Abstract
An accurate blood test for Alzheimer’s disease that is sensitive to preclinical proteinopathy and cognitive decline has clear implications for early detection and secondary prevention. We assessed the performance of plasma phosphorylated tau 217 () against brain PET markers of amyloid [-labelled Pittsburgh compound B (PiB)] and tau (MK-6240) and its utility for predicting longitudinal cognition. Samples were analysed from a subset of participants with up to 8 years follow-up in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention (WRAP; 2001–present; plasma 2011–present), a longitudinal cohort study of adults from midlife, enriched for parental history of Alzheimer’s disease. Participants were a convenience sample who volunteered for at least one PiB scan, had usable banked plasma and were cognitively unimpaired at first plasma collection. Study personnel who interacted with participants or samples were blind to amyloid status. We used mixed effects models and receiver–operator characteristic curves to assess concordance between plasma and PET biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease and mixed effects models to understand the ability of plasma to predict longitudinal performance on WRAP’s preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite (PACC-3). The primary analysis included 165 people (108 women; mean age = 62.9 6.06; 160 still enrolled; 2 deceased; 3 discontinued). Plasma was strongly related to PET-based estimates of concurrent brain amyloid ( = 0.83 (0.75, 0.90), P < 0.001). Concordance was high between plasma and both amyloid PET (area under the curve = 0.91, specificity = 0.80, sensitivity = 0.85, positive predictive value = 0.58, negative predictive value = 0.94) and tau PET (area under the curve = 0.95, specificity = 1, sensitivity = 0.85, positive predictive value = 1, negative predictive value = 0.98). Higher baseline levels were associated with worse cognitive trajectories ( = −0.07 (−0.09, −0.06), P < 0.001). In a convenience sample of unimpaired adults, plasma levels correlate well with concurrent brain Alzheimer’s disease pathophysiology and with prospective cognitive performance. These data indicate that this marker can detect disease before clinical signs and thus may disambiguate presymptomatic Alzheimer’s disease from normal cognitive ageing.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, amyloid beta, cognitively unimpaired, plasma, pTau117
Graphical Abstract
Jonaitis et al. report high correspondence between brain and plasma measures of Alzheimer’s pathology in 165 middle-aged, cognitively unimpaired adults. The area under the curves relating plasma to amyloid and tau PET positivity were 0.91 and 0.95 respectively. Results suggest promise as an early indicator of Alzheimer’s disease.
Introduction
Blood-based biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease that detect beta-amyloid () and phosphorylated tau (pTau) proteinopathy are rapidly developing.1,2 The utility and convenience of an accurate blood test has clear implications for accelerating and improving clinical research and practice.1–4 Several candidate markers exist including mass spectrometry5–7 and immunoassay8 measured and and their ratio and phosphorylated tau at threonine 217 (),9 181 (),10 and other phosphorylated sites,11 as well as non-specific markers of neurodegeneration and astrogliosis, including neurofilament light (NfL)12,13 and glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP).14–16
Recently, interest has turned to , as cerebrospinal fluid levels increase early in autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease17 and better discriminate Alzheimer’s disease from non-Alzheimer’s subgroups of cognitively impaired adults, compared to .18 In plasma, accurately differentiates persons with neuropathologically defined Alzheimer’s disease from other dementia.9,19 Further, in vivo plasma levels correlate with ex vivo protein levels and spatial burden in post-mortem brain tissue.19–21 Next, plasma levels discriminate diagnostic groups informed by amyloid PET. levels are elevated among impaired (Alzheimer’s disease or mild cognitive impairment (MCI)) participants compared to cognitively unimpaired (CU) participants,11,19 and plasma and tau PET signal show moderate to high agreement.9,11,22 Serial plasma levels also differentiate Alzheimer’s disease from non-Alzheimer’s MCI, remaining stable and non-elevated in patients and increasing over time in patients.23
The utility of plasma to identify amyloid and tau proteinopathy in a preclinical cohort is less well studied. Among older adults in the Swedish BioFINDER study (mean age = 72), levels increased over 6 years in CU, but not ,23 similar to findings in MCI. In this same cohort, baseline levels affected cognitive change.3 In the Australian Imaging, Biomarker & Lifestyle study (AIBL), among CU adults (mean age = 75), a 2-fold increase in levels of the marker in compared to was recently reported,11 although the correlation between this biomarker and centiloids was relatively weaker in CU than that in Alzheimer’s disease and MCI, perhaps due to restriction of range ( = 0.64 versus 0.45). In the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging, among CU adults (mean age = 79), a smaller fold increase of 0.49 was reported in compared to .24 The varying strength of these reported results may be due to measurement precision differences between instrument platforms and assays.18,25,26
Here we report a study from the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention (WRAP)27 in which we examine plasma trajectories in CU adults using Lilly’s immunoassay for the Meso Scale Discovery platform (Lilly-MSD).9 For this study, participants had a mean age of 63 at first plasma collection. We examined (i) whether changes in plasma levels over time track progression of Alzheimer’s disease proteinopathy ascertained from amyloid and tau positron emission tomography (PET) with -PiB for amyloid and MK-6240 for tau; (ii) whether plasma levels accurately differentiate people with varying degrees of amyloid and tau burden; and (iii) whether plasma levels are associated with longitudinal cognition.
