Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2023 Mar 22;18(3):e0281072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0281072

Survival of the fittest in the pandemic age: Introducing disease-related social Darwinism

Paul Nachtwey 1,¤,#, Eva Walther 1,*,#
Editor: Peter Karl Jonason2
PMCID: PMC10032491  PMID: 36947535

Abstract

COVID-19 was a harsh reminder that diseases are an aspect of human existence and mortality. It was also a live experiment in the formation and alteration of disease-related attitudes. Not only are these attitudes relevant to an individual’s self-protective behavior, but they also seem to be associated with social and political attitudes more broadly. One of these attitudes is Social Darwinism, which holds that a pandemic benefits society by enabling nature “to weed out the weak”. In two countries (N = 300, N = 533), we introduce and provide evidence for the reliability, validity, and usefulness of the Disease-Related Social Darwinism (DRSD) Short Scale measuring this concept. Results indicate that DRSD is meaningful related to other central political attitudes like Social Dominance Orientation, Authoritarianism and neoliberalism. Importantly, the scale significantly predicted people’s protective behavior during the Pandemic over and above general social Darwinism. Moreover, it significantly predicted conservative attitudes, even after controlling for Social Dominance Orientation.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has not only painfully reminded us that diseases are part of a human’s life and death, but was also a real-time experiment in disease-related attitude formation and change. These attitudes are not only relevant to an individual’s behavior but were a source of great political polarization in different countries around the world. This polarization was, for example, evident in politicians’ communication [1]. While some politicians around the world appealed to people’s sense of community and solidarity in order to fight the pandemic, others seemed to prefer to let the virus run its course in hopes of herd immunity and better economic outcomes. The Lt. Governor of Texas, for example, suggested that the elderly should risk their lives in order not to ruin America [2]. That is, he implied a willingness to sacrifice the “weaker” people for an alleged benefit to society. In other words, he seemed to refer to an attitude relatively unnoticed in Social Psychology so far: Social Darwinism.

Social Darwinism

Social Darwinists apply Darwin’s biological theory of natural selection to the evolution of human society. Hence, society is perceived as an organism that evolves through the process of natural selection [3]. A central idea of social Darwinism is expressed by the term “survival of the fittest”, introduced by Herbert Spencer as early as 1852 [4]. Accordingly, “for the good of the species, nature should be allowed to weed out the weak” [5 p1141], in other words: the struggle for resources should be maximized as a way of getting rid of the weaker members of society, which allows the naturally strong to thrive [6]. A key element of this thinking is that natural selection equals progress. Consequently, social Darwinists reject governmental interventions intended to strengthen the weak or to create social equality, as such attempts are seen as a threat to societal progress [7]. Hence, inequality is seen as both, inevitable and preferable for people holding social Darwinist attitudes [5].

Social Darwinism has influenced politics ever since the idea was first popularized by Spencer. Martin [8], for example, argued that social Darwinism was partly responsible for the justification of a laissez-faire approach to charity and social welfare throughout the 19th and 20th century in the US. Moreover, both Leyva [7] and Tienken [9] stated that certain educational reforms in the US (the No Child Left Behind Act) have been influenced by social Darwinist ideas packaged as neoliberalism. Leyva [7] extended this argument, stating that social Darwinism has constantly resurfaced in neoliberal economics and free-market policies in the US.

Social Darwinism and COVID-19

Thus, although social Darwinism has been widespread in societies for a long time, it can be assumed that the COVID-19 pandemic has increased the prevalence of this thinking. As Strobl [10] pointed out, the extreme right in Austria and Germany used social Darwinist arguments to popularize their ideology in the context of the pandemic. In a study of German anti-vaxxers and anti-maskers, more than 60% showed at least partial agreement with social Darwinist ideas [11]. Analyzing samples from Turkey and the US Kanık et al. [12] investigated the role of social Darwinism and ageism in predicting opposition to policies designed to protect elderly people during the pandemic. They found that social Darwinism predicts lower support for such policies through predicting higher levels of ageism.

Considering that pandemics, like COVID-19, have become increasingly likely over the last years [13,14], it is a vital question how pandemics change and form political attitudes. Even though social Darwinist ideas about diseases seem to have revitalized during the current pandemic, no measurement exists so far that directly assesses disease-related social Darwinism; a specification of general social Darwinist attitudes applied to the realm of diseases. To address this issue, we developed the Disease-Related Social Darwinism (DRSD) Scale, to investigate how a social Darwinist view of diseases relates to people’s health-related and political attitudes and behavior.

Disease-related social Darwinism

As the COVID-19 pandemic has illustrated, diseases are part of a human’s life and death and if they are not properly controlled, they can change the way we live together in a society. However, this is not only the case for diseases like COVID-19 but also for other infectious diseases like for example measles, if not enough children are vaccinated at the right time [1517]. This highlights the importance of people’s disease-related attitudes, not only for the individual’s health behavior but for collective behavior as well. An attitude that seems to have been widespread during the pandemic is DRSD as it frames diseases (especially pandemics) as having a positive function for society.

(Disease-related) social Darwinism and political orientation

During the pandemic differences in social distancing and other disease-related behaviors between groups became evident. Often it seemed, that people on the right of the political spectrum were less concerned about COVID-19 or even outright angry at governments restricting public life to stop its spread [18]. Using smartphone data, Alcott et al. [19], for example, showed that areas in the US with predominantly Republican voters engaged in less social distancing. This effect stayed significant even when controlling for other factors like public policies or population density. Republicans and Democrats also differed in their beliefs about the pandemic. Democrats perceived COVID-19 as more severe and social distancing as more effective than Republicans. Furthermore, majority Republican counties showed higher numbers of additional deaths due to COVID-19 than majority Democratic counties [20], this association also seems to hold on the individual level [21]. We argue that one attitude possibly explaining these differences is DRSD.

As pointed out earlier, a German study of anti-maskers and anti-vaxxers found high degrees of at least partial support for certain social Darwinist ideas [11]. The same study also found that these people were inclined to vote for the right-wing populist party “Alternative für Deutschland” (AfD; [22]) in the 2021 election. In a large representative German sample, social Darwinism was highly correlated with support for a right-wing authoritarian dictatorship [23]. And in a similar vein, a more recent investigation in Germany found that social Darwinism was most strongly endorsed by people identifying as right-wing and by people preferring the AfD [24]. It seems then that social Darwinism is an attitude more strongly pronounced on the right-wing end of the political spectrum, which should also be the case for DRSD as it refers to a specification of social Darwinism.

(Disease-related) social Darwinism and social attitudes

Previous research on social Darwinism has revolved mainly around a) investigating it as part of an extreme right-wing ideology in Germany [23] or b) investigating naïve social Darwinism/Competitive Jungle Beliefs in light of a dual-process model of motivation [25]. However, two recent studies by Saud [26] and Rudman and Saud [5] conceptualized social Darwinism as a central inequality justifying social attitude. The authors defined social Darwinism as the belief “that humans, like plants and animals, are engaged in a ruthless genetic competition” [5 p1141]. Utilizing this concept, Saud [26] found that social Darwinism, was related to prejudice against minorities, Social Dominance Orientation (SDO; [27]), and Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA;[28]). That social Darwinism is related to SDO is not surprising, because it refers to the degree to which an individual prefers that his or her in-group is dominant over other groups in society [27]. Similarly, RWA should be related because it refers to the acceptance of hierarchies, the submission to authorities who uphold them, and aggression against those who oppose them [28]. These associations provide the basis for our understanding of DRSD, as a specific form of social Darwinism, in the context of social and political attitudes. Importantly, Rudman and Saud [5] have shown that social Darwinism explains more variance of three forms of system justification (gender system, race system, economic system) than SDO and biological essentialism [29]. The authors suggested that social Darwinism explains more variance because it provides both a rational explanation for and positive evaluation of social hierarchies, while SDO and biological essentialism provide only one or the other. Rudman and Saud [5] concluded that when individuals believe that differences between people are the result of natural selection, attempts to create equality may be rejected as a threat to human welfare and progress, because, according to this view, inequality as a result of natural selection is both inevitable and preferable.

The role of DRSD during COVID-19

Based on this logic, people with disease-related social Darwinist attitudes should perceive restrictions imposed by the government to stop the spread of COVID-19 as unnecessary or even dangerous because they could be seen as “attempts to level the playing field” [5]. To investigate this hypothesis, we developed the DRSD scale and examined its usefulness in Study 1. In Study 2 (preregistered), we provide further validation and extend the findings to a different sample and broader political context. To our knowledge, there is not yet a measurement directly capturing social Darwinist ideas about diseases. Due to the importance of disease-related attitudes outlined above, we aim to close this gap by introducing the DRSD scale.

Ethics statement

The study was conducted following the 2016 American Psychological Association Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct [30]. As the project did not involve deception, vulnerable populations, identifiable data, intensive data, or interventions, it was exempt from ethical approval at the participating institution.

Study 1

The main goal of Study 1 was to develop the DRSD scale and as such to investigate its internal structure and validity. Regarding the validity of the scale, we expected the DRSD scale to be correlated with the social Darwinism scale [5] (Hypothesis 1a) because we theoretically consider DRSD a specification of the broader construct of social Darwinism applied to diseases. We also predicted that the DRSD scale would be related to SDO and RWA (Hypothesis 1b). DRSD should correlate with SDO, first; because social Darwinism can be seen as a hierarchy-enhancing myth, legitimizing inequality and hierarchies in society. Second, as Rudman and Saud [5] pointed out, SDO and social Darwinism share conceptual roots in biological determinism, which makes them “ideological cousins” (p1141). The DRSD scale should further correlate with authoritarianism because the concept of social Darwinism justifies the acceptance of authoritarian figures. It was further hypothesized that the DRSD scale should be related to neoliberal thinking (Hypothesis 1c). The ideal of an unregulated market is a cornerstone of this ideology [31,32] and has been deeply entwined with social Darwinism throughout history [3335]. Moreover, Azevedo et al. [32] have shown that the endorsement of neoliberal ideology correlates with SDO, which is, as already pointed out, deeply connected to social Darwinism. Furthermore, neoliberalism [32] and social Darwinism [5] can both be seen as justifications for inequality in society.

We also expect that political left-right orientation would predict DRSD (Hypothesis 2) because it has been shown that social Darwinism overlaps with (extreme) right-wing ideologies [23,24,36].

More importantly, however, we also want to show that the scale has incremental validity in comparison to the standard social Darwinism scale when predicting peoples’ protective behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic (Hypothesis 3a). Furthermore, we think that the relationship between party preference and behavior during the pandemic will be moderated by DRSD (Hypothesis 3b). For an overview of our hypotheses see Fig 1.

Fig 1. Hypotheses Study 1.

Fig 1

Method and materials

Participants

This study was conducted as an online study in German. Participants (N = 304) were recruited via several methods. University student groups (some with a political agenda) were contacted via e-mail. Of the 28 contacted groups, 8 agreed to share the link to the survey with their respective groups. The link was also shared on an official e-mail platform of the University and social media (WhatsApp, Instagram, Facebook). We decided to recruit as close to the sample size of Rudman & Saud [5] as possible. We aimed for at least around 300 participants because it has been shown correlation coefficients stabilize around 250 participants [37], and a sample of this size is sufficient for factor analyses with less than 40 variables [38]. A post-hoc sensitivity analysis revealed that our final sample of 300 participants had an 80% power for detecting a minimum change in R2 = .001 (when investigating on predictor added to one or two other predictors). Based on pre-testing of our study we determined that the minimum time it takes to at least read all items would be around 5 minutes, therefore, four participants were excluded from the analysis as they took less than 5 minutes to complete the survey. The median time participants took to complete the survey was 13.63 minutes.

Disease-related social Darwinism

If not indicated otherwise, participants gave their response on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” (1) to “strongly agree” (7).

