4.5)
xc Location parameter of RN’s (ONR’s) sigmoidal response function −0.6 (−0.45)
θp Threshold for population vector length 5
δr Trajectory’s step size in behavioral model 0.1

Once activated by light, PRCs inhibit connected groups of RN neurons on the same ambulacrum. RN neurons were assumed to be excitatory, with different RN neuron groups exciting one another. The same groups of RN neurons send excitatory projections to input stage inhibitory cells in the ONR (iONR cells). The latter inhibit the activity of downstream groups of excitatory ONR neurons (eONR cells) that could be interpreted as the output sublayer of ONR. In turn, these groups of eONR neurons excite one another via recurrent connections (Figure 1B). This allows us to integrate information across the five ambulacra, thus mediating a form of decentralized vision.

Photoreceptor cells

PRCs are located inside the tube feet protruding along each ambulacrum.16,18 The available anatomical description of PRCs, coming mostly from studies in the species S. purpuratus,14,15,16 is still incomplete. We consider in the model only the PRCs located at the base of the tube feet, near the test, whose position can be considered fixed in the coordinate system of the sea urchin (see STAR Methods for details on coordinate systems). We did not consider the PRCs located at the tips of the tube feet because, given their lack of screening pigment and their high motility,16 those PRCs cannot contribute to spatial vision (see also the discussion). In the absence of more detailed information about the spatial distribution of PRCs, we assumed a uniform distribution of PRCs in each ambulacrum; different choices, such as a Gaussian distribution, led to similar results (not shown). Since changes along the latitudinal direction are not relevant in our model, we focus again on the longitudinal direction φ (i.e., from west to east; see Figure 1A). Along the kth ambulacrum, we assume a uniform distribution of PRCs between φkδ and φk+δ, where φk is the position of the center of the ambulacrum (we used δ=15° in the main simulations; see Table 1). Each PRC had an acceptance angle (i.e., the full width at half maximum of the normalized angular sensitivity curve) of Δρ=30°. Other values of δ and Δρ could be used and we show later how our results depend on variations of these values (see effect of location and acceptance angle of PRCs on spatial vision). Our behavioral experiments7 suggest acceptance angles in the range 38°89°. As this estimate takes also into account the distribution width of PRC locations on each ambulacrum, here quantified by δ, we chose δ=15° and Δρ=30° so as to obtain an “effective” acceptance angle of Δρeff=Δρ+2δ=60° (see also the discussion).

Each PRC’s angular sensitivity curve was modeled as a Gaussian function normalized to have a unitary peak and cutoff at its tails.19 We approximated such a function with a cosine function that optimally matches the Gaussian function away from its tails, while naturally vanishing in the corresponding tail regions20,21 (see STAR Methods for details). The angular sensitivity curves are plotted in Figure 2A for PRCs in all five ambulacra (each ambulacrum in a different color).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Angular sensitivity curves and population vectors

(A) Angular sensitivity curves of PRC fik(φ) (Equation 3 of STAR Methods with Δρ=30° and δ=15°). Each color represents an ambulacrum. The light shades represent the angular sensitivity curves of all PRCs (uniformly distributed with half-width δ in each ambulacrum). The darker lines are examples of single angular sensitivity curves (one example for each ambulacrum).

(B) Plot of sigmoidal function S (Equation 2) used to model the output of RN and ONR neurons (here shown for the ONR neurons, β=4.5 and xc=0.45; see Table 1). Note that activation of PRCs results in a reduced overall input x, reducing the output of RN cells (see the text).

(C) Population vectors (readout of eONR cells, see Equation 11 of STAR Methods) for three stimuli: 40° bar, 29° DoG, and 69° DoG (from left to right). Each data point (red circle) corresponds to the tip of one population vector. Different population vectors were obtained by varying the orientation of the sea urchin with respect to the center of the stimulus (here, located at the top of the arena). All orientations (from 0° to 359°) are represented. The large blue circle is the threshold θp. If for a given orientation the population vector’s length exceeds θp, visual detection occurs and coherent motion is predicted along the direction of the population vector.

(D) Examples of single population vectors among those in panel C for specific orientations (ψ) of the animal with respect to the center of the stimulus (see Figure S1).

The angular sensitivity curve characterizes the response of a PRC to light coming from a punctiform source located at a given longitudinal angle φ. The response of the same PRC to an extended stimulus comprising light coming from all directions was obtained by integrating the stimulus intensity along the angular dimension φ, weighted by the angular sensitivity curve (see STAR Methods for details). For an animated illustration of PRCs activity in response to stimuli, see Data S4.

RN neurons

Radial nerve neurons are also distributed along ambulacra.18,22,23 In our model, we assumed that they receive inhibitory input from the PRCs (see below). PRCs on ambulacrum k project to their target group of RN cells on the same ambulacrum via a (negative) connection weight WRPk. RN cells are also connected to other RN cells on the same ambulacrum k via a (positive) connection weight WRRk (see Figure 1B). As illustrated in the figure, we assume that these “lateral” connections among RN neurons are mostly local, i.e., they exist among adjacent groups of RN neurons, although this hypothesis is more a matter of convenience than a crucial ingredient of the model. We also note that these connections may or may not be established via chemical synapses—an issue that has not been settled, not even at the neuromuscular junction,24,25 but one that is not essential at our level of description.

The activity of RN neurons in response to input coming from both PRCs and other RN neurons was modeled as a sigmoidal function of the input,

rik,RN=ri,maxk,RNS(Iik), (Equation 1)

where rik,RN is the output firing rate of the ith RN cell on ambulacrum k, ri,maxk,RN is the maximal firing rate (here, the same for all i), and S(x) is the sigmoidal function

S(x)=11+e2β(xxc), (Equation 2)

see Table 1 (plotted in Figure 2B). The input Iik comprises multi-unit activity from PRCs and RN cells targeting RN cell i on ambulacrum k (these are visualized, in Figure 1B, by the encircled groups of PRCs and RN neurons targeting the same group of RN neurons). The exact analytical form of the input is reported in the STAR Methods (section “radial nerve neurons”). Note that since the PRCs provide an inhibitory input to RNs, rik,RN is a monotonically decreasing function of the activity of the PRCs: the larger the PRC activity, the smaller the firing rate of RN neurons. In the absence of visual input, the RN neurons maintain a spontaneous firing rate S(0)×ri,maxk,RNri,maxk,RN even in the absence of lateral input from other RNs, since the sigmoidal function is saturated at x=0 (see Figure 2B). This assumption was made to explain the lesion studies reported by Yoshida in the sea urchin Temnopleurus toreumaticus3 (see discussion, “assumptions of the model”). For an animated illustration of RN neurons’ activity in response to stimuli, see Data S4.