Methods
Ethics
The research protocol was approved by the University of Wisconsin—Madison Health Sciences IRB (IRB00000366), and all participants provided written informed consent.
Participants
Plasma samples were analysed from WRAP participants with amyloid PET scan using Pittsburgh compound B (‘PiB’; see Imaging methods). Participants were included in the PiB+ sample if they had ≥ 1 global PiB distribution volume ratio (DVR) > 1.19 (centiloid equivalent = 21.6). The PiB- sample included all participants who had ≥ 2 PiB scans with all global DVR ≤ 1.1. We also examined samples from participants whose global PiB DVR trajectories indicated possible conversion from PiB- to PiB+ by virtue of initially low but recently subthreshold DVR values (1.16 < DVR ≤ 1.19). Most participants (N = 145) also had at least one tau scan. Primary analyses included only participants who were cognitively unimpaired (CU) at their first plasma collection and excluded one participant whose levels of were highly influential in models. Secondary analyses were conducted including these excluded participants (Sensitivity set 1) and excluding participants with measured below the lower limit of detection (Sensitivity set 2; see Plasma methods and Supplementary material).
Plasma methods
Thirty millilitres of blood was drawn from each participant into 310 mL lavender top EDTA tubes (BD 366643; Franklin Lakes, New Jersey, USA). Samples were mixed gently by inverting 10–12 times and were centrifuged 15 min at 2000 g at room temperature within 1 h of collection. Plasma samples were aliquoted into 2 mL cryovials (Wheaton Cryoelite W985863; Millville, New Jersey, USA). Aliquoted plasma was frozen at −80°C within 90 min and stored for up to 10 years.
Plasma concentration was measured at the Clinical Memory Research Unit, Lund University (Sweden) using immunoassay on a Meso Scale Discovery (MSD) platform developed by Lilly Research Laboratories.9 Samples were assayed in duplicates according to published protocols28 with biotinylated-IBA493 used as a capture antibody and SULFO-TAG-4G10-E2 as the detector. The assay was calibrated with a synthetic peptide. The mean intra-assay coefficient of variation (CV) was 7.11%. The inter-assay CV for three quality control samples included in every run was 10.3%. Plasma concentration was below the detection limit of the assay (0.11–0.17 pg/mL) for six cases. For each model, Sensitivity analysis 2 excluded these observations. Samples were arranged on plates according to a randomization scheme devised by author E.M.J., who had no contact with samples. All samples were analysed by staff blind to clinical and imaging data.
Imaging methods
Participants underwent T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging as well as amyloid (-PiB) and tau (-MK-6240) PET imaging at the University of Wisconsin—Madison. Detailed methods for radioligand synthesis and PET and MRI acquisition, processing and quantification and analysis were implemented as reported previously.29,30
Amyloid burden was assessed as a global cortical average -PiB DVR, and two DVR thresholds were applied for determining PiB positivity (PiB+): one at DVR > 1.19, based on previously published work,31 and another, lower threshold of DVR > 1.16 (corresponding to a centiloid of 17.7), previously shown to predict subsequent accumulation of amyloid.32 Estimated age of amyloid onset (EAOA) was obtained from observed global PiB DVR using a combination of group-based trajectory modelling and Bayes’ theorem33 using either the most recent PET scan (for those who were PiB-) or the scan closest to the PiB+ threshold. Amyloid duration was then estimated as age at plasma sample minus EAOA, and the corresponding estimated PiB DVR (GBTM-DVR) was calculated via linear transformation as described in Betthauser et al.34 Centiloids were estimated from these DVRs according to the following equation: .
The -MK-6240 standardized uptake volume ratio (SUVR) (70–90 min; cerebellum grey reference region excluding the superior medial vermis) tau burden was assessed visually by an expert reader (SCJ) using SUVR images overlaid on the coregistered MRI and scaled from 0 to 2.5. Images were classified as tau negative or tau positive for the medial temporal lobe (MTL; entorhinal cortex, amygdala or hippocampus) and for the neocortex (Neo; 1 or more cortical regions). The visual rating defined four classes: MK-, MK+ in MTL only, MK+ in neocortex only and MK+ in MTL and neocortex.
Neuropsychological assessment and cognitive status
Participants in WRAP completed a comprehensive cognitive battery at each visit, including tests of memory, executive function, language ability and other aspects of cognitive performance, alongside self- and informant-based measures of everyday functioning.27 Based on these measures, participant cognitive status at each visit was determined via consensus conference.35 Among those without clinically significant cognitive impairment (i.e. dementia or MCI), some were assigned a research diagnosis of ‘cognitively unimpaired-declining’ denoting performance within the range of normal, but suggestive of decline from baseline.36
Our measure of global cognition was a three-test version of the Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite37 including the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, sum of Trials 1–5; the Wechsler Memory Scale Logical Memory II, total score; and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Digit Symbol Substitution, total score. The tests were combined by rescaling and computing an unweighted average, scaled such that first observations in cognitively unimpaired indiduals were distributed . The Wide-Range Achievement Test Reading Subtest (standard score) was used as a measure of literacy.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed in R 4.0.5.38 Longitudinal trajectories were modelled using mixed effects models39 with participant-level random intercepts, which are robust to missingness when data are missing at random. Two such models were fit. First, to evaluate how well measurements reflect brain amyloid, we estimated the fixed effect of GBTM-DVR at each timepoint. Second, to compare age trends in people known to be accumulating amyloid versus people who are not,23 we estimated the fixed effects of age, amyloid status (PiB+ versus PiB-) and their interaction. An exploratory analysis estimated whether this relationship was moderated by education. Effect sizes were estimated by ,40 where was considered a small effect size, medium and large.41 Test–retest reliability was assessed using the intraclass correlation. Significance tests were evaluated using α = 0.05.
To establish potential thresholds with maximal correspondence between and binary brain amyloid (global PiB) and tau (MTL+ neocortical MK-6240) positivity, receiver–operator characteristic (ROC) curves42 were constructed on a sub-sample comprising one plasma observation per participant, acquired within 2 years of a PiB or MK-6240 scan, respectively. Thresholds were selected to maximize Youden’s index.43 Positive and negative predictive values for PiB- and MK-positivity assumed population prevalence of 25% and 10%, respectively. In a secondary analysis, we used a robust norms approach to identify an alternate threshold for pTau217 by first winnowing the sample to solidly PiB- individuals (DVR < 1.1 at all scans); computing the 2.5th and 97.5th percentiles; selecting all observations within this range; and recomputing the 97.5th percentile to obtain the robust norms threshold. To validate these thresholds, we classified GBTM-DVR at each plasma observation into PiB- (GBTM-DVR ≤ 1.19) and PiB+ (GBTM-DVR > 1.19) and compared this against positivity on according to each threshold.
To evaluate the relationship between baseline and cognitive trajectories, we fit a mixed effects model of longitudinal PACC-3, with linear and quadratic age terms and their interaction with baseline modelled as continuous fixed effects, and a participant-level random intercept. Age and terms were mean-centred. Sex, education, baseline literacy and number of prior exposures to the cognitive battery were included as covariates. For comparison, a covariate-only model was also fit. An exploratory analysis estimated whether this relationship was moderated by education.
To assess whether within-person change in predicts within-person change in cognition and explore the phasing of this relationship, an exploratory, repeated measures correlation analysis was performed.44,45 This analysis used a subset of data in which was paired variously with concurrent PACC-3 scores and with PACC-3 lagged by one or two study visits. Only participants with at least four timepoints were included (N = 46; = 93), to satisfy the constraints that each participant should contribute at least two observations, to assess within-person change, and that each such observation should allow for pairings with cognition under three lag conditions [e.g. at (A) Visits 1 and 2 with PACC-3, (B) Visits 2 and 3 and (C) Visits 3 and 4, in successive models lag = 0, lag = 1, lag = 2]. This exploratory analysis was repeated twice, first using PACC-3 and values from which age had been partialed out and then substituting GBTM-DVR for .
Data availability
Coded data may be shared at the request of any qualified investigator. R scripts underlying all analyses are available in the Supplementary material.
Results
Participants
A total of 173 participants had qualifying PiB scans and at least 1 plasma sample stored in EDTA. At their last PiB scan, 74 had PiB DVR > 1.19, and 99 had PiB DVR ≤ 1.19. Included in the second set were 84 having ≥ 2 PiB scans with all DVR ≤ 1.11 and 15 with values suggestive of possible conversion. From this set, eight participants were removed from primary analyses due to cognitive impairment at first plasma (N = 6), missing diagnosis at first plasma (N = 1) or measured levels found to be highly influential using Cook’s d (N = 1). Participant characteristics for the primary analysis sample are shown in Table 1. For each applicable aim, excluded participants were included in a sensitivity analysis (Sensitivity set 1; see Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Figs 1 and 3). Six observations had measured values below the lower limit of detection; these were excluded for a second sensitivity analysis (Sensitivity set 2; see Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Figs 2 and 4).
Table 1.
Variable | Value |
---|---|
Number of participants | 165 |
Number of plasma observations per participant, median (range) | 3 (1–5) |
Age at first plasma, mean (SD) | 62.94 (6.06) |
Age at last plasma, mean (SD) | 68.22 (6.10) |
Years of plasma follow-up, mean (SD) | 5.28 (1.62) |
Female, N (%) | 108 (65%) |
Male, N (%) | 57 (35%) |
White, N (%) | 158 (96%) |
Black, N (%) | 4 (2%) |
Native American, N (%) | 3 (2%) |
e2/e3, N (%) | 9 (5%) |
e2/e4, N (%) | 7 (4%) |
e3/e3, N (%) | 77 (47%) |
e3/e4, N (%) | 63 (38%) |
e4/e4, N (%) | 9 (5%) |
Cognitively unimpaired-stable at first plasma, N (%) | 131 (79%) |
Cognitively unimpaired-declining at first plasma, N (%) | 34 (21%) |
Cognitively unimpaired-stable at last plasma, N (%) | 134 (81%) |
Cognitively unimpaired-declining at last plasma, N (%) | 23 (14%) |
MCI at last plasma, N (%) | 7 (4%) |
Dementia at last plasma, N (%) | 1 (1%) |
PiB- at last plasma, N (%) | 95 (58%) |
PiB+ at last plasma, N (%) | 70 (42%) |
MK- at last plasma, N (%) | 111 (67%) |
MK+ MTL only at last plasma, N (%) | 6 (4%) |
MK+ Neo only at last plasma, N (%) | 2 (1%) |
MK+ MTL+ Neo at last plasma, N (%) | 26 (16%) |
MK+ missing, N (%) | 20 (12%) |
MCI = mild cognitive impairment; MK = -MK-6240 tau tracer; MTL = medial temporal lobe; Neo = neocortex; PiB = Pittsburgh compound B.
Longitudinal trajectories
Individual trajectories are displayed by age in Fig. 1A and by GBTM-DVR in Fig. 1B (secondary x-axis: centiloid conversions, at top). A strong relationship with MK-6240 PET is evident: those with tau in both medial temporal and neocortical regions appear distinct from other groups. The improved alignment in Fig. 1B supports strong correspondence between plasma and PiB PET.
The mixed effects models relating levels to GBTM-DVR are shown in Table 2. GBTM-DVR was a strong predictor of measured ( = 0.83, = 0.64). Intraclass correlations were moderate. Together, the results indicate good test–retest reliability and high sensitivity to true underlying change. The results of sensitivity analyses were substantially similar.
Table 2.
as a function of GBTM-DVR | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Primary set | Sensitivity set 1 | Sensitivity set 2 | |||||||
Predictors | Estimates | CI | P | Estimates | CI | P | Estimates | CI | P |
Intercept | 0.00 | −0.08–0.08 | 0.968 | 0.00 | −0.12–0.12 | 0.972 | 0.00 | −0.08–0.08 | 0.984 |
GBTM-DVR | 0.83 | 0.75–0.90 | <0.001 | 0.46 | 0.35–0.56 | <0.001 | 0.83 | 0.75–0.90 | <0.001 |
Random effects | |||||||||
σ2 | 0.12 | 0.32 | 0.12 | ||||||
τ00 | 0.25 Reggieid | 0.52 Reggieid | 0.25 Reggieid | ||||||
ICC | 0.67 | 0.62 | 0.67 | ||||||
N | 165 Reggieid | 173 Reggieid | 165 Reggieid | ||||||
Observations | 515 | 530 | 509 | ||||||
Marginal R2/conditional R2 | 0.648/0.885 | 0.200/0.695 | 0.649/0.885 | ||||||
as a function of age, moderated by binary PiB status | |||||||||
Intercept | 0.23 | 0.20–0.27 | <0.001 | 0.27 | 0.21–0.34 | <0.001 | 0.23 | 0.20–0.27 | <0.001 |
Amyloid positivity | 0.19 | 0.13–0.24 | <0.001 | 0.16 | 0.06–0.26 | 0.002 | 0.19 | 0.13–0.24 | <0.001 |
Age (centred), linear | 0.00 | −0.00–0.00 | 0.337 | 0.00 | −0.00–0.01 | 0.267 | 0.00 | −0.00–0.00 | 0.350 |
Amyloid positivity × age (centred), linear | 0.02 | 0.02–0.03 | <0.001 | 0.01 | 0.00–0.03 | 0.021 | 0.02 | 0.02–0.03 | <0.001 |
Random effects | |||||||||
σ2 | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.01 | ||||||
τ00 | 0.03 Reggieid | 0.09 Reggieid | 0.03 Reggieid | ||||||
ICC | 0.81 | 0.66 | 0.81 | ||||||
N | 165 Reggieid | 173 Reggieid | 165 Reggieid | ||||||
Observations | 515 | 530 | 509 | ||||||
Marginal R2/conditional R2 | 0.355/0.879 | 0.093/0.695 | 0.356/0.878 |
GBTM-DVR = group-based trajectory modelled distribution volume ratio from amyloid PET; ICC = intraclass correlation; = phosphorylated tau 217.
Bold text denotes statistical significance.
The mixed effects models relating levels to age, with binary PiB status (PiB DVR > 1.19) as a moderator, are shown in Table 2. A mid-sized, significant age by amyloid status interaction was observed such that levels of increased with age only in PiB+ participants, whereas in PiB- participants, the age-related slope estimate was indistinguishable from zero ( = 0.021; = 0.0016, = 0.022; = 0.11). One participant with high levels continued to be highly influential in this model. The model fit to the primary dataset is shown in Fig. 1C. The results of sensitivity analyses were similar. An exploratory analysis indicated that education was not a moderator of this effect ( = −0.00082, = 0; see Supplementary Table 1).
threshold estimation
Boxplots and ROC curves relating to binary PiB and MTL+ neocortical MK-6240 are shown in Fig. 2A–D. Correspondence was high for both PiB positivity thresholds [early positivity (DVR > 1.16), AUC = 0.90, PPV = 0.58, NPV = 0.94; late positivity (DVR > 1.19), AUC = 0.91, PPV = 0.58, NPV = 0.94], as well as for MTL+ neocortical MK-6240 positivity (AUC = 0.95, PPV = 1.00, NPV = 0.98). Estimated thresholds were lower for amyloid than tau (early PiB positivity: 0.27; late PiB positivity: 0.27; MTL+ neocortical MK-6240 positivity: 0.37). Our robust norms threshold approach identified a higher positivity boundary of 0.37. Analyses on the sensitivity datasets were similar (see Supplementary Figs 1 and 2).
When adjudicating both the late PiB positivity, ROC-based threshold and the robust norms threshold against the ground truth of concurrent GBTM-DVR > 1.19, the ROC-based threshold was more sensitive, but less specific (ROC threshold: sensitivity = 0.91, specificity = 0.75; robust norms threshold: sensitivity = 0.7, specificity = 0.96). Among individuals having plasma observation between these thresholds (N = 70), half were PiB+ at their last PET scan (N = 36).
Associations with longitudinal cognition
Figure 3A illustrates the mixed effects model relating baseline levels to PACC-3 trajectories. We observed a mid-sized, significant age by interaction ( = −0.075, = 0.10). Lower baseline levels were associated with a flatter cognitive trajectory, whereas moderate and higher levels were linked to faster decline. To facilitate deeper understanding of the model output, we used the betas from the sex main effect, the age terms and the interaction to estimate the ages at which men and women at the 10th and 90th percentiles (but with otherwise average values on the other model terms) would be expected to decline to a PACC-3 score of z = −1.5. According to this model, the predicted age for an average woman at the 10th percentile to reach this z-score is 91.5, compared with age 74.2 for an otherwise similar woman at the 90th percentile. For men, these estimated ages are 86.4 and 71.7, respectively. A likelihood ratio test comparing this model to a covariate-only version indicated better performance when including ( = 70.7, P < 0.0001). Model summaries for these two models are shown in Table 3. Sensitivity analyses were similar (see Supplementary Table 2 and Supplementary Figs 3 and 4). An exploratory model adding higher-level interactions with education was not a better fit ( = 5.9, P = 0.32), and three-way interactions with age were small and not significant ( = 0; see Supplementary Table 3).
Table 3.
Base model | With pTau217 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | Estimates | CI | P | Estimates | CI | P |
Intercept | −3.71 | −5.43–−1.99 | <0.001 | −4.14 | −5.83–−2.44 | <0.001 |
Sex (male) | −0.41 | −0.69–−0.13 | 0.004 | −0.49 | −0.77–−0.22 | 0.001 |
Education (years) | 0.09 | 0.01–0.16 | 0.024 | 0.10 | 0.03–0.17 | 0.007 |
Baseline literacy | 0.02 | 0.00–0.04 | 0.026 | 0.02 | 0.00–0.04 | 0.015 |
Practice | 0.12 | 0.05–0.19 | 0.001 | 0.14 | 0.07–0.21 | <0.001 |
Age (centred), linear | −0.07 | −0.10–−0.05 | <0.001 | −0.09 | −0.11–−0.06 | <0.001 |
Age (centred), quadratic | −0.00 | −0.00–−0.00 | 0.001 | −0.00 | −0.00–−0.00 | 0.015 |
Baseline pTau217 | −0.07 | −0.25–0.11 | 0.466 | |||
Baseline pTau217×age [centred], linear | −0.07 | −0.09–−0.06 | <0.001 | |||
Baseline pTau217 × Age (centred), quadratic | −0.00 | −0.00–0.00 | 0.666 | |||
Random effects | ||||||
σ2 | 0.24 | 0.20 | ||||
τ00 | 0.64 Reggieid | 0.63 Reggieid | ||||
ICC | 0.73 | 0.76 | ||||
N | 165 Reggieid | 165 Reggieid | ||||
Observations | 509 | 509 | ||||
Marginal R2/conditional R2 | 0.237/0.793 | 0.330/0.840 |
ICC = intraclass correlation; = phosphorylated tau 217.
Bold text denotes statistical significance.
Exploratory lagged repeated measures correlations on a subset of individuals with at least four observations (N = 46) suggested weak within-person correspondence between and concurrent cognition ( = −0.130) but stronger within-person relationships when a lag of one ( = −0.550) or two visits ( = −0.670; Fig. 3B–D) was imposed. This pattern held when repeated measures correlation was performed instead after age had been partialed out from both and cognitive scores ( = −0.0720; = −0.500; = −0.630). In comparison, relationships with modelled PiB DVR at the same lags were weaker ( = −0.0230; = −0.410; = −0.540).
Discussion
We characterized the temporal dynamics of Lilly-MSD in a cohort of late middle-aged adults without baseline clinical cognitive impairment at a mean age of 63. We observed a strong relationship between brain amyloid positivity and , with plasma biomarker trajectories rising with age only in PiB+ individuals. This is similar to a recent report from BioFINDER23 in a sample approximately 10 years older. We further observed strong relationships between trajectories and brain tau as measured via MK-6240: those with extensive tau deposition, all PiB+, exhibited increasing plasma levels of . This resembles recent findings in AIBL of a moderately strong cross-sectional correlation between and meta-temporal and mesial temporal MK-6240 SUVR in A+ older adults.11 However, in that analysis, the correlation was weaker in a subset of CU participants, whereas in the present analysis, which includes only CU individuals and again features a cohort 10 years younger, the relationship is strong.
Plasma biomarkers have potential for prescreening Alzheimer’s disease biomarker–positive participants in clinical trials.3 To that end, our results are encouraging, as we observed strong relationships between plasma and concurrent brain imaging biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease, with an AUC of approximately 0.91 for identifying PiB+ participants and 0.95 for identifying those who were MK+. These values are similar to those seen for the easier task of discriminating Alzheimer’s disease from CU groups and are high compared to other reports describing cognitively unimpaired elderly groups in AIBL,11 MCSA24 and BioFINDER.9 With our threshold for predicting PiB+, the PPV of was 0.58, which would reduce the number needed to screen to obtain a full sample. However, for other purposes, a more conservative threshold might be preferable. For MK, in contrast, the PPV of 1 and NPV of 0.98 are likely overestimates but suggest this threshold may work well for many purposes, in principle, in populations with prevalence close to our estimates.
Our two analyses relating baseline to PACC-3 scores were complementary, each suggesting important longitudinal relationships between this plasma biomarker and cognition. In our primary analysis, the by age interaction suggests those with higher baseline biomarker levels evince worse cognitive trajectories with age than do those with lower biomarker levels, whose cognitive trajectories appear flat. In our exploratory analysis using repeated measures correlation, although the within-person relationship between biomarker levels and concurrent cognitive performance is weak, by modelling a delayed effect using a lagged correlation, a robust negative relationship emerges. Although similar relationships have been found in older groups, our report establishes such relationships with biomarkers measured in late midlife.3,23,24 Given the interest in establishing valid surrogate outcomes for Alzheimer’s disease pharmaceutical research,46 our findings may inform the trial design in which the fitness of plasma for that purpose is evaluated.
Limitations
The chief limitation of the present analysis is our small, racially homogenous sample, drawn from a cohort that is convenience- and not population-based.27 The complexities of bringing plasma assays into use with heterogeneous clinical populations should not be discounted. However, recent work in WHICAP suggests relatively good concordance between plasma and clinical status, and no evident demographic biases.47 In CSF and in other plasma tau biomarkers, some have observed differences in various tau isoform levels between Black and White participants after controlling for cognitive status,48,49 but others have not.50 Future directions include assaying our extensive back catalogue of plasma and expanding our existing cohort with a more diverse group of research participants.
Conclusion
In this report, we extend previous findings of strong relationships between plasma levels of , concurrent PET Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers and prospective cognition to a preclinical dataset. These findings have strong implications for early detection, which is prerequisite for several major goals of Alzheimer’s disease research: understanding susceptibility and resilience factors that underlie prognosis; designing better primary and secondary Alzheimer’s disease prevention trials; and determining the relative timing and impact of Alzheimer’s disease and co-occurring pathologies on cognitive decline.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We extend our deepest thanks to the WRAP participants and staff for their invaluable contributions to the study.
Abbreviations
- =
amyloid beta
- AIBL =
Australian Imaging, Biomarker & Lifestyle study
- CL =
centiloid
- CSF =
cerebrospinal fluid
- CU =
cognitively unimpaired
- CV =
coefficient of variation
- DVR =
distribution volume ratio
- EAOA =
estimated amyloid onset age
- EDTA =
ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid
- GBTM-DVR =
group-based trajectory modelled distribution volume ratio at age of plasma assessment
- MCI =
mild cognitive impairment
- MK =
MK-6240 tau PET tracer (florquinitau)
- MSD =
Meso Scale Discovery
- MTL =
medial temporal lobe
- NfL =
neurofilament light-chain
- NPV =
negative predictive value
- PACC-3 =
Preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite
- PiB =
Pittsburgh imaging compound B
- PPV =
positive predictive value
- pTau217 =
phosphorylated tau 217
- ROC =
receiver–operator characteristic
- SUVR =
standardized uptake volume ratio
- WHICAP =
Washington Heights/Inwood Columbia Aging Project
- WRAP =
Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention
Contributor Information
Erin M Jonaitis, Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Institute, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53726, USA; Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA; Department of Medicine, Division of Geriatrics and Gerontology, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA.
Shorena Janelidze, Clinical Memory Research Unit, Department of Clinical Sciences Malmö, Lund University, Lund 205 02, Sweden.
Karly A Cody, Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA.
Rebecca Langhough, Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Institute, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53726, USA; Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA; Department of Medicine, Division of Geriatrics and Gerontology, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA.
Lianlian Du, Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA; Department of Biostatistics and Medical Informatics, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53726, USA.
Nathaniel A Chin, Department of Medicine, Division of Geriatrics and Gerontology, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA.
Niklas Mattsson-Carlgren, Clinical Memory Research Unit, Department of Clinical Sciences Malmö, Lund University, Lund 205 02, Sweden; Department of Neurology, Skåne University Hospital, Lund 222 42, Sweden; Wallenberg Center for Molecular Medicine, Lund University, Lund 221 84, Sweden.
Kirk J Hogan, Department of Anesthesiology, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA.
Bradley T Christian, Department of Medical Physics, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53705, USA; Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA.
Tobey J Betthauser, Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA; Department of Medicine, Division of Geriatrics and Gerontology, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA.
Oskar Hansson, Clinical Memory Research Unit, Department of Clinical Sciences Malmö, Lund University, Lund 205 02, Sweden; Memory Clinic, Skåne University Hospital, 20502, Malmö, Sweden.
Sterling C Johnson, Geriatric Research Education and Clinical Center of the Wm. S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, WI 53705, USA; Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Institute, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53726, USA; Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin—Madison, Madison, WI 53792, USA.
Supplementary material
Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.
Funding
Work at the University of Wisconsin was supported by the National Institutes of Health R01AG027161 (Johnson), National Institutes of Health RO1AG021155 (Johnson), Alzheimer’s Association Research Foundation 19614533 (Betthauser), National Institutes of Health S10 OD025245-01 (Christian) and National Institutes of Health UL1TR002375 (Cody; University of Wisconsin Institute for Clinical and Translational Research). Work at Lund University was supported by the Swedish Research Council (2016-00906), the Knut and Alice Wallenberg foundation (2017-0383), the Marianne and Marcus Wallenberg foundation (2015.0125), the Strategic Research Area MultiPark (Multidisciplinary Research in Parkinson’s disease) at Lund University, the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation (AF-939932), the Swedish Brain Foundation (FO2021-0293), the Parkinson foundation of Sweden (1280/20), the Konung Gustaf V:s och Drottning Victorias Frimurarestiftelse, the Skåne University Hospital Foundation (2020-O000028), Regionalt Forskningsstöd (2020-0314) and the Swedish federal government under the ALF agreement (2018-Projekt0279).
Competing interests
O.H. has acquired research support (for the institution) from ADX, Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Biogen, Eli Lilly, Eisai, Fujirebio, GE Healthcare, Pfizer and Roche. In the past 2 years, he has received consultancy/speaker fees from AC Immune, Amylyx, ALZpath, BioArctic, Biogen, Cerveau, Fujirebio, Genentech, Novartis, Roche and Siemens. S.C.J. has served as a consultant to Eisai and Roche Diagnostics, has received an equipment grant from Roche Diagnostics and has received support (sponsoring of an observational study and provision of precursor for tau imaging) from Cerveau Technologies. E.M.J., S.J., K.A.C., R.L.K., L.D., N.A.C., N.M.C., K.J.H., B.T.C. and T.J.B. have nothing to disclose.
References
- 1. Hansson O. Biomarkers for neurodegenerative diseases. Nat Med. 2021;27(6):954–963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Leuzy A, Mattsson-Carlgren N, Palmqvist S, Janelidze S, Dage JL, Hansson O. Blood-based biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. EMBO Mol Med. 2022;14(1):e14408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Cullen NC, Leuzy A, Janelidze S, et al. . Plasma biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease improve prediction of cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired elderly populations. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):3555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Bateman RJ, Blennow K, Doody R, et al. . Plasma biomarkers of AD emerging as essential tools for drug development: An EU/US CTAD task force report. J Prev Alzheimers Dise. 2019;6(3):169–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. West T, Kirmess KM, Meyer MR, et al. . A blood-based diagnostic test incorporating plasma Aβ42/40 ratio, ApoE proteotype, and age accurately identifies brain amyloid status: Findings from a multi cohort validity analysis. Mol Neurodegener. 2021;16(1):30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Ovod V, Ramsey KN, Mawuenyega KG, et al. . Amyloid β concentrations and stable isotope labeling kinetics of human plasma specific to central nervous system amyloidosis. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(8):841–849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Nakamura A, Kaneko N, Villemagne VL, et al. . High performance plasma amyloid-β biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. Nature. 2018;554(7691):249–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Palmqvist S, Janelidze S, Stomrud E, et al. . Performance of fully automated plasma assays as screening tests for Alzheimer disease-related β-amyloid Status. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76(9):1060–1069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Palmqvist S, Janelidze S, Quiroz YT, et al. . Discriminative accuracy of plasma phospho-tau217 for Alzheimer disease vs other neurodegenerative disorders. JAMA. 2020;324(8):772–781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Karikari TK, Pascoal TA, Ashton NJ, et al. . Blood phosphorylated tau 181 as a biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease: A diagnostic performance and prediction modelling study using data from four prospective cohorts. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(5):422–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Doré V, Doecke JD, Saad ZS, et al. . Plasma P217+tau versus NAV4694 amyloid and MK6240 tau PET across the Alzheimer’s continuum. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;14(1):e12307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Mattsson N, Cullen NC, Andreasson U, Zetterberg H, Blennow K. Association between longitudinal plasma neurofilament light and neurodegeneration in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76(7):791–799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Ashton NJ, Janelidze S, Al Khleifat A, et al. . A multicentre validation study of the diagnostic value of plasma neurofilament light. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):3400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Elahi FM, Casaletto KB, La Joie R, et al. . Plasma biomarkers of astrocytic and neuronal dysfunction in early- and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(4):681–695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Pereira JB, Janelidze S, Smith R, et al. . Plasma GFAP is an early marker of amyloid- but not tau pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2021;144(11):3505–3516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Benedet AL, Milà-Alomà M, Vrillon A, et al. . Differences between plasma and cerebrospinal fluid Glial fibrillary acidic protein levels across the Alzheimer disease Continuum. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(12):1471–1483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Barthélemy NR, Li Y, Joseph-Mathurin N, et al. . A soluble phosphorylated tau signature links tau, amyloid and the evolution of stages of dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Med. 2020;26(3):398–407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Karikari TK, Emeršič A, Vrillon A, et al. . Head-to-head comparison of clinical performance of CSF phospho-tau T181 and T217 biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease diagnosis. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17(5):755–767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Thijssen EH, La Joie R, Strom A, et al. . Plasma phosphorylated tau 217 and phosphorylated tau 181 as biomarkers in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A retrospective diagnostic performance study. Lancet Neurol. 2021;20(9):739–752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Wennström M, Janelidze S, Nilsson KPR, et al. . Cellular localization of p-Tau217 in brain and its association with p-Tau217 plasma levels. Acta Neuropathol Commun. 2022;10(1):3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Mattsson-Carlgren N, Janelidze S, Bateman RJ, et al. . Soluble P-tau217 reflects amyloid and tau pathology and mediates the association of amyloid with tau. EMBO Mol Med. 2021;13(6):e14022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Janelidze S, Berron D, Smith R, et al. . Associations of plasma phospho-Tau217 levels with tau positron emission tomography in early Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(2):149–156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Mattsson-Carlgren N, Janelidze S, Palmqvist S, et al. . Longitudinal plasma p-Tau217 is increased in early stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2020;143(11):3234–3241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Mielke MM, Frank RD, Dage JL, et al. . Comparison of plasma phosphorylated tau Species with amyloid and tau positron emission tomography, neurodegeneration, vascular pathology, and cognitive outcomes. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(9):1108–1117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Triana-Baltzer G, Moughadam S, Slemmon R, et al. . Development and validation of a high-sensitivity assay for measuring P217+tau in plasma. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;13(1):e12204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Barthélemy NR, Horie K, Sato C, Bateman RJ. Blood plasma phosphorylated-tau isoforms track CNS change in Alzheimer’s disease. J Exp Med. 2020;217(11):e20200861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Johnson SC, Koscik RL, Jonaitis EM, et al. . The Wisconsin registry for Alzheimer’s prevention: A review of findings and current directions. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;10:130–142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Janelidze S, Palmqvist S, Leuzy A, et al. . Detecting amyloid positivity in early Alzheimer’s disease using combinations of plasma Aβ42/Aβ40 and p-tau. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;18(2):283–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Johnson SC, Christian BT, Okonkwo OC, et al. . Amyloid burden and neural function in people at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35(3):576–584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Betthauser TJ, Cody KA, Zammit MD, et al. . In vivo characterization and quantification of neurofibrillary tau PET radioligand 18F-MK-6240 in humans from Alzheimer disease dementia to young controls. J Nucl Med. 2019;60(1):93–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Racine AM, Clark LR, Berman SE, et al. . Associations between performance on an abbreviated CogState battery, other measures of cognitive function, and biomarkers in people at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;54(4):1395–1408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Farrell ME, Jiang S, Schultz AP, et al. . Defining the lowest threshold for amyloid-PET to predict future cognitive decline and amyloid accumulation. Neurology. 2021;96(4):e619–e631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Koscik RL, Betthauser TJ, Jonaitis EM, et al. . Amyloid duration is associated with preclinical cognitive decline and tau PET. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2020;12(1):e12007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Betthauser TJ, Bilgel M, Koscik RL, et al. . Multi-method investigation of factors influencing amyloid onset and impairment in three cohorts. Brain. 2022;145(11):4065–4079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Koscik RL, La Rue A, Jonaitis EM, et al. . Emergence of mild cognitive impairment in late middle-aged adults in the Wisconsin registry for Alzheimer’s prevention. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2014;38(1-2):16–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Koscik R L, Hermann BP, Allison S, et al. . Validity evidence for the research category, “cognitively unimpaired—Declining,” as a risk marker for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Aging Neurosci. 2021;13:688478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Jonaitis EM, Koscik RL, Clark LR, et al. . Measuring longitudinal cognition: Individual tests versus composites. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;11:74–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. R Core Team . R: A language and environment for statistical computing; 2017Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- 39. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Soft. 2015;67(1):1–48. [Google Scholar]
- 40. Xu R. Measuring explained variation in linear mixed effects models. Stat Med. 2003;22(22):3527–3541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Ben-Shachar MS, Lüdecke D, Makowski D. Effectsize: Estimation of effect size indices and standardized parameters. J Open Source Softw. 2020;5(56):2815. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Robin X, Turck N, Hainard A, et al. . pROC: An open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves. BMC Bioinformatics. 2011;12:77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Youden W. Index for rating diagnostic tests. Biometrics. 1950;6(2):172–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Bakdash JZ, Marusich LR. Repeated measures correlation. Front Psychol. 2017;8:456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Bakdash JZ, Marusich LR. Rmcorr: Repeated Measures Correlation; 2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 46. Planche V, Villain N. US Food and drug administration approval of aducanumab-is amyloid load a valid surrogate End point for Alzheimer disease clinical trials? JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(11):1307–1308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Brickman AM, Manly JJ, Honig LS, et al. . Correlation of plasma and neuroimaging biomarkers in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2022;9(5):756–761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Garrett SL, McDaniel D, Obideen M, et al. . Racial disparity in cerebrospinal fluid amyloid and tau biomarkers and associated cutoffs for mild cognitive impairment. JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(12):e1917363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Howell JC, Watts KD, Parker MW, et al. . Race modifies the relationship between cognition and Alzheimer’s disease cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017;9(1):88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Windon C, Iaccarino L, Mundada N, et al. . Comparison of plasma and CSF biomarkers across ethnoracial groups in the ADNI. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;14(1):e12315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Coded data may be shared at the request of any qualified investigator. R scripts underlying all analyses are available in the Supplementary material.