DRSD was assessed using a 24-item scale. Items were developed by the authors based on Rudman and Saud [5]and other sources [35,39,40]. An example of an item reads as: “Diseases are a natural mechanism to strengthen our immune system. It would be better for our future if we wouldn’t try to prevent this” (for final scale and item statistics see online supplemental materials). The items were presented in a randomized order. Responses were averaged such that higher scores reflect stronger attitudes of DRSD. Because 5 of the final 14 items directly referred to rapidly changing official COVID-19 countermeasures, we dropped these items from further analysis. We used the remaining 9-item DRSD short scale for further analyses (for final short scale: α = .95, M = 2.99, SD = 1.63; more detailed descriptive statistics for relevant scales can be found in the online supplemental materials). Results, however, did not differ if the full 14-item scale was used. We believe that the high internal consistency of our scale provides evidence for the reliability of the scale.

Social Darwinism

We used a translation of the scale from Rudman and Saud [5]. The scale consists of 8 items (example item: “The fittest members of our society naturally rise to the top”). Responses were averaged such that higher scores reflected stronger endorsement of social Darwinist thinking (α = .88, M = 2.86, SD = 1.29).

Social Dominance Orientation (SDO)

Preference for social hierarchy was measured using a German translation of the SDO7(s) scale from Ho et al. [41, Eckerle personal communication] (sample item: “an ideal society requires some groups to be at the top while others are at the bottom”). Responses to the 8 items were reversed coded when needed and then averaged such that higher scores indicated a stronger preference for inequality between groups in society (α = .83, M = 2.56, SD = 1.08).

Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA)

RWA was assessed using the KSA-3 scale [42]. The KSA-3 is a German scale to measure authoritarianism based on the three subdimensions of authoritarianism proposed by, for example, Altemeyer [28]: authoritarian aggression (general aggression against others sanctioned by authority figures), authoritarian submissiveness (submission to authority figures and general acceptance of their statements and actions) and conventionalism (strong obedience to established societal conventions). The scale has been proven useful in a German sample [42]. Responses were averaged such that higher scores indicated stronger authoritarian tendencies (α = .82, M = 3.07, SD = 1.04).

Neoliberal ideology

We measured neoliberal ideology using items of two subscales from Groß and Hövermann [43]: entrepreneurial universalism (orig: unternehmerischer Universalismus; a generalized version of neoliberal self-optimization) and competitive ideology (orig: Wettbewerbsideologie; the idea that in order to make progress in society, there needs to be omnipresent competition). The entrepreneurial universalism scale consists of three items (sample item: “If someone is not willing to try something new it’s his own fault if he fails”), theescribtive ideology scale of two (sample item: “The key to success is to be better than everyone else”). Responses were averaged so that higher scores reflected a stronger endorsement of neoliberal ideology (α = .85, M = 3.69, SD = 1.37).

Behavior and attitudes during the COVID-19 pandemic

Two items were administered to measure whether participants took precautionary measures and followed official recommendations and guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. Participants were asked to indicate how strongly the two statements applied to themselves on a scale ranging from “does not apply at all” (1) to “applies completely” (7) (sample Item: “I follow the official guidelines regarding COVID-19”). Based on the high correlation (r = .831, p < .001), responses were averaged so that higher scores indicated more precaution and endorsement of official rules (α = .91, M = 5.58, SD = 1.42). We also administered one item to measure the attitudes of people regarding the official rules and policies concerning COVID-19. People were asked to indicate if they felt that the measures taken by the government were an overreaction on a scale ranging from “does not apply at all” (1) to “applies completely” (7) (M = 3.60, SD = 2.09).

Political orientation

Political orientation was measured using three items. Participants were asked to locate themselves on a scale from “very left” (1) to “very right (7) (M = 3.26, SD = 1.29), and they were also asked to rank themselves on a scale ranging from “very liberal” (1) to “very conservative” (7) (M = 2.97, SD = 1.28). Furthermore, we asked participants with which political party they could best identify (see Table 1). The parties were coded from most left (1) to most right (11); the options “other” and “none” were coded as (12) and (13). The left vs. right scale showed high correlation with party preference when people selecting “other” or “none” were excluded from analysis, justifying our classification of the parties (r = .706, p < .001, N = 236).

Table 1. Party Preference Study 1.
Party N Percent
Alliance 90/The Greens (“Bündnis 90 die Grünen“, Green politics) 82 27.3
None 49 16.3
Christian Democratic Union of Germany (“CDU”, liberal conservatism) 42 14
The Left (“Die Linke”, democratic socialism) 28 9.3
Free Democratic Party (“FDP”, classical liberalism) 28 9.3
Other 15 5
Social Democratic Party of Germany (“SPD”, social democracy) 14 4.7
Alternative for Germany (“AFD”, right-wing populism) 12 4
“Die Basis” (new party emerging from anti-vaxxer/masker communities) 10 3.3

All other options were favored by less than 10 participants and are thus not mentioned. One person mentioned one party first and then “Die Basis” as a political group; this person was counted as a voter for the first party mentioned.

Demographics

Participants were asked to indicate if they were currently enrolled at a university; 51.0% (N = 153) were not. Furthermore, participants were asked to specify their gender (N = 162, 54.0% identified as female; N = 136, 45.3% as male, and N = 2 or .7% as diverse) and age (M = 37.73, SD = 18.72). We also asked participants to indicate whether their mother tongue was German, with 97.7% (N = 293) indicating that their mother tongue was German.

Procedure

After clicking on the link to the survey, participants were greeted and thanked for their participation. Following this, the measurements of DRSD, social Darwinism, SDO, RWA, and neoliberal ideology were presented in a randomized order on separate pages. At the end of the survey, participants’ demographics were assessed. Afterward, participants were thanked and asked to share the survey with friends and family (for scales and instructions see online supplemental materials).

Results

If not indicated otherwise, analyses for both studies were performed using SPSS version 26 [44].

Factor analysis

We conducted an exploratory factor analysis with oblimin rotation (varimax rotation yielded nearly identical results) of the 24 original items of the DRSD scale. Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2 (276) = 6061.40, p < .001), providing justification for the factor analysis. The scree-plot clearly suggested a one-factor solution (Fig 2). This factor explained 55.92% of variance. 14 of the 24 original items loaded highly only on this one factor (s. online supplemental materials for factor loadings). Because five items referred to rapidly changing anti-COVID measures, these items were excluded from further analyses, so that the final analyses rested on a 9-item short version of the scale. However, results did not differ significantly when the full scale was used. The analyses with the full 14-item scale are reported in the online supplemental material.

Fig 2. Scree-plot of exploratory factor analysis.

Fig 2

We also calculated an additional exploratory factor analysis on the social Darwinism scale and the 9-item DRSD-scale to investigate their relationship. Two highly correlated (r = .534) factors with Eigenvalues higher than 1 emerged. The 9 DRSD items loaded on factor 1, while the items of the social Darwinism scale loaded on factor 2. These results support our conceptualization of DRSD as a structurally distinct part of general social Darwinism.

Correlations of disease-related social Darwinism

To test Hypotheses 1 a)–c), correlations between relevant variables were calculated (see, Table 2). Participants who scored higher on the DRSD scale also scored higher on the social Darwinism scale (r = .606, p < .001). Furthermore, participants with higher DRSD scores exhibited higher values of SDO (r = .473, p < .001), RWA (r = .217, p < .001), and neoliberal thinking (r = .436, p < .001).

Table 2. Correlations Study 1.
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. DRSD -
2. SDO .473** -
3. RWA .217** .461** -
4. Left/Right .459** .543** .536** -
5. Liberal/Conservative .378** .397** .420** .605** -
6. Social Darwinism .606** .681** .552** .580** .395** -
7. Neoliberalism .436** .599** .545** .624** .394** .695** -
8. Protective Behavior -.629** -.269** .058 -.193** -.163** -.361** -.199** -

N = 300

** p < .001.

To provide further evidence in support of our validity analyses, we performed a Harman’s single factor test to investigate potential common method bias. The resulting single factor explained about 32% of variance, suggesting that common method bias is not a concern.

Predicting disease-related social Darwinism

To test Hypothesis 2, a linear regression with the left-right self-assessment as predictor and DRSD as the dependent variable was conducted. Participants’ identification on the left-right axis significantly predicted DRSD (β = .459 t (298) = 8.919, p < .001), indicating that with increasing self-identification as politically right-wing, endorsement of DRSD also increased. The left-right axis explained about 21.1% of variance of the DRSD scale. A linear regression with party preference as the predictor yielded similar results (people who chose “other” or “none” were excluded from this analysis, N = 236); participants who preferred more right-wing parties tended to show higher scores of DRSD (see Table 3). We decided to calculate two separate regression analyses for left-right self-identification and party preference because even though the dimensions are correlated, someone identifying as more right-wing could prefer libertarian (economically right-wing), traditionalist/conservative (moderate right-wing) or populist right-wing parties. Hence, calculating two separate regression analyses should provide a more detailed picture.

Table 3. Regression analyses predicting DRSD.
Dependent Variable Predictors β B 95% CI R2 R2adj
DRSD Left/Right .459 .580 .452, .708 .211 .208
Party Pref. .466 .256 .194, .319 .217 .214

N = 300, NPartyPref. = 236.

Predicting behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic

To test Hypothesis 3 a), a hierarchical regression with self-reported behavior during the pandemic as the dependent variable was conducted. In Step 1, participants’ score on the social Darwinism scale was added as the only predictor. In Step 2, DRSD was added as the second predictor. Model 1 was significant (F (1, 298) = 44.787, p < .001); people with higher scores on the social Darwinism scale showed less precautionary behavior during the pandemic. Social Darwinism explained approximately 13.1% of variance in behavior. Model 2 was also significant (F (2, 297) = 97.613, p < .001) and explained about 39.7% of variance, which represents a significant increase of roughly 26.6% explained variance (change in F (1, 297) = 130.915, p < .001) through the addition of DRSD as a predictor (see Table 4 for all analyses predicting protective behavior).

Table 4. Regression analyses predicting protective behavior.
Dependent Variable Predictors β B 95% CI R2 R2adj Tolerance
Protective Behavior SD -.361 -.398 -.515, -.281 .131 .128
SD
DRSD
.032
-.648
.035
-.564
-.088, .157
-.662, -.467

.397

.393
.633
.633
Protective Behavior Left/Right
DRSD
Interaction
.291
-.416
-.389
.321
-.363
-.065
.111, .530
-.603, -.122
-.127, -.003

.416

.410
.210
.100
.055
Party Pref.
DRSD
Interaction
.352
-.222
-.637
.169
-.193
-.054
.063, .275
-.421, .034
-.086, -.022

.392

.385
.208
.150
.072
Left/Right ©
DRSD ©
Interaction
.115
-.659
-.094
.127
-.574
-.065
.018, .235
-.662, -.485
-.127, -.003

.416

.410
.786
.741
.935
Party Pref. ©
DRSD ©
Interaction
.015
-.638
-.173
.007
-.557
-.054
-.047, .062
-.657, -.457
-.086, -.022

.392

.385
.777
.774
.966

N = 300, NPartyPref. = 236, mean-centered predictors are marked with ©, for these regression analyses the interaction is the product of the mean-centered predictors.

To test Hypothesis 3 b), we computed an interaction variable as the product of party preference and DRSD, which was then used as one of the predictors in a multiple linear regression. The other two were DRSD and party preference (participants who chose the options “other” or “none” were excluded from this analysis). The dependent variable was self-reported behavior during the pandemic. The regression model was significant (F (3, 232) = 49.952, p < .001), explaining 39.2% of variance. The interaction variable was a significant predictor (β = -.637; t (232) = -3.328, p = .001). The negative values are obviously due to the coding of behavior. Introducing the interaction variable in a second step results in a significant increase in explained variance of 2.9% (Change in F(1, 232) = 11.078, p = .001). We centered the predictor variables and calculated the same regression analyses again to make the main effects interpretable. The interaction effect was obviously still significant (β = -.173; t (232) = -3.328, p = .001). DRSD was a significant negative predictor of protective behavior during the pandemic (β = -.638; t (232) = -10.964, p < .001), while party-preference was not (β = .015; t (232) = .266, p = .790). Conducting the same analysis with the person’s left-right identification instead of party-preference (N = 300) yields very similar results; the interaction is also significant (p = .042). This indicates that for people preferring more right-wing parties (and identifying as further right) the negative effect of DRSD on protective behavior during the pandemic is more pronounced.