ONR

We assumed that the ONR contains iONR and eONR neurons, with iONR neurons being the input stage of ONR (Figure 1B). The RN cells project to iONR neurons via connection weights WiOR, where the connection exists only between specific groups of iONR neurons and RN neurons in their vicinity, i.e., located on the ambulacrum closest to their target group. Groups of iONR neurons, in turn, inhibit groups of eONR neurons in their vicinity via negative connection weights. The overall effect of this connectivity pathway is inhibition of eONR neurons by RN input, and can be described as being mediated by effective (negative) connection weights WOR (Figure 1B). In turn, neighboring groups of eONR neurons are connected through (excitatory) weights WOO. These lateral connections allow the ONR to integrate information coming from RNs on different ambulacra. The output of eONR neurons, the firing rate riONR, is a sigmoidal function of their input, analogously to the output of RN neurons (see STAR Methods for details, section “oval nerve ring”). The firing rate riONR increases due to lateral ONR input and decreases due to RN input. For an illustration of ONR neurons’ activity in response to stimuli, see Data S4.

ONR readout and spatial vision

Sea urchins can move toward or away from specific visual stimuli that they are able to detect.3,4,5,6,7 In our model, the direction of movement was determined by pulling together the activity of eONR neurons to produce a specific vectorial readout named “population vector”. The population vector is built by assigning a “vote” to each eONR neuron according to its own “preferred direction”, which was defined as the direction of a narrow stimulus causing the maximal increase in activity in the cell, and is highly correlated with the cell’s angular location (see STAR Methods for details). When an eONR is active, its firing rate “votes” for its preferred direction. The directions associated with different eONR neurons are then added up as vectors, and the resulting vector (the population vector) is compared to the threshold θp. If the length of the population vector exceeds the threshold, the urchin detects a visual stimulus as coming from the same direction as the population vector. The longer the population vector, the more reliable the detection.

The population vectors in the presence of the three spatially extended stimuli utilized in the experiment of Kirwan et al.7 are shown in Figure 2C for all possible orientations of the animal with respect to the center of the stimulus (see Figure 3 for a description of the stimuli). This figure shows that a stimulus may or may not be visible depending on the relative positions of the sea urchin and the stimulus. Therefore, visible stimuli are those for which there is a subset of orientations for which the population vector exceeds θp (blue circle in figure). For animations of this figure as the animal and stimulus change their relative orientation, see Data S4.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Experimental setting and stimuli

(A) Behavioral experiment setting. The stimuli were attached on the outer wall of the arena. At the beginning of each trial, the animal was positioned at the center of the arena with a random orientation.

(B) Examples of the six stimuli used in this work with φstim=40° (mathematical definitions in Table S1). The ink value of black and white is 1 and 0, respectively. Panel A adapted from ref.7

Model prediction of object taxis in Diadema africanum

The model developed in the previous subsections was tested on the visually guided behavior of D. africanum in taxis experiments performed by Kirwan et al.7 In the experiments, individuals of this species were located at the center of an arena with printed stimuli attached to the outer wall of the arena in a way to completely surround it (a schematic illustration of the arena is shown in Figure 3A). Three different stimuli were used in the experiments: a 40° bar stimulus and two stimuli obtained from taking the difference of two Gaussian functions (“difference of Gaussians”, or DoG), each with a darker center subtending a 29° and a 69°, respectively. These stimuli are shown as the top two stimuli in Figure 3B and were used to match the model to the experimental data. The remaining stimuli shown in Figure 3B were used to make novel model predictions on future experiments (see Data S2 for the mathematical characterization of all the stimuli).

The behavioral results found by Kirwan et al.7 are shown in Figure 4A. In summary, the sea urchins moved randomly in the presence of a homogeneous stimulus of constant intensity that was used as control (Figure 4A, top left panel). Similarly results were obtained in the presence of a 29° DoG stimulus (Figure 4A, top right). Some coherent movement toward a 40° bar stimulus was detected; however, its statistical significance was unclear (Figure 4A, bottom left); and finally, there was clear (significant) movement toward to center of a 69° DoG stimulus (Figure 4A, bottom right).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Behavior: comparison of model with data

Behavior of model and Diadema africanum in the presence of 4 different stimuli.

(A) Experimental results from the study by Kirwan et al.7 Each blue semicircle represents the final position of the animal at the end of one trial, while a full blue circle represents the final position of two animals. Although all animals reached the wall of the arena, final identical positions were stacked to show all the data. The stimuli used were a stimulus with uniform intensity in all directions (control; top left panel); a 29° DoG stimulus (top right); a 40° bar (bottom left); and a 69° DoG (bottom right; see Figure 3 and Table S1 for details about the stimuli). In each panel, the stimulus is shown on the outer wall of the arena (large circle). The red arrow at the center of the arena is a measure of aggregate directional movement across each cohort of subjects (see below).

(B) Model results in simulations of the same tasks shown in panel A. Each blue circle corresponds to one predicted final position across a cohort of 100 animals with random initial orientations with respect to the center of the stimulus. Here, the final position was inferred from the population vector induced by the stimulus when the animal was at the center of the arena (see Figures 2C and 2D). In both panels, the red arrow at the center of each plot is circular mean vector of the final positions (see STAR Methods, Equation 14). The only significant circular mean vector was obtained with the 69° DoG stimulus: P=0.013 (V-test) and P=0.042 (Rayleigh test), in agreement with the analogous results of ref. 7 (P>0.5 for the remaining stimuli). Main parameters: Δρ=30°, δ=15°, θp=5 (see Table 1 for all other parameter values). Panel A adapted from ref.7

Significant aggregate behavior toward the 69° DoG stimulus was not merely due to a larger number of subjects used in this task (n=96) compared to the same task with the other two stimuli (see ref. 7 for details). The red vector at the center of the arena is the circular mean vector (see STAR Methods Equation 14). The direction of this vector indicates the circular mean orientation of all bearings, while the length of the arrow indicates the “mean resultant length”, a measure of the concentration of the points along the direction of the arrow (the longer the arrow, the more coherent directional motion in the direction of the arrow compared to the uniform distribution; a short arrow indicates random direction of movement7).

The experimental results are accurately predicted by the model, as shown in Figure 4B. Model sea urchins with random orientations were located at the center of the arena, and the final position in each case was inferred from the initial value of the population vector (the collective readout of eONR activity). For population vectors of length below a threshold θp=5, a random final position was inferred, otherwise the final position was drawn from a Gaussian distribution with the same mean as the population vector. The distribution of final positions in Figure 4B is due to random initial orientations across different bearings (i.e., visual detection depends on the relative position of animal and stimulus) and reflects the distribution of population vectors shown in Figure 2C (or a circularly uniform distribution if the populations vectors are all smaller than the threshold; see STAR Methods for details). This ability for spatial vision is the result of integrating light information coming from all PRCs distributed on the animal’s body, mediated by neural activity in each RN, and finally integrated in the ONR.

As shown in Figure 2C, the population vector is always below threshold for the 29° DoG and the 40° bar, predicting random motion as confirmed in Figure 4B. For the 69° DoG stimulus, the population vector length exceeds the threshold when the region of the animal’s body between two ambulacra is facing the “target region” of the stimulus (roughly, the dark region flanked by the white maxima; see Figure 2D, rightmost plot). This means that, averaging across all initial orientations, there will be detectable motion toward the target, as confirmed in Figure 4B (bottom right). The simultaneous activation of the activities in PRCs, RNs, and ONR neurons, together with the resulting population vector readout in the ONR, can be appreciated in the animations presented in Data S3.

Dynamic model of object taxis

In the previous section, we have inferred the final position of the animal based on its ability to detect the stimulus when located at the center of the arena. This method assumes that the initial population vector not only establishes whether the stimulus has been detected but it is also a proxy of the final position of each bearing. As the animal moves toward the target, however, stimulus detection will change due to a change in the relative position of stimulus and animal. Such relative position will change at every movement and can in turn produce adjustments in movement direction. It is therefore of interest to have a model of visually driven, step-by-step movement. This would also allow a better comparison with the experimental results shown in Figure 4A, where the final position was determined based on the projection, on the arena wall, of the line connecting the center of the arena with the location of the animal at one-quarter of the radius of the arena.7

To include a basic mechanism of movement, we discretized time into small steps and predicted the movement direction of the next step based on i) the stimulus as detected by the animal at the current position, and ii) the direction of previous movement (see STAR Methods for details). Figure 5 shows the simulated trajectories for the same four stimuli shown in Figure 4B (see Data S5 for full animations). Each colored curve is the trajectory in one out of 100 trials and the final position of the center of the body is marked by dot of the same color (radially projected as a gray dot on the arena wall). The final position of the body center was set at 1/4 of the distance between the wall and the center of the arena to take into account the long spikes of D. africanum, so that the edge of the body reaches the wall when the center is at such distance from it.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Simulated trajectories

Simulated trajectories of 100 bearings under control, 40° bar, 29° DoG, and 69° DoG stimuli. The outer circle represents the wall of the arena with the stimulus on it. Animals were placed at random orientations at the center of the arena and moved according to the behavioral model described in section “Model of visually induced movement”. The colored lines represent the trajectories covered by the center of each animal starting from the center of the arena and ending at the color-matched dot. The gray dots on the arena’s wall are the radial projections of the colored dots and represent the points where the animal’s body hits the wall. See Table 1 for model parameters.

The distributions of predicted final positions agree well with those in Figure 4B, suggesting that the initial movement produced at the center of the arena is, on average, a valid proxy for the final position reached using this simple model of behavior. Under control, the trajectories started off a random direction and did not change direction except for small random changes around the previous direction. For the 29° DoG, initial movement was random but it would occasionally turn toward the stimulus. This happened when the location of the animal was such that light coming from the white maxima of the DoG struck one ambulacrum, causing a strong activation of PRCs at that location. For the 40° bar, the animal moved randomly at first but changed movement direction later in several trials. These changes occurred close to the stimulus target (the black region), which became wider in reference to the size of the animal as the latter approached the arena. On the opposite side, the bright region of the stimulus became narrower so that fewer ambulacra were activated. This resulted in a longer population vector and a change of movement direction toward the opposite side (this is due to a reduced cancellation of visual information that would occur when uniform light comes from most directions). Compared to the “static” model of Figure 4B, we noticed a smaller fraction of animals reaching the wall near the target region, although this caused no appreciable difference in the circular mean vector. Finally, for the 69° DoG, the animal would turn toward the target region when the latter faced directly one ambulacrum. In this case, due to the 69° arc subtended by the stimulus, each of the two white maxima of the stimulus would face one ambulacrum, producing a large population vector in the direction of the target region (see the rightmost plot in Figure 2D). As a result, there was a significant concentration of final positions near the target stimulus, as also observed in Figure 4B (bottom right).

Effect of location and acceptance angle of PRCs on spatial vision

The results shown so far were obtained assuming a longitudinal distribution of PRCs with half-width δ=15° and acceptance angle (the half-width of the angular sensitivity function) of Δρ=30°. Together, this results in an effective acceptance angle of 60° (Figure 2A). Varying either δ or Δρ can affect detection acuity (the minimum angular width of a visual stimulus which can be detected). For example, the reason why the model fails to detect the 29° DoG stimulus is the small arc length separation of the white maxima of the stimulus (the latter can only activate PRCs located on one ambulacrum; see Figures 2C and 2D). A larger distribution width or a larger acceptance angle, however, could allow the activation of PRCs on two ambulacra, as it occurs in the 69° DoG. Is our model robust to variability of δ and Δρ? In other words, what range of values can be allowed for these parameters so that the model still captures the results of Figure 4A?

To answer this question, we computed the maximal length vmax of the population vector, Equation 11, across all possible orientations. This was done for all values of Δρ between 15° and 90° and δ between 5° and 20° (Figure 6). If, for a given pair (Δρ,δ), one has vmax>θp, then the stimulus can be detected (a more realistic criterion is to require a finite range of initial orientations for which the population vector length exceeds θp). Note that the range of Δρ values used here contains our previously estimated range based on contrast thresholds between 5% and 20%.7

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Effect of acceptance angle and location of PRCs on the model’s spatial vision

Each panel shows a heatmap of vmax, the maximal length of the population vector across initial orientations of the animal, for a given stimulus and a given pair of values for Δρ and δ. Each column shows the same stimulus for different arc widths of the target region (i.e., for different φstim, see Table S1), while each row shows the same φstim across different stimuli. If vmax<θp, the animal cannot detect the stimulus from any orientation. The red dotted line is the contour line where vmax=θp, while the black dotted line is the contour line where vmax=4. The white line is the collection of points with Δρ+2δ=60°. In each plot, Δρ ranged between 15° and 90° and δ ranged between 5° and 20°. Red stars mark the point (Δρ,δ)=(30°,15°), the parameter values used in the main simulations. DoG: Difference of Gaussians; FB: Flanked bar.

Figure 6 shows the value of vmax for different values of Δρ and δ for a wide range of stimuli, beyond those used in our previous experimental study.7 In each plot, the red dotted line is the contour line where vmax reaches the boundary defined by the threshold (θp). The animal is able to detect the stimulus for parameter values (Δρ,δ) inside the yellow region delimited by the red dotted line. The red stars mark the pair of parameter values (Δρ,δ)=(30°,15°) used in Figure 4B. The point defined by this pair is located outside the yellow region for the 40° bar and 29° DoG, whereas it is inside the yellow region for the 69° DoG stimulus. This implies an animal with (Δρ,δ)=(30°,15°) can detect the 69° DoG, but not the 40° bar and 29° DoG, as already established. Figure 6 also shows that our results are robust to some degree of parameter variation, in the sense that they hold in a finite region of parameters space around the point (Δρ,δ)=(30°,15°).

Our model makes other interesting predictions. For example, it predicts that the impact of Δρ on spatial vision depends on the stimulus. For some stimuli (e.g., the bar), detection improves for larger Δρ, while for other stimuli (e.g., the DoG and the Morlet wavelet), detection improves for smaller Δρ. Furthermore, the model can be used as a guide for choosing probing stimuli in future behavioral experiments. For example, the model predicts that a 69° bar stimulus should be easily visible, whereas a 69° Flanked bar, Haar wavelet, or first Hermitian wavelet stimulus should be hard to detect from any initial orientation. We emphasize that these predictions—which can be tested in experiment—depend on the neural model built in this study, and not just the properties of the PRCs.

Finally, note that in Figure 6, we have assumed an equal acceptance angle Δρ for all PRCs, which is unreasonable. To address this issue, we performed the same analysis using a random distribution of Δρ values across PRCs in each ambulacrum. As shown in Data S6, a random distribution of Δρ values does not alter the general picture shown in Figure 6 (compare Figure 6 with Figure S4).

Discussion

A model of decentralized vision in sea urchins

In this work, we propose a model of decentralized vision in sea urchins based on general anatomical features of their nervous system and assuming that light information is captured by PRCs distributed over the animals’ dermis around the ambulacra. PRCs have been found on the tube feet of sea urchins,14,15,16 which emerge from the test of the animal along the five ambulacra. The model considers the nervous system of the animal in some degree of detail, and provides a mechanistic model involving all stages of the system, from PRCs to neural activity in the ONR. As information flows from PRCs to RN neurons to ONR neurons, the model can be abstracted as comprising three major layers. The model has feedforward connections between the layers (PRCs to RN neurons and RN neurons to ONR neurons) as well as recurrent connections among neurons in the same RN, and within neurons of the ONR (Figure 1B). PRCs on the tube feet of the animal, once activated by light, inhibit RN neurons in the same ambulacrum. In turn, RN neurons inhibit excitatory neurons in the ONR (eONR). Lateral recurrent connections in the ONR allow to integrate the information coming from all ambulacra. The activity of ONR is readout as a “population vector”. The population vector collates the preferred directions and the activity of all eONR neurons in the presence of a specific visual stimulus. When the length of the population vector is large enough to exceed a threshold, enough evidence has been accrued about the location of a visual stimulus, and movement toward said location occurs with probability proportional to the length of the population vector. We discuss below the feasibility of all these putative mechanisms in light of the available experimental evidence.

The model explains in quantitative detail the behavior of D. africanum found in the taxis discrimination task of the study by Kirwan et al.,7 where taxis was found clearly only in 69° isoluminant DoG stimuli, but neither in narrower DoG nor in a 40° bar stimulus. The latter finding was somewhat surprising given that there is a much greater local contrast in the case of the bar stimulus and greater amplitude at low spatial frequencies (see Figure 4 of the study by Kirwan et al.7). Our model provides a mechanistic explanation of these results based on generic anatomical features of the nervous system of sea urchins combined with a specific mechanism of visual integration and readout at the level of ONR neurons.

Assumptions of the model

In building our model, we have considered the available experimental data on sea urchins anatomy and behavior, and turned to other systems when the experimental knowledge was insufficient. This has led us to combine elements from both invertebrate and vertebrate nervous systems, some of which inspired by mammalian physiology. Examples of invertebrate structures are the ganglia-like clusters of RNs connected to clusters of PRCs, and nearby clusters of RN nerves connected “laterally” (see Figure 1B).13,26 This local arrangement is compatible with the fact that axons of echinoderm neurons are normally small and unmyelinated, and are bundled in packages with a parallel arrangement.27 Examples from vertebrate animals include sigmoidal “tuning curves” to characterize the response of RN and ONR neurons (Equation 2). Neurons with similar tuning curve properties are found e.g. in cats and primates and include primary and secondary visual neurons,28,29 middle temporal neurons,28,30 parietal neurons,31 motor cortical neurons,20 and so on, suggesting perhaps that tuning curves, as a way to relay sensory information, may be widespread across different nervous systems, echinoderms included. Similarly, the idea of reading out the activity of ONR neurons via population vectors was borrowed from landmark studies in motor and premotor cortex of primates.20,32

While the existence of analogous properties in sea urchin neurons awaits experimental demonstration, we included these structures in our model for a number of reasons. First, some evidence in the literature suggests that we may be on the right track. For example, studies in other echinoderms such as brittle stars9 indicate that bidirectional connections between RNs (similar to our model’s lateral connections) are necessary for coordinated locomotion. However, the analogy with better studied marine invertebrates is often of very limited help. For example, starfish orienting in an odor plume lead with the rays facing the odor source and change leading ray multiple times during the approach to the source,33 whereas we did not reliably observe similar changes in our taxis experiments.7 Finally, it seems that some neural elements are indeed common to the mammalian and echinoid nervous systems. For example,34 Pentreath and Cobb have reported cholinergic and dopaminergic neurons in the central nervous system of echinoderms, with acetylcholine bound to synaptic vesicles morphologically similar to those present in the mammalian brain.

To summarize, our modeling choices were informed by arguments of plausibility and generality whenever the experimental information was inadequate. Our most salient choices are discussed in more detail below.

Model of tube feet photoreceptor cells

PRCs at the base of tube feet are located along the ambulacra, but their detailed distribution is not known. We have therefore chosen a uniform distribution of locations that spans 30° longitudinally. This feature of the model does not present a limitation, for two reasons: i) other distributions of PRC locations (such as a Gaussian distribution) result in a quantitatively equivalent model (not shown); and ii) the model’s behavior is robust to variations of parameter values such as location spread, δ, and acceptance angles, Δρ, of single PRCs (Figure 6).

The values chosen for these parameters reflect the position of the PRCs on the tube feet and other factors, such as depressions in the test that can hold space for r-opsin-expressing PRCs while also screening light reaching the PRCs,16 or the shading activity of opaque spines.4,5,17 Our behavioral experiments7 suggest acceptance angles in the range 38°89°. As this estimate takes also into account the distribution width of PRC locations on each ambulacrum, we chose δ=15° and Δρ=30° so as to obtain an “effective” acceptance angle of Δρeff=Δρ+2δ=60°.

We note that although a sizable density of PRCs is found on the tips of the tube feet, the latter lack any associated screening pigment and are highly motile.16 As a consequence, the PRCs located on the tube feet disks display continuously changing spatial properties and cannot provide the basis for spatial vision, unlike the PRCs at the base of the tube feet. Moreover, in a recent analysis of scRNA-seq data of early developmental stages of the sea urchin Paracentrotus lividus, different gene expression profiles were found for two sub-clusters of r-opsin-expressing PRCs.35 Those findings might reflect different functions of the two PRC clusters located, respectively, at the tip and base of the tube feet that had been initially reported in the study by Ullrich-Lüter et al.16

Finally, although sea urchins can presumably sense light gradients along the latitudinal (vertical) direction, we did not consider this possibility in our model, since our stimuli were vertically homogeneous and locomotion occurred always on a horizontal plane. We also grouped cells with similar properties in each layer (PRC, RN, or ONR) and averaged their behavior (input and output), so that we could consider a homogeneous group of neurons as the elementary processing unit in our model. This type of coarse-grained approach is typically successful in systems with redundant numbers of elements and it allows us to focus on the essential features of the anatomy.

Double inhibition of oval nerve ring neurons

In the model, light-induced excitation of eONR neurons occurs by double inhibition: PRCs inhibit RNs, which in turn project to iONR neurons which inhibit eONR neurons. In this model, the more the RNs are inhibited by light, the larger the response of target ONR neurons leading to stimulus detection. Although this is not the only possible mechanism compatible with our results (double excitation would work just as well), we favored double inhibition over double excitation because the former makes our model compatible with experimental findings reported in other species of sea urchins. Inhibition by light was invoked by Millott and Yoshida36 to explain the shadow reaction of Diadema antillarum Philippi, and by Yoshida3 to explain experiments performed by Yoshida and Kobayashi in the sea urchin T. toreumaticus (see our summary below). The putative inhibition of RN neurons in response to light is also reminiscent of the “off” response of isolated RNs observed in the sea urchin Diadema setosum,37 and in general, our model’s assumption that excitatory RN neurons are inhibited by a stimulus resonates with the notion, based on early electrophysiological experiments, that RNs are a locus for interaction between excitation and inhibition.38

In the experiments described in ref. by Yoshida 3, four ambulacra were surgically removed and the animal placed in dim light (under photographic safelight). Under these conditions, the majority of animals moved in the direction of the surgically operated area. Assuming positive phototaxis as the most likely behavior in this experiment (wherein the animal moves toward the light source), this finding could be explained if RN neurons inhibited ONR neurons: the removal of RNs would remove an inhibitory factor, increasing the activity of ONR neurons close to the operated area, and producing locomotion in that direction. In a related experiment, a light source was positioned outside of the animal facing the intact RN, and movement toward the light source usually ensued. This is consistent with inhibition of the intact RN by the photo-stimulated PRCs. This inhibition must cause a stronger excitation of the ONR neurons near the intact site, compared to the excitation caused by the absence of RNs on the (operated) opposite site.

When the light source was located internally and near the surgically operated site, locomotion in the direction of the intact RN was reduced but was still more likely than locomotion in the opposite direction. This may be because RNs can sense light directly, although to a smaller degree than when stimulated directly by PRCs. In our double inhibition model, this smaller light detection causes a smaller inhibition of the intact RN and therefore a smaller disinhibition of ONR neurons near the intact side, explaining (i) the preferential locomotion toward the intact side, but also (ii) the presence of locomotion toward the opposite side (caused by the absence of RNs on that side). Analogous experiments with two intact RNs (instead of one) led to analogous conclusions.3

By adopting a double inhibition model, we can quantitatively explain the locomotion experiments in D. africanum while also capturing, at least qualitatively, Yoshida’s observations in surgically operated sea urchins. In this sense, the model makes strong predictions on the patterns of connections between PRCs, RN neurons, and ONR neurons.

In the same article, Yoshida reported another finding that supports the integrative role ascribed by our model to the ONR. Specifically, transecting one RN near the ONR resulted in lack of motion in the direction of the transected nerve, when the animal was placed between two light sources facing each other. Thus, the ONR may play a key role in integrating visual stimuli from RNs. In our model, we have specified a possible mechanism of integration (discussed next) that can explain the results of the study by Kirwan et al.7

Model of visual detection and movement

In the absence of more detailed experimental evidence, we have chosen to follow a principle most precisely formulated in neuroscience,39 according to which sensory and motor functions can benefit from pulling together the activity of many neurons in key areas. In our case, pulling together the collective activity of RNs and ONR neurons determines the strength of visual detection and the stimulus direction. The specific mechanism we adopted makes use of the population vector, which is a natural and general construct for reading out the activity of populations of neurons.

We prefer not to commit to more complex interpretations of the population vector. For example, while the length of the population vector clearly reflects the animal’s ability to detect the stimulus, it may also incorporate the animal’s motivation to move toward (or away from) the stimulus, once detected. Therefore, resolving power could be greater than the level exhibited. Also, the same model could be used to move toward the light (as assumed here) or away from it, by simply reversing the meaning of the population vector’s direction. Thus, while the readout of the ONR neurons’ activity could represent additional variables related to motivation or other determinants of the behavior, we feel that it is premature to commit to any of them due to the lack of the necessary experimental data. At this stage, the population vector is best understood as the link between the neural activity produced by the nervous system of our model and the observed orienting behavior of D. africanum.

Questions for future experiments

It is an important question to determine experimentally to what degree our anatomical assumptions hold. Much information is lacking on the nervous system of Echinodermata, partly due to technical difficulties.27 In the absence of more detailed information, our assumptions allow an explanation of vision in D. africanum—at least pertaining to the experiments performed in7—and allow to make clear predictions for future experiments. In turn, the model presented here can be modified and improved on the basis of new experimental evidence, which may alter its functionality to different degrees. To this aim, future anatomical, morphological, and behavioral experiments should help clarify issues such as the impact of PRCs input onto clusters of RN neurons; the nature of neural connectivity, including the details of the excitatory vs. inhibitory action exerted by clusters of RN and ONR neurons on their target structures; the electrophysiological response of the same neurons to luminous stimuli (their “tuning” properties); and the detailed nature of ONR integration of RN input. Furthermore, the aggregate response of ONR neurons to luminous stimuli could reveal the details of the mechanism used by these organisms to produce visually driven behavior (here modeled via the population vector). Finally, comparison of our predictions with behavioral experiments conducted with various stimuli as shown in Figure 6 could give indirect information on the distribution of PRCs, their (effective) angular sensitivity, and more in general on the architecture of the model upon which the predictions are based.

Limitations of the study

In this study, we have built a model of decentralized vision in echinoderms guided by behavioral data acquired with D. africanum. Hence, although the principles underlying this model could apply to a larger class of echinoderms, the specific model studied here is limited to this particular species. Some assumptions of the model have not yet received experimental validation.

STAR★Methods

Key resources table

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER
Software and algorithms

Matlab Mathworks www.mathworks.com
Original code the authors https://github.com/lacameralab/diadema

Resource availability

Lead contact

Further information and requests for resources and code should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Giancarlo La Camera (giancarlo.lcamera@stonybrook.edu).

Materials availability

This study did not generate new materials except for computer code (see “data and code availability”).

Method details

Stimuli

The data analyzed and modeled in this work were obtained in Ref. 7 with the procedures summarized in Data S1 and using printed patterns surrounding the arena as stimuli. The printed patterns were greyscale printed images consisting of dark regions set against a lighter background (shown in Figure 3B of the main text). The stimuli were uniform in the vertical plane but varied in the horizontal plane along the longitudinal direction φ in the coordinate system of the arena. This variation was described by a function X(φ) which was the input to the model PRCs (described in detail in Data S2). The main patterns used in the experiment were a 40° bar stimulus and the difference of Gaussian functions (DoG) subtending either 29 or 69 degrees. In the bar stimulus, a region of homogeneously black stimulus subtending 40° was presented against a white background. In the DoG stimulus, the center of the stimulus was maximally dark, but of increasing reflectance towards the periphery of the stimulus (on the horizontal axis) and reaching the maximum achievable reflectance before darkening into the grey background. These stimuli were used in the model to obtain a match between model and experiment. The remaining stimuli shown in Figure 3B were used to make model predictions for future experiments. All stimuli were isoluminant with respect to the remainder of the patterns due to the lighter regions flanking the stimulus, i.e., it is not possible to detect the stimulus by simply comparing the radiance profile of different parts of the arena from the centre without having a spatial resolution equivalent to the arc subtended by the stimulus itself. Full details about the stimuli can be found in Data S2.

Photoreceptor cells

Each PRC’s angular sensitivity curve was modeled as a Gaussian function normalized to have a unitary peak and cut-off at its tails.19 We approximated such a function with a cosine function that optimally matches the Gaussian function away from its tails, while naturally vanishing in the corresponding tail regions:20,21

fik(φ)=[cos(φφi,dmsk,PRC)a1a]+. (Equation 3)

In this function, fik is the response of the ith PRC on ambulacrum k from punctiform light coming from a longitudinal angle φ; a=2cos(Δρ/2)1, where Δρ is the acceptance angle of the PRC; the symbol [·]+ means rectification ([x]+0 for x0, [x]+=0 otherwise); and φi,dmsk,PRC is the direction of maximum sensitivity of the ith PRC on ambulacrum k. For simplicity, we assume that light is screened so that φi,dmsk,PRC is also the actual location of the cell (i.e., fik(φ) is maximal in response to radially facing light). Equation 3 is plotted in Figure 2A of the main text for PRCs in all five ambulacra (each ambulacrum in a different color).

Note that in Equation 3 only the difference of two angles matters, which is independent of the coordinate system chosen. In the following we consider the angles φ given in the coordinate system of the sea urchin, which is simply the coordinate system of the arena rotated by a fixed angle ψ (the angular distance of the first ambulacrum from the center of the stimulus; see Figure S1). The function fik(φ) characterizes the response of PRCs to light coming from a punctiform source located at φ=φψ (where φ is the source’s location in the coordinate system of the arena). The response of the same PRC to a full stimulus comprising light coming from all directions φ with intensity X(φ), was obtained by integrating the input X along the angular dimension, weighted by the angular sensitivity curve fik:

rik,PRC=ri,maxk,PRCZik,PRC0360°dφX(φ)fik(φ), (Equation 4)

where Zik,PRC=0360°dφfik(φ) is a normalization factor to keep the output activity of PRCs in a physiological range (note that X(φ)1 for all stimuli; see Data S2 for mathematical details on the stimuli). The output is proportional to ri,maxk,PRC, a constant parameter with physical units of PRC activity. Note that for our choice of stimuli and parameters, rik,PRCri,maxk,PRC. For an illustration of PRCs activity in response to stimuli, see Data S4.

Radial nerve neurons

The activity of RNs is a sigmoidal function of the inhibitory input coming from the PRCs (through synapses WRP<0) and the excitatory input coming from adjacent RNs (through synapses WRR>0; see the main text). For convenience, we scaled the connection weights as the inverse of the square root of the number of projecting groups of cells:

WRPk(i,j)=1/NPRCk, (Equation 5)
WRRk(i,j)=aRR/NRNk, (Equation 6)

where i,j are the indices of the connected cells (with i being the index of the neuron on the receiving end of the connection), and the parameter aRR quantifies the strength of ‘lateral’ excitation in the RN layer of each ambulacrum. The scaling with the inverse square root of the total number of neurons echoes wisdom from mammalian physiology.40 In this model, however, this specific scaling is simply a means to parameterize the value of the connection strengths as a function of NPRCk and NRNk.

The activity of RN neurons in response to input coming from both PRCs and other RN neurons, measured as firing rate (the number of action potentials per second), was

rik,RN=ri,maxk,RNS(WRP,(i)krk,PRC+WRR,(i)krk,RN|WRP,(i)krmaxk,PRC|), (Equation 7)

where S(x) is the sigmoidal function Equation 2 (Figure 2B).

In Equation 7, rik,RN is the output firing rate of the ith RN cell on ambulacrum k, ri,maxk,RN is the maximal firing rate (here, the same for all i), rk,PRC and rk,RN are vectors of activities from PRCs and RN cells, respectively, targeting RN cell i on ambulacrum k (these are visualized in Figure 1B by the encircled groups of PRCs and RN neurons targeting the same group of RN neurons). Finally, we have used the notation W(i) for the ith row of matrix W, and W(i)r for the dot product of vectors W(i) and vector r: W(i)r=jWijrj.

Oval nerve ring

Similar to RN neurons, eONR neurons received inhibitory and excitatory input (from adjacent iONR and eONR neurons, respectively; see the main text). Also in this case we rescaled the connection weights by the inverse square root of the total number of afferent neurons (note that NONR=NRNk=15NNRk):

WOR(i,j)=1/NRN, (Equation 8)
WOO(i,j)=aOO/NONR. (Equation 9)

WOR is the effective, inhibitory synaptic weight connecting RN neurons to eONR neurons (see Figure 1B of the main text); WOO is the lateral excitatory synaptic weight connecting adjacent groups of eONR neurons (Figure 1B). Similarly to RN neurons, the output of eONR neurons is a sigmoidal function of their inputs,

riONR=ri,maxONRS(WOR,(i)rRN+WOO,(i)rONR|WOR,(i)rmaxRN|), (Equation 10)

where S(x) is Equation 2 (parameter values given in Table 1 of the main text). In Equation 10, the meaning of the symbols is analogous to the meaning of the corresponding symbols for RN neurons in Equation 7. In particular, rRN and rONR are vectors of activities from RN (on all ambulacra) and ONR cells, respectively, targeting (directly or indirectly) eONR cell i, according to the connectivity pattern shown in Figure 1B of the main text.

Population vector

The population vector of eONR cells was defined as the sum of vectors associated to each eONR neuron. The direction of each vector was the preferred direction of the corresponding neuron (defined below), while the length of the vector depended on the firing rate of the cell in response to the stimulus.

The preferred direction of eONR cell i, φi,prefONR, was defined as the direction of a narrow stimulus causing the maximal increase in activity in the cell. As the narrow stimulus, we chose a 2° white bar. The preferred direction can be identified with the unit vector ziONR=(cos(φi,prefONR),sin(φi,prefONR)), where we have used notation (x,y) for a vector with components x and y, respectively (note that ziONR does not depend on the current visual stimulus: it depends only on the anatomy and physiology of the model).

The population vector is the vector sum of the vectors ziONR, each weighted by the firing rate of the cell, riONR:

vpop=i=1NONRriONR(cos(φi,prefONR),sin(φi,prefONR)). (Equation 11)

Unlike the single cells preferred directions, the population vector depends on the stimulus X(φ) (via the firing rates riONR) and on the orientation of the stimulus with respect to the animal (see Figures 2C and 2D of the main text).

Note that the preferred directions are properties of the cells and do not depend on the extended stimuli X(φ) used to probe movement; they can be computed analytically and agree with the preferred directions obtained in simulations with the 2° white bar (we report the analytical details in Data S3). The preferred direction of an eONR cell was highly correlated with its angular position in the coordinate system of the animal, although, due to the lack of PRCs in the region between ambulacra, a mismatch between eONR cell position and preferred direction slowly accrues until it is zeroed at the onset position of the next ambulacrum (see Figure S2).

Each final position of the model on the wall of the arena shown in Figure 4B was sampled from a circularly uniform distribution if the population vector was below threshold, and from a Gaussian distribution narrowly centered around the population vector if |vpop|>θp (with standard deviation 1/(|vpop|θp)1; standard deviations 5 or 10 times larger gave the same results).

To compute the population vector in response to a visual stimulus we used Equation 11 of the main text. Due to the recurrent connections in RNs and ONR, the neural activities need time to converge to the stationary values used in Equation 11. Thus, we ran a simulation of the neural dynamics until the activity in the RN and ONR neurons reached the steady state. This process entails presenting the stimulus, obtaining the response of the PRCs, feeding this response to the RN neurons, feeding the RN neurons’ response to the iONR neurons, and so on until all neurons’ responses have been recorded. At the next step, the activity of the RN neurons is modified due to the lateral inputs coming from adjacent groups of RN neurons; and similarly for eONR neurons. The new activity in each group of neurons, in turn, modifies the activity of neighboring groups at the next time step, and so on. The cycle is repeated until a stable self-consistent activity state is found, i.e., a state where none of the neurons change their activity at the next time step. At this stage, we computed the population vector according to Equation 11. We considered the steady state reached when the firing rates in two consecutive steps were smaller than 105. Note that convergence to steady state occurred rapidly: typically, in less than 200 iterations and never more than 600. Assuming a time step of 0.1ms for each iteration (a customary choice in the literature on neural systems), this means that convergence occurred typically in about 20ms and never more than 60ms, well below the typical time scales of locomotion in D. africanum.

Model of movement

In brief, the model sea urchin will move in the direction of the population vector if the latter is sufficiently larger than the threshold θp=5, otherwise it will move randomly but with a bias along the direction of the previous step. In detail, movement direction is a random variable that may follow one of two Gaussian distributions, p1(μ1,σ1) or p2(μ2,σ2), depending on the length of the population vector, vpop=|vpop|. The distribution p1 is peaked around the direction of the population vector, while p2 is peaked around the previous direction of motion. In detail, p1(μ1,σ1) had a mean μ1 equal to the direction of vpop and σ1=max{10°vpopθp,ϵ}, where ϵ=(105)° is a small number to keep the standard deviation σ1 positive. Also, values of σ1 larger than 360° were set to 360° (uniform distribution on the circle). For the Gaussian distribution p2(μ2,σ2), μ2=φpre (the direction of motion at the previous step, say in the coordinate system of the sea urchin), with fixed standard deviation σ2=10° (on the first step, φpre was randomly sampled from a uniform distribution on the unit circle). The bias towards the previous direction helps to reduce the frequency of sharp directional changes, which are rarely observed experimentally.

The model followed p1 with probability

q(vpop)=11+e10(vpopθ), (Equation 12)

and it followed p2 with probability 1q(vpop), so that at every time step the movement is sampled from a mixture of two Gaussian distributions:

P(φnext|vpop,φpre)=q(vpop)×p1(φnext|vpop)+(1q(vpop))×p2(φnext|φpre), (Equation 13)

where P(φnext|vpop,φpre) is the probability of the next step φnext given the current value of the population vector (both direction and length) and the direction of the previous step, φpre. For large population vector lengths vpop, p1 is almost certainly chosen, and it will be narrowly peaked around the direction of vpop; on the other hand, if vpop is below the threshold, the most likely movement will be a random step in a range of about 10 degrees from the previous direction. The probabilistic nature of this model is especially relevant for peri-threshold stimuli, for which one expects the largest behavioral variability since p1,2 have an equal probability of being selected.

To simulate the behavioral trajectories shown in Figure 5 of the main text, each trial started with the animal located at the center of the arena with a random orientation with respect the center of the stimulus. The movement of the animal occurred in incremental steps covering 10% of the arena’s radius. The length of each step roughly mimics the number of steps required to reach the wall of the arena during the experiments. We also assumed the animal does not rotate during movement, as observed in experiment. After each step, we updated the position of the animal by sampling the new position from the distribution Equation 13. Each trial ended when the animal reached the wall of the arena, i.e., when the center of the sea urchin reached a distance 3/4 from the center of the arena, as explained in the main text. Full details of the procedure, together with animations of behavioral trajectories, are given in Data S5.

To obtain a dataset including M animals, we simulated M trials for each stimulus, each time with an animal starting from the center of the arena and with a uniformly random orientation of the first ambulacrum. As an aggregate measure of the final positions across a population of model sea urchins, we took the circular mean vector4,7

rf=1Nk=1N(cosαk,sinαk), (Equation 14)

where αk is the final position of model sea urchin #k (relative to the line joining the center of the area to the center of the stimulus), N is the total number of sea urchins, and we have used again notation (x,y) for a vector with components x and y, respectively. The circular mean vector rf is shown as the red vector in Figure 4B of the main text. For a narrow concentration of final positions around a given direction, rf will point towards that direction and will have a large length (i.e., close to 1). Conversely, for a uniform distribution of final positions, rf will have a length close to zero. The Rayleigh test and V-test of circular statistics were conducted to test the significance of simulated object taxis at α=0.05 confidence level (Figure 4B).

Quantification and statistical analysis

Model simulations and statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB using custom code. The Rayleigh test and V-test of circular statistics were conducted to test the significance of simulated object taxis (Figure 4B). The statistics were reported as mean values across 100 simulated experiments with 100 subjects in each experiment (where each subject is located at the center of the arena with a random initial orientation). Statistical details can be found in the figure legend.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a Research Grant from the Human Frontier Science Program (Ref.-No: RGP0002/2019). We thank Drs. Esther Ullrich-Lüter and Carsten Lüter for very useful discussions, Drs. Esther Ullrich-Lüter and Michael Bok for a careful reading of the manuscript, and Dr. Esther Ullrich-Lüter for expert guidance on the literature on echinoderms.

Author contributions

T.L. built the model, wrote the computer code, performed the numerical simulations, prepared the figures, and helped write the manuscript. J.K. wrote some computer code and provided materials. M.I.A. helped conceive the research and provided materials. D.E.N. helped conceive the research and provided materials; G.L.C. conceived the research, supervised the research, wrote the manuscript, and provided materials. All authors provided intellectual contribution to the research and the manuscript.

Declaration of interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Published: February 28, 2023

Footnotes

Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2023.106295.

Supplemental information

Document S1. Figures S1–S4, Tables S1–S3, and Data S1–S6
mmc1.pdf (613.4KB, pdf)

Data and code availability

  • This study did not generate new resource data.

  • All original code has been deposited at GitHub and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOIs are listed in the key resources table.

  • Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.

References

  • 1.Holmes S.J. Phototaxis in the sea urchin, arbacia punctulata. J. Anim. Behav. 1912;2:126–136. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Millott N., Takahashi K. The shadow reaction of diadema antillarum philippi. IV. spine movements and their implications. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 1963;246:437–469. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Yoshida M. In: Physiology of Echinodermata. Boolootian R.A., editor. John Wiley and Sons; 1966. Photosensitivity; pp. 435–464. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Blevins E., Johnsen S. Spatial vision in the echinoid genus echinometra. J. Exp. Biol. 2004;207:4249–4253. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Yerramilli D., Johnsen S. Spatial vision in the purple sea urchin Strongylocentrotus purpuratus (Echinoidea) J. Exp. Biol. 2010;213:249–255. doi: 10.1242/jeb.033159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Al-Wahaibi M.K., Claereboudt M.R. Extraocular vision in the sea urchin diadema setosum. Mar. Freshw. Behav. Physiol. 2017;50:31–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kirwan J.D., Bok M.J., Smolka J., Foster J.J., Hernández J.C., Nilsson D.-E. The sea urchin diadema africanum uses low resolution vision to find shelter and deter enemies. J. Exp. Biol. 2018;221:jeb176271. doi: 10.1242/jeb.176271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rodríguez A., Hernández J.C., Clemente S., Coppard S.E. A new species of diadema (echinodermata: Echinoidea: Diadematidae) from the eastern atlantic ocean and a neotype designation of diadema antillarum (philippi, 1845) Zootaxa. 2013;3636:144–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Clark E.G., Kanauchi D., Kano T., Aonuma H., Briggs D.E.G., Ishiguro A. The function of the ophiuroid nerve ring: how a decentralized nervous system controls coordinated locomotion. J. Exp. Biol. 2019;222:jeb192104. doi: 10.1242/jeb.192104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Beer S., Wentzel C., Petie R., Garm A. Active control of the visual field in the starfish acanthaster planci. Vis. Res. 2016;127:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Li L., Connors M.J., Kolle M., England G.T., Speiser D.I., Xiao X., Aizenberg J., Ortiz C. Multifunctionality of chiton biomineralized armor with an integrated visual system. Science. 2015;350:952–956. doi: 10.1126/science.aad1246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Garm A., Nilsson D.-E. Visual navigation in starfish: first evidence for the use of vision and eyes in starfish. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2014;281:20133011. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.3011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hyman L.H. McGraw-Hill; 1955. The Invertebrates Vol. IV Echinodermata. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Agca C., Elhajj M.C., Klein W.H., Venuti J.M. Neurosensory and neuromuscular organization in tube feet of the sea urchin Strongylocentrotus purpuratus. J. Comp. Neurol. 2011;519:3566–3579. doi: 10.1002/cne.22724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lesser M.P., Carleton K.L., Böttger S.A., Barry T.M., Walker C.W. Sea urchin tube feet are photosensory organs that express a rhabdomeric-like opsin and pax6. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2011;278:3371–3379. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ullrich-Lüter E.M., Dupont S., Arboleda E., Hausen H., Arnone M.I. Unique system of photoreceptors in sea urchin tube feet. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:8367–8372. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018495108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Woodley J.D. In: Echinoderms: Proceedings of the International Conference, Tampa Bay. Lawrence J.M., editor. Balkema; 1982. Photosensitivity of Diadema antillarum: does it show scototaxis? p. 61. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Burke R.D., Angerer L.M., Elphick M.R., Humphrey G.W., Yaguchi S., Kiyama T., Liang S., Mu X., Agca C., Klein W.H., et al. A genomic view of the sea urchin nervous system. Dev. Biol. 2006;300:434–460. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2006.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Land M.F., Nilsson D.-E. 2nd ed. Oxford University Press; 2012. Animal Eyes. Oxford Animal Biology Series. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Georgopoulos A.P., Kalaska J.F., Caminiti R., Massey J.T. On the relations between the direction of two-dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 1982;2:1527–1537. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-11-01527.1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Salinas E., Abbott L.F. Vector reconstruction from firing rates. J. Comput. Neurosci. 1994;1:89–107. doi: 10.1007/BF00962720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cobb J.L. The significance of the radial nerve cords in asteroids and echinoids. Z. Zellforsch. Mikrosk. Anat. 1970;108:457–474. doi: 10.1007/BF00339653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Formery L., Orange F., Formery A., Yaguchi S., Lowe C.J., Schubert M., Croce J.C. Neural anatomy of echinoid early juveniles and comparison of nervous system organization in echinoderms. J. Comp. Neurol. 2021;529:1135–1156. doi: 10.1002/cne.25012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kawaguti S. Electron microscopic structures of the podial wall of an echinoid with special references to the nerve plexus and the muscle. Biol. J. Okayama Univ. 1964;10 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Florey E., Cahill M.A. Cholinergic motor control of sea urchin tube feet: evidence for chemical transmission without synapses. J. Exp. Biol. 1980;88:281–292. doi: 10.1242/jeb.88.1.281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Matheson T. Encyclopedia of Life Sciences. Macmillan Publishers Ltd, Nature Publishing Group; 2002. Invertebrate nervous systems. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ortega A., Olivares-Bañuelos T.N. Neurons and glia cells in marine invertebrates: an update. Front. Neurosci. 2020;14:121. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Albright T.D. Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area mt of the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 1984;52:1106–1130. doi: 10.1152/jn.1984.52.6.1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Maunsell J.H., Van Essen D.C. Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. i. selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. J. Neurophysiol. 1983;49:1127–1147. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.49.5.1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Britten K.H., Shadlen M.N., Newsome W.T., Movshon J.A. Responses of neurons in macaque mt to stochastic motion signals. Vis. Neurosci. 1993;10:1157–1169. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800010269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Fanini A., Assad J.A. Direction selectivity of neurons in the macaque lateral intraparietal area. J. Neurophysiol. Jan 2009;101:289–305. doi: 10.1152/jn.00400.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kalaska J.F., Caminiti R., Georgopoulos A.P. Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 1983;51:247–260. doi: 10.1007/BF00237200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dale. J. Coordination of chemosensory orientation in the starfish asterias forbesi. Mar. Freshw. Behav. Physiol. 1999;32:57–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Pentreath V.W., Cobb J.L. Neurobiology of echinodermata. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 1972;47:363–392. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185x.1972.tb00977.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Paganos P., Ullrich-Lüter E., Caccavale F., Zakrzewski A., Voronov D., Fournon-Berodia I., Cocurullo M., Lüter C., Arnone M.I. A new model organism to investigate extraocular photoreception: opsin and retinal gene expression in the sea urchin paracentrotus lividus. Cells. 2022;11:2636. doi: 10.3390/cells11172636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Millott N., Yoshida M. The shadow reaction of diadema antillarum philippi. II inhibition by light. J. Exp. Biol. 1960;37:376–389. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Takahashi K. Eletrical responses to light stimuli in the isolated radial nerve of the sea urchin, Diadema setosum (leske) Nature. 1964;201:1343–1344. doi: 10.1038/2011343a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Millott N., Okumura H. The electrical activity of the radial nerve in diadema antillarum philippi and certain other echinoids. J. Exp. Biol. 1968;48:279–287. doi: 10.1242/jeb.48.2.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Pouget A., Dayan P., Zemel R. Information processing with population codes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2000;1:125–132. doi: 10.1038/35039062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Barral J., D Reyes A. Synaptic scaling rule preserves excitatory-inhibitory balance and salient neuronal network dynamics. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1690–1696. doi: 10.1038/nn.4415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Document S1. Figures S1–S4, Tables S1–S3, and Data S1–S6
mmc1.pdf (613.4KB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

  • This study did not generate new resource data.

  • All original code has been deposited at GitHub and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOIs are listed in the key resources table.

  • Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.


Articles from iScience are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES