Abstract
A central goal of computational psychiatry is to identify systematic relations between transdiagnostic dimensions of psychiatric symptomatology and the latent learning and decision-making computations that inform individuals' thoughts, feelings, and choices. The vast majority of psychiatric disorders emerge prior to adulthood, yet little work has extended these computational approaches to study the development of psychopathology. Here, we lay out a roadmap for future studies implementing this approach by developing empirically and theoretically informed hypotheses about how developmental changes in model-based control of action and Pavlovian learning processes may modulate vulnerability to anxiety and addiction. We highlight how insights from studies leveraging computational approaches to characterize the normative developmental trajectories of clinically relevant learning and decision-making processes may suggest promising avenues for future developmental computational psychiatry research.
Introduction
During adolescence, individuals undergo pronounced social, sexual, and intellectual changes, with the adolescent brain exhibiting corresponding structural and functional changes to adapt to these new demands (1). Adolescence is also a time of increased vulnerability to a wide array of mental disorders including anxiety, substance use disorders, mood disorders, psychosis, eating disorders, and personality disorders (2). Indeed, twenty percent of adolescents develop a mental disorder that persists into adulthood (3), with early emergence associated with greater clinical severity of mental illness (4,5). Despite widespread recognition that most forms of psychopathology can be conceptualized as developmental disorders (2), a mechanistic account of adolescents’ psychiatric vulnerability has proven elusive. Recent theoretical proposals argue that focusing on learning – the process through which experiences modify subsequent behavior – may be critical for achieving a more mechanistic understanding of the emergence of psychopathologies (6).
The field of computational psychiatry leverages computational methods in order to better understand and treat psychiatric symptomatology (7). A primary goal of the field of computational psychiatry is to identify latent learning processes that underpin clinically relevant dimensions of behavior (8,9). A central premise of this approach is that psychiatric disorders can be conceptualized as constellations of transdiagnostic behavioral phenotypes that reflect latent neurocognitive computations (10). A growing literature in adults has identified systematic relations between specific psychiatric symptom dimensions and latent learning and decision-making computations that inform individuals' thoughts, feelings, and choices (8,11). The extension of this approach to study the development of psychopathology is only in its nascent stages, with initial theoretical efforts delineating putative computational mechanisms underlying the development of autism (12) and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) (13), and a few studies successfully linking learning phenotypes to clinical symptoms in developmental populations (14,15). Despite the sparsity of empirical findings in developmental computational psychiatry, there may be value in integrating findings in adults that link specific learning computations to psychiatric symptom dimensions, with the growing body of research that has begun to characterize the normative development of these learning phenotypes using computational approaches (16). Here, we lay out a roadmap for future research leveraging such computational approaches to study the development of addiction and anxiety – two classes of disorders that have a clear peri-adolescent developmental trajectory (17), significant comorbidity, overlapping transdiagnostic symptomatology (18), and are proposed to involve aberrations in learning computations (19,20).
Addiction is a chronic, relapsing condition that typically begins during adolescence, characterized by cycles of craving, intoxication, binging, and withdrawal (21). Over forty percent of teens experiment with drugs (22) and while the majority reach adulthood without developing substance-use disorders, for some, experimentation rapidly progresses into problem use (23). Anxiety is “a future-oriented mood state associated with preparation for possible, upcoming negative events” (distinct from fear, an alarm response to present or imminent danger) (24). Anxiety disorders can be subdivided into conditions that involve heightened threat response, such as panic disorder, phobias, or posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and conditions that involve increased worry and rumination, such as generalized anxiety (24). Anxiety has a lifetime prevalence of over thirty percent (25), with typical emergence between early school age and adolescence, and the majority of diagnoses made prior to young adulthood (17). Addiction and anxiety exhibit symptomatic overlap (see Figure 1.A) which may stem, in part, from common underlying learning mechanisms (see Figure 1.B). This review focuses on two learning computations that are implicated in both disorders, and exhibit developmental changes across adolescence: model-based control of action and Pavlovian learning processes (additional learning computations are discussed in Box 1). For each learning process, we detail its normative developmental trajectory and review studies in adults highlighting its roles in addiction and anxiety. We then discuss potential relations between developmental changes in computational phenotypes and symptom expression (see Figure 1.C for graphical illustration, and Table 1 for a summary of key empirical findings and theory-based hypotheses).
Box 1: Additional computational phenotypes of interest.
Valence asymmetries
The relative weight individuals place on positive versus negative outcomes may confer risk or resilience to psychopathology. Whereas healthy adults tend to overweight recent positive experiences relative to negative experiences (143,144), anxious individuals show the opposite tendency (145). Recent computational work assessing developmental changes in valence asymmetries without confounding task demands (16,146) has observed that adolescents overweight negative, relative to positive, outcomes in learning and in memory, more than children or adults (147-150). Such a tendency might contribute to internalizing disorders through increased encoding of negative events (151).
Adaptation of learning rates
Adapting one’s learning to dynamic environmental statistics allows for the optimization of behavior (152). Anxious individuals are less able to dynamically adjust learning rates in volatile environments (138,139,153,154), which may contribute to their difficulties coping with conditions of high uncertainty. Few studies have examined developmental change in the optimal adjustment of learning rates, though recent work suggests that the ability to adapt valenced learning rates may improve with age (155).
Metacognition
Systematic distortions in metacognition – the ability to reflect on and evaluate one’s behavior (156) – may be a transdiagnostic factor associated with poor mental health (157). A tendency to be overconfident in one’s performance (irrespective of the actual performance) is associated with heightened compulsivity, while a tendency toward underconfidence is associated with anxiety-depression symptoms (158). Metacognitive abilities improve from childhood through adolescence (159,160). Future studies should examine the developmental emergence of these metacognitive distortions, and how they may relate to the progression of anxiety symptomatology and addictive behaviors (161,162).
Table 1:
Computational phenotype |
Relation to addiction | Relation to anxiety | Developmental findings |
Hypothesized mechanisms of developmental vulnerability to psychopathology |
---|---|---|---|---|
Model-based control | Drug exposure decreases model-based control. 𝔼A(41-44) 𝕋(38) Model-based control is reduced in drug-dependent individuals. 𝔼H(46-50) 𝕋(38) Reduced model-based control is associated with greater propensity toward compulsive behavior. 𝔼H (54,55) Reduced model-based control predicts the emergence of compulsive drug consumption. 𝔼A(45) 𝔼H(58) |
Worry and rumination may depend in part on model-based simulation processes. 𝔼H(68) 𝕋(65) | Reliance on model-based control increases with age. 𝔼H(15,29,32-36) 𝕋(28) | Vulnerability to developing addiction may be greater at younger ages due to reduced model-based control. Drug exposure during development might attenuate the normative development of model-based control. Age-related improvement in mental simulation abilities might lay the cognitive foundation for heightened worry and rumination (in interaction with negative valence biases in information processing). |
Pavlovian processes | ||||
Pavlovian-instrumental transfer | Pavlovian-instrumental transfer is greater in drug-dependent individuals and high-risk drinkers. 𝔼H(89-93) Drug exposure increases Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. 𝔼A(84-88) |
Anxious individuals exhibit greater Pavlovian-instrumental transfer.𝔼H(98,99) | Pavlovian-instrumental transfer decreases from childhood into adolescence 𝔼H(100) and may stabilize from adolescence into adulthood 𝔼H(100,101). | Developmental changes in Pavlovian-instrumental transfer may modulate the influence of valenced environmental exposures on symptom expression (e.g. cue-induced craving/drug seeking). |
Latent state inference | Alteration in latent state inference relates to anxious symptomatology.𝔼H(117,118) Propensity to infer multiple latent states is associated with extinction-resistance of threat associations 𝔼H(114). |
The tendency to infer distinct latent states based on shifts in environmental statistics may increase with age 𝕋(113). | Anxiety in younger individuals might reflect difficulty discriminating between threat and safety states due to a tendency to infer fewer latent states. At later ages, anxiety might reflect extinction-resistance of threat associations due to a tendency to infer multiple latent states. |
Model-based control and its development
Two classes of algorithms can guide instrumental action selection: A “model-based” system evaluates actions by simulating their potential outcomes using a cognitive model of environmental states, which specifies how one transitions between states and the outcomes associated with each state; in contrast, a “model-free” system instead estimates and stores the mean values of actions based on previously experienced rewards and punishments (26). These two evaluative processes have different advantages and shortcomings. Model-free value computation, a process proposed to give rise to habitual actions (27), enables rapid, reflexive repetition of previously rewarded actions, but is not immediately sensitive to changes in current goals or action-outcome contingencies. In contrast, model-based evaluation is a slower but more flexible deliberative process, which confers greater sensitivity to environmental changes and supports counterfactual reasoning.
Convergent cross-species findings in diverse tasks suggest that reliance on model-based control increases with age (28). A common assay of model-based control is outcome devaluation. In this paradigm, individuals first learn to perform a rewarded action, which then gets devalued. For example, by allowing the individual to consume the reward until sated. An individual who learns and updates action-outcome relations will reduce performance of an action that now leads to undesirable reward, reflecting goal-directed consideration of the expected outcome (e.g., open the fridge only if I currently want food), rather than a habitual response based on a cached action value (e.g., open the fridge because it is a good thing to do). In a study examining outcome devaluation in one- to four-year-old children, sensitivity to devaluation of video clips increased with age (29). A study in adolescent and adult rodents found both age groups exhibited sensitivity to devaluation of food reward outcomes, but only adult rodents exhibited sensitivity to degradation of the action-outcome contingency, a manipulation that probes the sensitivity of actions to the strength of causal relation between action and outcome (30). These results suggest that these components of model-based cognition exhibit unique developmental trajectories. Model-based behavior can also be assessed in sequential decision-making tasks that index the degree to which an individual uses a mental model of the task structure to maximize reward, rather than simply repeating previously rewarded actions (e.g. the “two-step task” (31)). Several studies using such tasks in human participants have found that model-based control of action increases with age from middle childhood through young adulthood (32-36), an effect also evident within individuals longitudinally across adolescence (15). Moreover, while adults increase their recruitment of model-based control when it is incentivized (37), this capacity for “meta-control” is absent in children (32) and may increase gradually across adolescence into adulthood (33). Collectively, these findings suggest that model-based control increases across development, and that the age at which model-based control is recruited may depend upon the complexity of learning, updating, and planning with a mental model in a particular environment.
Reduced model-based control and the development of addiction
Reduced model-based control and increased dominance of model-free evaluation has been hypothesized to play a mechanistic role in the emergence of addiction. The progression of addiction typically involves a narrowing of goals to focus on drug seeking and consumption, even despite adverse consequences. Drug consumption is proposed to reflect, in part, a shift from goal-directed action, guided by positive hedonic consequences, to habitual, compulsive behavior that is insensitive to negative outcomes (38) (although there are compelling alternative accounts (39,40)).
Cross-species evidence corroborates theoretical proposals that habitual drug use is associated with decreased model-based control. Self-administration of alcohol in rodents that is initially sensitive to outcome devaluation becomes increasingly insensitive with prolonged consumption (41). Animals exposed to cocaine or methamphetamine similarly exhibit decreasing sensitivity to devaluation (42-44), suggesting drug consumption decreases model-based control of action. Notably, the degree to which drug exposure reduces model-based control may predict subsequent escalation of drug seeking and use (45). Relative to control participants, both alcohol- (46) and cocaine-dependent (47) patients exhibit decreased sensitivity to devaluation of actions associated with monetary rewards. Human studies using sequential decision-making tasks have similarly observed decreased model-based control in alcohol misuse and dependence (48,49) and various substance use disorders (50), as well as in gambling addiction (51). These convergent findings (but see (52,53)) suggest that reduced model-based control is associated with an increased propensity toward compulsive behavior (54,55), a transdiagnostic behavioral dimension characteristic of diverse psychiatric disorders.
Decreased engagement of model-based control at younger ages may confer a heightened propensity toward compulsivity that increases vulnerability to addiction (for a graphical depiction of this hypothesis see Figure 1.C.I). Indeed, earlier initiation of substance use predicts a greater likelihood of dependence (5), a more rapid progression to addiction (56), as well as greater addiction severity (57). Heightened compulsivity may increase the propensity for escalation of substance use during developmental periods in which experimentation is common. Indeed, model-based control amongst adolescents and young adults is associated with heightened subclinical compulsivity (15), and decreased model-based control at age 18 predicts greater increases in binge drinking behavior over a three-year period (58). Notably, while low model-based control at a given age may heighten vulnerability to addiction following drug experimentation, greater model-based control may confer resilience (Figure 1.C.II). Conversely, drug experimentation may also influence the longitudinal developmental trajectory of model-based control. Future studies should examine whether there are sensitive developmental periods during which drug exposure is particularly consequential (see Figure 1.C.III).
Heightened compulsivity in children is typically viewed as a developmentally normative behavioral characteristic. Parents typically restrict access to rewards for which children tend to have difficulty regulating their consumption (e.g., candy, video games). Such parental oversight makes it unclear whether youth itself represents a risk factor for the development of behavioral addictions. Online gaming provides one testbed for such hypotheses. A substantial proportion of children and teens engage in video-game play (59), and during the COVID pandemic, relaxation of parental restrictions on playtime were widespread (60). One recent study in Japanese middle-school students found that earlier age of regular video-game play was indeed associated with more problematic gaming (61). This preliminary evidence should be corroborated, and whether variation in model-based control in youth is associated with the development of problematic gaming should be directly examined.
Importantly, a heightened propensity toward reward-driven habit formation may also be harnessed to develop healthy routines that facilitate resilience. Youths highly engaged in school activities, academics, and sports are at lower risk for problematic substance use, potentially because these rewarding activities prevent exploration of less healthy reinforcers (62). A mechanistic understanding of the emergence of addictive behaviors will require characterizing how normative developmental trajectories of learning computations interact with specific aspects of individuals’ daily experiences (63).
A potential role for model-based control in the development of anxious symptomatology
Recent speculative theoretical proposals suggest that model-based simulation processes may underpin ruminative symptoms characteristic of several forms of anxiety and depressive disorders (64,65). Model-based control enables simulation of potential states of the world including counterfactual alternatives to past actions or potential future actions and the outcomes they might yield (66). This computational capacity supports adaptive planning but may become overly active and biased toward negative, low-probability events in anxious individuals (67), yielding excessive worry and rumination (65). Thus, anxious rumination likely reflects an interaction between heightened model-based control and negatively biased simulation (see Box 1 and Figure 1.C.IV).
Currently, there is little empirical evidence for the role of model-based simulation in anxiety. A recent study found that severity of social anxiety symptoms was associated with increased deliberative evaluation, indexed by a computational counterfactual updating parameter (68). However, a large-scale study observing no relation between anxious symptomatology and model-based control (54) suggesting that any potential relation with anxiety may be more complex.
Both model-based control and the tendency to engage in rumination increase with age from pre-adolescence into adolescence (69) possibly reflecting a common underlying developmental improvement in mental simulation ability (70). Developmental increases in model-based simulation may be largely adaptive, as deliberative anticipatory or retrospective processing of events can facilitate future planning. However, alterations in both the content and regulation of these deliberative processes may give rise to rumination and worry: anxious children overestimate the probability of rare negative events (67)and report being unable to terminate worry until perceived threats are removed (71).
Heightened rumination during adolescence prospectively predicts increases in anxiety following stressful life events (72), and accounts for comorbidity between anxiety and depression (73). Given the clear clinical significance of rumination, an important goal for future developmental studies is to understand how the development of model-based simulation abilities interact with environmental factors to modulate maladaptive deliberation.
Pavlovian learning processes
Through Pavlovian learning, cues that predict the presence of motivationally significant positive and negative events come to elicit reflexive and valence-dependent consummatory or defensive behaviors that can promote survival (74). Reward-associated cues typically drive approach responses and invigoration of ongoing behavior, whereas threat-associated cues typically drive withdrawal and behavioral inhibition. Pavlovian learning processes are centrally implicated in the etiology of both addiction and anxiety (75,76). Through Pavlovian-instrumental transfer, reward value assigned to drug-related cues and contexts can invigorate the instrumental behaviors involved in drug seeking and consumption. In addition, recent computational work suggests that anxiety may be associated with alteration in the process of inferring the latent states to which negative value is assigned during Pavlovian learning, rendering anxious individuals’ Pavlovian threat associations highly robust, resistant to change, and readily generalized to related stimuli and contexts. Below, we review findings illustrating how these two aspects of Pavlovian learning processes — Pavlovian-instrumental transfer and latent-state inference — are implicated in addiction and anxiety, and discuss how knowledge of the developmental trajectories of these processes may contribute to a mechanistic understanding of the emergence of these disorders.
The development of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer and its role in addiction
Interactions between Pavlovian and instrumental learning play a critical role in the cycle of addiction. Drug-predictive cues (e.g., sights, smells, contexts) acquire Pavlovian reward value through hedonic drug consumption experiences. Such cues can then elicit behavioral invigoration, fostering approach, attentional capture, and physiological arousal, which collectively facilitate craving and drug-seeking behavior (77). Through Pavlovian-instrumental transfer, positive value assigned to a reward-predictive stimulus can reflexively invigorate performance of instrumental actions previously learned to yield either that specific reinforcer or rewards more generally (78).
Drug-predictive cues tend to increase performance of reward-related instrumental actions (79-83). Moreover, drug exposure strengthens the influence of non-drug Pavlovian reward cues on instrumental action (84-88). Human studies s point to a key role of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer in addiction. Relative to healthy controls, alcohol-dependent individuals(89-91) and high-risk young drinkers (92,93) exhibit greater difficulty learning actions that conflict with the valence-dependent behavioral responses elicited by Pavlovian learning processes. The strength of this effect predicts long-term relapse (94-96), potentially by facilitating drug-related approach behaviors (97). These suggest that t Pavlovian biases may be a risk factor for the escalation of drug use and is further exacerbated by drug consumption. Notably, Pavlovian-instrumental transfer may also play a role in the heightened behavioral inhibition characteristic of anxiety ((98,99) see Box 1). However, to date, few studies have examined these relations computationally.
Two recent studies examined the developmental trajectory of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer using computational approaches. In a small cohort of participants, adolescents exhibited the smallest degree of Pavlovian bias on instrumental learning, relative to children and young adults (100). However, a larger study that tested teenagers and young adults, but not children, observed no age-related change in the degree of transfer (101), suggesting that Pavlovian-instrumental transfer may decrease from childhood into adolescence andthen stabilize into young adulthood. Given this sparse evidence base, future studies spanning a broad age range and employing more diverse computational assays of Pavlovian influence (e.g., Pavlovian pruning of decision trees (102), Pavlovian shaping of goal-aligned behaviors (103)) will be important for clarifying this developmental trajectory.
Studies of food consumption suggest that high levels of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer in childhood drive consummatory behaviors in response to reward-related cues. Children who attended more to advertisements for calorie-dense snacks during play consumed more of the snack afterwards (104). Moreover, food-cue-evoked salivation predicted consumption in overweight, but not normal-weight children (105), suggesting that such transfer effects may drive unhealthy consumption. Adolescent drinkers who exhibited heightened reactive responses to alcohol cues in the laboratory also reported greater craving and consumption in real-world contexts associated with use (106). Pavlovian-instrumental transfer may be a mechanism contributing to such escalation of reward-seeking and consumption. Individuals who initiate drug experimentation during developmental periods when Pavlovian-instrumental transfer is typically robust (or who exhibit atypically high levels of transfer) may be more prone to develop compulsive use. This underscores the importance of examining how this computational process is shaped by environmental factors and modulates the emergence of addictive behaviors (Figure 1.C.II). Moreover, diverse forms of Pavlovian learning reflecting model-free versus model-based underlying computations (i.e., sign-tracking versus goaltracking) (107) are differentially associated with addiction risk (108), highlighting the importance of investigating these learning dimensions in future developmental studies (109).
Latent state inference in aversive Pavlovian learning and the development of anxiety
Aversive Pavlovian learning has been proposed as a model for the learning processes through which threat expectations are acquired in anxiety (110). Aversive Pavlovian learning is commonly studied using paradigms in which a neutral stimulus (the conditioned stimulus, or CS) is repeatedly paired with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g., mild electrical shock), after which CS presentation elicits reflexive defensive conditioned responses (e.g., freezing). The capacity to alter such associations is commonly studied using extinction paradigms, in which the CS is no longer paired with the US, and expression of conditioned responses typically decreases. However, extinguished responses can return under a number of circumstances, including a change in context (renewal), re-exposure to a US (reinstatement), or the mere passage of time (spontaneous recovery) (111). Through generalization, stimuli that share some degree of perceptual or conceptual similarity to the CS, but were never directly associated with the US, can come to evoke a conditioned response. Generalization typically decays as the dissimilarity between the CS and other stimuli increases (112).
Changes in the process of inferring the latent environmental states (or contexts) to which negative or positive value is assigned may contribute to variation in the acquisition, extinction, and generalization of Pavlovian learning. Under this computational account, learners segment their experience into latent states that capture regularities in the configuration of observed stimuli (e.g., CS and US) (113). Mismatch between the current configuration and the learned prototypical configuration of a state (akin to a prediction error) provides evidence of a new latent state. Sensitivity to the deviation between the stimulus configurations observed during acquisition and extinction may cause trials from these phases to be assigned to distinct latent states (i.e., separate threat and safety memories), whereas lower sensitivity allows reinforced acquisition trials and unreinforced extinction trials to be assigned the same latent state, effectively “overwriting” the original memory and eliminating the possibility that the conditioned response could later reemerge. Consistent with this account, computational analysis of participants’ conditioned responses during aversive learning and extinction (114) demonstrated that only individuals who appeared to infer two latent states showed spontaneous recovery following extinction training. Variation in latent-state inference can also account for stimulus generalization. If a reinforced CS and another similar stimulus are clustered within the same latent state, they will both acquire valenced expectations, as they will share an association with the US.
Relative to control participants, anxious individuals acquire more robust conditioned responses, exhibit attenuated extinction learning and stronger reinstatement, and show increased generalization of conditioned responding to unreinforced stimuli (115,116). Two recent studies suggest that these alterations in Pavlovian learning may relate to differences in latent-state inference. In an unselected sample, shock expectation ratings of high-trait-anxiety individuals were better explained by a model inferring multiple latent states, conferring the ability to reinstate previously learned threat associations, while ratings of low-trait-anxiety individuals were better explained by a single-state learning model (117). A second study found that PTSD patients who exhibited greater generalization of loss expectations and higher levels of avoidance symptoms were more likely to assign stimulus observations to a single underlying cause (118). These seemingly contradictory findings may identify two modes of latent-state inference that correspond to distinct dimensions of anxious symptomatology (persistent threat response versus overgeneralization) that differ in prevalence across these samples. However, these findings should be replicated and extended in order to better reconcile these apparent discrepancies.
Although aversive Pavlovian expectations are similarly acquired and expressed across development, both the extinction and generalization of these associations change markedly (119). In juvenile rodents, conditioned responding decreases during extinction, but unlike in adult animals, these responses do not tend to re-emerge (120-122). Extinction learning in juveniles also does not engage neural processes associated with threat-response inhibition (123), but instead appears to modify the original threat association (122,124), suggesting that extinction at this developmental stage may be more akin to unlearning. In contrast, adolescent rodents and humans exhibit greater difficulty extinguishing threat associations and heightened reemergence of conditioned responses following extinction, relative to pre-adolescents and adults (125,126). Aversive generalization also exhibits systematic changes across development. Relative to adolescents and adults, younger children show broader generalization of threat responses to novel stimuli. With age, the ability to discriminate between a CS associated with threat and unreinforced neutral stimuli gradually improves (127-130).
We do not know of studies that have used formal approaches to test whether latent-state inference exhibits systematic developmental changes. However, the changes in extinction and generalization described above are consistent with a proposal that the tendency to infer new latent states may be lower at younger ages, and increase with development. Moreover, a greater tendency to cluster conceptually or perceptually similar stimuli into the same latent state may contribute to the heightened generalization observed in younger individuals (127-131). Consistent with broader developmental improvements in memory specificity across adolescence (132), the tendency to infer distinct latent threat and safety states based on shifts in environmental statistics may increase with age, enabling extinguished responses to reemerge. Future studies using computational modeling approaches should test this proposal, with a particular focus on identifying the specific mechanisms that might underpin attenuated extinction and heightened reemergence of threat associations during adolescence (133).
Systematic developmental changes in latent-state inference might underpin corresponding age differences in anxiety-related symptomatology, and suggest avenues for tailoring treatment. A heightened tendency to cluster together threat-associated cues or contexts with similar, safe states may be a key driver of symptoms in anxious children (134,135), making therapies that facilitate discrimination ability particularly efficacious. As differentiation of threat and safety states improves with age, extinction-based exposure therapies might be modified to increase generalization between extinguished threat associations and earlier experiences, in order to prevent fear reemergence. For example, exposure protocols modeled after gradual extinction training, in which the frequency of CS-US pairing slowly decreases, may diminish the mismatch signals that drive new state inference and reduce spontaneous recovery and reinstatement effects (136,137). More generally, such cognitive heterogeneity across individuals highlights the importance of tailoring behavioral therapies to target specific types of maladaptive inferences.
Conclusion
Here, we synthesized knowledge about computational processes implicated in the etiology of addiction and anxiety with findings about the developmental trajectories of these computations. We highlighted areas where we have strong knowledge of both the clinical significance and the normative developmental trajectory of a cognitive process (e.g., the development of model-based control and its relevance to compulsivity), and those for which both developmental and clinical data are lacking (e.g., latent-state inference).
Going forward, computational psychiatry approaches may prove particularly valuable for characterizing developmental mechanisms of psychopathology. Latent cognitive computations that underpin clinically relevant behaviors likely contribute to the emergence, maintenance, or escalation of symptoms over developmental time. The objective behavioral metrics that this approach provides (e.g., model parameter estimates) can enable researchers to chart normative developmental changes in these computations, study their underlying neural mechanisms, and characterize the dynamic relation between computational phenotypes and clinical symptoms longitudinally. Importantly, normative trajectories of clinically relevant objective phenotypes may provide critical tools for diagnosis and intervention, circumventing challenges posed by reliance on clinical interviews and self-report measures. This may be especially true for children and adolescents, whose verbal abilities and metacognitive awareness may be less robust than adults’ and whose expression of symptoms can often be either atypical or masked by caregiving environments. Further, these measures may eventually be able to serve as actionable early indications of aberrant developmental trajectories that can indicate whether — and which — interventions should be considered in order to prevent the emergence or worsening of symptoms.
Several topics remain beyond the scope of this review but may be critical for progress in the field. In this paper, we have treated computational phenotypes as stable, trait-like properties that change on a developmental timescale. However, computational phenotypes can also change dynamically in response to shifts in environmental conditions (138,139), stress (see Box 2), changes in affective state (140), or other factors. Characterizing both the factors that elicit such dynamic changes in computational phenotypes and the temporal dynamics of these changes across both local and developmental time scales (7), are important avenues for future research. Understanding periods of vulnerability to psychopathology will also require more thorough investigation of the relation between neuroplasticity and learning. For example, a study detailing the brain mechanisms underlying the opening and closure of a developmental critical period for social reward learning (141) suggested a potential biological mechanism of efficacy of a drug that is used to treat PTSD (142). Such convergent insights illustrate how neurobiological research into the mechanisms of learning development can inform psychiatric treatment. Characterizing developmental periods during which the tuning of additional learning processes, and their underlying neural circuits, are most sensitive to environmental inputs may be critical for identifying targets for psychiatric intervention.
Box 2: Stress effects on transdiagnostic learning computations.
Threats, either real or perceived, can induce stress. Stress responsivity is increased during childhood and adolescence, with stress exposure yielding heightened physiological responses, lasting effects on developing cortical-subcortical circuitry, and impairments in stress coping in adulthood (163-166). Early-life stress is a well-established risk factor for the development of both anxiety and addiction (167,168), and mental illness more generally (169).
Across species, stress reduces model-based control, fostering increased habitual behavior (170-175). Stress does not appear to facilitate appetitive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (176-178). However, it does promote the reemergence of previously extinguished reward associations (179,180), which may allow drug cues to regain their influence over behavior following periods of abstinence, consistent with stress-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior (181).
Stress exerts marked effects on aversive Pavlovian learning that exacerbate anxious symptomatology. Stress enhances the consolidation of aversive associations and impairs extinction learning and retention (182). Acute stress increases Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (183), especially in anxious individuals (98), by fostering greater behavior inhibition in the presence of threat, compromising the flexibility of instrumental behavior. Moreover, traumatic stress exposure during mid-to-late adolescence can yield persistent increases in such passive avoidance behavior (99).
These demonstrations that learning computations are profoundly altered by stress exposure suggest that the environmental conditions in which learning occurs are a key determinant of psychiatric outcomes. This may be particularly true at earlier stages of development when sensitivity to stress is heightened.
Acknowledgments
The work presented in this manuscript was supported by the NIMH R01MH125564 (E.E., G.S., C.H), R01MH124092 (E.E.), R01MH126183 (C.H.), US-Israel BSF grant 2019801 (E.E.), ISF grant 1094/20 (E.E.) and the NYU Vulnerable Brain Project (C.H.). We thank Kate Nussenbaum for helpful feedback on the manuscript.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Financial Disclosures
Dr. Goldway, Dr. Eldar, Dr. Shoval, and Dr. Hartley all reported no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Patton GC, Sawyer SM, Santelli JS, Ross DA, Afifi R, Allen NB, et al. (2016): Our future: a Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. Lancet 387: 2423–2478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN (2008): Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 947–957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE (2005): Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62: 593–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ramsawh HJ, Weisberg RB, Dyck I, Stout R, Keller MB (2011): Age of onset, clinical characteristics, and 15-year course of anxiety disorders in a prospective, longitudinal, observational study. J Affect Disord 132: 260–264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hingson RW, Heeren T, Winter MR (2006): Age at drinking onset and alcohol dependence: age at onset, duration, and severity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 160: 739–746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hauser TU, Will G-J, Dubois M, Dolan RJ (2019): Annual Research Review: Developmental computational psychiatry. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 60: 412–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hitchcock PF, Fried El, Frank MJ (2022): Computational Psychiatry Needs Time and Context. Annu Rev Psychol 73: 243–270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Huys GJM, Maia TV, Frank MJ (2016): Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nat Neurosci 19: 404–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Patzelt EH, Hartley CA, Gershman SJ (2018): Computational Phenotyping: Using Models to Understand Individual Differences in Personality, Development, and Mental Illness. Personal Neurosci 1: e18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cuthbert BN (2014): The RDoC framework: facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry 13: 28–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Moutoussis M, Eldar E, Dolan RJ (2017): Building a New Field of Computational Psychiatry. Biol Psychiatry 82: 388–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jacob S, Wolff JJ, Steinbach MS, Doyle CB, Kumar V, Elison JT (2019): Neurodevelopmental heterogeneity and computational approaches for understanding autism. Transl Psychiatry 9: 63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Loosen AM, Hauser TU (2020): Towards a computational psychiatry of juvenile obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 118: 631–642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Moutoussis M, Garzón B, Neufeld S, Bach DR, Rigoli F, Goodyer I, et al. (2021): Decision-making ability, psychopathology, and brain connectivity. Neuron 109: 2025–2040.e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Vaghi MM, Moutoussis M, Váša F, Kievit RA, Hauser TU, Vértes PE, et al. (2020): Compulsivity is linked to reduced adolescent development of goal-directed control and frontostriatal functional connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117: 25911–25922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nussenbaum K, Hartley CA (2019): Reinforcement learning across development: What insights can we draw from a decade of research? Dev Cogn Neurosci 40: 100733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Beesdo-Baum K, Knappe S (2012): Developmental epidemiology of anxiety disorders. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 21: 457–478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Regier DA, Rae DS, Narrow WE, Kaelber CT, Schatzberg AF (1998): Prevalence of anxiety disorders and their comorbidity with mood and addictive disorders. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 24–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bishop SJ, Gagne C (2018): Anxiety, Depression, and Decision Making: A Computational Perspective. Annu Rev Neurosci 41: 371–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gueguen MCM, Schweitzer EM, Konova AB (2021): Computational theory-driven studies of reinforcement learning and decision-making in addiction: What have we learned? Curr Opin Behav Sci 38: 40–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Koob GF, Volkow ND (2010): Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 217–238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Swendsen J, Burstein M, Case B, Conway KP, Dierker L, He J, Merikangas KR (2012): Use and abuse of alcohol and illicit drugs in US adolescents: results of the National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescent Supplement. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69: 390–398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wagner FA, Anthony JC (2002): From first drug use to drug dependence; developmental periods of risk for dependence upon marijuana, cocaine, and alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology 26: 479–488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Craske MG, Rauch SL, Ursano R, Prenoveau J, Pine DS, Zinbarg RE (2011): What is an anxiety disorder? Focus 9: 369–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen H-U (2012): Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 21: 169–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sutton RS, Barto AG (1998): Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction. IEEE Trans Neural Netw 9: 1054–1054. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dickinson A (1985): Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 308: 67–78. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Raab HA, Hartley CA (2018): The Development of Goal-Directed Decision-Making. Goal-Directed Decision Making. Retrieved from https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128120989000139 [Google Scholar]
- 29.Klossek UMH, Russell J, Dickinson A (2008): The control of instrumental action following outcome devaluation in young children aged between 1 and 4 years. J Exp Psychol Gen 137: 39–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Naneix F, Marchand AR, Di Scala G, Pape J-R, Coutureau E (2012): Parallel Maturation of Goal-Directed Behavior and Dopaminergic Systems during Adolescence. 32: 16223–16232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Daw ND, Gershman SJ, Seymour B, Dayan P, Dolan RJ (2011): Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron 69: 1204–1215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Smid, Kool, Hauser (2020): Model-based decision-making and its metacontrol in childhood. Preprint at PsyArXiv https. Retrieved from https://www.dcp-lab.org/s/Smid_Jan2020_Manuscript.pdf [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bolenz F, Eppinger B (2022): Valence bias in metacontrol of decision making in adolescents and young adults. Child Dev 93: e103–e116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Decker JH, Otto AR, Daw ND, Hartley CA (2016): From creatures of habit to goal-directed learners: Tracking the developmental emergence of model-based reinforcement learning. Psychol Sci 27: 848–858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Potter TCS, Bryce NV, Hartley CA (2017): Cognitive components underpinning the development of model-based learning. Dev Cogn Neurosci 25: 272–280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nussenbaum K, Scheuplein M, Phaneuf CV, Evans MD, Hartley CA (2020): Moving developmental research online: comparing in-lab and web-based studies of model-based reinforcement learning. Collabra: Psychology 6. Retrieved from https://online.ucpress.edU/collabra/article/6/1/17213/114338?utm_source=TrendMD&utm_medium=cpc&utm_campaign=Collabra%253A_Psychology_TrendMD_0 [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kool W, Gershman SJ, Cushman FA (2017): Cost-Benefit Arbitration Between Multiple Reinforcement-Learning Systems. Psychol Sci 28: 1321–1333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Voon V, Reiter A, Sebold M, Groman S (2017): Model-Based Control in Dimensional Psychiatry. Biol Psychiatry 82: 391–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hogarth L (2020): Addiction is driven by excessive goal-directed drug choice under negative affect: translational critique of habit and compulsion theory. Neuropsychopharmacology 45: 720–735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bornstein AM, Pickard H (2020): “Chasing the first high”: memory sampling in drug choice. Neuropsychopharmacology 45: 907–915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Corbit LH, Nie H, Janak PH (2012): Habitual alcohol seeking: time course and the contribution of subregions of the dorsal striatum. Biol Psychiatry 72: 389–395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Zapata A, Minney VL, Shippenberg TS (2010): Shift from goal-directed to habitual cocaine seeking after prolonged experience in rats. J Neurosci 30: 15457–15463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schmitzer-Torbert N, Apostolidis S, Amoa R, O’Rear C, Kaster M, Stowers J, Ritz R (2015): Post-training cocaine administration facilitates habit learning and requires the infralimbic cortex and dorsolateral striatum. Neurobiol Learn Mem 118: 105–112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nelson A, Killcross S (2006): Amphetamine exposure enhances habit formation. J Neurosci 26: 3805–3812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Giuliano C, Puaud M, Cardinal RN, Belin D, Everitt BJ (2021): Individual differences in the engagement of habitual control over alcohol seeking predict the development of compulsive alcohol seeking and drinking. Addict Biol 26: e13041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sjoerds Z, de Wit S, van den Brink W, Robbins TW, Beekman ATF, Penninx BWJH, Veltman DJ (2013): Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Transl Psychiatry 3: e337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ersche KD, Gillan CM, Jones PS, Wiliams GB, Ward LHE, Luijten M, et al. (2016): Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction. Science 352: 1468–1471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sebold M, Deserno L, Nebe S, Schad DJ, Garbusow M, Hagele C, et al. (2014): Model-based and model-free decisions in alcohol dependence. Neuropsychobiology 70: 122–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Doñamayor N, Strelchuk D, Baek K, Banca P, Voon V (2018): The involuntary nature of binge drinking: goal directedness and awareness of intention. Addict Biol 23: 515–526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Voon V, Derbyshire K, Rück C, Irvine MA, Worbe Y, Enander J, et al. (2015): Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol Psychiatry 20: 345–352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Wyckmans F, Otto AR, Sebold M, Daw N, Bechara A, Saeremans M, et al. (2019): Reduced model-based decision-making in gambling disorder. Sci Rep 9: 19625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.van Timmeren T, Quail SL, Balleine BW, Geurts DEM, Goudriaan AE, van Holst RJ (2020): Intact corticostriatal control of goal-directed action in Alcohol Use Disorder: a Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer and outcome-devaluation study. Sci Rep 10: 4949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Nebe S, Kroemer NB, Schad DJ, Bernhardt N, Sebold M, Müller DK, et al. (2018): No association of goal-directed and habitual control with alcohol consumption in young adults. Addict Biol 23: 379–393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Gillan CM, Kosinski M, Whelan R, Phelps EA, Daw ND (2016): Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control. Elite 5. 10.7554/eLife.11305 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Gillan CM, Kalanthroff E, Evans M, Weingarden HM, Jacoby RJ, Gershkovich M, et al. (2020): Comparison of the Association Between Goal-Directed Planning and Self-reported Compulsivity vs Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder Diagnosis. JAMA Psychiatry 77: 77–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Behrendt S, Wittchen H-U, Höfler M, Lieb R, Beesdo K (2009): Transitions from first substance use to substance use disorders in adolescence: is early onset associated with a rapid escalation? Drug Alcohol Depend 99: 68–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Chen C-Y, Storr CL, Anthony JC (2009): Early-onset drug use and risk for drug dependence problems. Addict Behav 34: 319–322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Chen H, Mojtahedzadeh N, Belanger MJ, Nebe S, Kuitunen-Paul S, Sebold M, et al. (2021): Model-Based and Model-Free Control Predicts Alcohol Consumption Developmental Trajectory in Young Adults: A 3-Year Prospective Study. Biol Psychiatry 89: 980–989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD (2012): Online gaming addiction in children and adolescents: A review of empirical research. J Behav Addict 1: 3–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Han TS, Cho H, Sung D, Park M-H (2022): A systematic review of the impact of COVID-19 on the game addiction of children and adolescents. Front Psychiatry 13: 976601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Nakayama H, Matsuzaki T, Mihara S, Kitayuguchi T, Higuchi S (2020): Relationship between problematic gaming and age at the onset of habitual gaming. Pediatr Int 62: 1275–1281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Audrain-McGovern J, Rodriguez D, Tercyak KP, Epstein LH, Goldman P, Wileyto EP (2004): Applying a behavioral economic framework to understanding adolescent smoking. Psychol Addict Behav 18: 64–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Trucco EM (2020): A review of psychosocial factors linked to adolescent substance use. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 196: 172969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Olatunji BO, Naragon-Gainey K, Wolitzky-Taylor KB (2013): Specificity of rumination in anxiety and depression: A multimodal meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 20: 225–257. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Gagne C, Dayan P, Bishop SJ (2018): When planning to survive goes wrong: predicting the future and replaying the past in anxiety and PTSD. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 24: 89–95. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Dolan RJ, Dayan P (2013): Goals and habits in the brain. Neuron 80: 312–325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Muris P, van der Heiden S (2006): Anxiety, depression, and judgments about the probability of future negative and positive events in children. J Anxiety Disord 20: 252–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hunter LE, Meer EA, Gillan CM, Hsu M, Daw ND (2022): Increased and biased deliberation in social anxiety. Nat Hum Behav 6: 146–154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Jose PE, Brown I (2008): When does the Gender Difference in Rumination Begin? Gender and Age Differences in the Use of Rumination by Adolescents. J Youth Adolesc 37: 180–192. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Hartley CA, Nussenbaum K, Cohen AO (2021): Interactive Development of Adaptive Learning and Memory. Annual Review of. Retrieved from https://www.katenuss.com/papers/HartleyNussenbaumCohen_2021_AnnualReview.pdf [Google Scholar]
- 71.Szabó M, Lovibond PF (2004): The cognitive content of thought-listed worry episodes in clinic-referred anxious and nonreferred children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 33: 613–622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Michl LC, McLaughlin KA, Shepherd K, Nolen-Hoeksema S (2013): Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. J Abnorm Psychol 122: 339–352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.McLaughlin KA, Nolen-Hoeksema S (2011): Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety. Behav Res Ther 49: 186–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Rescorla RA (1988): Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci 11: 329–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2016): Drug Addiction: Updating Actions to Habits to Compulsions Ten Years On. Annu Rev Psychol 67: 23–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Craske MG, Hermans D, Vervliet B (2018): State-of-the-art and future directions for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxiety. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 373. 10.1098/rstb.2017.0025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993): The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 18: 247–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Lovibond PF (1983): Facilitation of instrumental behavior by a Pavlovian appetitive conditioned stimulus. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9: 225–247. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Corbit LH, Janak PH (2007): Ethanol-associated cues produce general pavlovian-instrumental transfer. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 31: 766–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Corbit LH, Janak PH (2016): Changes in the Influence of Alcohol-Paired Stimuli on Alcohol Seeking across Extended Training. Front Psychiatry 7: 169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Corbit LH, Fischbach SC, Janak PH (2016): Nucleus accumbens core and shell are differentially involved in general and outcome-specific forms of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer with alcohol and sucrose rewards. Eur J Neurosci 43: 1229–1236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Glasner SV, Overmier JB, Balleine BW (2005): The role of Pavlovian cues in alcohol seeking in dependent and nondependent rats. J Stud Alcohol 66: 53–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Shiflett MW (2012): The effects of amphetamine exposure on outcome-selective Pavlovian-instrumental transfer in rats. Psychopharmacology 223: 361–370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Saddoris MP, Stamatakis A, Carelli RM (2011): Neural correlates of Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer in the nucleus accumbens shell are selectively potentiated following cocaine self-administration. Eur J Neurosci 33: 2274–2287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.LeBlanc KH, Maidment NT, Ostlund SB (2014): Impact of repeated intravenous cocaine administration on incentive motivation depends on mode of drug delivery. Addict Biol 19: 965–971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.LeBlanc KH, Maidment NT, Ostlund SB (2013): Repeated cocaine exposure facilitates the expression of incentive motivation and induces habitual control in rats. PLoS One 8: e61355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Ostlund SB, LeBlanc KH, Kosheleff AR, Wassum KM, Maidment NT (2014): Phasic mesolimbic dopamine signaling encodes the facilitation of incentive motivation produced by repeated cocaine exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 2441–2449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Wyvell CL, Berridge KC (2001): Incentive sensitization by previous amphetamine exposure: increased cue-triggered “wanting” for sucrose reward. Journal of Neuroscience. Retrieved from https://www.jneurosci.org/content/21/19/7831.short [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Garbusow M, Schad DJ, Sommer C, Jünger E, Sebold M, Friedel E, et al. (2014): Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer in alcohol dependence: a pilot study. Neuropsychobiology 70: 111–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Garbusow M, Schad DJ, Sebold M, Friedel E, Bernhardt N, Koch SP, et al. (2016): Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer effects in the nucleus accumbens relate to relapse in alcohol dependence. Addict Biol 21: 719–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Sommer C, Garbusow M, Jünger E, Pooseh S, Bernhardt N, Birkenstock J, et al. (2017): Strong seduction: impulsivity and the impact of contextual cues on instrumental behavior in alcohol dependence. Transl Psychiatry 7: e1183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Garbusow M, Nebe S, Sommer C, Kuitunen-Paul S, Sebold M, Schad DJ, et al. (2019): Pavlovian-To-lnstrumental Transfer and Alcohol Consumption in Young Male Social Drinkers: Behavioral, Neural and Polygenic Correlates. J Clin Med Res 8. 10.3390/jcm8081188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Chen H, Nebe S, Mojtahedzadeh N, Kuitunen-Paul S, Garbusow M, Schad DJ, et al. (2021): Susceptibility to interference between Pavlovian and instrumental control is associated with early hazardous alcohol use. Addict Biol 26: e12983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Sommer C, Birkenstock J, Garbusow M, Obst E, Schad DJ, Bernhardt N, et al. (2020): Dysfunctional approach behavior triggered by alcohol-unrelated Pavlovian cues predicts long-term relapse in alcohol dependence. Addict Biol 25: e12703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Sebold M, Nebe S, Garbusow M, Guggenmos M, Schad DJ, Beck A, et al. (2017): When Habits Are Dangerous: Alcohol Expectancies and Habitual Decision Making Predict Relapse in Alcohol Dependence. Biol Psychiatry 82: 847–856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Chen K, Schlagenhauf F, Sebold M, Kuitunen-Paul S, Chen H, Huys QJM, et al. (2022): The association of non-drug-related Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer effect in nucleus accumbens with relapse in alcohol dependence: a replication. Biol Psychiatry. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Chen K, Garbusow M, Sebold M, Kuitunen-Paul S, Smolka MN, Huys QJM, et al. (2022): Alcohol Approach Bias Is Associated With Both Behavioral and Neural Pavlovian-to-Instrumental Transfer Effects in Alcohol-Dependent Patients. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science. 10.1016/j.bpsgos.2022.03.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Mkrtchian A, Aylward J, Dayan P, Roiser JP, Robinson OJ (2017): Modeling Avoidance in Mood and Anxiety Disorders Using Reinforcement Learning. Biol Psychiatry 82: 532–539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Ousdal OT, Huys QJ, Milde AM, Craven AR, Ersland L, Endestad T, et al. (2018): The impact of traumatic stress on Pavlovian biases. Psychol Med 48: 327–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Raab HA, Hartley CA (2020): Adolescents exhibit reduced Pavlovian biases on instrumental learning. Sci Rep 10: 15770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Moutoussis M, Bullmore ET, Goodyer IM, Fonagy P, Jones PB, Dolan RJ, et al. (2018): Change, stability, and instability in the Pavlovian guidance of behaviour from adolescence to young adulthood. PLoS Comput Biol 14: e1006679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Huys QJM, Eshel N, O’Nions E, Sheridan L, Dayan P, Roiser JP (2012): Bonsai trees in your head: how the pavlovian system sculpts goal-directed choices by pruning decision trees. PLoS Comput Biol 8: e1002410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Lieder F, Chen OX, Krueger PM, Griffiths TL (2019): Cognitive prostheses for goal achievement. Nat Hum Behav 3: 1096–1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Folkvord F, Anschütz DJ, Wiers RW, Buijzen M (2015): The role of attentional bias in the effect of food advertising on actual food intake among children. Appetite 84: 251–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Jansen A, Theunissen N, Slechten K, Nederkoorn C, Boon B, Mulkens S, Roefs A (2003): Overweight children overeat after exposure to food cues. Eat Behav 4: 197–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Ramirez J, Miranda R Jr (2014): Alcohol craving in adolescents: bridging the laboratory and natural environment. Psychopharmacology 231: 1841–1851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Dayan P, Berridge KC (2014): Model-based and model-free Pavlovian reward learning: revaluation, revision, and revelation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 14: 473–492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Flagel SB, Akil H, Robinson TE (2009): Individual differences in the attribution of incentive salience to reward-related cues: Implications for addiction. Neuropharmacology 56 Suppl 1: 139–148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Sebold M, Schad DJ, Nebe S, Garbusow M, Jünger E, Kroemer NB, et al. (2016): Don’t Think, Just Feel the Music: Individuals with Strong Pavlovian-to-lnstrumental Transfer Effects Rely Less on Model-based Reinforcement Learning. J Cogn Neurosci 28: 985–995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Pittig A, Treanor M, LeBeau RT, Craske MG (2018): The role of associative fear and avoidance learning in anxiety disorders: Gaps and directions for future research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 88: 117–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Bouton ME (2004): Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn Mem 11: 485–494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Shepard RN (1987): Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237: 1317–1323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Gershman SJ, Blei DM, Niv Y (2010): Context, learning, and extinction. Psychol Rev 117: 197–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Gershman SJ, Hartley CA (2015): Individual differences in learning predict the return of fear. Learn Behav 43: 243–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Cooper SE, van Dis EAM, Hagenaars MA, Krypotos A-M, Nemeroff CB, Lissek S, et al. (2022): A meta-analysis of conditioned fear generalization in anxiety-related disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 47: 1652–1661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Duits P, Cath DC, Lissek S, Hox JJ, Hamm AO, Engelhard IM, et al. (2015): Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety 32: 239–253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Zika O, Wiech K, Reinecke A, Browning M, Schuck NW (2022, April 28): Trait anxiety is associated with hidden state inference during aversive reversal learning. bioRxiv. p 2022.04.01.483303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Norbury A, Brinkman H, Kowalchyk M, Monti E, Pietrzak RH, Schiller D, Feder A (2021): Latent cause inference during extinction learning in trauma-exposed individuals with and without PTSD. Psychol Med 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Hartley CA, Lee FS (2015): Sensitive periods in affective development: nonlinear maturation of fear learning. Neuropsychopharmacology 40: 50–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Kim JH, Richardson R (2007): A developmental dissociation in reinstatement of an extinguished fear response in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 88: 48–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Yap CSL, Richardson R (2007): Extinction in the developing rat: an examination of renewal effects. Dev Psychobiol 49: 565–575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Gogolla N, Caroni P, Lüthi A, Herry C (2009): Perineuronal nets protect fear memories from erasure. Science 325: 1258–1261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Kim JH, Hamlin AS, Richardson R (2009): Fear extinction across development: the involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex as assessed by temporary inactivation and immunohistochemistry. J Neurosci 29: 10802–10808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Kim JH, Richardson R (2008): The effect of temporary amygdala inactivation on extinction and reextinction of fear in the developing rat: unlearning as a potential mechanism for extinction early in development. J Neurosci 28: 1282–1290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Pattwell SS, Duhoux S, Hartley CA, Johnson DC, Jing D, Elliott MD, et al. (2012): Altered fear learning across development in both mouse and human. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 16318–16323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Baker KD, Den ML, Graham BM, Richardson R (2013): A window of vulnerability: Impaired fear extinction in adolescence. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Lau JY, Britton JC, Nelson EE, Angold A, Ernst M, Goldwin M, et al. (2011): Distinct neural signatures of threat learning in adolescents and adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: 4500–4505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Michalska KJ, Shechner T, Hong M, Britton JC, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Fox NA (2016): A developmental analysis of threat/safety learning and extinction recall during middle childhood. J Exp Child Psychol 146: 95–105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Glenn CR, Klein DN, Lissek S, Britton JC (2012): The development of fear learning and generalization in 8–13 year-olds. Developmental. Retrieved from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.eom/doi/abs/10.1002/dev.20616?casa_token=kL_qcyimigIAAAAA:_GHfyxR7WC2rbBFYXzmh4OtmCFh7EEYLexC2Hu9zSpg-76EEuxFIMvnjc_ZvY2fOTnPyAk8QkpUb8XA [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Schiele MA, Reinhard J, Reif A, Domschke K, Romanos M, Decked J, Pauli P (2016): Developmental aspects of fear: Comparing the acquisition and generalization of conditioned fear in children and adults. Dev Psychobiol 58: 471–481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Reinhard J, Slyschak A, Schiele MA, Andreatta M, Kneer K, Reif A, et al. (2022): Fear conditioning and stimulus generalization in association with age in children and adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 31: 1581–1590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Keresztes A, Ngo CT, Lindenberger U, Werkle-Bergner M, Newcombe NS (2018): Hippocampal Maturation Drives Memory from Generalization to Specificity. Trends Cogn Sci 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Gershman SJ, Monfils M-H, Norman KA, Niv Y (2017): The computational nature of memory modification. Elite 6. Retrieved from http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=28294944&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Dymond S, Schlund MW, Roche B, Whelan R (2014): The spread of fear: symbolic generalization mediates graded threat-avoidance in specific phobia. Q J Exp Psychol 67: 247–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.de Vries YA, Al-Hamzawi A, Alonso J, Borges G, Bruffaerts R, Bunting B, et al. (2019): Childhood generalized specific phobia as an early marker of internalizing psychopathology across the lifespan: results from the World Mental Health Surveys. BMC Med 17: 101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Song M, Jones CE, Monfils M-H, Niv Y (2022, May 10): Explaining the effectiveness of fear extinction through latent-cause inference. arXiv [q-bio.NC]. Retrieved from https://arxiv.org/abs/2205.04670 [Google Scholar]
- 137.Gershman SJ, Jones CE, Norman KA, Monfils M-H, Niv Y (2013): Gradual extinction prevents the return of fear: implications for the discovery of state. Front Behav Neurosci 7: 164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Browning M, Behrens TE, Jocham G, O’Reilly JX, Bishop SJ (2015): Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments. Nat Neurosci 18: 590–596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Gagne C, Zika O, Dayan P, Bishop SJ (2020): Impaired adaptation of learning to contingency volatility in internalizing psychopathology. Elite 9. 10.7554/eLife.61387 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Eldar E, Rutledge RB, Dolan RJ, Niv Y (2016): Mood as Representation of Momentum. Trends Cogn Sci 20: 15–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Nardou R, Lewis EM, Rothhaas R, Xu R, Yang A, Boyden E, Dölen G (2019): Oxytocin-dependent reopening of a social reward learning critical period with MDMA. Nature 569: 116–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Mitchell JM, Bogenschutz M, Lilienstein A, Harrison C, Kleiman S, Parker-Guilbert K, et al. (2021): MDMA-assisted therapy for severe PTSD: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled phase 3 study. Nat Med 27: 1025–1033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Sharot T, Garrett N (2016): Forming Beliefs: Why Valence Matters. Trends Cogn Sci 20: 25–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Palminteri S, Lebreton M (2022): The computational roots of positivity and confirmation biases in reinforcement learning. Trends Cogn Sci 26: 607–621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Pike AC, Robinson OJ (2022): Reinforcement Learning in Patients With Mood and Anxiety Disorders vs Control Individuals: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.0051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Eckstein MK, Master SL, Xia L, Dahl RE, Wilbrecht L, Collins AGE (2022, June 17): The Interpretation of Computational Model Parameters Depends on the Context. bioRxiv. p 2021.05.28.446162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Habicht J, Bowler A, Moses-Payne ME, Hauser TU (2022): Children are full of optimism, but those rose-tinted glasses are fading—Reduced learning from negative outcomes drives hyperoptimism in children. J Exp Psychol Gen 151: 1843–1853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Rosenbaum GM, Grassie HL, Hartley CA (2022): Valence biases in reinforcement learning shift across adolescence and modulate subsequent memory. Elite 11. 10.7554/eLife.64620 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Moutsiana C, Garrett N, Clarke RC, Lotto RB, Blakemore S-J, Sharot T (2013): Human development of the ability to learn from bad news. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110: 16396–16401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Chowdhury R, Sharot T, Wolfe T, Düzel E, Dolan RJ (2014): Optimistic update bias increases in older age. Psychol Med 44: 2003–2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Rouhani N, Niv Y (2019): Depressive symptoms bias the prediction-error enhancement of memory towards negative events in reinforcement learning. Psychopharmacology 236: 2425–2435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Walton ME, Rushworth MFS (2007): Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nat Neurosci 10: 1214–1221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Huang H, Thompson W, Paulus MP (2017): Computational Dysfunctions in Anxiety: Failure to Differentiate Signal From Noise. Biol Psychiatry 82: 440–446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Lamba A, Frank MJ, FeldmanHall O (2020): Anxiety Impedes Adaptive Social Learning Under Uncertainty. Psychol Sci 31: 592–603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Nussenbaum K, Velez JA, Washington BT, Hamling HE, Hartley CA (2022): Flexibility in valenced reinforcement learning computations across development. Child Dev 93: 1601–1615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Flavell JH (1979): Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. Am Psychol 34: 906–911. [Google Scholar]
- 157.Hoven M, Lebreton M, Engelmann JB, Denys D, Luigjes J, van Holst RJ (2019): Abnormalities of confidence in psychiatry: an overview and future perspectives. Transl Psychiatry 9: 268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Seow TXF, Rouault M, Gillan CM, Fleming SM (2021): How Local and Global Metacognition Shape Mental Health. Biol Psychiatry 90: 436–446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Weil LG, Fleming SM, Dumontheil I, Kilford EJ, Weil RS, Rees G, et al. (2013): The development of metacognitive ability in adolescence. Conscious Cogn 22: 264–271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Moses-Payne ME, Habicht J, Bowler A, Steinbeis N, Hauser TU (2021): I know better! Emerging metacognition allows adolescents to ignore false advice. Dev Sci 24: e13101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Ellis DM, Hudson JL (2010): The metacognitive model of generalized anxiety disorder in children and adolescents. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 13: 151–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Silvers JA, Squeglia LM, Rømer Thomsen K, Hudson KA, Feldstein Ewing SW(2019): Hunting for What Works: Adolescents in Addiction Treatment. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 43: 578–592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Cohodes EM, Kitt ER, Baskin-Sommers A, Gee DG (2021): Influences of early-life stress on frontolimbic circuitry: Harnessing a dimensional approach to elucidate the effects of heterogeneity in stress exposure. Dev Psychobiol 63: 153–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Berens AE, Jensen SKG, Nelson CA 3rd (2017): Biological embedding of childhood adversity: from physiological mechanisms to clinical implications. BMC Med 15: 135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C (2009): Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 434–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Eiland L, Romeo RD (2012): Stress and the developing adolescent brain. NSC 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Juruena MF, Eror F, Cleare AJ, Young AH (2020): The Role of Early Life Stress in HPA Axis and Anxiety. In: Kim Y-K, editor. Anxiety Disorders: Rethinking and Understanding Recent Discoveries. Singapore: Springer Singapore, pp 141–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Sinha R, Jastreboff AM (2013): Stress as a common risk factor for obesity and addiction. Biol Psychiatry 73: 827–835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.McLaughlin KA, Greif Green J, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC (2012): Childhood adversities and first onset of psychiatric disorders in a national sample of US adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69: 1151–1160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Dias-Ferreira E, Sousa JC, Melo I, Morgado P, Mesquita AR, Cerqueira JJ, et al. (2009): Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science 325: 621–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Schwabe L, Tegenthoff M, Höffken O, Wolf OT (2012): Simultaneous glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity disrupts the neural basis of goal-directed action in the human brain. J Neurosci 32: 10146–10155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Schwabe L, Wolf OT (2010): Socially evaluated cold pressor stress after instrumental learning favors habits over goal-directed action. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35: 977–986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Schwabe L, Wolf OT (2009): Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. J Neurosci 29: 7191–7198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Cremer A, Kalbe F, Gläscher J, Schwabe L (2021): Stress reduces both model-based and model-free neural computations during flexible learning. Neuroimage 229: 117747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Otto AR, Raio CM, Chiang A, Phelps EA, Daw ND (2013): Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110: 20941–20946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Morgado P, Silva M, Sousa N, Cerqueira JJ (2012): Stress Transiently Affects Pavlovian-to-Instrumental Transfer. Front Neurosci 6: 93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Pielock SM, Braun S, Hauber W (2013): The effects of acute stress on Pavlovian-instrumental transfer in rats. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 13: 174–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Steins-Loeber S, Lörsch F, van der Velde C, Müller A, Brand M, Duka T, Wolf OT (2020): Does acute stress influence the Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer effect? Implications for substance use disorders. Psychopharmacology 237: 2305–2316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Karimi S, Attarzadeh-Yazdi G, Yazdi-Ravandi S, Hesam S, Azizi P, Razavi Y, Haghparast A (2014): Forced swim stress but not exogenous corticosterone could induce the reinstatement of extinguished morphine conditioned place preference in rats: involvement of glucocorticoid receptors in the basolateral amygdala. Behav Brain Res 264: 43–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Armstrong A, Rosenthal H, Stout N, Richard JM (2022): Reinstatement of Pavlovian responses to alcohol cues by stress. Psychopharmacology. 10.1007/S00213-022-06255-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Mantsch JR, Baker DA, Funk D, Lê AD, Shaham Y (2016): Stress-Induced Reinstatement of Drug Seeking: 20 Years of Progress. Neuropsychopharmacology 41: 335–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Raio CM, Phelps EA (2015): The influence of acute stress on the regulation of conditioned fear. Neurobiol Stress 1: 134–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Mkrtchian A, Roiser JP, Robinson OJ (2017): Threat of shock and aversive inhibition: Induced anxiety modulates Pavlovian-instrumental interactions. J Exp Psychol Gen 146: 1694–1704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]