Discussion

Study 1 revealed encouraging results regarding the DRSD scale. The short DRSD scale showed great reliability (α = .95) and validity as it related to similar political attitudes. As expected, DRSD increased with a higher preference for the political right. Importantly for the current purpose, DRSD was found to be related but not identical to general social Darwinism. Furthermore, the substantial correlations with social Darwinism and SDO, as well as the high inter-item correlations (s. online supplemental material) provide further justification for our constructed items. Moreover, the findings support the notion that DRSD is one of the attitudes formed or changed during the pandemic that not only explains people’s health-related behavior but also relates to a broader societal/political dimension. To corroborate these findings, we conducted a second, high-powered and preregistered study in a different cultural context using a representative sample.

Study 2

As pointed out in the introduction, we conceptualized social Darwinism and hence DRSD in the context of social and political attitudes. Therefore, to establish validity, we think it is very important to test our scale in a broader political context and in association with broader political attitudes. Furthermore, we think that in the “pandemic age” disease-related attitudes are gaining importance and thus also relate to broader political attitudes. Perceived threat due to COVID-19, for example, has been associated with more anti-Asian prejudice [45] and moderated the effects of RWA on nationalism and anti-immigrant attitudes during the pandemic [46]. Hence, Study 2 was designed to locate DRSD in the broader political context and investigate its associations with broader political attitudes. It was part of a larger investigation using a representative US sample. Our main goal was to replicate and extend our findings in a preregistered large study.

Based on Study 1, we expected DRSD to be correlated with RWA, SDO (Hypothesis 1a), and the reactivated F-scale (Hypothesis 1b), which was included as another measurement of authoritarianism. Moreover, we expected DRSD to correlate with participants’ sympathy for republican politicians and their self-identification as more right-wing and conservative (Hypothesis 1c). Most importantly, we hypothesized that DRSD would predict political attitudes classified as more conservative, even when SDO (a related concept, which has been shown to predict political conservatism; [47]) is controlled for (Hypothesis 2).

A relatively recent attempt to explain why people support right-wing populist politicians and parties has focused on the idea of relative need deprivation [22]. The theory posits that globalization and modernization have led to feelings of relative deprivation of certain psychological needs (e.g., for certainty or a coherent identity). Right-wing populists seem to signal opportunities to cope with those deprived needs. We exploratively assessed DRSD in relation to relative need deprivation [21], with the expectation that frustrated needs would predict higher sympathy for republican candidates (Hypothesis 3a). Moreover, we hypothesized that this relationship would be moderated by DRSD since social Darwinism is often associated with right-wing attitudes and ideologies (see Study 1, Hypothesis 3b). Additionally, policies like the No Child Left Behind Act implemented by Republicans have been described as social Darwinist [7,9], the same goes for Donald Trump’s worldview [48], so people with frustrated psychological needs should be even more inclined to sympathize with Republican politicians (like Trump) when they simultaneously show high scores of DRSD. We expected this interaction to be stronger in people with stronger specific relative deprivation (Hypothesis 3c).

Method and materials

Participants

The study was conducted between 28.01.2022 and 30.01.2020, using Prolific. The sample was representative of the US population in terms of age, gender, and ethnicity. In total 601 participants were recruited. Participants were asked if they were familiar with 10 politicians presented to them (8 real, 2 fake). As described in the pre-registration, participants were excluded if they indicated to be familiar with one or both fake politicians. This would indicate either a lack of knowledge about US politics or a lack of concentration. Both would have negative effects on the validity of our dependent variable. On this basis, 68 participants were excluded; hence, the final sample consisted of N = 533 (Mage = 45.75, SD = 16.2). N = 266 identified as female, while N = 7 either indicated they identified as another gender or did not indicate gender at all. 72% of the sample identified as White/Caucasian, 12.6% as African-American, 6.6% as Asian and 4.3% as Hispanic. A post-hoc sensitivity analysis revealed that our final sample of 533 participants had an 80% power to detect a minimum change in R2 < .001 (when adding one predictor to one or two other predictors). Again, as in Study 1, this sample can be considered sufficiently big in terms of stability of correlation coefficients [37].

Disease-related social Darwinism

If not indicated otherwise participants gave their response on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 6 (“strongly agree”).

DRSD was assessed using a translated version of the 14-item scale from Study 1. The scale was translated by the authors and checked by native speakers (s. online supplemental materials). Items were presented in a fixed-randomized order. Responses were averaged so that higher scores indicated a stronger endorsement of DRSD. For the same reasons as in Study 1, we used the short version of the scale (results using the full scale are reported in the online supplemental materials). A confirmatory factor analysis indicated that one item was not useful in measuring DRSD (We think the translation was sub-optimal), so it was excluded from further analyses. Items were presented in a fixed-randomized order (For short 8-item scale: α = .96; M = 2.36; SD = 1.37).

Right-Wing Authoritarianism

RWA was assessed using the revised, short version of the RWA scale proposed by Zakrisson [49]. Items were presented in a fixed-randomized order. Responses were reverse-coded when needed and averaged so that higher scores indicated a stronger endorsement of authorities (α = .89; M = 2.7; SD = .95).

Authoritarianism

To investigate all facets of authoritarianism, we also used the reactivated F-scale [50]. This scale was designed to measure the remaining 6 subscales of the original F-scale [51] not captured with RWA. The scale consists of 20 items, which were presented in a fixed-randomized order. Items were averaged so that higher scores indicated higher approval of authoritarianism (α = .91; M = 2.97; SD = .89).

Social Dominance Orientation

Preference for social hierarchies was measured using the same scale as in study 1 [41]. Items were presented in a fixed-randomized order. Responses to the 8 items were reverse coded when needed and then averaged so that higher scores indicated a stronger preference for inequality between groups in society (α = .91; M = 2.09; SD = 1.09).

Relative deprivation

Need deprivation was measured using 36 items consisting of two subscales: general and specific need deprivation with 18 items each. Both subscales are comprised of three scales representing the three types of psychological needs that can be deprived (existential needs, epistemic needs, and identity needs, compare [52]) each measured using 6 items. Items were presented in a fixed-randomized order. Items were reverse-coded as needed and then averaged so that higher scores reflected more strongly frustrated needs (αgeneral = .83, Mgeneral = 3.71, SDgeneral = .7; αspecific = .94, Mspecific = 3.06, SDspecific = 1.12). The distinction between specific and general need deprivation was not pre-registered, because their relation was unknown at that time.

Political orientation

Several different measures were used to assess political orientation and behavior. Participants were asked to indicate their position on a scale ranging from left to right (M = 2.93, SD = 1.41) and on a scale ranging from liberal to conservative (M = 2.94, SD = 1.44). Preference for political parties was assessed separately for the Democratic Party and the Republican Party (MDem = 3.48, SDDem = 1.55; MRep = 2.39, SDRep = 1.5), and participants were asked who they voted for in the 2020 presidential election (NBiden = 298, NTrump = 99, Nnon-voter = 108).

Furthermore, participants were asked to indicate if they were familiar with 10 politicians (4 Democrats, 4 Republicans and 2 fake politicians), and then to indicate how strongly they agreed with the views of these politicians (politicians were not presented as options if participants indicated they did not know them). A preference score was calculated as Preference = (Omar + Ocasio-Cortez)–(Trump + Taylor Greene) + 10, resulting in a scale ranging from 0 (strong preference for Republicans) to 20 (strong preference for Democrats; M = 14.07, SD = 5.84). We have decided to calculate the preference for politicians score in this way because we think that it helps us cover the whole political spectrum and thus provide a more nuanced picture. By using more “extreme” politicians for each party, we cannot only differentiate between the moderate center but also between people more on the “extreme” ends of the political spectrum. A scale consisting of 17 political statements was used to measure people’s political views. Items were reverse-coded when needed and averaged so that higher scores reflected more conservative political views (α = .94; M = 2.32; SD = 1.18). All items were presented in a fixed-randomized order.

Demographic variables

In addition to the demographic variables automatically provided by Prolific, we assessed people’s age, gender, ethnicity and their highest level of completed education.

Other instruments

There were other measures used in this study that are mentioned here but are not relevant to the hypotheses but that may, however, be used for exploratory analyses. A scale consisting of 12 items measuring left-wing attitudes was assessed (α = .86; M = 3.56; SD = .94) and participants were told that they could choose a nonprofit organization to which $100 would then be donated. They had three options: NAACP (National Association for the Advancement of Colored People; N = 172), American Red Cross (N = 328), NRA (National Rifle Association; N = 33). All items were presented in a fixed-randomized order.

Procedure

Participants were welcomed and thanked for their participation before they received some general information about the study (topic, duration, data protection, etc.). On the same page, participants were also informed that after completing the survey they would be given the chance to donate $100 to one of three organizations. Following this, participants had to provide their Prolific ID, after which the reactivated F-scale, the left-wing attitudes scale, RWA and SDO were presented with items in a fixed-randomized order. On the following page, the frustrated needs scales were presented followed by items concerning political views. Participants were asked if they were familiar with 10 politicians (Biden, Ocasio-Cortez, Omar, Sanders, Trump, Taylor Green, Romney, Liz Cheney & two fake politicians). The following items (how strongly do you agree with the views of the following people) were only presented for those politicians that participants indicated they were familiar with. Party preference and voting behavior were assessed on the next page. The political attitude scale and the DRSD scale followed on separate pages. Afterward, participants had a choice between three organizations to which $100 would be donated to and finally, demographics were administered on the final page of the survey.

Results

Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA)

We conducted confirmatory Factor Analysis using the package “lavaan” [53] in R [54] for the 14-item scale and the 9-item short scale to test for a one-factor solution (results for the 14-item scale can be found in the online supplemental materials). Cutoff values for Criteria of fit were based on Hu and Bentler [55] who proposed values close to .95 for CLI and TLI. Following Kenny et al. [56], we think that in our case RMSEA values should not be considered too important, since RMSEA tends to penalize models with lower df with a positive bias. Considering that our model only had one factor with few variables, we think a focus on CFI and TLI is justified. Nevertheless, we report RMSEA values for transparency. The 9-item short scale showed a relatively good fit to a one-factor solution (CFI = .962, TLI = .949, RMSEA = .112), however, one item did not seem to fit the factor (this was the case for both the 14-item and short scale), so it was excluded from further analyses. The 8 remaining items showed good fit to a one-factor solution (CFI = .974, TLI = .963, RMSEA = .105). The SRMR for both models looked proper according to Hu and Bentler [55], with SRMR = .031 for the 9-item model and even better for the 8-item model with SRMR = .021. Overall, the CFA supported the notion of the scale as a one-dimensional measurement.

Correlates of DRSD

To test Hypotheses 1 a)-c), correlations between the relevant variables were calculated (see Table 5). Correlations indicated again that participants scoring higher on the DRSD scale also showed higher scores of RWA (r = .524, p < .001) and SDO (r = .598, p < .001). The same can be said for the reactivated f-scale (r = .676, p < 001). Participants exhibiting higher scores of DRSD also showed a stronger preference for Republican politicians (r = -.760, p < .001; the negative correlation is obviously due to the calculation of the preference score), self-identification as more right (r = .591, p < .001) and more conservative (r = .587, p < .001). Hypotheses 1 a)-c) were supported.

Table 5. Correlations Study 2.
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. DRSD -
2. SDO .598** -
3. RWA .524** .551** -
4. Left/Right .591** .609** .667** -
5. Liberal/Conservative .587** .606** .682** .943** -
6. F-Scale .676** .651** .802** .668** .675** -
7. Politician Preference -.760** -.676** -.648** -.834** -.827** -.708** -

N = 533

NPolPref = 322

** p < .001, Politician Preference was coded such that lower scores indicate a stronger preference for Republican politicians.

Again, to provide further evidence in support of our validity analyses, we performed a Harman’s single factor test to investigate potential common method bias. The resulting single factor explained about 32% of variance, suggesting that common method bias is not a concern.

Predicting conservative political orientation

To test Hypothesis 2, we conducted a linear regression with DRSD and SDO as the predictors and the 17-item political attitudes scale as the dependent variable. The model was significant, explaining 62.4% of variance (F (2, 530) = 439.542, p < .001). SDO (β = .467; t (530) = 14.039, p < .001) and DRSD (β = .417; t (530) = 12.533, p < .001) were both significant predictors of conservative political orientation. When this is calculated as a hierarchical regression with DRSD being added in step 2, the addition of DRSD as a predictor explains a significant 11.1% of variance in addition to SDO (change in F (1, 530) = 157.088, p < .001). We, therefore, found confirming evidence for Hypothesis 2 (for all regression analyses see Table 6).

Table 6. Regression Analyses predicting conservative attitudes.
Dependent Variable Predictors β B 95% CI R2 R2adj Tolerance
Conservative Attitudes SDO .716 .776 .712, .841 .512 .511
SDO
DRSD
.467
.417
.506
.359
.435, .577
.303, .416

.624

.622
.642
.642

N = 533.

Predicting preference for politicians

To test Hypothesis 3 a) we calculated two linear regressions. The predictor was general or specific frustrated needs. General frustrated needs significantly predicted politician preference (β = .176; t (320) = 3.201, p = .002), as did Specific frustrated needs (β = -.799; t (320) = -23.767, p < .001). Hypothesis 3 a) was partly supported. To test Hypotheses 3 b) and c) we calculated interaction variables of DRSD and specific/general need deprivation as the product of the respective two predictors. We performed linear regressions with three predictors: DRSD, general (Model 1) or specific (Model 2) need deprivation, and the corresponding interaction variable. DRSD was a significant predictor in both models (Model 1: β = -1.076; t (318) = -5.750, p < .001; Model 2: β = -.212; t (318) = -1.978, p = .049), general need deprivation were not (β = .032; t (318) = .483, p = .629), specific needs deprivation were (β = -.420; t (318) = -5.464, p < .001). The interaction variable was not significant in both models (Model 1: β = .339; t (318) = 1.766, p = .078; Model 2: β = -.252; t (318) = -1.659, p = .098; when calculated with the 14-item scale the interaction of DRSD and general frustrated needs was significant (s. online supplemental materials)). When added in an extra step the interaction variable explained 0.4% of additional variance in Model 1 and 0.3% of additional variance in Model 2. Hypotheses 3 b) and c) were not supported.

Because some of the betas are higher than 1 which is most likely due to collinearity being high, we decided to recalculate the regression analysis with mean-centered variables. Therefore, we mean-centered DRSD, general frustrated needs and specific frustrated needs before calculating their interaction variables as described above. These analyses were not pre-registered since we did not expect such high collinearity. Again, DRSD was a significant predictor in both models (Model 1: β = —.754; t (318) = -21.25, p < .001; Model 2: β = -.353; t (318) = -7.514, p < .001). General need deprivation (β = .140; t (318) = 3.884, p < .001), and specific needs deprivation were both significant (β = -.510; t (318) = -11.065, p < .001). The interaction variable was not significant in both models (Model 1: β = .064; t (318) = 1.766, p = .078; Model 2: β = -.060; t (318) = -1.659, p = .098). See Table 7 for detailed results.

Table 7. Regression Analyses predicting politician preference.
Dependent Variable Predictors β B 95% CI R2 R2adj Tolerance
Politician Preference Frustrated Needs (gen) .176 1.454 .560, 2.347 .031 .028
Frustrated Needs (spec) -.799 -4.240 -4.591, -3.889 .638 .637
Frustrated Needs (gen)
DRSD
Interaction
.032
-1.076
.339
.262
-4.709
.380
-.806, 1.330
-6.320, -3.097
-.043, .804

.598

.594
.292
.036
.034
Frustrated Needs (spec)
DRSD
Interaction
-.420
-.212
-.252
-2.230
-.927
-.202
-3.033, -1.427
-1.848, -.005
-.441, .037

.704

.701
.157
.081
.040
Frustrated needs (gen) (C)
DRSD (C)
Interaction
.140
-.754
.064
1.159
-3.298
.380
.572, 1.747
-3.605, -2.991
-.043, .804

.598

.594
.967
.995
.968
Frustrated needs (spec) (C)
DRSD (C)
Interaction
-.510
-.353
-.060
-2.705
-1.543
-.202
-3.186, -2.224
-1.947, -1.139
-.441, .037

.704

.701
.439
.423
.722

NPolPref = 322; Politician Preference was coded such that lower scores indicate a stronger preference for Republican politicians, mean-centered predictors are marked with ©, for these regression analyses the interaction is the product of the mean-centered predictors.

Explorative analyses

The following analyses are not mentioned in the pre-registration as they comprise exploratory investigations of our data and should be interpreted as such. The measurements we use, however, are mentioned in the pre-registration and the Measures section of Study 2.

To investigate the role of DRSD in participants’ self-reported voting behavior in the 2020 presidential election we performed a binominal logistical regression with DRSD as the predictor. The dependent variable was recoded so that voting for Donald Trump (1; N = 99) was compared to voting for Joe Biden (0, N = 298), while other answers to the original item were coded as missing. The regression model was significant (χ2 (1) = 133.463, p < .001). The model explained between 28.6% (Cox & Snell R2) and 42.3% (Nagelkerkes R2) of variance. The model had an overall percentage of accurate classification of 82.9%, with a sensitivity of 51.5% and specificity of 93.3%. DRSD significantly predicted voting for Donald Trump (p < .001), leading to a higher probability of voting for Donald Trump (vs. Joe Biden; OR = 3.031). We calculated the same binominal logistical regression and controlled for RWA and SDO as additional predictors. All correlations of the predictors were lower than r = .8, indicating that multicollinearity was not a confounding factor in this model [57]. We added RWA and SDO in the first step and DRSD in the second step. Adding DRSD to the model increased the overall percentage of accurate classification by 2.5% and increased sensitivity by 7.1%. DRSD leads to a higher probability of voting for Donald Trump (vs. Joe Biden; OR = 1.943) even when controlling for RWA and SDO.

We calculated similar binominal logistical regressions with participants’ donation behavior as the dependent variable, coded as 0 (donating to the NAACP, N = 172) and 1 (donating to the NRA, N = 33). The model including just DRSD was significant (χ2 (1) = 112.638, p < .001), explaining between 42.3% (Cox & Snell R2) and 72.1% (Nagelkerkes R2) of variance. The model had an overall percentage of accurate classification of 91.7%, with a sensitivity of 72.7% and specificity of 95.3%. DRSD significantly predicted donating to the NRA (p < .001), leading to a higher probability of donating to the NRA (vs. the NAACP; OR = 6.944). Adding DRSD to RWA and SDO in a second step increased the overall percentage of accurate classification by 3.9% and increased sensitivity by 9.1%. DRSD leads to a higher probability of donating to the NRA (vs. the NAACP; OR = 3.875) even when controlling for RWA and SDO.

We checked for possible outliers using both leverage (values higher than .2 [58]) and Cooks-distance (values higher than 1 [59]) in all four models. Only in the model with DRSD, SDO, and RWA predicting donation behavior we found some outliers based on leverage but not based on Cooks-distance, which is why we decided to not exclude them.

Discussion

Investigating DRSD in a large representative US sample confirmed the findings of the previous study. Across both investigations, DRSD scale was found to be a valid measurement of disease-related social Darwinist thinking, a specific form of social Darwinist thinking particularly relevant in the pandemic. Furthermore, Study 2 indicated that the scale was useful in a different cultural and political context, as an important construct in explaining more generalized political attitudes outside of COVID-19 related political behaviors. Exploratory analyses point to a possible role of DRSD in voting decisions and deciding to support certain organizations like the NRA.

General discussion

The COVID-19 pandemic has changed the way people think and live together in societies. If experts are correct in predicting that ecological issues like deforestation and extinctions make future pandemics ever more likely [60], we might already live in a pandemic world. This highlights the importance of understanding how political attitudes are formed and changed during pandemics and how they relate to societal life.

The present paper aims at addressing this topic by introducing Disease-Related Social Darwinism as a disease-related belief characterized by the wish to let nature run its course for progress to occur. We generally defined social Darwinism as an application of Darwin’s theory of natural selection to the development of human societies. Specifically, social Darwinism is characterized by the idea that inequality is inevitable and preferable because it is a result of natural selection. According to this logic, natural selection equals societal progress because, if permitted, the weaker members of a group are weeded out and the group is strengthened accordingly. Hence, diseases (particularly pandemics) are perceived as a natural engine of evolution and progress since, if not controlled or counteracted, they would kill mostly the supposedly “weaker” members of a group. Therefore, DRSD, as a specification of general social Darwinism, provides justification for politicians not to protect vulnerable members of society and for citizens to not protect themselves and others. To address this way of thinking, we investigated the quality and usefulness of the DRSD scale. Results of two studies indicate that the scale is reliable, valid and useful in predicting people’s protective behavior and their political attitudes.

In Study 1 (conducted in Germany), we investigated DRSD in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Besides great reliability, the DRSD scale showed significant relations with established political measures such as general social Darwinism, RWA, SDO and neoliberalism thus indicating the scale’s validity. The high inter-item correlations (s. online supplemental material) in both studies also justify the constructed items. Most importantly, DRSD significantly predicted people’s behavior during the pandemic, even when general social Darwinism [5] was controlled for. People endorsing a view of diseases as a natural engine of evolution reported less protective behavior during the pandemic. DRSD also moderated the relation between people’s party preference and behavior. People sympathizing with more right-wing parties showed less protective behavior, especially if they also had higher scores of DRSD.

Preregistered Study 2 confirmed the reliability and validity of the DRSD scale in a different national (i.e., US) and representative sample. Correlations with SDO and RWA were replicated, and other significant correlations corroborated the validity of the scale. The central finding is that DRSD significantly predicted people’s political attitudes, after controlling for SDO, which has been linked to more conservative attitudes [27]. People who perceive diseases in a social Darwinist way tended to exhibit more conservative political attitudes.

With our findings, we build on the fundamental work by Saud [26] and Rudman and Saud [5] investigating social Darwinist beliefs in political contexts. We replicate central findings from their studies concerning the relationships between social Darwinism, SDO and RWA. However, most importantly, contributing to the social Darwinism framework, we introduce a new measurement of social Darwinist beliefs about diseases, which is not only reliable and valid but also very economical (8-items for the English short scale).

Applications, limitations and avenues for future research

The studies presented here provide a solid basis for future research on the role of DRSD and its association with political attitudes and behavior. Considering that social Darwinist ideas arose in western societies during the Gilded Age (Degler, as cited in [5]), future research should investigate social Darwinist beliefs in non-western cultures to better understand their cultural aspect.

We also believe that the DRSD scale contributes to the landscape of political attitude measures because it provides answers to basic societal questions such as how societal progress should occur and whether equality is preferable. More generally, it touches on the foundational tenets of how individuals perceive human life and the question of whether human beings should be considered as rather similar or quite different from each other. Furthermore, until now there has not been a measurement directly capturing the social Darwinist idea that diseases can serve a positive function for society. The incremental validity our scale showed compared to social Darwinism and SDO in predicting political behavior and orientation proves that our scale captures a specific form of social Darwinist attitudes not adequately measured by existing measures. Exploratory analyses in Study 2 point to a possible role of DRSD in voting decisions and more directly behavioral decisions relating to donating $ 100 to a specific organization. DRSD increased the likelihood of reporting having voted for Donald Trump (vs. Joe Biden) in the 2020 election and wanting to donate to the NRA (vs. NAACP). These analyses provide fertile ground for future research on DRSD as a social/political attitude. For exploratory reasons we also investigated the relationship between DRSD and general/specific relative deprivation which resulted in an inconsistent pattern. Hence, the motivational underpinnings of DRSD need further investigation.

Regarding the construction of the scale, we found that all reverse-coded items loaded on a different factor. This might be due to the fact it is difficult to define opposites to social Darwinist beliefs. Nevertheless, this aspect should be addressed in future studies. Additionally, the CFA in Study 2 yielded high values of RMSEA. Even though we think that in our case this should not be considered a great threat to model fit [56], especially considering the proper CFI, TLI, and SRMR values, future research should nonetheless investigate the structure of our scale further.

Is disease-related social Darwinism an individual, intergroup, or societal phenomenon?

Our results suggest that DRSD justifies ideas of inequality on almost every social dimension, starting from the very individual and reaching to societal levels. On the individual dimension, DRSD relates to people’s behavior and their conceptions of what is right and wrong (e.g., “Should I protect vulnerable members of my group?”). On the intragroup dimension, DRSD justifies a hierarchical (vs. egalitarian) organization of an individual’s own in-group, as indicated by the significant correlations with RWA in both studies. As already shown by Rudman and Saud [5] and replicated in the present study, social Darwinism and hence DRSD justify why certain groups should dominate others in society, as illustrated by the significant correlations with SDO. From the significant prediction of conservative political attitudes, it can be assumed that DRSD also plays a role on a societal dimension, justifying an individual’s perception of policy decisions and party/politician preference. We think that the multi-dimensional influence DRSD seems to have is a good argument to consider it as a foundational attitude underlying conservatism and right-wing political attitudes.

How useful is the DRSD scale outside of the pandemic?

In the present investigation, DRSD significantly predicted people’s self-reported protective behavior during the pandemic. Showing that people, who perceive diseases as having a positive function for societies, reported less self-protective behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. A recent study confirmed that self-reported measures of social distancing (even short two-item measures like the one we used) are valid measures of actual social distancing behavior during the pandemic, assessed with GPS tracking [61]. Nevertheless, it can be asked whether the DRSD would be useful outside of the context of COVID-19 and the accompanying pandemic. Even if experts predicting a pandemic future are not correct, the COVID-19 pandemic has demonstrated that people’s disease-related attitudes are not only relevant to an individual’s health but also have a great impact on a societal/political dimension, being used to popularize certain ideologies or positions. With the DRSD scale we provide a useful tool to capture such an attitude, finally used to justify the death of vulnerable people. Individuals holding social Darwinist beliefs perceive diseases as an opportunity for growth of the group/society rather than a risk. Hence, vaccinations and other treatments preventing severe illness may be rejected as threats to human progress even outside the current pandemic context. Accordingly, DRSD may have a great impact on a societal level, since it can be a reason for people’s vaccine hesitancy, which can cause broader societal problems [16,17]. Furthermore, DRSD can serve as a justification for political decisions regarding diseases which makes it an interesting attitude to consider in the realm of political orientation. Moreover, the already-mentioned incremental validity our scale possesses in comparison to existing measurements, demonstrates that our scale is relevant for future research on people’s disease-related attitudes and their effects. Such future research should investigate the relation of DRSD to actual disease-related behavior by using, for example, smartphone location data or step-tracking devices to assess social-distancing behavior.

Data Availability

All Data, SPSS and R Syntax, the preregistration for Study 2 and the online supplemental materials are available on OSF: DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/28XPG.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Green J, Edgerton J, Naftel D, Shoub K, Cranmer SJ. Elusive consensus: Polarization in elite communication on the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci Adv. 2020;6(28). doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abc2717 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Levin B. Texas Lt. Governor: Old People Should Volunteer to Die to Save the Economy. VanityFair. 2020. Mar 24. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Glumm K, Johnson JD. Creating the “Unfit”: Social Darwinism or Social Triage?: Constructing a Supply of Patients in Private Psychiatric Hospitals. J Appl Behav Sci. 2001;37(2):154–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rogers JA. Darwinism and social Darwinism. J Hist Ideas. 1972;33(2):265–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rudman LA, Saud LH. Justifying Social Inequalities: The Role of Social Darwinism. Pers Soc Psychol Bull [Internet]. 2020;46(7):1139–55. Available from: doi: 10.1177/0146167219896924 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Weikart R. Laissez-Faire Social Darwinism and Individualist Competition in Darwin and Huxley. The European Legacy. 1998;3(1):17–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Leyva R. No Child Left behind: A neoliberal repackaging of social Darwinism. Journal for Critical Education Policy Studies. 2009;7(1):365–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Martin ME. Philosophical and religious influences on social welfare policy in the United States: The ongoing effect of Reformed theology and social Darwinism on attitudes toward the poor and social welfare policy and practice. Journal of Social Work. 2012;12(1):51–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tienken CH. Neoliberalism, Social Darwinism, and Consumerism Masquerading as School Reform. Interchange. 2013;43(4):295–316. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Strobl N. Feinde finden Vorabdruck. Sozialdarwinismus– Ökofaschismus–Verschwörungsideologien. Die extreme Rechte reagiert in verschiedener Weise auf die Coronakrise. junge Welt. 2021. Jan 8. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Nachtwey O, Schäfer R, Frei N. Politische Soziologie der Corona-Proteste. 2020. Dec 22. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kanık B, Uluğ ÖM, Solak N, Chayinska M. “Let the strongest survive”: Ageism and social Darwinism as barriers to supporting policies to benefit older individuals. Journal of Social Issues. 2022;78(4):790–814. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jones KE, Patel NG, Levy MA, Storeygard A, Balk D, Gittleman JL, et al. Global trends in emerging infectious diseases. Nature. 2008;451(7181):990–3. doi: 10.1038/nature06536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Madhav N, Oppenheim B, Gallivan M, Mulembakani P, Rubin E, Wolfe N. Pandemics: risks, impacts, and mitigation. 2018. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bundesministerium für Gesundheit (German Ministry of Health). Masern [Internet]. 2021. Available from: https://www.bundesgesundheitsministerium.de/service/begriffe-von-a-z/m/masern.html.
  • 16.Jacobson RM, Sauver JLS, Rutten LJF. Vaccine hesitancy. In: Mayo Clinic Proceedings. Elsevier; 2015. p. 1562–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dubé E, Laberge C, Guay M, Bramadat P, Roy R, Bettinger JA. Vaccine hesitancy: an overview. Hum Vaccin Immunother. 2013;9(8):1763–73. doi: 10.4161/hv.24657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jarynowski A, Semenov A, Belik V. Protest perspective against COVID-19 risk mitigation strategies on the German Internet. In: International Conference on Computational Data and Social Networks. Springer; 2020. p. 524–35. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Allcott H, Boxell L, Conway J, Gentzkow M, Thaler M, Yang D. Polarization and public health: Partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. J Public Econ. 2020;191. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104254 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sehgal NJ, Yue D, Pope E, Wang RH, Roby DH. The Association Between COVID-19 Mortality And The County-Level Partisan Divide In The United States: Study examines the association between COVID-19 mortality and county-level political party affiliation. Health Aff. 2022;41(6):853–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wallace J, Goldsmith-Pinkham P, Schwartz JL. Excess death rates for Republicans and Democrats during the COVID-19 pandemic. National Bureau of Economic Research; 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Walther E, Isemann S. Einleitung: Psychologische Erklärungen für den Erfolg der AfD. In: Walther E, Isemann S, editors. Die AfD–psychologisch betrachtet. 1st ed. Wiesbaden: Springer; 2019. p. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Decker O, Hinz A, Geißler N, Brähler E. Fragebogen zur rechtsextremen Einstellung–Leipziger Form (FR-LF). In: Rechtsextremismus der Mitte Eine sozialpsychologische Gegenwartsdiagnose. 2013. p. 197–212. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Küpper B, Zick A, Rump M. Rechtsextreme Einstellungen in der Mitte 2020/21. In: Zick A, Küpper B, editors. Die Geforderte Mitte—Rechtsextreme und Demokratiegefährdende Einstellungen in Deutschland 2020/21. Bonn: Friedrich-Ebert-Stiftung; 2021. p. 75–111. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Radkiewicz P, Skarżyńska K. Who are the ‘social Darwinists’? On dispositional determinants of perceiving the social world as competitive jungle. PLoS One. 2021;16(8):e0254434. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Saud LH. Perceptions of Immigrant Threats: The Role of Social Darwinism. Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Pratto F, Sidanius J, Stallworth LM, Malle BF. Social Dominance Orientation: A Personality Variable Predicting Social and Political Attitudes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67(4):741. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Altemeyer B. Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing authoritarianism. Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing authoritarianism. San Francisco, CA, US: Jossey-Bass; 1988. (The Jossey-Bass social and behavioral science series and The Jossey-Bass public administration series.). [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Keller J. In genes we trust: The biological component of psychological essentialism and its relationship to mechanisms of motivated social cognition. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88(4):686. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.American Psychological Association. Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct (2002, amended effective June 1, 2010, and January 1, 2017) [Internet]. 2017. Available from: http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/index.html.
  • 31.Beattie P, Bettache K, Chong KCY. Who is the Neoliberal? Exploring Neoliberal Beliefs across East and West. Journal of Social Issues. 2019;75(1):20–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Azevedo F, Jost JT, Rothmund T, Sterling J. Neoliberal Ideology and the Justification of Inequality in Capitalist Societies: Why Social and Economic Dimensions of Ideology Are Intertwined. Journal of Social Issues. 2019;75(1):49–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Weikart R. The Origins of Social Darwinism in Germany, 1859–1895. J Hist Ideas. 1993;54(3):469–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Paul DB. Darwin, social Darwinism and eugenics. In: The Cambridge Companion to Darwin. 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dennis RM. Social Darwinism, Scientific Racism, and the Metaphysics of Race. J Negro Educ. 1995;64(3):243–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Decker O, Kiess J, Brähler E. Die Untersuchung 2014—Starke Wirtschaft, gefestigte Mitte. In: Rechtsextremismus der Mitte und sekundärer Autoritarismus. 2015. p. 35–69. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Schönbrodt FD, Perugini M. At what sample size do correlations stabilize? J Res Pers. 2013;47(5):609–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Comrey AL. Factor-analytic methods of scale development in personality and clinical psychology. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(5):754. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.5.754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Claeys G. The “survival of the fittest” and the origins of social Darwinism. J Hist Ideas. 2000;61(2):223–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Leonard TC. Origins of the myth of social Darwinism: The ambiguous legacy of Richard Hofstadter’s Social Darwinism in American Thought. J Econ Behav Organ. 2009;71(1):37–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ho AK, Sidanius J, Kteily N, Sheehy-Skeffington J, Pratto F, Henkel KE, et al. The Nature of Social Dominance Orientation: Theorizing and Measuring Preferences for Intergroup Inequality Using the New SDO₇ Scale. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;109(6):1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Beierlein C, Asbrock F, Kauff M, Schmidt P. Die Kurzskala Autoritarismus (KSA-3): Ein ökonomisches Messinstrument zur Erfassung dreier Subdimensionen autoritärer Einstellungen. GESIS—Working Papers. 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Groß EM, Hövermann A. Marktförmiger Extremismus-ein Phänomen der Mitte? In: Zick A, Klein A, editors. Fragile Mitte-Feindselige Zustände. Bonn: Dietz Verlag; 2014. p. 102–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh. New York: IBM Corp; 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lantz B, Wenger MR, Mills JM. Fear, Political Legitimization, and Racism: Examining Anti-Asian Xenophobia During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Race Justice. 2022;21533687221125816. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hartman TK, Stocks TVA, McKay R, Gibson-Miller J, Levita L, Martinez AP, et al. The authoritarian dynamic during the COVID-19 pandemic: Effects on nationalism and anti-immigrant sentiment. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2021;12(7):1274–85. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Sidanius J, Pratto F, Bobo L. Social Dominance Orientation and the Political Psychology of Gender: A Case of Invariance? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67(6):998. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Chait J. Social Darwinism Is What Truly Guides Trump. New York Magazine. 2017. Jun 26. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Zakrisson I. Construction of a short version of the Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) scale. Pers Individ Dif. 2005;39(5):863–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Heidemeyer P, Wesser-Sallfrank A, Banse R, Hoffmann L, Quinten L. The California F Scale Revisited—Development and Validation of a Contemporary Version. Department of Psychology, University of Bon; n. 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Adorno TW, Frenkel-Brunswik E, Levinson DJ, Sanford RN. The authoritarian personality. The authoritarian personality. Oxford, England: Harpers; 1950. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Webber D, Kruglanski AW. Psychological factors in radicalization: A “3N” approach. The handbook of the criminology of terrorism. 2016;1:33. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):1–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kenny DA, Kaniskan B, McCoach DB. The performance of RMSEA in models with small degrees of freedom. Sociol Methods Res. 2015;44(3):486–507. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Pituch KA, Stevens JP. Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences: Analyses with SAS and IBM’s SPSS. Routledge; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Huber PJ. Robust statistics. In: International encyclopedia of statistical science. Springer; 2011. p. 1248–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Heiberger RM, Heiberger RM, Burt Holland BH. Statistical Analysis and Data Display An Intermediate Course with Examples in R. Springer; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Tollefson J. Why deforestation and extinctions make pandemics more likely. Nature. 2020. Mar 16;584:175+. doi: 10.1038/d41586-020-02341-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Gollwitzer A, McLoughlin K, Martel C, Marshall J, Höhs JM, Bargh JA. Linking self-reported social distancing to real-world behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2022;13(2):656–68. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Peter Karl Jonason

22 Sep 2022

PONE-D-22-19515Survival of the fittest in the pandemic age: Introducing Disease-Related Social DarwinismPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Nachtwey,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. I was able to get one evolutionary psychologist and one prejudice researcher with EP leanings. Both identified major issues that they think warrant your attention prior to publication but they suggest there is "light at the end of the tunnel" for this paper.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 05 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Peter Karl Jonason

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. You indicated that ethical approval was not necessary for your study. Could you please provide further details on why your study is exempt from the need for approval and confirmation from your institutional review board or research ethics committee (e.g., in the form of a letter or email correspondence) that ethics review was not necessary for this study? Please include a copy of the correspondence as an ""Other"" file.

3. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure: 

"The authors did not receive funding for this work."

At this time, please address the following queries:

a) Please clarify the sources of funding (financial or material support) for your study. List the grants or organizations that supported your study, including funding received from your institution. 

b) State what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role in your study, please state: “The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

c) If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funders.

d) If you did not receive any funding for this study, please state: “The authors received no specific funding for this work.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The manuscript entitled „Survival of the fittest in the pandemic age: Introducing Disease-Related Social Darwinism” describes two studies on the Disease-Related Social Darwinism (DRSD) as a new concept inspired by the changes in societal attitudes toward protective measures and politics during the COVID-19 pandemic. The Authors described this construct (DRSD) in the context of sociopolitical attitudes (SDO; RWA; SD) and examined the expectations regarding political preferences related to this belief (DRSD). Since this new construct seems to be interesting development of social Darwinism, I have some suggestions concerning the description of the studies and its methodology.

#1. The Authors tested a lot of hypotheses concerning various groups of variables possibly corelated with DRSD. I think that the introduction would benefit from dividing it into sections concerning the associations between groups of such variables and DRSD (e.g., Political preferences and DRSD; DRSD and the COVID-19 preventive measures). The Authors could also include a figure in order to illustrate their hypotheses regarding DRSD. I could help in making the manuscript easier to follow.

#2. The abstract includes the sentences which are the copies of sentences present within the manuscript. I suggest to correct it in order to avoid repetitions.

#3. Throughout the manuscript the Authors are using wording suggesting causal role of the DRSD for political attitudes (e.g., p. 2; l. 29; p. 16; l. 344-346). Their studies are cross-sectional and correlational. Such a design gives no argument for causal interpretations.

#4. The first paragraphs of the introduction should be concise and avoid unnecessary references. I think that the Authors could simply introduce the DRSD and illustrate the beliefs similar to DRSR during the pandemic with one example. I am also wondering whether personal references to politicians are necessary.

#5. The suggestions that the DRSD beliefs “have been formed or changed during the pandemic” could be premature. The statements that arose during the pandemic and seem to be the examples of the DRSD could be better see globally, but could be previously present e.g. in individual’s attitudes toward the health care systems in their countries. The sentences such as “although social Darwinism has been widespread in societies for a long time, it can be assumed that the COVID-19 pandemic has increased the prevalence of this thinking” better express the possible situation associated with the DRSD beliefs.

#6. Study 1 has numerous goals. Moreover, the Authors stated: “Most importantly, however, we want to show that the scale has incremental validity in comparison to the standard social Darwinism scale when predicting people’s protective behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic (Hypothesis 3a)”. In my opinion, the central goal of the study 1 was to develop the measure of the DRSD. Thus, inspection of its internal structure and validity should be the main goal of the Study 1.

#7. The better justifications of sample size in both studies are necessary. The sentences such as “We decided to recruit around 300 participants based on Rudman and Saud (2020), who recruited around 400 participants for most of their studies but also computed more complex calculations than we planned for” are not enough. How the number of the participant could be described in the context of the measure development? Or in the context of stability of correlation coefficients?

#8. Better descriptions of exclusion and inclusion criteria are missing in both studies.

#9. Did the Authors use also the parallel analysis in order to inspect the correct number of factors in the DRSD measure?

#10. According to the Authors hypothesis that the DRSD is a variant of SD, the hypotheses about associations between the DRSD, SDO, and RWA, should be tested by using partial correlation (controlled for SD). SDO and SD are correlated stronger that the DRSD and SDO. The Authors should also mention such results in the discussion. In my opinion some CFA testing whether the DRSD and SD are different construct could be useful.

#11. The justification of using separate RAs to examine the associations between party preferences and left/right political orientation should be provided in the manuscript. Which were the correlations between these variables? In general, the authors can provide statistics concerning collinearity in their RA analysis.

#12. The Authors used probably a hierarchical regression (see l. 312). Did they centered variables before calculating interactions? Were interactions entered into regression models in separate step? Did that step introduce a significant change in R square? How were the interactions unpacked?

#13. The description of the goals of Study 2 introduces new construct which were not previously mentioned in the introduction (needs deprivation). I suggest to elaborate on these predictions in separate section in the introduction. In both studies some moderations appeared as a goals of the examinations. However, they were not elaborated and introduce properly in the introduction. Such a situation create a sense of chaotic argumentation and multiple goals which the Authors try to obtained using the one sample.

#14. The political orientation measure used in Study 2 should be better described. It consists of 4 Democrats and 4 Republicans, but the calculation is based only on 2 of each category. Please, justify this score calculation. Please also include some data on the validity.

#15. The other instruments again create a feeling that the Authors include many measures in their study and tried to find some significant associations with the DRSD. It would be better to clearly justify why these instruments were used. I am wondering why the Authors did not controlled for SD in the study 2? Moreover, the study 2 is less associated with the exact wording of the title, which suggest testing the DRSD in the context of the pandemic. Study 2 is more about the associations between the DRSD and political preferences.

#16. The CFA brought inconsistent results. Both CFI and TLI seem proper (but please provide also criteria of fit which were used), but RMSEA is clearly problematic. Moreover, the Authors mention two-factor solution, which was not previously described. Did the Authors tested the differences between chi square of both one- and two-factor solutions?

#17. The hierarchical RA in Study 2 looks problematic. First, when the goals is to analyze interaction, the Authors have to state how they computed interaction term, and did he model explained more variance when the interaction term was entered. Some standardized betas are higher than 1.00 – please explain such situation. Again, collinearity statistics are necessary.

#18. The sections entitled “Is social Darwinism an individual, intergroup, or societal phenomenon?” seem misleading in the discussion. The studies were about the DRSD, not the SD itself. Thus, I suggest to keep to the results of the Authors’ studies. However, the structure of this section could be used to structure the introduction in terms of correlates of the DRSD.

Reviewer #2: I would like to thank for possibility to review this paper on subject of creation of disease-specific social Darwinism scale.

Even general social Darwinism is a rather narrow variable, disease-specific social Darwinism very much so. I think that constructing such a measurement tool, very specific in scope, brings limited novelty to the literature. In my opinion, such a niche scale should be of utmost quality in order to be consider worthy of publication.

Theoretical part of the article is well-written and concise; the same could be said about discussion. Scale has good reliability. Method chosen for testing scale’s validity are proper, but Authors do not test validity by any other way than other questionnaires, which is unfortunate. The same applies to predicting behavior during pandemic “over and above” general social Darwinism. It is great idea, but unfortunately behavior was measured by 2 simple, self-report questions.

In my opinion, authors should implement at least one non-self-report measure to check scale’s validity. It is harder to do, but if scales are validated only by other paper-and-pencil measures, we cannot be fully sure what is their relation to real world.

In summary, I think this article is just not giving enough: in case of small, narrow contribution, it should met the highest standards. I would recommend publishing this article only if authors provide non-paper-and-pencil proves of scale validity and predictive utility (which may require additional study).

I also have two minor comments:

1. I couldn’t find information about national identity of the participants of the first study; Authors inform in the abstract that they collected data from two nationalities, but it is not evident what is the first one

2. Beta’s should not be written in italics

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Marcin Moroń

Reviewer #2: Yes: Jarosław Piotrowski

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Mar 22;18(3):e0281072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0281072.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


8 Nov 2022

Dear Dr. Jonason and dear Reviewers,

we would like to thank you for the constructive and helpful feedback on our manuscript. We are very happy for the opportunity to resubmit a revised manuscript version.

We believe that the reviewers' and your comments have helped us substantially to improve our manuscript and we were particularly grateful for the many concrete suggestions. Based on these comments and suggestions, we have made numerous changes to the manuscript, which are all detailed in the responses (in italics) below. We think that we were able to address all of the raised concerns. However, if anything remains unsatisfactory, we will happily undertake more revisions.

We are looking forward to hearing from you! And thanks again.

Sincerely,

Paul Nachtwey & Eva Walther

Note: the line numbers refer to the unmarked manuscript version, as the line numbers are subject to change in the marked-up version of the document depending on how changes are made visible in Microsoft Word.

Journal Requirements

1. You indicated that ethical approval was not necessary for your study. Could you please provide further details on why your study is exempt from the need for approval and confirmation from your institutional review board or research ethics committee (e.g., in the form of a letter or email correspondence) that ethics review was not necessary for this study? Please include a copy of the correspondence as an ""Other"" file.

We appreciate the concern of PLOS ONE for ethical research. Separate ethical approval from the University was not necessary for our study because we strictly followed the ethical guidelines of the German Society for Psychology (DGPs; Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie, 2016) to conduct ethical psychological research. Hence, we received an “Exempt from Ethics” from the ethics committee of the Universität Trier which we uploaded as an extra file with the revised version of our manuscript.

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

"The authors did not receive funding for this work."

At this time, please address the following queries:

a) Please clarify the sources of funding (financial or material support) for your study. List the grants or organizations that supported your study, including funding received from your institution.

b) State what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role in your study, please state: “The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

c) If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funders.

d) If you did not receive any funding for this study, please state: “The authors received no specific funding for this work.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf

The Authors received no specific funding for this work

Reviewer 1

1. The Authors tested a lot of hypotheses concerning various groups of variables possibly corelated with DRSD. I think that the introduction would benefit from dividing it into sections concerning the associations between groups of such variables and DRSD (e.g., Political preferences and DRSD; DRSD and the COVID-19 preventive measures). The Authors could also include a figure in order to illustrate their hypotheses regarding DRSD. I could help in making the manuscript easier to follow.

We thank Reviewer 1 for this helpful comment. We agree that the number of hypotheses with different variables made the introduction difficult to follow. We thus included more subheadings in the introduction section (see, l. 82, 105, 128, 153) and we have added a part about DRSD and political orientation for clarification (see, l. 105-127).

Based on the suggestion from the reviewer, we have now added a figure (see, l. 194) illustrating the hypotheses of Study 1 because we agree that this will enhance the comprehensibility of the manuscript and theoretical assumptions respectively. This figure can also be found at the end of this Letter.

2. The abstract includes the sentences which are the copies of sentences present within the manuscript. I suggest to correct it in order to avoid repetitions.

We appreciate this suggestion from Reviewer 1. We have changed the sentences in our abstract to avoid repetition (see, l. 25-30, 34f).

3. Throughout the manuscript the Authors are using wording suggesting causal role of the DRSD for political attitudes (e.g., p. 2; l. 29; p. 16; l. 344-346). Their studies are cross-sectional and correlational. Such a design gives no argument for causal interpretations.

We are thankful for these observations. For obvious reasons, we did not want to create the idea of causality in our manuscript, so we went through the whole manuscript and carefully changed every instance of misleading wording (see, l. 28, 95, 382, 650, 674, 692, 725).

4. The first paragraphs of the introduction should be concise and avoid unnecessary references. I think that the Authors could simply introduce the DRSD and illustrate the beliefs similar to DRSR during the pandemic with one example. I am also wondering whether personal references to politicians are necessary.

We agree with the reviewer’s critique that the first paragraph of our introduction may have been lengthy. We have thus shortened it considerably and included as few references as possible (see, l. 49-60). We also dropped references to individual politicians. As the reviewer has suggested, we have illustrated the beliefs similar to DRSD with one example (see, l. 56f). We have added parts of the deleted introduction to the part about DRSD and political Orientation (see, l. 109-114).

5. The suggestions that the DRSD beliefs “have been formed or changed during the pandemic” could be premature. The statements that arose during the pandemic and seem to be the examples of the DRSD could be better see globally, but could be previously present e.g. in individual’s attitudes toward the health care systems in their countries. The sentences such as “although social Darwinism has been widespread in societies for a long time, it can be assumed that the COVID-19 pandemic has increased the prevalence of this thinking” better express the possible situation associated with the DRSD beliefs.

Thanks for this comment. We did not intend to raise premature conclusions. Due to the restructuring of our opening paragraph (see point 4 raised by Reviewer 1), the critical sentence 'have been formed or changed during the pandemic' is no longer part of our manuscript. However, because there is empirical evidence supporting the sentence: ‘although social Darwinism has been widespread in societies for a long time, it can be assumed that the COVID-19 pandemic has increased the prevalence of this thinking’ this part remains in the manuscript (see, l. 83f).

6. Study 1 has numerous goals. Moreover, the Authors stated: “Most importantly, however, we want to show that the scale has incremental validity in comparison to the standard social Darwinism scale when predicting people’s protective behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic (Hypothesis 3a)”. In my opinion, the central goal of the study 1 was to develop the measure of the DRSD. Thus, inspection of its internal structure and validity should be the main goal of the Study 1.

We agree with the Reviewer’s comment that the most important goal of Study 1 is the inspection of structure and validity of our scale. Hence, we reformulated the respective sentences accordingly (see, l. 169f, 189).

7. The better justifications of sample size in both studies are necessary. The sentences such as “We decided to recruit around 300 participants based on Rudman and Saud (2020), who recruited around 400 participants for most of their studies but also computed more complex calculations than we planned for” are not enough. How the number of the participant could be described in the context of the measure development? Or in the context of stability of correlation coefficients?

We appreciate the Reviewer’s concern regarding the justification of the sample size.

For Study 1, we decided to recruit as close to the sample size of Rudman and Saud (2020) as possible. A post hoc sensitivity analysis revealed that a sample of 300 participants has an 80% power of detecting a minimum Change in R2 = .001 (when investigating one predictor added to one or two other predictors). It has also been shown that correlation coefficients stabilize around 250 participants (Schönbrodt & Perugini, 2013), and that 200 participants are sufficient for factor analyses with less than 40 variables (our item-pool consisted of 24 items; Comrey, 1988). Hence, for the main goal of inspecting internal structure and validity of our scale (using EFA, correlational and regression analyses), 300 participants should be sufficient.

Regarding Study 2, post-hoc sensitivity analyses revealed that a sample of 533 participants has an 80% power of detecting a minimum Change in R2 < .001 (when investigating one predictor added to one or two other predictors).

To be more transparent, we have now added these explanations in part to the manuscript (see, l. 203-209, 433-437).

8. Better descriptions of exclusion and inclusion criteria are missing in both studies.

Again, we sympathize with the concern of Reviewer 1 about transparency concerning the sample size.

For Study 1, based on pre-testing of the study we decided that the minimum time it takes to at least read all items is around 5 minutes. Therefore, 4 participants that took less than 5 minutes were excluded from the sample. This more detailed description has also been added to the manuscript (see, l. 209-211).

For Study 2, participants were presented with 10 politicians (8 real + 2 fake ones) and then asked to indicate if they knew these politicians. Participants were excluded if they expressed, they were familiar with one or both fake politicians because this implies either a lack of concentration or a serious lack of knowledge regarding US politics that might lead to flawed results. This more detailed justification has also been added to the manuscript (see, l. 425-430).

9. Did the Authors use also the parallel analysis in order to inspect the correct number of factors in the DRSD measure?

We thank Reviewer 1 for the reminder about this possibility. However, after reinspecting the scree plot, we were positive that it very clearly suggests a one-factor solution and thus no parallel analysis would be necessary. To make this decision more transparent we have added the scree-plot as a figure to our manuscript (see, l. 314). We have also added it at the end of this letter for clarification.

10. According to the Authors hypothesis that the DRSD is a variant of SD, the hypotheses about associations between the DRSD, SDO, and RWA, should be tested by using partial correlation (controlled for SD). SDO and SD are correlated stronger that the DRSD and SDO. The Authors should also mention such results in the discussion. In my opinion some CFA testing whether the DRSD and SD are different construct could be useful.

We thank Reviewer 1 for this comment. However, based on our theoretical reasoning, we assume that DRSD is a specification of the broader concept of Social Darwinism applied to the realm of diseases (see added Figure 1). Hence, partializing out Social Darwinism or computing the proposed CFA would not be appropriate. We have added a new paragraph into the introduction to make this consideration clearer and we also refer to this in the discussion section (see, l. 92f, 126f, 171ff, 660f).

11. The justification of using separate RAs to examine the associations between party preferences and left/right political orientation should be provided in the manuscript. Which were the correlations between these variables? In general, the authors can provide statistics concerning collinearity in their RA analysis.

Thanks for this important comment. Because only one predictor was used in relevant RA, we think that collinearity concerns are unfounded. However, we added collinearity statistics for regression with multiple predictors (see Tables 4,6,7).

We decided to calculate two separate RAs because we think that differentiating between self-placement on a left-right spectrum and preference for political parties in our RAs provides a more nuanced picture of the results. This is because, previous research indicates that these dimensions do not necessarily measure the same construct (Lesschaeve, 2017). Even though the dimensions are correlated (r = .396 when no participants are excluded, r = .706 when participants selecting no or another party were excluded), someone self-identifying as more right-wing could prefer libertarian parties (economically right-wing), traditionalist-conservative parties (moderate right-wing), or populist right-wing parties (van Assche et al., 2019). So even though there is overlap between these variables, they are by no means identical. This more detailed justification has been added to the manuscript (see, l. 334-338).

12. The Authors used probably a hierarchical regression (see l. 312). Did they centered variables before calculating interactions? Were interactions entered into regression models in separate step? Did that step introduce a significant change in R square? How were the interactions unpacked?

Thank you for raising these questions. The reviewer’s assumption is correct we used a hierarchical regression. We apologize for the mistake in wording and corrected the term throughout the manuscript (see, l. 343, 561). In the previous version of the manuscript, we did not center variables for the regression analyses since we were mainly interested in the interaction (centered variables do not change results for the interaction analyses). Based on your comment, we additionally calculated the regression analyses with centered variables to be able to interpret the main effects (instead of calculating two separate RAs as we did in the previous version of the manuscript). These additional analyses have been added to the manuscript (see, l. 365-369).

Based on your suggestion, we have also added multicollinearity statistics for regression analyses with multiple predictors to our manuscript (see e.g., Table 5).

We added interaction terms in an additional step; this is now also mentioned in the manuscript (see, l. 363). That step introduced a significant change in R2, which has also been added to the manuscript (see, l. 364).

If we are correct in understanding 'unpacked' as interpreted, then we appreciate the comment of Reviewer 1 and agree that the interaction should be explained briefly. We have thus added a couple of sentences to explain that for people preferring more right-wing parties the negative effect of higher DRSD on protective behavior was more pronounced (see, l. 371ff).

While calculating the RAs with mean-centered variables, we noticed a mistake in the computing of our original interaction variable of DRSD and party-preference. We have corrected this mistake, and changed the results accordingly (see, l. 361f). Importantly, the results remain the same, that is, the interaction is still significant.

13. The description of the goals of Study 2 introduces new construct which were not previously mentioned in the introduction (needs deprivation). I suggest to elaborate on these predictions in separate section in the introduction. In both studies some moderations appeared as a goals of the examinations. However, they were not elaborated and introduce properly in the introduction. Such a situation create a sense of chaotic argumentation and multiple goals which the Authors try to obtained using the one sample

We thank Reviewer 1 for this comment. As indicated in the original manuscript, examining the interaction of needs and DRSD was exploratory in nature. To clarify this, we have added a more extensive explanation of the idea of need deprivation to the introduction of Study 2 (see, l. 406-410).

14. The political orientation measure used in Study 2 should be better described. It consists of 4 Democrats and 4 Republicans, but the calculation is based only on 2 of each category. Please, justify this score calculation. Please also include some data on the validity.

Thanks for this helpful comment. We calculated politician preference score in this way because we think it creates a more differentiated picture of political preference. Because we calculated the score only using the more 'extreme' politicians in their respective party (Ocasio-Cortez and Ilhan Omar for the Democrats; Donald Trump and Majorie Taylor Greene for the Republicans), we can fully cover the political spectrum and not only differentiate between people in the more moderate center. We have also added this more detailed justification in our manuscript (see, l. 490-494).

15. The other instruments again create a feeling that the Authors include many measures in their study and tried to find some significant associations with the DRSD. It would be better to clearly justify why these instruments were used. I am wondering why the Authors did not controlled for SD in the study 2? Moreover, the study 2 is less associated with the exact wording of the title, which suggest testing the DRSD in the context of the pandemic. Study 2 is more about the associations between the DRSD and political preferences.

We appreciate the concern of Reviewer 1 about the instruments included in Study 2. We strictly followed pre-registration in this Study, or extensively explained differences from pre-registration, which should reduce the Reviewer’s concerns. Study 2 is important, because we consider DRSD as a political and social attitude that should be investigated in the broader political context. The newly added paragraph about DRSD and political orientation (see, l. 105-127) and the slightly reworked paragraph about DRSD and social attitudes (see, l. 141ff) should strengthen this point. In fact, we believe it is important for the validity of our scale to locate it in the broader political context and in association to broader political attitudes. In the ‘pandemic age’, disease-related attitudes are gaining importance (Hartman et al., 2021; Lantz et al., 2022) and thus also influence general political attitudes (see, l. 387-396).

16. The CFA brought inconsistent results. Both CFI and TLI seem proper (but please provide also criteria of fit which were used), but RMSEA is clearly problematic. Moreover, the Authors mention two-factor solution, which was not previously described. Did the Authors tested the differences between chi square of both one- and two-factor solutions?

We appreciate the comments of Reviewer 1 on the CFA in Study 2. First, criteria of fit were originally based on Hu and Bentler (1999) who suggested cut-off values for TLI and CFI close to .95. This has also been added to the manuscript (see, l. 529ff). Following Kenny et al. (2015), we think that in our specific case a high RMSEA should not be considered a great threat to model fit since RMSEA tends to penalize models with lower df with a positive bias. Considering that our model only had one factor with few variables, we consider our CFA as a model with relatively few df. Therefore, the focus should be more on the proper CFI and TLI values. We have also added information on SRMR values for further confidence in our model (see, l. 539ff). Nonetheless, future research should investigate the factor structure of our scale in more depth. We have added this consideration of the high RMSEA in the manuscript (see, l. 531-534) as well as a part in the limitation section discussing the CFA further (see, l. 717-720).

Originally the two-factor model was included in the manuscript to show that an alternative model did not show better fit to the data than the hypothesized one factor model. In this two-factor model we had all items directly mentioning Covid-19 load on the one factor and all other items on the other factor. However, in response to the reviewer’s comment we decided to delete this part in the manuscript and instead rely on the justification based on Kenny et al. (2015).

17. The hierarchical RA in Study 2 looks problematic. First, when the goals is to analyze interaction, the Authors have to state how they computed interaction term, and did he model explained more variance when the interaction term was entered. Some standardized betas are higher than 1.00 – please explain such situation. Again, collinearity statistics are necessary.

We appreciate Reviewer 1’s comment on the RA in Study 2. Based on this comment we have added an explanation of how we calculated the interaction term to the manuscript (which was done in the same way as in Study 1; see, l. 573f). Standardized Betas are most likely higher than 1 in some cases because of high multicollinearity. To counteract this, we have added the same RAs with mean centered variables as predictors to the manuscript (see, l. 586-596). We created a new table for the interaction analyses (see Table 7). We have mentioned the explained variance through adding the interaction in a separate step (see, l. 583ff). We also added collinearity statistics to the table for this RA (see Table 6 & 7).

18. The sections entitled “Is social Darwinism an individual, intergroup, or societal phenomenon?” seem misleading in the discussion. The studies were about the DRSD, not the SD itself. Thus, I suggest to keep to the results of the Authors’ studies. However, the structure of this section could be used to structure the introduction in terms of correlates of the DRSD.

We thank Reviewer 1 for this very helpful observation. We agree and hence we have changed the heading in the Discussion (see, l. 721f).

All in all, we thank Reviewer 1 for the very helpful ideas and suggestions that really helped stimulate thoughts about our manuscript.

Reviewer 2

1. In my opinion, authors should implement at least one non-self-report measure to check scale’s validity. It is harder to do, but if scales are validated only by other paper-and-pencil measures, we cannot be fully sure what is their relation to real world.

We thank Reviewer 2 for this comment. Study 2 included two more behavioral measures: people's vote in the 2020 US election and a donation question (participants were able to choose one of three organizations to which they would donate $ 100). Using logistical regression analyses, DRSD significantly predicts voting for Donald Trump (vs. Joe Biden) and significantly predicts donating to the National Rifle Association (vs. to the National Association for the Advancement of Colored People), even when controlling for SDO and RWA. We have added these analyses as exploratory analyses to our manuscript to further illustrate the predictive validity of DRSD (see, l. 601-635). And we have mentioned them in the discussion section of Study 2 (see, l. 643f), as well as in the General Discussion (see, l. 705-710).

Furthermore, as indicated by recent research, we think, that self-reported behavior can serve as a good approximation for actual behavior. Allcott et al. (2020), for example, showed that significant differences in self-reported social distancing behavior between Republicans and Democrats in the US, were also evident in actual social-distancing behavior assessed via smartphone location data. Also, Gollwitzer et al. (2022) showed that self-reported measures of social distancing present valid measures of actual social distancing behavior. In their study, even a short two-item self-report of social distancing (comparable to the one we used in our study) was valid in predicting actual social-distancing assessed using GPS tracking. This gives us additional confidence that our findings are valid and extend to real-world behavior. We have added this line of argumentation to our discussion section (see, l. 738-743). Because we fully share the reviewer’s opinion that actual behavioral measures would provide great validation for our scale, we added this as a direction for future research (see, l. 759-762).

2. I couldn’t find information about national identity of the participants of the first study; Authors inform in the abstract that they collected data from two nationalities, but it is not evident what is the first one

We thank Reviewer 2 for this observation and comment. We have added the information that Study 1 was conducted in German to the manuscript (see, l. 199, 666).

3. Beta’s should not be written in italics

We appreciate Reviewer 2’s attention to detail and pointing out this mistake. We have adjusted this throughout our manuscript.

We also corrected some typos that we realized during the last proofreading process.

Data, Syntaxes and the Online Supplemental Materials have been updated on the OSF.

Figure 1 (Hypotheses)

Figure 2 (Scree-Plot)

References

Allcott, H., Boxell, L., Conway, J., Gentzkow, M., Thaler, M., & Yang, D. (2020). Polarization and public health: Partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. Journal of Public Economics, 191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104254

Comrey, A. L. (1988). Factor-analytic methods of scale development in personality and clinical psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(5), 754.

Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie. (2016). Berufsethische Richtlinien des Berufsverbandes Deutscher Psychologinnen und Psychologen e.V. und der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie e.V.

Gollwitzer, A., McLoughlin, K., Martel, C., Marshall, J., Höhs, J. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2022). Linking self-reported social distancing to real-world behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13(2), 656–668.

Hartman, T. K., Stocks, T. V. A., McKay, R., Gibson-Miller, J., Levita, L., Martinez, A. P., Mason, L., McBride, O., Murphy, J., & Shevlin, M. (2021). The authoritarian dynamic during the COVID-19 pandemic: Effects on nationalism and anti-immigrant sentiment. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(7), 1274–1285.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55.

Kenny, D. A., Kaniskan, B., & McCoach, D. B. (2015). The performance of RMSEA in models with small degrees of freedom. Sociological Methods & Research, 44(3), 486–507.

Lantz, B., Wenger, M. R., & Mills, J. M. (2022). Fear, Political Legitimization, and Racism: Examining Anti-Asian Xenophobia During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Race and Justice, 21533687221125816.

Lesschaeve, C. (2017). The predictive power of the left-right self-placement scale for the policy positions of voters and parties. West European Politics, 40(2), 357–377.

Rudman, L. A., & Saud, L. H. (2020). Justifying Social Inequalities: The Role of Social Darwinism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(7), 1139–1155. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219896924

Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 609–612.

Tung, H. H., Chang, T.-J., & Lin, M.-J. (2022). Political ideology predicts preventative behaviors and infections amid COVID-19 in democracies. Social Science & Medicine, 308, 115199.

van Assche, J., van Hiel, A., Dhont, K., & Roets, A. (2019). Broadening the individual differences lens on party support and voting behavior: Cynicism and prejudice as relevant attitudes referring to modern‐day political alignments. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(1), 190–199.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Peter Karl Jonason

12 Dec 2022

PONE-D-22-19515R1Survival of the Fittest in the Pandemic Age: Introducing Disease-Related Social DarwinismPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Nachtwey,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 26 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Peter Karl Jonason

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The revised version of the manuscript was substantially improved. The Authors put a great effort in the revision and adressed all suggestions of the reviewers. I have now only a few suggestions:

#1. There are some possible problems with collinearity presented in Table 4 (DV: protective behavior; IV: Left/Right; DRSD; Interaction). Similar situation is present in Table 7 (see a lot of Tolerance is below .100). I think that the Authors should at least comment on this possible problem and, preferably, explain it.

#2. Given the study model which indicates that DRSD is a part/form of social darwinism, I think that the Authors could indicate how these two phenomenons are structurally separate. I think that for the clarity, additional EFA on the items of both DRSD and SD could be beneficial. In example, the Authors could include this analysis only as a note.

#3. In Study 1 and Study 2, e.g. a Harman's test for detection of common method bias could be an additional information in favor of the Authors standing that the tested variables were indeed independent and the validity analysis was conducted properly.

Reviewer #2: I would like to congratulate Author(s) on the good work done. In my opinion, manuscript is much better now and deserves for publication as is.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Marcin Moroń

Reviewer #2: Yes: Jarosław Piotrowski

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Mar 22;18(3):e0281072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0281072.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


12 Jan 2023

Journal Requirements

1. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

We appreciate PLOS ONE’s attention to detail. We have checked our reference list and did not find articles that were retracted or needed to be removed. However, while working on the comments made by Reviewer 1, we found a recently published paper investigating social Darwinism during the COVID-19 pandemic, specifically its relationship to ageism (Kanık et al., 2022). Because we think that this paper is relevant to our line of argumentation, we have added a short paragraph in the introduction citing said paper (l. 94-97). Apart from adding this paper to our reference list, no other changes have been made.

Reviewer 1

1. The revised version of the manuscript was substantially improved. The Authors put a great effort in the revision and adressed all suggestions of the reviewers. I have now only a few suggestions:

There are some possible problems with collinearity presented in Table 4 (DV: protective behavior; IV: Left/Right; DRSD; Interaction). Similar situation is present in Table 7 (see a lot of Tolerance is below .100). I think that the Authors should at least comment on this possible problem and, preferably, explain it.

First, we want to thank Reviewer 1 for the positive feedback regarding our revisions after the first round of peer-review. Second, we thank Reviewer 1 for bringing up the low Tolerances in some of the regression tables. As can be seen, the low Tolerances in both tables only concern the regression analyses with uncentered predictors. While the tolerances in the regression analyses with centered predictors (marked by © in the tables) are satisfactory. To prevent future confusion, we have added a note to both tables explaining this (see Table 4 &7).

2. Given the study model which indicates that DRSD is a part/form of social darwinism, I think that the Authors could indicate how these two phenomenons are structurally separate. I think that for the clarity, additional EFA on the items of both DRSD and SD could be beneficial. In example, the Authors could include this analysis only as a note.

We thank Reviewer 1 for his comments on the relationship of DRSD and SD and suggesting an additional EFA. Because we agree with Reviewer 1, we have calculated the proposed EFA and found that this produces two highly correlated (r = .5) though distinct factors; Factor 1 containing all items of DRSD and Factor 2 all items of SD. This provides support for our conception of DRSD as a structurally distinct part of SD. We have added this analysis in a short paragraph to our manuscript (l. 377-382).

3. In Study 1 and Study 2, e.g. a Harman's test for detection of common method bias could be an additional information in favor of the Authors standing that the tested variables were indeed independent and the validity analysis was conducted properly.

We thank Reviewer 1 for bringing up the concern about common method bias. Even though the Harman’s test has been criticized a lot in the literature (e.g., Aguirre-Urreta & Hu, 2019; Baumgartner et al., 2021), we agree with Reviewer 1 that it could provide additional evidence in our favor. Hence, we have calculated the test for both studies and in both studies the single factor extracted explained around 32 % of total variance. This can be interpreted as additional information giving us confidence in our analyses. We have added a short section describing the test for both Studies (l.392-395 & l. 643-646)

References

Aguirre-Urreta, M. I., & Hu, J. (2019). Detecting common method bias: Performance of the Harman’s single-factor test. ACM SIGMIS Database: The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems, 50(2), 45–70.

Baumgartner, H., Weijters, B., & Pieters, R. (2021). The biasing effect of common method variance: some clarifications. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 49(2), 221–235.

Kanık, B., Uluğ, Ö. M., Solak, N., & Chayinska, M. (2022). “Let the strongest survive”: Ageism and social Darwinism as barriers to supporting policies to benefit older individuals. Journal of Social Issues, 78(4), 790–814.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 2

Peter Karl Jonason

16 Jan 2023

Survival of the Fittest in the Pandemic Age: Introducing Disease-Related Social Darwinism

PONE-D-22-19515R2

Dear Dr. Nachtwey,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Peter Karl Jonason

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Peter Karl Jonason

27 Feb 2023

PONE-D-22-19515R2

Survival of the Fittest in the Pandemic Age: Introducing Disease-Related Social Darwinism

Dear Dr. Nachtwey:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Peter Karl Jonason

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All Data, SPSS and R Syntax, the preregistration for Study 2 and the online supplemental materials are available on OSF: DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/28XPG.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES