Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Mar 23;18(3):e0283221. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0283221

Sleeping with time in mind? A literature review and a proposal for a screening questionnaire on self-awakening

Laura Verga 1,2,*, Giada D’Este 3,4, Sara Cassani 3,4, Caterina Leitner 3,4, Sonja A Kotz 2, Luigi Ferini-Strambi 3,4, Andrea Galbiati 3,4,*
Editor: Manuel Spitschan5
PMCID: PMC10035927  PMID: 36952462

Abstract

Some people report being able to spontaneously “time” the end of their sleep. This ability to self-awaken challenges the idea of sleep as a passive cognitive state. Yet, current evidence on this phenomenon is limited, partly because of the varied definitions of self-awakening and experimental approaches used to study it. Here, we provide a review of the literature on self-awakening. Our aim is to i) contextualise the phenomenon, ii) propose an operating definition, and iii) summarise the scientific approaches used so far. The literature review identified 17 studies on self-awakening. Most of them adopted an objective sleep evaluation (76%), targeted nocturnal sleep (76%), and used a single criterion to define the success of awakening (82%); for most studies, this corresponded to awakening occurring in a time window of 30 minutes around the expected awakening time. Out of 715 total participants, 125 (17%) reported to be self-awakeners, with an average age of 23.24 years and a slight predominance of males compared to females. These results reveal self-awakening as a relatively rare phenomenon. To facilitate the study of self-awakening, and based on the results of the literature review, we propose a quick paper-and-pencil screening questionnaire for self-awakeners and provide an initial validation for it. Taken together, the combined results of the literature review and the proposed questionnaire help in characterising a theoretical framework for self-awakenings, while providing a useful tool and empirical suggestions for future experimental studies, which should ideally employ objective measurements.

Introduction

Human sleep is a complex physiological function. Behaviourally, it alternates with waking state and is characterised by an increased threshold to sensory input, reduction of motor output, typical changes in central and peripheral physiology, and diminished conscious awareness [1]. Importantly, from a psychological standpoint, sleep plays a crucial role in the regulation of daytime cognitive functions like memory and emotion [24]. Moreover, other phenomena (e.g., lucid dreaming) suggest that high order cognitive processing may be active during sleeping. This suggests that awareness may re-emerge during this apparently inactive brain state. For example, lucid dreaming, namely the ability of being aware of dreaming while dreaming, assumes a form of volitional control in sleep [5]. Another understudied experience related to a regained cognitive activity during sleep is self-awakening. In daily life, people use alarm clocks to wake up at a specific time in the morning. Interestingly, some report the ability to awaken without the help of any timekeeper; we refer to this phenomenon as self-awakening. Although the experience is commonly reported, some specifications are warranted to identify and understand the phenomenon (see Fig 1). First, awakening from sleep might be induced by external stimuli but can also occur spontaneously. Second, spontaneous awakening might be subdivided into natural awakening, caused by a “natural” cessation of sleep due to the dissipation of the physiological drive to sleep, or self-awakening due to the willingness to wake up. Third, self-wakening itself needs to be disambiguated: self-awakening might be habitual (i.e., a person waking up every day at the same time), in which case the interaction between circadian rhythms and learned behaviour is the driving factor, or it can be induced by volition [6]. This latter phenomenon represents the topic of interest here.

Fig 1. Schematic representation of a taxonomy for awakenings.

Fig 1

Self-awakening remains largely understudied; however, the first systematic investigation of self-awakening dates to the end of the 19th century. In his work entitled “Statistics of Unconscious Cerebration”, Charles Manning Child investigated this ability in a survey by asking the following question: “Can you wake precisely at a given hour determined upon before going to sleep, without waking up many times before the appointed time?”. The author found that 59% of the participants (200 in total) reported waking up at a predetermined time without disturbed sleep [7]. Yet, the first empirical study on self-awakening was conducted by Nicolae Vaschide at the beginning of 20th century [8]. Participants, who habitually woke up at 8 a.m., were asked to wake up at three different times during the night (at 3, 5, and 6 a.m.) without the help of any external cue. Interestingly, the mean error (i.e., the discrepancy between target time and actual wake-up time) was dependent on a specific wake-up time. Participants woke up more accurately during the last part of the night. This might reflect the influence of slow wave sleep, which predominates the first part of the night and is characterised by minimal consciousness [9]. Accordingly, one possible explanation for the phenomenon of self-awakening might involve the subjects’ ability to disengage from slow wave sleep [10].

The study of self-awakening is not only interesting, but it could also inform on the presence of specific cognitive processes even at minimal or absent levels of consciousness. There are at least three cognitive operations a sleeping person must manage to awaken at the right point in time during sleep: first, one must encode and store the target time, which must be readily accessible; second, the time elapsed during sleep should be constantly estimated and compared to the target time; third, one must regain volitional control during sleep to awake. All these operations must be carried out while the sleeping subject is in a state (i.e., sleep) typically characterised by minimal levels of consciousness. This suggests that some cognitive processes might be active during sleep in self-awakeners. However, before addressing this issue, it is necessary to properly characterise the concept of self-awakening and to streamline the methods to study this phenomenon. Up to now, self-awakening has been defined and studied using different subjective and objective methodologies, including surveys, sleep diaries, actigraphy, and polysomnography. The aim of this article is to review the scientific literature on self-awakening intended as an ability, in line with the taxonomy reported in Fig 1. Furthermore, based on the literature review, we propose a quick paper-and-pencil questionnaire that could provide a first screening tool for non-clinical self-awakeners.

Methods

Literature review

This review follows the procedure and guidelines of the Preferred reporting items for reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) statement, elaboration, and explanation [1114].

Search method

Two researchers (GD, SC) independently searched the relevant literature up to October 6th, 2022, in the following databases: Pubmed, Web of Science, Embase, Scopus, PsycInfo, and ProQuest. Search operators (e.g., AND; the proximity operator NEAR/0 was employed to find records where the search terms joined by the operator were adjacent) were used to combine the following the key-terms: self-awakening, self NEAR/0 awakening, forced-awakening, forced NEAR/0 awakening, self-awakening AND nap, self-awakening AND daily sleep, self NEAR/0 awakening AND nap, self NEAR/0 awakening AND daily sleep. Subsequently, other key-terms were searched in the same databases: ability to wake from sleep, ability to awaken from sleep, ability to awake from sleep, awakening at a pre-set time, awakening at a preselected time, wakening at a preselected time, waking from sleep at a preselected time, awakening from sleep at a pre-set time, (time NEAR/0 perception) AND (REM NEAR/0 sleep). The relevant key terms could appear anywhere in a manuscript (e.g., title, abstract, body). Peer-reviewed and non-peer reviewed materials (e.g., conference abstracts appearing in Special Issues) were considered to avoid publication bias; yet only full manuscripts were retrieved from the database search. We allowed the search to include manuscripts written in languages other than English; however, only English studies were retained in the final set of suitable papers based on the criteria outlined in the following section.

Selection of studies

Abstract were first scanned and evaluated for eligibility. The reference list of the selected papers was carefully inspected to identify other suitable manuscripts. Manuscripts were selected if they i) contained experimental studies conducted either in a sleep laboratory or at home (sleep diaries, questionnaires) and/or ii) evaluated spontaneous awakenings during daytime or night-time sleep; they were excluded if they were i) non-experimental (e.g., observational) or ii) in case of abstracts, they coincided with published journal articles. Disagreements between the two independent evaluators were reviewed and solved by internal discussion. The selection procedure is summarized in the PRISMA flowchart (Fig 2).

Fig 2. PRISMA flow diagram summarizing the selection procedure.

Fig 2

Quality evaluation

The quality of the studies selected for the literature review was assessed using the Critical Appraisal Skills Program (CASP) for cohort studies tool. This checklist aims at reliably evaluating each study in a systematic way. Each of the 12 questions assesses a specific aspect of the study, namely: clarity of the research question (Question 1), sample recruitment (Question 2), classification bias (Question 3, 4), confounding factors (Question 5a, 5b), follow-up characteristics (Question 6a, 6b), preciseness, validity, and applicability of the results (Question 7, Question 8, Question 9, Question 10), fitness of the results within the literature (Question 11), implications of the study (Question 12).

The evaluators independently rated the quality of each study; results of this assessment were compared, and disagreements were solved by internal discussion. Each study could obtain a maximum of 14 points; the average quality score for the selected studies was 10.71 (median = 11; range = 9–13). Since all studies scored above 50% of the maximum result, none was excluded from subsequent analyses based on the quality ratings. Cohen’s Kappa statistic was calculated and the overall agreement between two raters was moderated (k = 0.58).

Self-Awakening Questionnaire (SAQ)

A screening questionnaire was created by LV and AG to target and identify possible non-clinical self-awakeners. The questionnaire included 5 questions based on the reviewed literature on self-awakeners and was translated into both English (Table 1) and Italian (see S1 File). SAQ was originally created in Italian by the first and last authors (LV and AG) and subsequently translated into English. The English version was checked by two native speakers of American English and by several colleagues using English daily as their main working language. The final version was then re-translated in Italian by the other co-authors.

Table 1. Self-Awakening Questionnaire (English version).

The questionnaire consists of five questions aiming at probing the ability of a subject to wake up spontaneously (Question 1), and accurately (Question 4), independently from habit or circadian preference (Question 2, Question 5), or underlying conditions (e.g., stress, anxiety; Question 3).

Self-Awakening Questionnaire (SAQ)
Aim Under some circumstances⎯for example on the night before a test or an important meeting at work⎯some people are able to wake up spontaneously at a specific time, anticipating the sound of their alarm clock or even without the need to set one. The following questions aim at characterising this behaviour.
Instructions Read each question carefully.
Select the response that better defines your personal experience.
Please answer ALL questions.
Q1 Have you ever spontaneously woken up at a desired time, without the help of an alarm clock or other external devices?
  • yes

  • no

Q2 If yes, how frequently?
  • Every day

  • At least once a week

  • Only on nights before important events (Which ones? Please specify)

Q3 When you experience these spontaneous awakenings, do you wake up more than once throughout the night?
  • Yes

  • No, just in the morning before the alarm clock

Q4 On average, how many minutes before the desired time do you wake up?
  • 5 minutes before or less

  • Between 5 and 15 minutes

  • Between 15 and 30 minutes

  • More than 30 minutes (please specify …………………‥)

Q5 In your experience, these spontaneous awakenings are more likely to occur:
  • When you have to wake up at the usual time (e.g., 7 a.m. on a working/school day)

  • When you have to wake up at an unusual time (e.g., 3 a.m.)

The five questions aimed at characterising a self-awakener as a person capable of spontaneously waking up from sleep at a preferred time (Table 1, Question 1 and 2) and accurately (Table 1, Question 4), not simply because of a habit (i.e., not at the usual waking time; Table 1, Question 2 and 5), or due to disrupted sleep caused by stress, anxiety, or other causes (i.e., multiple awakenings during the night; Table 1, Question 3).

The questionnaire was administered to 164 students at Maastricht University (Maastricht, the Netherlands). All students were enrolled in the English track and used English daily as their main study language. Given the paucity of previous work on self-awakening and the exploratory nature of our study, we did not perform an a-priori power analysis to estimate an appropriate sample size. Yet, a number of participants > 150 can be considered a reasonable sample size for most types of analyses, especially for simple statistics as the ones performed in the current study. The study was advertised locally on bulletin boards across campus and on the SONA system of Maastricht University. Interested participants could either contact the experimenters directly via email or could select the study from those available on the SONA system, a participant pool management system used by many universities (https://www.sona-systems.com/). In SONA, interested students can easily find and enrol in ongoing studies in exchange for university credits or cash. All the participants in the current study were compensated with university credits. Recruitment stopped when no further participants would sign up for the experiment after 2 months since the enrolment of the last participant. Ethical approval was granted by the local ethical committee of Maastricht University, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience (agreement nr. OZL_207_04_04_2019). Every subject gave written informed consent to participate in the study.

The questionnaire was complemented by additional tests to probe sleep-related aspects (e.g., sleep quality, insomnia, circadian preference), as well as psychological traits and disorders known to influence sleep (e.g., anxiety, depression, arousal). The following tests were administered: 1) Insomnia Severity Index (ISI [15]), 2) Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ [16]), 3) Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI [17]), 4) Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ [18]), 5) State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI [19]), 6) Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D Scale [20]), 7) Ruminative Response Scale (RRS [21]), 8) Pre-Sleep Arousal Scale (PSAS [22]). All questionnaires were administered online via Qualtrics (Qualtrics, Provo, UT); total testing time was approximately 30 minutes.

A score from 0 to 3 was assigned to each response given to SAQ, leading to a maximum total score of 13 (see S1 Table in S1 File for details on the score assigned to each question). We set the cut-off to define a self-awakener at 11/13, based on our theoretical definition of self-awakener as well as on the score distribution. More specifically, the combination of the answers and scores needed to identify a self-awakener was as follows:

  • Question 1: “yes”, 1 POINT

  • Question 2: “only on nights before important events”, 3 POINTS

  • Questions 3: “No, just in the morning before the alarm clock”, 3 POINTS

  • Question 4: stratified answer, 1, 2, or 3 POINTS

  • Question 5: “When you have to wake up at an unusual time”, 3 POINTS

Thus, the minimum score needed to be defined as a self-awakener is 11, with some variability granted by Question 4.

Based on this cut-off, we divided participants in two groups: self-awakeners (SA, n = 9) and non-self-awakeners (nSA, n = 144). To evaluate possible differences between these two groups (self-awakeners vs. non-self-awakeners), separate non-parametric Mann-Whitney tests were performed using the outcome of each test of the battery as dependent variable. To account for the difference in sample size between the two groups, we bootstrapped each Mann-Whitney test (1000 repetitions); at each repetition, the nSA sub-group was randomly resampled (with replacement) from the global sample of 144 nSA individuals.

In addition, for the whole group of participants (i.e., without grouping them into SA and nSA), Spearman rank correlation tests were conducted to evaluate possible relationships between the SAQ scores and the answers given to other tests in the battery. Correlations tests were Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons (p = .05/number of performed tests).

Lastly, we evaluated the impact of circadian preference, as it has been reported that circadian preference might play a role in self-awakening [6, 23, 24]. Participants were divided into three groups (morning, intermediate, and evening type) based on their MEQ responses. Their responses to each SAQ question were analysed separately by means of a non-parametric ANOVA (Kruskal-Wallis test) to account for differences in data distribution between the three groups (S1 Table in S1 File).

All statistical analyses were conducted in R (version 4.1.2, https://www.R-project.org/ [25]) running in R studio (version 1.3.959, http://www.rstudio.com/ [26]).

Results

Literature review

Based on the search criteria, the database research identified 29 papers targeting the topic of self-awakening. 17 of these were published in peer-reviewed journals, 9 appeared as conference abstracts, and 3 reported non-experimental questionnaires or surveys. All retrieved articles and abstract were written in English. Most conference abstract (6 out of 9) coincided with published articles; the remaining 3 abstract reported non-experimental studies. Thus, only the 17 peer-reviewed journal articles were retained in the review (Table 2).

Table 2. Results of the literature review.

“Experiment type”: location of the experiment and type of objective sleep validation (PSG = polysomnography, EEG = electroencephalography; EOG = electrooculography; Act = Actigraphy); in case of multiple studies within a paper, S1 indicates the first and S2 the second study. “Sleep type”: nocturnal or diurnal sleep. “Nr. Participants”: total experimental sample size. “Age (SD)”: age range or average for the whole participant group and corresponding standard deviations (SD) when available. “Successful criterion”: criterion used to define a successful self-awakener, usually a time window around the expected awakening time (absolute value in minutes). “Nr. SA”: number of self-awakeners explicitly reported in the study or whether the study investigated successful self-awakening events. “% SA/total”: percentage of self-awakeners on the study sample size either reported in the manuscript or calculated based on the available data. “SA age (SD)”: age of self-awakeners and its standard deviation. Additional notes, symbols, and abbreviations: *15 participants were recruited but only 12 underwent EEG. ** depending on the questionnaire question and concerning the ability to wake-up with or without an alarm; *** five participants follow within the second criterion and are considered “moderately successful”; § = studies designed to select only self-awakeners; # = 9 self-awakeners were identified but data of one were lost, leaving 8 participants; ## = PSG failed for one participant, leaving 8 participants (6 F; mean age = 21.8 ±.6 years).

Article Experiment type Sleep type Nr. Participants (males / females) Age (SD) Successful criterion |minutes| Nr. SA (males/females) % SA/total SA age (SD)
1 Tart, 1970 [27] S1: home exp nocturnal 10 |10| events
S2: sleep lab (PSG) nocturnal 3 (2 /1) 18 (—) |10| events
2 Zung & Wilson, 1971 [28] sleep lab nocturnal 22 (4 / 18) 20–45 |10| events
3 Lavie et al., 1979 [29] S1: sleep lab (PSG) nocturnal 7 (6 / 1) 21–30 |10|
|20|
|30|
|40|
events
S2: sleep lab (PSG) nocturnal 2 |20| events
4 Bell, 1980 [30] home exp nocturnal 38 (20 /18) events
5 Zepelin, 1986 [31] S1: sleep lab (EEG, EOG) nocturnal 15 * (7 / 8) 15–32 |15| 11 73
S2: sleep lab (EEG, EOG) nocturnal 2 events
6 Hawkins, 1989 [32] home exp nocturnal 84 (31 / 53) 22.7 (6.7) |30| 22 26
7 Hawkins & Shaw, 1990 [33] home exp nocturnal 146 (38 / 108) 22.9 (6.2) |30| 15 10
8 Moorcroft et al., 1997 [34] S1: telephone survey nocturnal 269 (129 /140) 21–84 23, 29, 24 **
S2: home exp (Act) nocturnal 15 (6 / 9) 19–62 |10|
|15|
5
5 ***
33
9 Kaida, Nakano, et al., 2003 § [35] sleep lab (PSG) diurnal 11 (4 / 7) 21.7 (1.25) |5| 9 (3 / 6) 21.6 (1.24)
10 Kaida, Nittono, et al., 2003 [36] sleep lab (PSG) diurnal 14 (6 / 8) 21.3 (1.3) |5| 10 (4 / 6) 71
11 Kaida et al., 2005 § [37] home exp (sleep diary, PSG) diurnal 10 |5| 9 (7 / 2) 74.1 (5.01)
12 Kaida et al., 2006 § [38] home exp (sleep diary, PSG) diurnal |5| 9 (7 / 2) 74.1 (5.0)
13 Ikeda & Hayashi, 2008 § [39] sleep lab (PSG) nocturnal 10 (3 / 7) 21.7 (.06) |30| 8 # (2 / 6) 21.8 (0.7)
14 Matsuura & Hayashi, 2009 [40] sleep lab (PSG) nocturnal 17 (10 / 7) 19.7 (1.55) |30| 11 (5 / 6) 65 19.0 (1.73)
15 Ikeda & Hayashi, 2010 [41] sleep lab (PSG) nocturnal 10 (3 / 7) 21.8 (.6) |30| 9 ## (2 / 7) 90 21.8 (0.6)
16 Aritake et al., 2012 [10] sleep lab (PSG) nocturnal 15 (15 / 0) 22.1 (.7) |30| 7 (7 / 0) 47
17 Ikeda et al., 2014 [42] sleep lab (PSG) nocturnal 15 (15 / 0) 40.5 (6.9) |30| events

Study type

The majority of retained papers (n = 13/17, 76%) included an objective sleep evaluation being either PSG (n = 11/17, 65%), a combination of Electroencephalography (EEG) and Electrooculography (EOG) (n = 1/17, 6%), or actigraphy (n = 1/17, 6%); four papers (24%) included studies conducted either at home (n = 3/17, 18%) or in a sleep lab (n = 1/17, 6%) without objective validation. 13 out of 17 papers (76%) investigated self-awakenings from nocturnal sleep; the remaining 4 papers (24%) focused on diurnal periods of sleep (naps). Most papers (n = 13/17, 76%) described a single study, while four papers (24%) reported two studies; of these, three used the first study to select or confirm self-awakeners to be tested in the second study and only one tested a different sample in the second study.

Participants’ characteristics

All experiments combined tested 715 participants, of whom 299 were males (42%) and 392 were females (55%); for 24 participants (3%), this information was not specified. 14 papers reported the age of participants: 10 reported the average age (23.24 years, standard deviation (SD) = 6.24) in one or both studies described therein, yet, only 9 of these also reported the SD (average reported SD = 2.81 years); 4 papers reported an age range (20–45, single study; 21–30, single study; 15–32, single study; 21–84 for study 1 and 19–62 for study 2); 3 papers did not report any information on participants’ age.

Self-awakeners: Definition and details

Most papers (n = 16/17, 94%) reported a time criterion to define a successful self-awakener (i.e., the spontaneous awakening had to occur within a specific time window around the expected time to represent a hit), while one paper (6%) did not report any criterion. Studies reporting a criterion employed a single criterion (n = 14/17 papers, 82%) or, more rarely, multiple criteria (n = 2/17 papers, 12%). Of those employing a single criterion, the most employed criterion defined a self-awakener as a person who spontaneously wakes up within 30 minutes of a defined wake-up time (7 papers); other criteria targeted a time of 10 minutes (2 papers), 15 minutes (1 paper), or 5 minutes (4 papers) around the desired waking time. This narrow time criterion (5 minutes) was used by all the studies targeting diurnal naps of short duration (typically 20 minutes). Papers adopting multiple criteria opted for four-time windows around an expected time (10, 20, 30, and 40 minutes), or two-time windows (10 or 15 minutes).

11 papers reported the number of self-awakeners identified based on their criteria, leading to a total of 125 self-awakeners out of 715 total subjects (17%); 4 papers only reported the number of times successful self-awakening occurred, but did not define self-awakeners based on this result; 2 papers reported neither. The percentage of self-awakeners on the total number of participants in each study, either explicitly reported in the papers or calculated based on the available data, ranged from a minimum of 10% to a maximum of 90% (average percentage = 52%). This percentage is much higher than the 17% based on our calculations.

Eight papers reported the number of males (n = 37) and females (n = 35) among their self-awakeners. These numbers indicate a no difference in the gender of self-awakeners when pooling across studies (males = 30%, females = 28%, unknown = 42%). However, when looking at the percentages of females and males self-awakeners within each single paper, several studies reported a higher percentage of females compared to males (n = 5; f/m %: 67/33; 60/40; 75/25; 55/45; 78/22), while only two papers reported the opposite pattern (n = 2; f/m %: 22/78; 22/78), and one paper only included male participants (n = 1; f/m %: 0/100). 5 papers reported the average age in the SA participants and their SD (average reported mean = 38.73 years, average reported SD = 2.38).

SAQ questionnaire results

SAQ was administered to 164 students from Maastricht University (Maastricht, the Netherlands). Of these, 153 fully completed the testing battery and were retained for analyses (mean age ± SD: 20.62 ± 2.06, 135 F, 18 M, 1 not specified). A descriptive summary of the responses for each item and the proposed score is presented in S1 Table in S1 File.

The maximum score obtainable in the SAQ is 13; to define a possible self-awakener, we propose a cut-off equal to a score of 11 or higher (S1 Table in S1 File). Based on this classification, the current sample included 9/153 possible self-awakeners (6%, mean age ± SD: 21.57 ± 1.91, 7F, 2M).

Self-awakeners vs. non-self-awakeners

U-statistics and p-values were calculated as the mean results across the 1000 repetitions of the Mann-Whitney performed on the scores of each test in the battery for the SA and nSA groups. Results were non-significant for all the test scores (all p-values > .451; Fig 3).

Fig 3. Differences between Self-Awakeners (n = 9) and non-Self-Awakeners (n = 144) by test scores.

Fig 3

The difference between groups was non-significant for all measures. For all boxplots, each box contains the interquartile range (i.e., the range containing 50% of all observations) and the median value (middle line), while the whiskers represent values extending outside 50% of the observations. Abbreviations: ISI = Insomnia Severity Index; MEQ = Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire; PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; PSWQ = Penn State Worry Questionnaire STAI = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory RRS = Ruminative Response Scale; PSAS = Pre-Sleep Arousal Scale; CES = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (see S1 File for details).

Correlations between SAQ and other questionnaires

Spearman correlations showed significant negative correlations between SAQ and ISI scores (rs = -.19, p = .020) and between SAQ and PSQI (rs = -.22, p = .007). However, these results did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni corrected p-value: .05/11 = .005).

SAQ and circadian preference

Based on the MEQ scores, participants were divided into a morning (n = 16), an intermediate (n = 100), and an evening (n = 37) groups; their responses to each SAQ question were analysed separately (S1 Table in S1 File). The Kruskal-Wallis test showed no significant differences between the three groups (chi-squared = 0.16, df = 2, p = .93; Fig 4).

Fig 4. Distribution of the SAQ scores depending on the respondent’s circadian preference group.

Fig 4

Each boxplot contains the interquartile range (i.e., the range containing 50% of all observations) and the median value (middle line), while the whiskers represent values extending outside 50% of the observations.

Discussion

We reviewed publications investigating the phenomenon of self-awakening up to October 2022. We retrieved 17 papers, written in English, testing a total of 715 participants. Based on these data, we calculated a percentage of self-awakeners corresponding to the 17% of the total pool of participants. The paucity of peer-reviewed papers, as well as the low calculated percentage of self-awakeners, are indicative of self-awakening as a relatively understudied and moderately rare phenomenon. Further, the review revealed that several subjective and objective methods are typically employed in the study of self-awakening.

Although self-awakening is frequently observed in the general population, as reported in the seminal work by Child [7], where 59% of the participants (200 in total) claimed this ability, the definition of the phenomenon is far from uncontroversial or unambiguous. To clarify the phenomenon of interest, we proposed a theoretical disambiguation of this construct (Fig 1), starting from the assumption that self-awakening may represent a privileged model to study the recovery of volition during sleep, a state characterised by a minimal but reversible level of consciousness. Consequently, habitual self-awakening (i.e., waking up every day at the same time without the aid of external stimuli; e.g., [43]) cannot be considered the topic of our study as circadian factors would be a confounding factor.

In addition to issues related to definition, the current scientific approach to self-awakening is undermined by several methodological problems. For example, objective sleep assessments (e.g., PSG or actigraphy) are rarely used in the literature; yet, because they can reliably certify each participant’s wake-up time, they should be the first choice in the study of self-awakenings. Instead, subjective questionnaires and sleep diaries should only be used for screening purposes. Next to objective sleep assessment, one of the biggest methodological issues, acting also as an important source of variability between studies, is represented by the temporal criterion adopted to define a successful self-awakening. Several studies considered different time intervals to define a successful awakening, ranging from ± 5 minutes to ± 40 minutes around the target time. In addition, in some studies, participants were paid based on their accuracy (defined as the difference between target time and actual wake-up time [28, 29]). Importantly, success rate was also influenced when the target time was set close to one’s usual habitual wake-up time [32, 34]. All these variables create huge variability across studies.

The neural mechanisms underlying self-awakening are far from being understood. Studies investigating polysomnographic alterations right before self-awakening reported discrepant results. Although it could be possible that the attempt to awake at predetermined time during the night could lighten sleep by reducing slow waves, previous findings do not clearly support this view. Born and collaborators [44] found that time spent in different stages, sleep stages’ time distribution, and arousals showed no alteration. Similarly, also Zung and Wilson [28] reported no significant difference in sleep macrostructure between control and experimental nights. Remarkably, a study by Aritake and colleagues [10] investigated the neural correlates of self-awakening through near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) and polysomnography. They also found no differences in any sleep stages between experimental and control conditions. However, they found an increase in oxyhemoglobin in the right prefrontal cortex and a decrease in delta power 30 minutes before the self-awakening. The increase of oxyhemoglobin in this area is important, since some studies reported its involvement in time recognition [45, 46]. This finding suggests the importance of the activation of brain areas related to time estimation abilities and the disengagement from deep sleep to successfully perform self-awakening.

The second part of this work aimed at developing a questionnaire for screening participants in the general population and for identifying possible self-awakeners, who would then need to be confirmed in an empirical study. According to the proposed scoring method and cut-off score, we identified 9 out of 153 possible self-awakeners. This low percentage (5%) is even more surprising when considering that most participants (98%) answered yes to Question 1 of the SAQ (“Have you ever spontaneously woken up at a desired time, without the help of an alarm clock or other external devices?”). This suggests that most people think they can self-awake or can directly experience it. However, we speculate, the rather low percentage observed when applying the proposed criteria may be indicative of a naive conception of the phenomenon that does not adhere to the proposed definition. Once more, this result underlines the need for a careful disambiguation of the phenomenon, when screening the general population with subjective and retrospective measures. When investigating multiple awakenings before a target time (Question 3: “When you experience these spontaneous awakenings, do you wake up more than once throughout the night?”), 42% of the sample answered positively. This outcome is consistent with a possible deterioration of sleep before a self-awakening as negative sleep changes (increased arousal, wake after sleep onset, and sleep stage 1) are often reported [6]. Yet, the presence of conscious or unconscious awakenings (for example, due to checking the clock) should be excluded. Another important aspect concerns target time: as previously discussed, a person should be able to wake up easily when a target time is set close to their habitual wake-up time, in particular during the final hours of a sleep period. Question 5 aimed at investigating this aspect (“In your experience, these spontaneous awakenings are more likely to occur…”). Interestingly, 76% of the sample answered that the phenomenon is more likely to occur when waking up at an unusual time (e.g., 3 a.m.). Notably, we observed a weak negative correlation between the score obtained for SAQ, ISI, and PSQI. This relation suggests that self-awakeners (i.e., individuals with a high SAQ score) do not show signs of either insomnia (as evidenced by the low ISI scores) or low sleep quality (as evidenced by the low PSQI scores). This result complies with the definition of self-awakening as an independent phenomenon, unrelated to concomitant sleep disorders. While poor sleep quality or insomnia may also cause spontaneous awakenings, the presence of these disorders should constitute an important exclusion criterion when studying self-awakening.

The investigation of self-awakening is rather relevant, because it might represent an interesting model for studying specific cognitive processes even in states of minimal or absent consciousness. We proposed that three basic cognitive operations may be involved: (i) the encoding and storing of the target time, which must be readily accessible; (ii) the comparison between actual time and target time, which presumes the existence of a psychobiological ultradian timekeeper; (iii) the restoration of volitional control during sleep to wake up (Fig 5).

Fig 5. Proposed schematic model of the cognitive operations potentially involved in self-awakening.

Fig 5

Although simple, the model proposed in Fig 5 highlights several important points, in particular the lack of a reliable model for time estimation in the minutes-to hours range. This could be explained by the poor knowledge on brain ultradian oscillators (i.e., biorhythm having a period of less than 24 hours) in relation to sleep perception during sleep. Rather, processes involved in the internal timekeeping of circadian rhythms have been extensively studied. These are biologically implemented in a relatively small set of ~10,000 neurons in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus and oscillate with a 24-hour rhythm [47]. Little is known about the timekeepers of sleep (ultradian and not circadian). Some studies directly or indirectly investigated the relationship between time perception and sleep state but with rather varied results. For example, Aritake-Okada and colleagues [48] showed that time estimation ability during sleep is associated with slow wave sleep. In particular, deep sleep led to an overestimation of elapsed time. A recent article [49] challenged the common notion that subjective sleep depth, and hence elapsed time, is associated with slow wave sleep, pointing to the relevance of dream-like conscious experiences in perceiving sleep depth. Strikingly, none of these studies directly evaluated the contribution of oscillatory brain activity of time perception during sleep. Future insights into this issue will be fundamental to understand and explain the phenomenon of self-awakening.

Lastly, we put forward some limitations of the current research. First, as stated in the introduction, there are relatively few studies on self-awakening; consequently, the conclusions that can be drawn from our systematic review are preliminary. We sincerely hope that the current manuscript may stimulate further research and lead to a more in-depth understanding of this fascinating phenomenon. Second, the number of self-awakeners compared to non-self-awakeners in our student sample was rather low: Because of the lack of a screening instrument and the relatively low percentage of self-awakeners in the general population, we could not select directly self-awakeners to match non-self-awakeners, nor was it possible for us to test indefinitely. While we partially addressed this issue with our statistical approach, the development of a screening questionnaire–such as the one presented here–may tackle this problem more effectively, by allowing researchers to pre-select and match groups of non- and self-awakeners. Third, our study lacked an objective sleep evaluation to confirm whether the self-awakeners identified by SAQ were indeed capable of spontaneously waking up at a desired time. Objective sleep evaluation by means of PSG is of paramount importance in the study of self-awakening, and we strongly advise future studies to include these measurements.

In conclusion, 17 studies were selected for a review of the literature on self-awakening. These studies reported self-awakening as a relatively rare and understudied phenomenon. However, important methodological differences are present between studies, ranging from the experimental designs to the criteria for defining successful self-awakening and evaluating its accuracy. These differences significantly impact the interpretation of current results. The proposed questionnaire offers first interesting insights on self-awakening and might represent a useful screening tool for studies targeting self-awakening and, more in general, volition during sleep. Future studies employing objective measurement such as EEG, PSG, and actigraphy, with the help of a comprehensive and innovative study design and a clear definition of self-awakening, are needed for a rigorous scientific study of this fascinating phenomenon.

Supporting information

S1 File. Additional results, details, and SAQ, Italian version.

(PDF)

Data Availability

Data and code are available in OSF (https://osf.io/kaunw/). All the material is public and freely available for download and use.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Stickgold R, Walker M. The neuroscience of sleep. San Diego: Academic Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tempesta D, Socci V, De Gennaro L, Ferrara M. Sleep and emotional processing. Sleep Med Rev. 2018;40: 183–195. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Klinzing JG, Niethard N, Born J. Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22: 1598–1610. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0467-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Walker MP. The Role of Sleep in Cognition and Emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1156: 168–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04416.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Baird B, Mota-Rolim SA, Dresler M. The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;100: 305–323. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hayashi M, Matsuura N, Ikeda H. Preparation for Awakening: Self-Awakening vs. Forced Awakening: Preparatory Hanges in the Pre-Awakening Period. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2010;93: 109–127. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(10)93005-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Child CM. Statistics of “Unconscious Cerebration”. Am J Psychol. 1892;5: 249. doi: 10.2307/1410868 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Vaschide N. Le sommeil et les rêves. Flammation. Paris; 1911.
  • 9.Tononi G, Massimini M. Why Does Consciousness Fade in Early Sleep? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129: 330–334. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Aritake S, Higuchi S, Suzuki H, Kuriyama K, Enomoto M, Soshi T, et al. Increased cerebral blood flow in the right frontal lobe area during sleep precedes self-awakening in humans. BMC Neurosci. 2012;13: 153. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gotzsche PC, Ioannidis JPAA, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. BMJ. 2009;6: b2700. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2700 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Page MJ, Moher D, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372: n160. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. Updating guidance for reporting systematic reviews: development of the PRISMA 2020 statement. J Clin Epidemiol. 2021;134: 103–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372: n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bastien CH, Vallières A, Morin CM. Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2001;2: 297–307. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(00)00065-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4: 99–110. Available: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2015-49334-001 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28: 193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Meyer TJ, Miller ML, Metzger RL, Borkovec TD. Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1990;28: 487–495. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1: 385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Roelofs J, Muris P, Huibers M, Peeters F, Arntz A. On the measurement of rumination: A psychometric evaluation of the ruminative response scale and the rumination on sadness scale in undergraduates. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2006;37: 299–313. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nicassio PM, Mendlowitz DR, Fussell JJ, Petras L. The phenomenology of the pre-sleep state: The development of the pre-sleep arousal scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1985;23: 263–271. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(85)90004-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Matsuura N, Hayashi M, Hori T. Comparison of sleep/wake habits of university students with or without a habit of self-awakening. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;56: 223–224. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2002.00973.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Crabb PB. Technology and self-regulation: The case of alarm clock use. Soc Behav Pers. 2003;31: 343–348. doi: 10.2224/SBP.2003.31.4.343 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria; 2020. https://www.r-project.org/
  • 26.RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. Boston, MA; 2020. http://www.rstudio.com/
  • 27.Tart CT. Waking from sleep at a preselected time. J Am Soc Psychosom Dent Med. 1970;17: 3–16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Zung WWK, Wilson WP. Time estimation during sleep. Biol Psychiatry. 1971;3: 159–164. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lavie P, Oksenberg A, Zomer J. It’s time, you must wake up now. Percept Mot Skills. 1979;49: 447–450. doi: 10.2466/pms.1979.49.2.447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bell CR. Awakening from sleep at a pre-set time. Percept Mot Skills. 1980;50: 503–508. doi: 10.2466/pms.1980.50.2.503 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Zepelin H. REM sleep and the timing of self-awakenings. Bull Psychon Soc. 1986;24: 254–256. doi: 10.3758/BF03330132 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hawkins J. Sleep disturbance in intentional self-awakenings: behavior-genetic and transient factors. Percept Mot Skills. 1989;69: 507–510. doi: 10.2466/pms.1989.69.2.507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hawkins J, Shaw P. Sleep satisfaction and intentional self-awakening: An alternative protocol for self-report data. Percept Mot Skills. 1990;70: 447–450. doi: 10.2466/pms.1990.70.2.447 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Moorcroft WHW, Kayser KHKH, Griggs AJ. Subjective and objective confirmation of the ability to self-awaken at a self-predetermined time without using external means. Sleep. 1997;20: 40–45. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.1.40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kaida K, Nakano E, Nittono H, Hayashi M, Hori T. The effects of self-awakening on heart rate activity in a short afternoon nap. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2003;114: 1896–1901. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00167-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kaida K, Nittono H, Hayashi M, Hori T. Effects of self-awakening on sleep structure of a daytime short nap and on subsequent arousal levels. Percept Mot Skills. 2003;97: 1073–1084. doi: 10.2466/pms.2003.97.3f.1073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kaida K, Ogawa K, Hayashi M, Hori T. Self-awakening prevents acute rise in blood pressure and heart rate at the time of awakening in elderly people. Ind Health. 2005;43: 179–185. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.43.179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kaida K, Ogawa K, Nittono H, Hayashi M, Takahashi M, Hori T. Self-awakening, sleep inertia, and P3 amplitude in elderly people. Percept Mot Skills. 2006;102: 339–351. doi: 10.2466/pms.102.2.339-351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ikeda H, Hayashi M. Electroencephalogram activity before self-awakening. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2008;6: 256–259. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2008.00366.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Matsuura N, Hayashi M. Effects of habitual self-awakening on nocturnal sleep, autonomic activity prior to awakening, and subjective condition after awakening. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2009;7: 172–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2009.00398.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ikeda H, Hayashi M. The effect of self-awakening from nocturnal sleep on sleep inertia. Biol Psychol. 2010;83: 15–19. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ikeda H, Kubo T, Kuriyama K, Takahashi M. Self-awakening improves alertness in the morning and during the day after partial sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2014;23: 673–680. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12176 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Malloggi S, Conte F, Albinni B, Gronchi G, Ficca G, Giganti F. Sleep and psychological characteristics in habitual self-awakeners and forced awakeners. Chronobiol Int. 2022;39: 547–556. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2021.2003375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Born J, Hansen K, Marshall L, Mölle M, Fehm HL. Timing the end of nocturnal sleep. Nature. 1999;397: 29–30. doi: 10.1038/16166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Maquet P, Lejeune H, Pouthas V, Bonnet M, Casini L, Macar F, et al. Brain Activation Induced by Estimation of Duration: A PET Study. Neuroimage. 1996;3: 119–126. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Coull JT, Vidal F, Nazarian B, Macar F. Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science. 2004;303: 1506–1508. doi: 10.1126/science.1091573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hastings MH, Maywood ES, Brancaccio M. Generation of circadian rhythms in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19: 453–469. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0026-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Aritake-Okada S, Uchiyama M, Suzuki H, Tagaya H, Kuriyama K, Matsuura M, et al. Time estimation during sleep relates to the amount of slow wave sleep in humans. Neurosci Res. 2009;63: 115–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2008.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Stephan AM, Lecci S, Cataldi J, Siclari F. Conscious experiences and high-density EEG patterns predicting subjective sleep depth. Curr Biol. 2021;31: 5487–5500.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Manuel Spitschan

2 Sep 2022

PONE-D-22-15227Sleeping with time in mind? A literature review and a proposal for a screening questionnaire on self-awakeningPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Verga,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

 Please address all comments raised by the reviewer.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Oct 17 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Manuel Spitschan

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

3. Please note that supplementary tables (should remain/ be uploaded) as separate "supporting information" files".

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Summary:

The paper by Verga et al. titled “Sleeping with time in mind? A literature review and a proposal for a screening questionnaire on self-awakening” investigates the ability to self-awake at a pre-set time without external help, i.e. the ability to end one’s sleep episode voluntarily. The authors provide a systematic literature review following the PRISMA guidelines, summarise the results of the selected studies, present a working definition for “self-awakening” and present their own screening questionnaire they developed based on the literature review. Their questionnaire was designed in English and translated to Italian and administered to 164 students at Maastricht University.

Altogether, the authors find substantial diversity in the 17 reviewed studies in terms of quality, study design and methods. Furthermore, the concept of “self-awakening” has been operationalised and framed differently across studies and the authors present a working definition for this phenomenon. Based on the review and their own study results, the ability to self-awake does not seem to be a common phenomenon albeit this results is very much limited by the small sample sizes across reviewed studies.

The study has several strength which are the adherence to PRISMA guidelines, access to the data, clarification and proposal of the concept behind “self-awakening”, clear language and writing, nice presentation of the results and a succinct summary. Limitations/weaknesses include the missing PRISMA flowchart and no detail on the quality of the reviewed studies despite this being mentioned, low sample size in their own study, raw data only available upon request and in general a relatively superficial handling and discussion of the phenomenon, possible mechanisms, underlying neurobiological facts or discussion of other known facts from related fields, such as time keeping mechanisms when awake.

I recommend major revisions which have to include a more in-depth discussion of the phenomenon including possible mechanisms.

Major issues:

Introduction:

1. The authors seem to make the point that sleep is still viewed as an inactive brain state. This view is outdated and I advise to discuss this more in light of the current evidence concerning sleep architecture, possibly functions of sleep, circadian mechanism/sleep homeostatic components, and cognitive aspects of sleep and during sleep

2. Please add references to the claim that habitual self-awakening, i.e. waking up at the same time every day, is indeed driven by circadian mechanism. I’m unsure whether the evidence is clear on this.

3. In general, more references and in-depth background to the current topic is warranted. For example, what could be the biological differences between habitual self-awakening and self-awakening ability and so on?

Methods:

4. Could the authors please provide information about the program used for statistical analyses, including the version number and used packages if applicable

5. The data are not really openly available, they are only available upon request. Why?

6. Could the authors please clarify what is meant by “NEAR/0” (lines 120-128)

7. It is unclear why each study could obtain a max. of 13 points (line 152). Please clarify.

8. Cohen’s kappa for the inter-rater reliability was rather low, please comment on the difficulties

9. It in unclear how the sampling procedure for their own study was done, nor why they stopped at a certain amount of participants

10. Why was the cut-off for their SAQ at 11; Could the authors provide an explanation?

11. Lines 191-195: More information is needed to understand what was actually carried out: 1 analysis, 9 separate analyses, or something completely different?

12. SAQ “Aim” section: The information given under “aim” in Table 1 points towards “night before a test or important meeting” -> this is confounded by possibly anxiety feelings or feeling nervous; yet the authors suggest that they are probing for self-awakening explicitly without underlying conditions such as stress or anxiety. Could the authors clarify why they think this is still valid?

13. SAQ Q2 and Q5: I think that a neutral answer option should be given here, e.g. “neither” (Q5) and the scale is not adequate in Q2 (every day vs at least 1/week or only on nights before important events). What about other timescales?

Results:

14. Please clarify what is meant by “ps” (line 298) and “rs” (line 301). Multiple p values and r values? If so, maybe giving a range would be more appropriate.

15. Lines 305: It is unclear to me what the MANOVA actually tested; what were the dependent and what the independent variables?

16. Lines 306-314: Could the term “univariate test” be replaced with the actual name of the test that was conducted?

17. Fig 2: The colour-coding legend “self-awakener” is currently 0 and 1 without explanation. I suggest to change to yes/no instead. Also, I strongly recommend to plot the underlying data points, report the sample size per item if not the same across all plots and give an explanation of the errors bars (SD?). The individual tests are only reported with abbreviations and should be explained in the Figure legend.

18. Fig. 3: Similar to Fig. 2, underlying data points to be added, as well as a description of the error bars and sample size.

19. Could it be that in S1 Q5 the 2 numbers (36 vs 117) in the “all” column gut confused?

Discussion:

19. Line 355 talks about the seminal work of Child but does not explain in detail what it was (this was first presented in the Introduction but could be repeated here for clarity)

20. In general, the discussion is nice to read and succinct. I feel though that it is substantially missing a lot of background information, especially on the context of circadian influences, possible mechanisms, time keeping mechanisms that happen outside of sleep, theoretical underpinnings etc. I strongly urge the authors to provide more information on possible mechanisms

21. Lines 416-421: this section needs more additional references and in depth-description of the underlying mechanisms

22. The discussion does not contain a limitation section. Could the authors reflect critically on their work and mention some limitations?

23. The working definition of their concept should be discussed more carefully in this discussion. What does it contain? What does it not contain? What could the underlying mechanisms be? Why is it so difficult to find a common definition, and so on…

#####

Minor issues:

Introduction:

23. Lines 92-96: The authors could expand this section to meet my recommendations above: this is an interesting observation and provides first hints towards possible mechanisms

24. Line 88: 51% of participants - what was the overall sample size?

25. Lines 101-102: It would be nice to discuss the concept of volition. What is it? What are the brain mechanisms involved? How would this be related to certain sleep stages etc?

Methods:

25. Lines 120-121: Unfortunately, the search is a little bit outdated, maybe the authors should consider re-doing this to check if new studies have been published meanwhile?

26. Lines 120-130: Could the authors please present the search terms differently to facilitate reading?i

27. Lines 131: I don’t quite understand the argument concerning language bias: if only English publications were selected anyways, why does this reduced language bias?

28. Line 139: What is meant with “non-experimental”?

29. Line 153: What was the median and the range?

30. SAQ: Please provide more information about the translation. How many people were involved? Was it back translated into English? Was a professional translator involved?

31. Lines 171-176: please provide information about the individual scales and their interpretation. Do higher number mean more severe insomnia etc?

32. Lines 184-189: The big difference in sample sizes between self-awakeners (n=9) and non self-awakeners (n=144) is problematic. Did the authors consider any measures to increase the sample size in general? On the very positive side, they used bootstrapping methods to account for this limitation.

Results:

33. Did the authors of the reviewed studies try to explain underlying mechanisms? If so, please review them when reviewing the results of the reviewed studies.

34. Line 254: Does the term “significantly higher” refer to a statistical test or does it just mean “much higher”?

35. Lines 256-261: I don’t understand how the 8 papers which looked at gender differences found no gender difference but “within each paper’s self-awakeners’ samples, percentages of females and males […] showed a predominance of studies with a higher % of females compared to males”. What is the difference? How do these 2 sentences come together?

36. Table 2 legend: rather long. Instead I propose to explain some of the terms in the column headers or in the cells (e.g. instead of using the abbreviation “E” just write “events”)

37. If using the MEQ and not the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire, I would rather refer to circadian preference and not chronotype. These two are slightly different concepts.

38. Fig. 3: I would suggest to increase the amount of ticks of the y-Axis, e.g. 0,2,4,6,8 etc to facilitate reading the values

Discussion:

39. Line 349: I would suggest to report the actual number of participants, not just more than 700

40. Line 351-352: “This result suggests self-awakening as a relatively understudied […] phenomenon”. Which result suggest this? It reads like the 17% but this does not make sense. Please clarify.

Reviewer #2: This review summarized 17 studies examining self-awakening and proposes a screening questionnaire for self-awakeners. Out of 715 total participants, 125 (17%) reported being self-awakeners, with an average age of 23.24 years and a slight predominance of males compared to females. 9 out of the 153 Dutch students are possible self-awakeners. These results reveal that studies and findings on self-awakening are a relatively rare phenomenon.

Although the topic is intriguing, several serious concerns arise and are listed below:

1.I would delete the part on lucid dreaming lines 62-67.

2.Line 76 can’t it be a learned behavior and/or circadian rhythm issue?

3.Lines 72-78 need references.

4.Line 108 given your proposed taxonomy which type of self-awakening is being studied? This should be specified.

5.Lines 126-128 search terms cover aspects beyond the self-awaking ability. Also, if different languages were allowed in the search, what were the search terms to prevent bias? Please note, Lines 214-215 only mention English papers and abstracts.

6.Line 120-121 an update of the literature is needed.

7.Line 131 was not elaborated on in later sections

8.Line 143 Quality appraisal – the applicability, hence validity, of the tool is very questionable given that it is designed for cohort studies. How many of your study designs were cohort studies?

9.Line 159 Was there any forward/backward translation done? Foremost if this was applied at Maastricht university, was a Dutch version used? Also, what are the psychometric properties of the items selected from the literature, and the SAQ as well? The tools from which the items were selected should be discussed in the Introduction or even Methods. The re-using of items from the literature needs to be based on their psychometric property and not merely face validity.

10.What is the purpose of an Italian version?

11.Line 171-177 was any randomization applied?

12.Line 183 If SAQ contains 5 questions, and each item is scored as a 0 to 3, how can the maximum score be 13? This is misleading, especially when reading Table 1. A justification for the scoring method is needed. A report of several psychometric properties for the SAQ is lacking.

13.How does the response of Q3 “No, just in the morning before the alarm clock” discriminate sufficiently toward self-awaking ability versus habitual self-awakening? For 58% of the sample, this is the answer. Responses like Q3 prime different interpretations throughout your questionnaire, which will affect the validity and reliability of your SAQ.

14.A power calculation of the needed sample size is required because this study is underpowered. The literature estimate is 17% whereas your sample has 6%.

15.The MANOVA is not valid and should be discarded. Lines 308,311, 312 etc , no Bonferroni correction?

16.Information is needed on the validity/reliability of the test battery applied to Dutch students.

17.Is Line 367 not contradictory to your purpose, i.e., creating the SAQ? Namely, is a screening tool not an outcome measure?

Minor

Line 368, and Line 400 sentences need some English correction

Line 382-383, line 405-410 is speculative, and so is Figure 4.

Authors should state the statistical software used for the analyses

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Manuel Spitschan

8 Feb 2023

PONE-D-22-15227R1Sleeping with time in mind? A literature review and a proposal for a screening questionnaire on self-awakeningPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Verga,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

Please include the points raised by Reviewer 1 in the paper – not just in the rebuttal.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 25 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Manuel Spitschan

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Additional Editor Comments:

Please include the points raised by Reviewer 1 in the paper – not just in the rebuttal.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you very much for the careful revision of your article and integrating my suggestions. I recommend publication based on the condition that you simply add some of your answer to my questions also to the document since I believe that these might also help other readers to better understand your article. These include the following:

- the explanation about power analysis and why recruitment stopped (point 9) should also be included in the methods

- maybe also include the description of point 10

- Fig. 2 is very nice, thank you for adopting my suggestions. I still miss the different sample sizes for both self-awakeners and non self-awakeners; even though they don’t change, as you suggest, one has to go back and consult somewhere in the text

- point 20: thanks for adding this information to the discussion, this improves it

- point 28: I guess my point was to specify the study design (i.e. observational study) instead of defining it by what it is NOT, i.e. non-experimental

- point 30: please add this information about translation in the method section

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 2

Manuel Spitschan

6 Mar 2023

Sleeping with time in mind? A literature review and a proposal for a screening questionnaire on self-awakening

PONE-D-22-15227R2

Dear Dr. Verga,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Manuel Spitschan

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Manuel Spitschan

13 Mar 2023

PONE-D-22-15227R2

Sleeping with time in mind? A literature review and a proposal for a screening questionnaire on self-awakening

Dear Dr. Verga:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Manuel Spitschan

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Additional results, details, and SAQ, Italian version.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PLOS response to reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: R2_response_to_reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Data and code are available in OSF (https://osf.io/kaunw/). All the material is public and freely available for download and use.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES