Abstract
An increasing number of studies suggest that implicit attitudes towards food and body shape predict eating behaviour and characterize patients with eating disorders (EDs). However, literature has not been previously analysed; thus, differences between patients with EDs and healthy controls and the level of automaticity of the processes involved in implicit attitudes are still matters of debate. The present systematic review aimed to synthesize current evidence from papers investigating implicit attitudes towards food and body in healthy and ED populations. PubMed, EMBASE (Ovid), PsycINFO, Web of Science and Scopus were systematically screened and 183 studies using different indirect paradigms were included in the qualitative analysis. The majority of studies reported negative attitudes towards overweight/obese body images in healthy and ED samples and weight bias as a diffuse stereotypical evaluation. Implicit food attitudes are consistently reported as valid predictors of eating behaviour. Few studies on the neurobiological correlates showed neurostimulation effects on implicit attitudes, but how the brain automatically processes implicit evaluations remains an open area of research. In conclusion, implicit attitudes are relevant measures of eating behaviour in healthy and clinical settings, although evidence about their neural correlates is limited.
Keywords: implicit attitudes, food preference, body image evaluation, weight bias, eating disorders
Introduction
Implicit attitudes are behaviours and judgements driven by automatic evaluations, which are triggered independently from conscious control (Greenwald et al., 1998). In the context of eating behaviour, implicit attitudes towards food and body images are indices of individual preference for different categories of food and affective evaluation of bodies in self and other representations. In particular, there is evidence that implicit food attitudes predict the subsequent actual choice for different types of food (e.g. healthy or unhealthy food) and individuals’ eating behaviours with modulatory effects of hunger state, craving and eating disorder (ED) symptomatology (Perugini, 2005; Ellis et al., 2014; Richard et al., 2019). On the other hand, implicit attitudes towards underweight or overweight body shape have been related to internalization of thin ideal, body dissatisfaction, drive for thinness and ED symptoms (e.g. Ahern et al., 2008; Cserjési et al., 2010; Heider et al., 2015). The theoretical framework posits that implicit attitudes rely on associative spontaneous processes (in contrast to propositional processes underling explicit attitudes) that can be triggered automatically and independently from the overt consideration that a person has on the same association in explicit evaluations (Gawronski and Bodenhausen, 2006). Different models have been proposed accounting for the relationship between implicit and explicit attitudes in predicting behaviour, and studies on eating behaviour reported food choice as an example of spontaneous decisions predicted by implicit evaluations (Perugini, 2005; Conner et al., 2007). In addition, strong affective evaluation of food and body image may represent a key aspect of dysfunctional eating and body dissatisfaction in patients with ED (Spring and Bulik, 2014).
However, previous literature on implicit attitudes and eating behaviour is not always consistent since some studies did not replicate findings on the predictive validity of implicit tasks above explicit measures (Ahern and Hetherington, 2006; Ayres et al., 2012), and there are contrasting results from studies assessing implicit attitudes in patients with different ED diagnoses (Roefs et al., 2005b; Khan and Petróczi, 2015; Smith et al., 2018). Crucially, implicit attitudes are measured by indirect tasks, which assess associations between target stimuli and attributes without directly inquiring participants’ beliefs and thus are less influenced by social desirability and strategies of self-presentation compared to explicit questionnaires (De Houwer, 2002). This is particularly relevant in clinical or sub-clinical populations with EDs, which tend to have a low level of therapeutic adherence and to mask symptoms as body dissatisfaction, food restraint or craving to avoid interventions and hospitalization (Halmi, 2013). Interestingly, discrepancy between explicit and implicit measures has been reported in individuals with obesity and predicted disinhibited eating (Goldstein et al., 2014; Cserjesi et al., 2016). Moreover, implicit attitude towards body images is a widely used measure of weight bias, i.e. the stigmatizing concept that people with obesity or overweight are lazy and lacking in self-control, which predicts prejudice and misbehaviour (O’Brien et al., 2008; Flint et al., 2016). Notably, weight bias is present also in healthcare professionals and in individuals with overweight with relevant clinical consequences (Anselmi et al., 2013; Phelan et al., 2015a; Tomiyama et al., 2015). Indeed, self-directed weight bias in patients with obesity or binge eating disorder (BED), referred as internalization of weight bias, has been related to higher depressive symptoms, perceived stress and worse overall health (Pearl et al., 2013; Hilbert et al., 2014; Phelan et al., 2015a). Considering healthcare professionals, including eating behaviour specialists, there is evidence of weight bias impact on interpersonal relationship, decision-making on treatments and quality of patiwents’ care, although reciprocal influences between implicit and explicit weight bias of patients and healthcare providers are a recent matter of investigation (Forhan and Salas, 2013; Phelan et al., 2015c).
Crucially, studies investigating implicit attitudes typically use indirect tasks as measures of automatic associations and affective evaluations of target stimuli, although it is still an issue of debate which is the level of automaticity of the processes involved in task execution and which are the underpinning brain mechanisms (De Houwer et al., 2009; Forbes et al., 2012). Results from neuroimaging studies and patients with brain lesions have shown that the Implicit Association Tests (IATs; Greenwald et al., 1998), one of the most diffuse tasks to assess implicit attitudes, recruits structural and functional networks involved in cognitive control and inhibition processes as well as automatic self-representation and semantic representation of concepts, including the dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, the insula and the anterior temporal lobe (Chee et al., 2000; Gozzi et al., 2009; Forbes et al., 2012). Moreover, electrophysiological studies with event-related potentials (ERPs) have reported effects related to congruency of blocks and score at the IAT in early time-windows at 90- to 130-ms post-target onset or at the N200 component, supporting the automaticity of brain responses for implicit associations (Forbes et al., 2012; Healy et al., 2015). Whether these automatic processes are common to implicit attitudes in different contexts, or have specific features related to the stimuli eliciting the bias, remains an open question. Research with neuromodulation techniques can be informative on this aspect, providing evidence of a causal relationship between induced modulation or interference in a target region and changes in implicit attitudes. Indeed, it has been shown that transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) applied to a different portion of the prefrontal cortex interfered with IAT on gender stereotype or food, whereas parietal stimulation affected implicit religiousness (Cattaneo et al., 2011; Crescentini et al., 2014; Mattavelli et al., 2015). As stated above, implicit attitudes towards food and body are relevant in predicting eating behaviour; thus, it is of interest to investigate the neural bases of these attitudes as potential targets for therapeutic neuromodulation treatments. The reasons for combining neuromodulation and assessment with indirect tasks are twofold: on one hand, implicit measures can represent a feasible alternative to explicit questionnaires to evaluate patients with EDs with difficulties in recognizing/reporting their symptoms; on the other hand, applying neurostimulation on the neural correlates of automatic processes contributing to the maintenance of ED could boost the effect of therapeutic treatment.
Different paradigms have been proposed to evaluate implicit attitudes. The IAT (Greenwald et al., 1998) measures associations of two opposite target categories (e.g. healthy vs unhealthy food) with opposite valence attributes, asking participants to categorize target stimuli and attributes congruently (e.g. healthy-positive and unhealthy-negative) and incongruently paired (e.g. healthy-negative and unhealthy-positive) in subsequent blocks. The Single Category IAT (SC-IAT; Karpinski and Steinman, 2006) consists in the same paradigm with opposite attributes to be associated with one category of stimuli. The affective priming (AP) task (Fazio et al., 1986) is another frequently used paradigm based on reaction times in categorizing positive and negative targets (words or pictures) preceded by prime stimuli (e.g. food stimuli). Responses to targets are informative of participant’s affective evaluation of primes, assuming faster responses when prime and target share the same valence (congruent trials). A modified version of the IAT is the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (EAST; De Houwer, 2003). In this case, participants are asked to categorize valence words presented in white colour as positive/negative words and target stimuli based on their colour (e.g. blue/green). The EAST is based on the prediction that participants are faster in categorizing with the positive key coloured words that they consider positive. Other indirect tasks are the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP; Barnes-Holmes et al., 2010), which requires to select between two response options (e.g. true/false) for samples and target stimuli presented together (e.g. ‘I am’ + ‘slim’, Heider et al., 2015), and the Affective Misattribution Procedure (AMP; Payne et al., 2005), which asks participants to evaluate Chinese characters as pleasant or unpleasant ignoring the preceding stimuli and the difference in responses provide a measure of how the affect evoked by the first image is misattributed to the Chinese character. All these tasks have been used to assess food and body implicit attitudes in healthy and clinical populations.
In the past decade, the assessment of implicit attitudes related to eating behaviour has become widespread, but a review is missing so far. Since the introduction of the IAT by Greenwald et al. (1998) and its early application to assess weight bias and predict eating behaviour (Teachman and Brownell, 2001; Perugini, 2005), different tasks have been proposed and used in healthy, sub-clinical and clinical populations. Previous reviews focused on cognitive bias with food- and body-related stimuli in Stroop or attentional tasks and reported greater bias in patients with EDs compared to control participants (Johansson et al., 2005; Brooks et al., 2011). However, attentional bias is related to the salience of the stimuli, whereas the indirect tasks used to evaluate implicit attitudes involved affective associations and the automatic evaluation in terms of valence of the stimuli (De Houwer, 2002; Brooks et al., 2011).
This study presents a systematic review of studies published up to May 2019 (i.e. when literature search was conducted) concerning implicit attitudes towards food and body in different areas of research. The aim is to analyse and summarize methodological aspects and findings in healthy and clinical populations with EDs. To this purpose, we will provide readers a comprehensive guide of literature in this field giving particular attention to neuroscientific methodological approaches and recent evidence of neurobiological correlates related to implicit attitudes in eating behaviour.
Method
Design and eligibility criteria
The systematic review was conducted following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines (Moher et al., 2015; Shamseer et al., 2015). Specific eligibility criteria were considered to systematically select and appraise studies (Grant and Booth, 2009). In particular, we included original studies, while systematic reviews, narrative reviews, meta-analysis, conference proceedings, case reports and conference abstracts were excluded. Only peer-reviewed papers published in English were selected. We were interested in studies on implicit attitudes towards food or body images in healthy populations or in patients with EDs. Thus, studies reporting assessment of implicit attitudes towards different targets were excluded (even if sample with EDs was involved). Similarly, studies assessing unconscious processing of food or body images, but with paradigm not related to implicit attitudes, were excluded.
Search strategy and studies selection
The following terms were combined to collect records from PubMed, EMBASE (Ovid), PsycINFO, Web of Science and Scopus: ‘implicit attitudes’, ‘implicit association’, ‘affective priming’, ‘eating disorder’, ‘anorexia nervosa’, ‘bulimia nervosa’, ‘binge eating disorder’, ‘obesity’, ‘food preference’, ‘thin ideal’ and ‘fat phobia’ (the search strategies are shown in Appendix 1). After removing duplicates, the records were independently filtered based on title and abstract by three researchers (G.M., A.G. and L.D.M.). To blind the process, we used Rayyan web-based reviews manager (Ouzzani et al., 2016), which allows to screen the records as ‘include’, ‘exclude’ or ‘maybe’. When records were eligible for exclusion, specific labels were added to justify the reason of exclusion. At the end of the screening, the blind mode was turned off and conflicts were resolved by consensus. Researchers (G.M., A.G. and L.D.M.) independently reviewed the full text of the records in the ‘include’ and ‘maybe’ categories. Conflicting decisions were solved based on researchers’ consensus.
Out of 633 screened papers, conflicts or unsure decisions for inclusion were 259 (i.e. the two raters gave different decisions of inclusion, exclusion or maybe). Percentage agreement among raters was computed (as suggested in Kottner and Streiner, 2011), resulting in a value of 59.1%, which is considered a moderate agreement (House et al., 1981; Nurjannah and Siwi, 2017). However, indecisions or disagreement were solved by consensus; thus, at the end of the screening procedure, a unanimous agreement on all papers was reached. A detailed presentation of decision percentages is reported in Supplementary Table S1.
Quality assessment
The Cochrane Collaboration’s Risk-of-Bias Tool (Higgins et al., 2011) was used to assess the methodological quality of the studies yielded by the search process. The Cochrane Tool allows to rate the following sources of bias as ‘high’, ‘low’, ‘unclear’ or ‘not applicable’: random sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding strategy, incomplete outcome data and selective outcome reporting. Three researchers independently evaluated the quality of the records included in the different sections (G.M. studies on neural correlates and food attitudes, A.G. studies on body image and L.D.M. studies on weight bias) by computing, for each Cochrane item, the percentage of the most frequent rating across the retrieved studies. Risk of bias of papers testing implicit attitudes on both food and body was calculated once.
Results
Studies selection and quality assessment
The systematic search retrieved 2840 papers. After removing duplicates, 633 papers were screened based on the abstract and 343 were excluded. The remaining 290 papers were examined as full-text readings and 107 were excluded. The most common reason for exclusion was lack of consistency with the review topic. In total, 183 studies met our inclusion criteria and were included in our review. Figure 1 summarizes the search procedure.
Fig. 1.
Flow chart showing the selection process of papers.
Results are presented on the basis of the main topics that emerged from studies meeting inclusion criteria. A small number of studies investigated the neurobiological correlates of implicit food or body attitudes in healthy individuals or patients with EDs and are described in a specific section (‘Studies on neurobiological correlates’). Other studies were grouped depending on whether implicit measures were used to assess attitudes towards food (‘Studies on food attitudes’), body image (‘Studies on body image’) or weight bias (‘Studies on weight bias’). Methodological aspects and main results are reported to capture the contribute of the studies in each specific sub-topic. In particular, papers included in body image or weight bias sections were grouped considering the common focus across the studies on the implicit evaluation of different types of body images and shapes or on the assessment of the stigmatizing (and self-stigmatizing) concept of overweight or obesity (i.e. weight bias).
Regarding the quality evaluation, Table 1 reports the risk of bias assessment according to the Cochrane Collaboration’s Risk-of-Bias Tool (Higgins et al., 2011). Overall, considering only the applicable Cochrane items, the food topic had the best quality compared to the others.
Table 1.
Risk of bias evaluation of the papers extracted from the systematic review procedure
Cochrane items | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Selection bias | Performance bias | Detection bias | Attrition bias | Reporting bias | |||
Studies topic | Random sequence generation | Allocation concealment | Blinding of participantsa | Blinding of personnel | Blinding of outcome | Incomplete outcome data | Selective reporting |
Neural correlates | 67% □ | 83% □ | 83% ↓ | 67% □ | 67% □ | 100% ↓ | 100% ↓ |
Body image | 78% □ | 74% □ | 89% ↓ | 81% □ | 81% □ | 59% ↓ 41% ↑ |
96% ↓ |
Food | 58% ↓ 40% □ |
50% □ 44% ↓ |
71% ↓ | 53% □ 29% ↑ |
51% □ 46% ↑ |
96% ↓ | 97% ↓ |
Weight bias | 74% □ | 74% □ | 91% ↓ | 87% □ | 74% □ | 74% ↓ | 92% ↓ |
↑ = high risk; ↓ = low risk; ■ = unclear; □ = not applicable;
blinding of participants was rated as ‘low risk’ both when participants were randomly assigned to experimental groups and when participants were blinded about the purposes and procedure of the implicit attitudes’ assessment.
Studies on neurobiological correlates
Six studies investigated the neurobiological underpinnings of implicit attitudes toward food or body images (Table 2). Three studies were carried out with healthy participants (Mattavelli et al., 2015; Cazzato et al., 2017; Hall et al., 2018a), two involved patients with EDs (Blechert et al., 2011; Mattavelli et al., 2019) and one study assessed patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) treated with dopaminergic replacement (Terenzi et al., 2018). Four of these studies investigated the possibility to modulate IAT performances with non-invasive brain stimulation techniques applied to different cortical regions (Mattavelli et al., 2015, 2019; Cazzato et al., 2017; Hall et al., 2018a). In particular, the study by Mattavelli et al. (2015) applied TMS to the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and left parietal cortex while healthy participants were submitted to three different IATs on food, self and flowers/insects. Results demonstrated the causal role of mPFC in monitoring implicit food attitudes and highlighted the impact of individual variability in modulating the effect of neurostimulation. Differently, neuromodulation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) by means of a continuous theta burst protocol (cTBS) did not affect the IAT score in the study by Hall et al. (2018a). The other study on healthy participants (Cazzato et al., 2017) applied transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on the extrastriate body area (EBA) in the right and left hemispheres and assessed participants’ anti-fat bias with valence-IAT and aesthetic-IAT. tDCS was applied in anodal, cathodal and sham mode on each hemisphere. Only the cathodal stimulation (applied to induce a decrease in cortical excitability) of right EBA showed a significant effect in reducing anti-fat bias, measured by the valence-IAT, in male participants. Differently, female participants did not show a reliable anti-fat bias and their performance was not modulated by tDCS. Only one study investigated neurostimulation effects on IAT in patients with ED (Mattavelli et al., 2019) applying anodal or sham tDCS to the mPFC and right EBA and assessing participants’ implicit attitudes toward food, body images and flowers/insects. Results showed that the occipito-temporal stimulation increased the implicit preference for high-fat tasty food in patients with ED and both mPFC and EBA stimulation increased reaction times in incongruent trials (i.e. tasty food—negative attributes and tasteless food—positive attributes associations). The effect was specific for food attitudes in patients, whereas no modulatory effects resulted for healthy participants and in the other IATs.
Table 2.
Details of studies on neurobiological correlates of implicit attitudes toward food or body shape
Studies | Sample (N) | Mean age (SD) | Implicit measure | Target categories | Method |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Blechert et al., 2011 | Study 1 AN (20, f = 20) BN (20, f = 20) H (20, f = 20) Study 2 |
23.1 (4.64) 26.5 (7.78) 25.4 (4.80) |
AP | Primes sentences on body shape and weight concern, self-esteem related word target | EEG (study 1) |
H-UNRES (21, f = 21) | 23.6 (5.02) | ||||
H-RES (18, f = 18) | 22.6 (3.27) | ||||
Cazzato et al., 2017 | H (25, f = 13) | f: 22.08 (0.73) m: 22 (0.6) |
IAT | Fat, slim body images | tDCS |
Hall et al., 2018a | H (37, f = n.r.) | 21.6 (n.r.) | IAT | Flavourful snack, Flavourless foods | cTBS |
Mattavelli et al., 2015 | H (36, f = 21) | 23.25 (2.88) | IAT | Tasty and high-fat, tasteless and low-fat food | TMS |
Mattavelli et al., 2019 | AN/BN/EDNOS (36, f = 36) H (36, f = 36) |
25.53 (8.09) 24.03 (3.48) |
IAT | Tasty high-fat, tasteless low-fat food; Underweight, overweight body images. |
tDCS |
Terenzi et al., 2018 | PD + BE (16, f = 8) PD (15, f = 7) H (20, f = 10) |
67.1 (8.2) 64.9 (12.9) 68 (6.4) |
AP | Food, non-food |
AN = anorexia nervosa; AP = affective priming; BN = bulimia nervosa; cTBS = continuous theta burst protocol; EDNOS = eating disorder not otherwise specified; f = female; H = healthy participants; H-RES = healthy restrained eaters; H-UNRES = healthy unrestrained eaters; IAT = Implicit Association Test; n.r. = not reported; PD = Parkinson’s disease; PD + BE = Parkinson’s disease with binge eating; tDCS = transcranial direct current stimulation; TMS = transcranial magnetic stimulation.
Only one study investigated the electrophysiological correlates of AP measuring the association of self-related targets with body shape and weight primes (Blechert et al., 2011). ERPs were recorded in healthy participants and patients with anorexia nervosa (AN) or bulimia nervosa (BN) during the AP task. Results showed a significant congruent–incongruent difference in the N400 amplitude only in the BN group, suggesting that the stronger association of self-evaluation with body shape and weight concept in ED is encoded in early stage of brain processes in BN patients.
Finally, Terenzi et al. (2018) used an AP on food to evaluate reward sensitivity in patients with PD treated with dopaminergic medications and BED. These patients are at risk of developing impulse control disorder and represent a neurobiological model for striatal dopaminergic anomalous functioning (Dagher and Robbins, 2009). Patients with PD and BED showed reduced priming effect compared to controls for sweet foods, but no differences in explicit rating on liking and wanting the foods.
Studies on food attitudes
Implicit attitudes toward food were investigated in 75 studies (Table 3). Among these, three studies that considered both food and body stimuli (Spring and Bulik, 2014; Khan and Petróczi, 2015; Moussally et al., 2015) are presented in the body section and also in details in Table 4. Most of these studies (N = 53) measured implicit attitudes in healthy normal-weight participants using the two-categories IAT (N = 26). Ten studies used the SC-IAT, whereas three studies tested participants both with the two-categories IAT and the SC-IAT (Friese et al., 2008; Houben et al., 2010; Guidetti et al., 2012). Five studies used the AP task, one the semantic priming task (Misener and Libben, 2017), three the AMP (Ellis et al., 2014; Woodward and Treat, 2015; Woodward et al., 2017) and two the EAST or similar variants (Hoefling and Strack, 2008; Veenstra and de Jong, 2010). Four studies combined more than one task, to investigate the role of different implicit measures in predicting behaviour (Roefs et al., 2005a; Conner et al., 2007; Seibt et al., 2007; Genschow et al., 2017). One study assessed recovered and currently diagnosed patients with AN and healthy controls by means of AMP procedure including high- and low-calorie food as well as thin- and fat-related body images (Spring and Bulick, 2014; see Table 4). Nineteen studies evaluated food implicit attitudes in participants with overweight and obesity. With this population, seven studies employed the two-categories IAT, four studies the SC-IAT, three studies the AP and one a semantic priming paradigm (Werrij et al., 2009). The EAST was used in two studies (Craeynest et al., 2005, 2008a) and the IRAP in one (McKenna et al., 2016). The study by Roefs et al. (2005) reported two separate experiments which compared performance at the AP task between patients with AN vs control participants and people with obesity vs control participants, respectively. Concerning the type of stimuli, 47 out of 75 studies used images of food in the implicit tasks, whereas 27 studies employed words of food representing different types of target categories and a study (Becker et al., 2015) used both words or images in different tasks (see Table 3).
Table 3.
Details of studies on implicit attitudes toward food
Studies | Sample (N) | Mean age (SD) | Implicit measure | Target categories | Manipulation |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Healthy participants | |||||
Ackermann and Palmer, 2014 | H (101, f = 62) | n.r. | IAT | Healthy food, fast food | - |
Adams et al., 2017 | H (143, f = 134) | 22.92 (n.r.) | SC-IAT | Chocolate | Response inhibition training |
Alblas et al., 2018 | H (125, f = 79) | 20.17 (1.88) | IAT | Chocolate, fruits | Evaluative conditioning (health video-games) |
Alkozei et al., 2018 | H (17, f = 8) | 24.53 (4.2) | IAT | High calorie, low-calorie food | Sleep deprivation |
Ashby and Stritzke, 2013 | H (132, f = 96) H (127, f = 97) |
18.67 (3.3) 18.75 (3.1) |
IAT | High-fat, low-fat food | Priming procedure |
Ayres et al., 2012 | H (80, f = 67) H (98, f = 80) |
22.1 (7.3) 23 (4.6) |
IAT | Chocolate, fruits | - |
Becker et al., 2015 | H (52, f = 52) H (104, f = 104) H (103, f = 103) |
20.47 (2.34) 20.77 (2.94) 21.94 (3.59) |
IAT AP AP |
healthy, unhealthy food Healthy, unhealthy food, objects Chocolate, objects |
Approach-avoidance training |
Bongers et al., 2013 | H (112, f = 112) | 20.34 (2.24) and 19.83 (1.83) per condition | SC-IAT | High-caloric food | Emotion induction during milk shake consumption |
Conner et al., 2007 | H (123, f = 76) H (104, f = 84) |
23.7 (5.8) 23.2 (4.9) |
IAT EAST IAT |
Sweets, shapes Sweets Chocolate, fruits |
- |
Coricelli et al., 2019 | H (45, f = 22) | 23.22 (3.12) | IAT | Natural food, utensils Transformed food, utensils |
- |
Ellis et al., 2014 | H (107, f = 59) | 27 (11.9) | AMP | Fruits | - |
Eschenbeck et al., 2016 | H (90, f = 79) | 21.56 (3.83) | IAT | Healthy, unhealthy food | High and low distraction situations |
Friese et al., 2008 | H (88, f = 88) 69 (f = 69) 48 (f = 0) |
23.19 (4.29) 22.48 (4.59) 24.11 (4.41) |
IAT SC-IAT SC-IAT |
Chocolate, fruits Chips Beer |
Cognitive load or self-regulatory resources during food choice |
Genschow et al., 2017 | H (91, f = 73) | 22.80 (5) | EAT AP MT |
Chocolate, fruits | Affective or cognitive focus before food choice |
Guidetti et al., 2012 | H (85, f = 75) | 18.86 (0.97) | SC-IAT IAT |
Fruits Sweet, savoury snacks |
- |
Hensels and Baines, 2016 | H (95, f = 70) | 24.88 (6.16) | IAT | Healthy, unhealthy food | Evaluative conditioning |
Hoefling and Strack, 2008 | H (66, f = 37) | 23.9 (3.7) | EAST | High-calorie, low-calorie food | Food deprived or satiated between groups |
Hollands et al., 2011 | H (134, f = 101) | 24.2 (n.r.) | IAT | Fruits, snacks | Evaluative conditioning |
Houben et al., 2010 | H (59, f = 59) H (63, f = 63) |
31.44 (9.61) 34.71 (13.28) |
IAT SC-IAT |
Snacks, fruits Snacks |
- |
Houben et al., 2012 | H (112, f = 112) | 24.88 (5.92) | SC-IAT | High-caloric food moderate-caloric food Low caloric food |
- |
Kakoschke et al., 2017 | H (240, f = 240) | 20.61 (2.43) | SC-IAT | Unhealthy food | Approach-avoidance training combined with Go/No-go training |
Kraus and Piqueras-Fiszman, 2016 | H (108, f = 68) | 22.9 (2.9) | IAT-RF | Sandwich, sweets | Hunger state |
Lamote et al., 2004 | H (26, f = 17) H (29, f = 25) |
36.96 (4.03) 19.55 (1.84) |
AP AP |
Individually positive and negative rated food | (Study 2) Strong or moderate prime condition |
Lebens et al., 2011 | H (85, f = 85) | E 34.14 (12.87) C 34.23 (13.49) |
SC-IAT | Snack | Evaluative conditioning |
Mai and Hoffman, 2015 | H (203, f = 81) | 23.0 (2.58) | IAT | Energy-dense, energy-poor food | - |
Mayer et al., 2008 | H (50, f = 50) | n.r. (undergraduate students) | IAT | High-caloric food, low-caloric food | Disgusting odour |
Maas et al., 2017 | H (83, f = 72) | n.r. (undergraduate students) | SC-IAT | High-fat food | - |
McConnell et al., 2011 | H (56, f = 35) | 19.25 (n.r.) | IAT | Apple, chocolate | - |
Misener and Libben, 2017 | H (115, f = 115) | 19.9 (1.47) | SP | ED, non-ED prime-target relation | - |
Nederkoon et al., 2010 | H (51, f = 51) | 19.5 (2.2) | SC-IAT | Snack food | - |
Papies et al., 2009 | H (91, f = 91) H (100, f = 65) |
20.5 (2.2) 20.07 (2.60) |
AP AP |
High-fat palatable, neutral, unpalatable food High-fat palatable, neutral food |
- |
Pavlovic et al., 2016 | H (89, f = 73) H (40, f = 40) |
21.7 (n.r.) 2.4 (n.r.) |
IAT | Fruits, sweets | - |
Pechey et al., 2015 | H (732, f = 371) | 51 (14) | SC-IAT | Fruit Chees Cake |
- |
Perugini, 2005 | H (113, f = 62) | 25.1 (6.8) | IAT | Snacks, fruits | - |
Richard et al., 2019 | H (66, f = 66) | 20.3 (2.34) | SC-IAT | Chocolate | - |
Roefs et al., 2005a | H restrained (32, f = 32) H unrestrained (37, f = 37) H restrained (26, f = 26) H unrestrained (30, f = 30) |
19.5 (2) 19.5 (1.8) 19.6 (2.2) 19.3 (1.1) |
AP EAST |
High-fat, low-fat food High-fat palatable, high-fat Unpalatable, low-fat palatable, low-fat unpalatable food |
- |
Raghunathan et al., 2006 | H (131, f = n.r.) | n.r. (undergraduate students) | IAT | Healthy, unhealthy food | - |
Seibt et al., 2007 | H (29, f = n.r.) H (74, f = n.r.) |
n.r. n.r. (undergraduate students) |
IAT EAST |
Food, sport Food, flower, non-words |
Deprived vs satiated between participant assessment |
Stafford and Scheffler, 2008 | H (30, f = 24) | 30.4 (7.4) | IAT | Food, furniture | Pre-lunch vs post-lunch between participants assessment |
Sato et al., 2016 | H (34, f = 16) | 23.3 (4.5) | AP | Fast food, Japanese diet food | - |
Sato et al., 2017 | H hungry (28, f = 13) H satiated (28, f = 13) |
22.9 (4.4) 23.4 (4.7) |
AP | Fast food, Japanese diet food | Hungry/satiated between groups conditions |
Schakel et al., 2018 | H (120, f = 97) | 21.3 (2.4) | IAT | Healthy, unhealthy food | Gamified approach avoidance training and verbal suggestions |
Songa and Russo, 2018 | H (60, f = 38) H (80, f = 44) |
32 (n.r.) 28 (n.r.) |
IAT | High-energy, low-energy food | - |
Storr and Sparks, 2016 | H (183, f = 183) | 27.29 (10.84) | IAT | high-calorie, Low-calorie food |
Self-affirmation and ego-depletion |
Trendel and Werle, 2016 | H (283, f = 163) H (142, f = 74) |
20.6 (n.r.) 20.6 (n.r.) |
IAT | Chocolate, apple | Cognitive load during food choice |
Van Dessel et al., 2018 | H (389, f = 219) H (184, f = 59) |
34 (13) 20 (2) |
IAT | Healthy, unhealthy food | Approach avoidance training |
Veenstra and de Jong, 2010 | H (55, f = 55) | n.r. (undergraduate students) | AST-voice | High-fat, low-fat food | - |
Wang et al., 2011 | H (100, f = 100) | 21.3 (2.4) | SC-IAT | Chocolate | Ego-depletion |
Werle et al., 2013 | H (94, f = 50) | 19.6 (n.r.) | IAT | Healthy, unhealthy food | - |
Werntz et al., 2016 | H (10 115, f = 76.6%)1 | 27.6 (11.4) | IAT | High-fat, low-fat food | - |
Woodward et al., 2015 | H (238, f = 238) | 18.98 (1.72) | AMP | Food | - |
Woodward et al., 2017 | H (238, f = 238) | 19.08 (1.40) | AMP | Food | - |
Yen et al., 2010 | PMDD (60, f = 60) H (59, f = 59) |
23.03 (2.45) 22.70 (2.37) |
IAT | High-sweet-fat, high-salted-fat food | - |
Overweight/Obesity | |||||
Alabduljader et al., 2018 | HW (33, f = 28) OB (20, f = 14) |
38.85 (11.36) 40.25 (9.51) | IAT | Unhealthy, healthy food | - |
Craeynest et al., 2005 | HW (38, f = 21) OB (38, f = 21) |
13.53 (2.52) 13.69 (2.63) |
EAST | Healthy, unhealthy food; Sedentary, moderate intense, high intense physical activity |
- |
Craeynest et al., 2006 | HW (39, f = 22) OB (39, f = 23) |
14.00 (2.40) 14.12 (2.43) |
IAT | Fat, non-fat food; Exercise, inactive child |
- |
Craeynest et al., 2007 | HW (40, f = 29) OW (40, f = 29 = |
14.83 (0.64) 14.83 (0.75) |
IAT | Palatable food, hobby Palatable healthy, palatable unhealthy food |
- |
Craeynest et al., 2008a | OB (19, f = 12) | 12.79 (2.68) | EAST | Healthy, unhealthy food; Sedentary, moderate intense, high intense physical activity |
12-month multi-component inpatient programme |
Craeynest et al., 2008b | HW (29, f = 16) OW (29, f = 17) HW (29, f = 14) OB (29, f = 17) |
14.34 (1.11) 14.59 (1.27) 13.07 (2.09) 13.21 (2.11) |
IAT | Fat, lean food | - |
Cserjesi et al., 2016 | HW (15, f = 7) OB (15, f = 7) |
38.4 (9.5) 37.8 (9.5) |
AP | Small, medium, large portion of a typical fast food | - |
Czyzewska and Graham, 2008 | UW (9, f = 9) HW (51, f = 51) OW (12, f = 12) OB (11, f = 11) |
21.11 (2.80) 21.61 (3.97) 21.75 (2.30) 24.36 (3.96) |
AP | High-calorie non-sweet, high-calorie sweet, low-calorie food, food-related items | - |
Ferentzi et al., 2018 | OB (129, f = 62) | 48 (9.45) | SC-IAT | Unhealthy food | Four sessions of approach-avoidance training |
Goldstein et al., 2014 | HW/OW (95, f = 95) | 19.87 (2.16) | SC-IAT | Chocolate | - |
Kemps and Tiggeman, 2015 | HW (56, f = 56) OB (56, f = 56) |
44.95 (11.82) | IAT | Food (healthy and unhealthy items), non-food | - |
McKenna et al., 2016 | HW (24, f = 11) OB (25, f = 15) HW (32, f = 16) OB (25, f = 15) HW (42, f = 20) OB (32, f = 16) |
20.92 (3.11) 41.57 (8.84) 20.81 (1.36) 41.36 (9.91) 20.76 (3.67) 36.88 (9.76) |
IRAP | Healthy, unhealthy | Food restriction |
Roefs and Jansen, 2002 | HW (31, f = 25) OB (30, f = 24) |
40.5 (14.4) 46.3 (14.8) |
IAT | High-fat, low-fat food | - |
Roefs et al., 2005b | HW (27, f = 27) AN (19, f = 19) HW (27, f = 27) OB (27, f = 27) |
20.4 (5.8) 20.6, (6.3) 36.6 (8.7) 36.5 (8.8) |
AP | High-fat palatable, low-fat palatable, high-fat unpalatable, low-fat unpalatable food | - |
Roefs et al., 2006 | HW (26, f = 26) OB (33, f = 33) HW (29, f = 29) OB (27, f = 27) |
41.8 (8.1) 41.7 (6.9) 35.8 (10.0) 36.4 (9.9) |
AP | High-fat palatable foods, low-fat palatable foods, high-fat unpalatable foods, low-fat unpalatable foods | (i) Focus attention on palatability or health aspects of food (ii) Food craving induction |
Sartor et al., 2011 | HW (22, f = 15) OW/OB (11, f = 4) Study 2 H (12, f = 7) |
23.1 (2.9) 22.2 (1.6) 26 (6) |
IAT | Sweet, non-sweet foods and drink | (Study 2) 4 weeks soft drink supplementation |
Verbeken et al., 2018 | OB (44, f = 23) | 12.58 (1.43) | SC-IAT | Unhealthy food | 10 sessions of approach-avoidance training |
Warschburger et al., 2018 | OW/OB (59, f = 33) | 13.23 (1.93) | SC-IAT | High energy snacks | Six sessions of approach-avoidance training |
Werrij et al., 2009 | HW (19, f = 19) OW/OB (24, f = 24) HW (29, f = 29) OB (28, f = 28) |
41 (12) 42 (10.3) 37 (8.5) 37 (8.9) |
SP | Palatable food, neutral words, disinhibition words Palatable food, neutral words, restraint words |
- |
AMP = affective misattribution procedure; AP = affective priming; AST = Affective Simon Task; C = control group; E = experimental group; EAST = extrinsic affective Simon test; ED = eating disorder; H = healthy participants; HW = healthy weight; IAT = Implicit Association Test; IAT-RF = recording free variant of IAT; IRAP = Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure; MT = manikin task; n.r. = not reported; OB = obese; OW = overweight; PMDD = premenstrual dysphoric disorder; SC-IAT = Single category IAT; SP = semantic priming; UW = underweight; f = female. 1part of a larger web-based data collection on other mental health domains.
Table 4.
Details of studies on implicit attitudes toward body image
Studies | Sample (N) | Mean age (SD) | Implicit measure | Target categories | Manipulation |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Healthy participants | |||||
Ahern et al., 2006 | H (86, f = 86) | Range 16–25 years | IAT | Fat and thin silhouettes | _ |
Ahern et al., 2008 | H (105, f = 105) | 18.01 (0.15) | IAT | Underweight and normal-weight body images | |
Benas and Gibb, 2011 | H (202, f = 202) | 18.93 (1.17) | IAT | Fat and thin related words | _ |
Elran-Barak and Bar-Anan, 2018 | H (66 799, f = 47.265) | 27.88 (11.9) | IAT | Fat and thin silhouettes | _ |
Expósito et al., 2015 | H (34, f = 34) | 23.35 (1.35) | IRAP | Fat and thin women silhouettes | _ |
Glashouwer et al., 2018 | H (72, f = 72) | 20.05 (1.41) | RRT | Actual or ideal body image related sentences | _ |
Heider et al., 2015 | H (68, f = 68) | 18.72 (2.12) | IRAP | Actual or ideal body image related sentences | _ |
Heider et al., 2018 | H (68, f = 68) | 18.72 (2.12) | RRT | Actual or ideal body image related sentences | _ |
Juarascio et al., 2011 | H (80, f = 80) | 18.24 (0.68) | IRAP | Fat and thin silhouettes | _ |
Lydecker et al., 2018 | H (657, f = 488), | 36.81 (7.96) | IATs | Thin or fat children related words | _ |
Marini, 2017 | H (4.806, f = 3.253/67.7%) | 28.32 (11.65) | IAT | Underweight, normal-weight, overweight and obese body images | _ |
Martijn et al., 2013 | Study 1 H (66, f = 66) Study 2 H (39, f = 39) |
18.94 (0.99) 21.51 (1.83) |
IAT Explicit ratings |
Images of supermodels vs normal-sized models | Evaluative conditioning task |
Matharu et al., 2014 | H (129, f = 91) | 25.2 (2.9) | IAT | Fat and thin silhouettes | Standard lecture vs medical humanities intervention |
Moussally et al., 2015* | H (121, f = 121) | 23.97 (4.80) | AMP | Thin and overweight women picture; permitted and forbidden food | _ |
Ritzert et al., 2016 | H (99, f = 75) | 18.6 (1.0) | IRAP | Attractiveness vs disgust/fear related sentences | _ |
Robstad et al., 2018 | H (30, f = 30) | 46.53 (n.r.) | IATs | Words related to the concept of obese vs normal body shape | _ |
Sabin et al., 2015 | H (75, f = 41) | 48 (-) | IAT | Fat and thin silhouettes | _ |
Watts et al., 2008 | Study 1 H (87, f = 87) Study 2 H (72, f = 72) |
18.91 (2.65) 19.28 (1.83) |
AP | Images of body parts and body shapes | _ |
EDs | |||||
Anselmi et al., 2011 | OB (43, f = 25) H (331, f = 214) |
34.30 (10.01) 26.16 (8.07) |
IAT | Faces of thin and fat people | _ |
Cserjési et al., 2010 | AN (35, f = 35) H (35, f = 35) |
19.61 (3.42) 20.27 (3.93) |
AP | Ultra-thin, average-size, overweight bodies | _ |
Izquierdo et al., 2019 | FP-AN (39, f = 39) NFP-AN (13, f = 13) Low-weight ARFID (10, f = 10) H (32, f = 32) |
19.5 (2.5) 18.3 (3.6) 15.2 (3.6) 17.4 (3.1) |
IATs | EDI-3 statements; underweight or normal-weight body pictures |
_ |
Keng and Ang, 2019 | AN/BN/BED (81, f = 81) | Range 18–55 years | IAT | Body-related words | Mindful breathing exercise vs resting condition |
Khan and Petróczi, 2015* | AN (14, f = 14) BN (24, f = 24) EDNOS (16, f = 16). At-risk ED (41, f = 41) H (23, f = 23) |
27.00 (12.95) 21.54 (6.19) 20.19 (5.69) 21.56 (7.83) 23.00 (4.97) |
IBI-BIAT PBI-BIAT FP-AAT |
Normal vs thin silhouettes; High-fat foods vs low-fat foods |
_ |
Parling et al., 2012 | AN (12, f = 12) Sub-AN (5, f = 5) H (17, f = 17) |
25.33 (6.0) 23.40 (4.4) 24.76 (5.5) |
IRAP | ‘Pro-thin/anti-fat’ or ‘anti-thin/pro-fat’ words | _ |
Smith et al., 2014 | AN (30, f = 30) H (29, f = 29) |
20.03 (2.83) 18.93 (1.41) | AP | ‘Beauty’ words (e.g. glamorous, beautiful and attractive), ‘ugly’ words (e.g. hideous, gross and disgusting), ‘neutral’ word (e.g. mailbox, cloud and desktop), ‘positive’ words (e.g. happy, cheerful and elated) in a lexical decision task |
Emaciation prime (photos of emaciated looking women) vs thin prime (photos of thin women) |
Smith et al., 2018 | AN/sub-AN (32, f = 31) BN/sub-BN (37, f = 36) Other ED (23, f = 21) H (85, f = 39) |
28.34 (7.88) 32.00 (12.93) 40.35 (12.00) 36.38 (10.75) |
AMP | ED-symptom images (emaciation, binge eating, hard exercise and vomiting); body stimuli images (average weight female bodies); eating stimuli images (women eating and words related to eating, e.g. dining, snacking and eating) | _ |
Spring and Bulik, 2014* | AN (9, f = 9) Recovered AN (14, f = 14) H (29, f = 29) |
21.4 (5.79) | AMP | High/low calorie food images and fatness/thinness related images | _ |
AN = anorexia nervosa; AMP = Affective Misattribution Procedure; AP = affective priming; ARFID = avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder; BED = binge eating disorder; BN = bulimia nervosa; ED = eating disorder; EDNOS = eating disorder not otherwise specified; f = female; FP-AAT = Food Preference Approach Avoidance Task; FP-AN = fat-phobic anorexia nervosa; H = healthy participants; IAT = Implicit Association Test; IBI-BIAT = Ideal Body Image Brief Implicit Association Test; IRAP = Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure; NFP-AN = non-fat phobic anorexia nervosa; n.r. = not reported; OB = obese; OW = overweight; PBI-BIAT = Personal Self Identification Body Image Brief Implicit Association Test; RRT = Relational Responding Task. *Studies measuring implicit attitudes on both body and food (data on food implicit preferences are presented in the food section).
Studies investigating food attitudes in healthy normal-weight participants could be grouped on the basis of three main objectives: (i) to assess implicit and explicit measures as predictors of eating behaviour; (ii) to measure differences in implicit food attitudes between individuals or groups with different features in eating behaviour and their relationship with ED symptoms; (iii) to evaluate the impact of manipulations regarding the context, the hunger state or the type of stimuli on implicit food attitudes. Similarly, studies including participants with obesity or overweight aimed at (i) exploring between-group differences in implicit food attitudes or (ii) assessing the effect of manipulations and training programmes on eating behaviour and attitudes.
Implicit attitudes as predictors of eating behaviour.
A first study by Perugini (2005) measured implicit and explicit attitudes toward snacks and fruits to test their validity to predict the following behaviour in food choice. Results are discussed as supporting a double dissociation model of the impact of implicit and explicit attitudes on behaviour, since the IAT was significantly related to spontaneous food choice whereas explicit questionnaire was significantly related to self-reported behaviour. Following studies supported this model (Pavlović et al., 2016; Songa and Russo, 2018) and showed the moderating effect of ED symptoms (Ellis et al., 2014) and of individual differences in habit in food behaviour and need for cognition (Conner et al., 2007) on the relative predictive impact of explicit and implicit measures for the subsequent food choice. In line with this evidence, the study by McConnell et al. (2011) reported that both IAT and explicit preference predicted the actual affective experience for eating, but affective forecasting errors were predicted only by the implicit measure suggesting the role of unconscious evaluation in blind affective predictions. Moreover, implicit attitudes have been shown to predict everyday chocolate consumption with interactive patterns of relationship with hunger and craving (Richard et al., 2019). Differently, other studies did not show incremental validity of implicit over explicit measures in predicting food choice, eating behaviour or body mass index (BMI) (Ayres et al., 2012; Ackermann and Palmer, 2014; Woodward and Treat, 2015; Maas et al., 2017; Woodward et al., 2017)
Seven studies investigated how the relationship between implicit and explicit measures and food choice behaviour was influenced by different conditions of cognitive load or self-regulatory resources (Friesen et al., 2008; Eschenbeck et al., 2016; Trendel and Werle, 2016; Wang et al., 2016), positive or negative priming manipulation (Ashby and Stritzke, 2013), emotional induction (Bongers et al., 2013) or manipulation of cognitive or affective focus (Genschow et al., 2017). Six of these studies consistently reported the predictive validity of implicit measures on the following choice behaviour and also found significant effects of the different manipulations (Friesen et al., 2008; Ashby and Stritzke, 2013; Bongers et al., 2013; Eschenbeck et al., 2016; Trendel and Werle, 2016; Wang et al., 2016). Similarly, Nederkoorn et al. (2010) showed that implicit preference for snack food interacted with response inhibition capacity in predicting weight change over a year. In contrast, the most recent study by Genschow et al. (2017) did not replicate the significant effect of different implicit measures [EAST, AP and manikin task (MT)] in predicting the actual behaviour, neither found significant impact of conditions focusing participants’ attention on cognitive or affective aspects.
Another line of research on food perception demonstrated the automaticity of unhealthy–tasty food association and in turn its relationship with body fat (Raghunathan et al., 2006; Mai and Hoffmann, 2015), although the opposite healthy–tasty food association has been reported in a French sample, suggesting intercultural differences in food evaluation (Werle et al., 2013). Moreover, a relevant role of food type has been suggested by Coricelli et al. (2019), who showed different implicit attitudes related to restrictive eating habits and healthiness explicit evaluation, for natural and transformed food matched for calories content. Finally, one study did not assess the actual behaviour, but investigated food attitudes transmission in social contexts testing dyads composed by students with a parent or a friend: results revealed that students’ implicit, but not explicit, attitudes toward fruits were associated with those of parents, whereas explicit, but not implicit, attitudes toward snacks were associated with those of friends (Guidetti et al., 2012).
Individual differences and relation with ED symptoms.
Five studies compared groups of restrained vs unrestrained eaters using implicit paradigms on high-fat and low-fat foods (Roefs et al., 2005a; Papies et al., 2009; Houben et al., 2010, 2012; Veenstra and de Jong, 2010). Different results are reported depending on stimuli and tasks. Indeed, positive association with high-fat palatable food in both restrained and unrestrained groups is reported in studies using Simon paradigm tasks and AP (Roefs et al., 2005a; Veenstra and de Jong, 2010), although stronger AP in unrestrained eaters is reported by Papies et al. (2009). No positive associations with high-caloric snacks and no group differences emerged using IAT with fruit and snack targets (Study 1 in Houben et al., 2001); in contrast, positive associations with snacks resulted using SC-IAT and this positivity was larger in restrained eaters (Study 2 in Houben et al., 2001). Restrained eaters also showed more positive associations than unrestrained eaters with palatable food in SC-IAT, independently from the caloric food density (Houben et al., 2012).
One study compared women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) and controls assessed with an IAT on high-sweet-fat and high-salted-fat food (Yen et al., 2010). Changes in appetite, overeating and increased food craving are some of PMDD diagnostic criteria; accordingly, the study reported higher implicit positive responses to high-sweet-fat food and higher craving response to food in PMDD group and in women in luteal phase, supporting differences in women’s appetite related to menstrual cycle, the relevance of eating assessment in PMDD and the feasibility of implicit measure to evaluate these aspects (Yen et al., 2010).
Four studies investigated the relationship between implicit food preferences and individual variables (Pechey et al., 2015; Sato et al., 2016; Werntz et al., 2016; Misener and Libben, 2017). Sato et al. (2016) showed significant AP effect in evaluating faces subliminally primed by food or mosaic images, with no differences between high-fat and low-fat food and reported that this effect correlated with individual external eating. In line with this, a semantic priming paradigm showed that priming effect did not differ between ED-related word pairs and non-ED-related word pairs across the whole sample of female students, but higher scores on ED questionnaires and body dissatisfaction were associated with increased priming for ED-related word pairs as compared to non-ED-related word pairs (Misener and Libben, 2017). Moreover, data from website studies showed that implicit preferences for fruit measured with a SC-IAT were significantly related to socioeconomic variables and gender (Pechey et al., 2015), and IAT with high-fat vs low-fat foods and shameful vs acceptable attributes predicted symptoms and concerns related to EDs with stronger high-fat food to shameful association in participants reporting higher score in a EDs questionnaire (Werntz et al., 2016).
Finally, contrasting results come from studies measuring both food and body attitudes: correlations between thin preference and positive evaluation of permitted and forbidden foods are reported by Moussally et al. (2015), in line with results from Spring and Bulik (2014) of higher negative implicit affect toward high-fat food and overweight body images in patients with AN compared to healthy controls; in contrast, Khan and Petróczi (2015) showed that body implicit attitude discriminated between ED and control individuals whereas no differences emerged for food attitudes.
Manipulation effects on implicit attitudes.
One study investigated the AP effect manipulating the strength of the positive or negative food prime stimuli, which were individually selected to be extremely or moderately related to positive and negative affects (Lamote et al., 2004). Results showed that the significant AP effect was not moderated by the evaluative extremity of the prime.
Five studies measured implicit food attitudes in participants with different hunger states (Seibt et al., 2007; Hoefling and Strack, 2008; Stafford and Scheffler, 2008; Kraus and Piqueras-Fiszman, 2016; Sato et al., 2017). Administering both motivational and evaluation IATs, Kraus and Piqueras-Fiszman (2016) highlighted that hunger state affected the first, but not the latter implicit attitudes measure. Other studies reported that participants in high vs low hunger state showed increased food preference in an IAT with food and non-food stimuli (Seibt et al., 2007; Stafford and Scheffler, 2008) and increased food preference in supraliminal and subliminal AP tasks with food and mosaic images (Sato et al., 2017). Similarly, hunger state increased the positive association with both high- and low-calorie foods compared to control stimuli in an EAST paradigm (Hoefling and Strack, 2008). This last study compared groups of restrained and unrestrained eaters and reported the same effect of hunger state manipulation in both groups, although restrained eaters had larger positive automatic associations with high-calorie food (Hoefling and Strack, 2008).
One study investigated the effect of sleep restriction on IAT with high- and low-calorie food and reported that food attitudes were not affected by sleep condition in the whole sample, whereas a significant sex by condition interaction revealed that males had greater association of low-calorie food with positive attributes than females in rest condition; no sex difference appeared in sleep restricted condition (Alkozei et al., 2018).
Eleven studies investigated the impact of different conditioning procedures on IAT: evaluative conditioning (EC; Hollands et al., 2011; Lebens et al., 2011; Hensels and Baines, 2016; Alblas et al., 2018), approach-avoidance training (Becker et al., 2015; Kakoschke et al., 2017; Schakel et al., 2018; Van Dessel et al., 2018), response inhibition training (Adams et al., 2017), ego-depletion and self-affirmation procedures (Storr and Sparks, 2016) and disgusting odour exposure (Mayer et al., 2008). These studies used between groups designs comparing participants randomly assigned to different experimental procedures. EC procedures paired images of unhealthy foods with images of potential adverse health consequences, negative facial expressions or body shapes (Hollands et al., 2011; Lebens et al., 2011; Hensels and Baines, 2016) or used video games to strengthen positive associations with fruits and negative associations with chocolate snacks (Alblas et al., 2018). Similarly, video games or computer tasks were used to train participants to approach healthy food and avoid unhealthy food (Bleckert et al., 2015; Kakoschke et al., 2017; Schakel et al., 2018; Van Dessel et al., 2018). Most of studies assessing implicit attitudes following experimental trainings or EC reported healthier food preference in experimental groups, who received manipulations aimed at increasing positive association to healthy food (Lebens et al., 2011; Hensels and Baines, 2016; Kakoschke et al., 2017; Schakel et al., 2018; Van Dessel et al., 2018). Differently, a study investigating the effect of a response inhibition training on implicit food attitudes reported the absence of manipulation-related modulations (Adams et al., 2017), and one study reported that approach-avoidance training did not affect implicit preferences (Bleckert et al., 2015). Other studies administered the IAT in pre- and post-assessment: Hollands et al. (2011) reported significant interaction between baseline IAT and intervention, with EC reducing preference for snacks vs fruits in individuals with stronger preference at baseline; Alblas et al. (2018) showed that preference for fruit vs chocolate decreased in the control group, but not in the EC group. Disgusting odours spread in the room while participants performed IAT with high- and low-calorie foods did not affect IAT score (Mayer et al., 2008). Finally, Storr and Sparks (2016) reported stronger positive associations with high-calorie foods and negative associations with low-calorie foods in unrestrained compared to restrained eaters, but ego-depletion and self-affirmation procedures did not differently affect restrained and unrestrained eaters in implicit food attitudes.
Group differences in obesity and overweight.
Different studies compared healthy-weight participants and participants with obesity for their implicit attitudes towards unhealthy-fat vs healthy-low-fat foods with different types of tasks (see Table 3). Seven of these papers consistently reported more positive attitudes towards healthy food both in healthy-weight adult participants and adult participants with obesity (Roefs and Jansen, 2002; Roefs et al., 2005b; Alabduljader et al., 2018) as well as in children with obesity and healthy-weight (Craeynest et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008b), with no differences between groups in implicit measures. The absence of group differences was also reported in a study showing that both women with obesity and healthy-weight associated high-fat food to restraint concepts (Werrij et al., 2009). On the other hand, participants with obesity responded slower to the high-fat/positive combination than controls in an IAT (Roefs and Jansen, 2002) and showed more positive implicit attitude towards both healthy and unhealthy food in an EAST paradigm (Craeynest et al., 2005). Five other studies reported group differences, with participants with obesity showing greater approach bias to food in a IAT with food and non-food categories and approach vs avoidance attributes (Kemps and Tiggemann, 2015), higher implicit preference for large fast food portion in an AP paradigm (Cserjesi et al., 2016) and higher preference for sweet than non-sweet food in an IAT (Sartor et al., 2011). Moreover, women with obesity showed significantly more negative attitudes to high-calorie sweet foods and positive attitudes to high-calorie savoury foods compared to healthy- and over-weight participants, with inconsistent results from explicit preference (Czyzewska and Graham, 2008). In line with this result, discrepancy between IAT score and explicit attitudes for chocolate predicted disinhibited eating in healthy- and over-weight participants in a following study (Goldstein et al., 2014).
Effects of manipulations and training on implicit attitudes in obesity.
One study reported that manipulation of focus of attention or craving induction affected AP of participants with obesity or lean participants in the same direction with no significant differences between groups (Roefs et al., 2006). Differently, McKenna et al. (2016) reported that participants with obesity and normal-weight participants differed in the automatic responses to healthy and unhealthy food in an IRAP on hunger state and that the groups were differently affected by manipulation of food restriction prior to the assessment. The same paper reported the absence of group differences on IRAP measuring automatic food wanting (McKenna et al., 2016). Five studies used implicit measures as outcome measures following treatments for individuals with obesity (Craeynest et al., 2008a; Sartor et al., 2011; Ferentzi et al., 2018; Verbeken et al., 2018; Warschburger et al., 2018). Craeynest et al. (2008a) reported that children with obesity reduced positive attitude towards healthy and unhealthy food in post-treatment EAST. The other studies showed that IAT score was not affected by intervention of soft drink supplementation (Sartor et al., 2011) and approach avoidant training in adults and children with obesity (Ferentzi et al., 2018; Warschburger et al., 2018; Verbeken et al., 2018).
Studies on body image
Our search retrieved 27 studies about implicit attitudes toward body image (Table 4). Studies using body images to measure weight bias or to investigate neural correlates involved in body representation are reported in the weight bias and neurobiological section, respectively. Eighteen studies involved healthy normal-weight participants, whereas nine studies included samples with EDs. Considering the first groups, most of the studies recruited participants from general population (N = 14), whereas the other four studies involved special categories: parents of children with obesity (Lydecker et al., 2006), nurses (Robstad et al., 2018), medical students (Matharu et al., 2014) and clinicians (Sabin et al., 2015). Nine out of 18 studies measured implicit attitudes using the two-categories IAT, whereas in one study participants underwent a multiple-categories IAT (Marini, et al., 2017). Two studies (Lydecker et al., 2006; Robstad et al., 2018) combined two different IATs to test both implicit valence attitudes (by using valence related words, i.e. good/bad) and stereotypes (by using judgement-related words, i.e. stupid/smart or lazy/motivated). Four studies used the IRAP (Exposito et al., 2015; Juarascio et al., 2011; Heider et al., 2015; Ritzert et al., 2016), two the Relational Responding Task (Glashouwer et al., 2018; Heider et al., 2018), one the AMP (Moussally et al., 2015) and one the AP (Watts et al., 2008). Concerning the type of stimuli, 10 out of 18 studies compared thin and fat body images (N = 7) or related words (N = 3). Two studies used underweight and normal-weight silhouettes (Ahern et al., 2008; Martijn et al., 2013), one study tested implicit attitudes toward underweight, normal-weight and overweight/obese categories (Marini et al., 2017), whereas four studies used sentences to elicit implicit attitudes toward actual and ideal body images (Heider et al., 2015 2018; Glashouwer et al., 2018) or attractiveness vs disgust/fear toward being thin or fat (Ritzert et al., 2016). One study considered images of body parts and of body shape (Watts et al., 2008). The impact of experimental manipulations on implicit attitudes toward body images was explored in two studies (Matharu et al., 2014; Martijn et al., 2013).
Looking at results, 7 out of 18 papers reported negative implicit attitude towards overweight/obese body images (Ahern et al., 2006; Lydecker et al., 2006; Robstad et al., 2018; Elran-Barak and Bar-Anan, 2018; Sabin et al., 2015; Moussally et al., 2015; Watts et al., 2008), when the stimuli were presented both for a short and a long period of time, likely reflecting more automatic or more controlled responses, respectively (Watts et al., 2008). In one study, the self-thin attractive bias was stronger than the self-fat attractive bias (Ritzert et al., 2016). Two studies showed stronger implicit preferences for normal-weight than underweight or overweight/obese body images (Ahern et al., 2008; Marini, 2017), whereas one study did not report a defined bias towards body image as participants showed similar pro-slim and pro-fat implicit attitudes (Exposito et al., 2015). The experimental manipulations were demonstrated to effectively decrease the positive implicit attitude towards underweight people (Martijn et al., 2013) or the negative implicit attitude towards fat people (Matharu et al., 2014). Four studies showed that body implicit attitudes could predict disordered eating, body image dissatisfaction and changes in weight (Juarascio et al., 2011; Heider et al., 2015 2018; Glashouwer et al., 2018). In particular, a study reported that individuals faster in categorizing own actual, but not ideal, body image as fat showed higher body dissatisfaction compared to individuals who represented their actual body image as slim (Glashouwer et al., 2018). One study showed that eating-relevant implicit associations were valid variables to test the negative effects of stereotype (e.g. weight-related peer teasing; Benas and Gibb, 2011). Most of the studies investigating body attitudes in healthy normal-weight participants reported no correlation between implicit and explicit measures. In contrast, four studies showed pro-slim/anti-fat attitudes at both implicit and explicit levels (Robstad et al., 2018; Elran-Barak and Bar-Anan, 2018; Sabin et al., 2015; Benas and Gibb, 2011), and such correlation was confirmed by Matharu et al. (2014) at the baseline measure, but not after experimental manipulation. Three studies did not investigate the correspondence between implicit and explicit attitudes (Watts et al., 2008; Martijn et al., 2013; Moussally et al., 2015). Among the two out of 18 studies testing the predictive value of implicit and explicit attitudes on the behavioural measures, one study reported a negative correlation between implicit and explicit anti-fat bias and the possibility of helping patients with obesity (Robstad et al., 2018). The other study showed that explicit, but not implicit, actual and ideal body image predicted food selection, caloric intake and restraint eating (Glashouwer et al., 2018).
Considering the studies that tested implicit attitudes toward body image in samples of individuals with EDs, most of the studies (N = 7) evaluated body implicit attitudes in patients with AN and BN vs healthy controls. One study involved participants with obesity (Anselmi et al., 2011), while another study considered patients with AN, BN and BED (Keng and Ang, 2019). Four studies measured implicit attitudes using the two-categories IAT (Izquierdo et al., 2019; Anselmi, 2011; Keng and Ang, 2019; Khan and Petróczi, 2015). Among these, Izquierdo et al. (2019) added a questionnaire-based IAT using items from the Eating Disorders Inventory (Garner, 2004), whereas Khan and Petróczi (2015) tested both the ideal body image and the personal internalized body image. Two studies used the AP (Cserjési et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2014), other two the AMP (Spring and Bulik, 2014; Smith et al., 2018) and one study applied the Relational Responding Task (Parling et al., 2012). Regarding the type of stimuli, three studies measured implicit attitudes towards underweight or normal-weight silhouettes (Khan and Petróczi, 2015; Izquierdo et al., 2019) or towards thin- or fat-related images (Spring and Bulik, 2014); one study compared thin and fat face images (Anselmi et al., 2011). Parling et al. (2012) used words to elicit body shape and weight concerns, while Keng and Ang (2019) used body-related and -unrelated words. The other three studies used ultra-thin, average-size, overweight body images (Cserjési et al., 2010), ED-symptom and body images (Smith et al., 2018) and beauty-related words in a lexical decision task (Smith et al., 2014). Records testing whether an experimental manipulation affected body implicit attitudes showed that exposure to primes of emaciated bodies increased pro-thin bias in women with AN (Smith et al., 2014), while exposure to mindfulness exercises did not alter implicit body dissatisfaction of patients with ED compared to resting condition (Keng and Ang, 2019).
Three studies reported that patients with AN and BN showed pro-thin implicit bias compared to healthy subjects (Izquierdo et al., 2019; Smith et al., 2018; Khan and Petróczi, 2015); similar results were shown by Anselmi et al. (2011) in a group of patients with obesity, compared to healthy controls. Conversely, Spring and Bulik (2014) reported that patients with AN, compared to recovered AN individuals and healthy controls, showed negative implicit affect towards overweight stimuli, but not an automatic attraction to thinness. Both pro-thin and anti-fat bias in patients with AN were reported by Parling et al. (2012), whereas only anti-fat bias emerged in Cserjési et al. (2010), and healthy samples of both studies were characterized by pro-thin preference, but not by anti-fat preference.
Concerning the correlation between implicit and explicit body attitudes, four studies out of nine reported mixed results (Parling et al., 2012; Khan and Petróczi, 2015; Cserjési et al., 2010; Spring and Bulik, 2014). While one study showed consistent null effect of mindfulness on both implicit and explicit body dissatisfaction (Keng and Ang, 2019), the other four records did not investigate possible correlation between implicit and explicit measures. Among the records on ED samples, only one study reported the predictive value of the implicit attitudes on the ED behaviours (Smith et al., 2018).
Studies on weight bias
Weight bias towards overweight and individuals with obesity has been investigated in 78 studies, using both explicit and implicit measures of preference for ‘fat’ or ‘thin’ attributes (Table 5). In order to measure weight stigma, the majority of these studies (N = 66) used the two-categories IAT. A minority of studies used either the SC-IAT (Lynagh et al., 2015; Aweidah et al., 2016), the AMP (Pryor et al., 2013; Skinner et al., 2017; Karsay and Schmuck, 2017) or the IRAP (Baker et al., 2017), whereas two studies (Roddy et al., 2010 2011) paired the two-categories IAT with the IRAP and four papers employed the AP (Degner and Wentura, 2009; Brochu et al., 2011; Glock et al., 2016; Rudolph and Hilbert, 2017). Concerning the type of stimuli employed in the implicit tasks, 27 out of 78 studies used images of thin and fat people, two studies (Anselmi et al., 2013; Penney et al., 2013) used morphed faces of normal-weight and overweight individuals and four studies employed pictures of average-weight and overweight children. Forty-five studies employed words representing different types of target categories whereas one study (Teachman et al., 2003) used both images and words as target categories in different IATs. Regarding the samples, five studies (Carels et al., 2009a, 2009b, 2010, 2011, 2014) recruited adults with overweight or obesity engaged in a behavioural weight-loss treatment. Five studies (Solbes and Enesco, 2010; Bissell and Hays, 2010; Hutchison et al., 2018; Skinner et al., 2017; Thomas et al., 2007) involved healthy children, whereas 13 studies involved adult professionals treating obesity. Twenty-six studies analysed the weight bias in healthy university students, whereas two studies (Agerström and Rooth, 2011; Flint et al., 2016) focused on samples of managers and employees. Eight studies included normal-weight, under-weight, over-weight participants or individuals with obesity (Wang et al., 2004; Schwartz et al., 2006; Brochu and Morrison, 2007; Degner and Wentura, 2009; Anselmi et al., 2013; Brauhardt et al., 2014; Brewis et al., 2016; Jiang et al., 2017). One study (Grover et al., 2003) focused on gender differences in weight bias, recruiting normal-weight and overweight males and females. One study (Brewis and Wutich, 2012) involved a sample of healthy women from Paraguay, whereas Hart et al. (2016) recruited two samples of healthy African American and non-Hispanic white women. Marini et al. (2013) recruited volunteers from 71 different nations. Fifteen papers investigated the effect of different manipulations on weight stigma in healthy students and volunteers.
Table 5.
Details of studies on weight bias
Studies | Sample (N) | Mean age (SD) | Implicit measure | Target categories | Manipulation |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Agerström et al., 2007 | Study 2A: Students (88, f = 58) Study 2B: Employers (166, f = 82) |
22.25 (5.73) 43.19 (10.45) |
IAT | Obese/normal weight | - |
Agerström and Rooth, 2011 | Managers (153, f = 77) | 40.0 (10.41) | IAT | Obese/normal-weight | Manipulation of weight status of job candidates |
Anselmi et al., 2013 | UW (53) HW (331) OW (83) OB (43) f = 327 of the entire sample |
27.06 (8.99) | IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Aweidah et al., 2016 | Diagnostic radiography clinical educators (37, f = 23) | n.r. | SC-IAT | Fat | - |
Baker et al., 2017 | Five classes of medical students | No demographic data were collected | IRAP | Overweight/obese/slim/thin | - |
Bissell and Hays, 2011 | H (601, f = 331) | n.r. | IAT | Overweight/thin children | Exposure to images of overweight/thin children |
Brauhardt et al., 2014 | BED (26, f = 21) OB (26, f = 21) HW (26, f = 21) |
34.77 (10.29) 35.19 (11.08) 34.65 (10.70) |
Weight Bias-IAT; Self-Esteem-IAT |
Thin/fat Self/other |
- |
Brewis and Wutich, 2012 | H from Paraguay (200, f = 200) H from the USA (66, f = 48) |
38.9 (13.4) 23.68 (11.2) |
IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Brewis et al., 2016 | Study 1: HW (116) OW (61) OB (27) f = 103 of the entire sample |
23 (n.r.) | IAT | Slim/Fat | - |
Brochu and Morrison, 2007 | UW (2) AW (75) OW and OB (17) f = 61 of the entire sample |
20.11 (4.0) | IAT | Average-weight/overweight people | - |
Brochu et al., 2011 | University students (80, f = 53) | 19.74 (5.05) | AP | Over-weight/normal-weight | - |
Carels et al., 2009a | OW and OB (58, f = 52) | 47.6 (10.3) | IAT | Fat/thin people | Weight loss intervention |
Carels et al., 2009b | OW and OB (58, f = 52) | n.r. | IAT | Fat/thin people | Weight loss intervention |
Carels et al., 2010 | OW and OB (54, f = 44) | 48.7 (11.7) | IAT | Fat/thin people | Weight loss intervention |
Carels et al., 2011 | OW and OB (53, f = 41) | 47.15 (14.1) | Weight Bias-IAT; Self-Esteem-IAT |
Fat/thin people Self/other |
Weight loss intervention |
Carels et al., 2013 | OW/OB (42, f = 30) following a weight loss intervention OW/OB (47, f = 38) before a weight loss intervention |
46.9 (13.0) 53.7 (13.2) |
Stereotype congruent IAT; Stereotype incongruent IAT |
Fat/thin people | - |
Carels et al., 2014 | OW/OB (44, f = 37) | 53.2 (13.6) | IAT | Obese/thin people | Weight loss intervention |
Cazzato and Makris, 2019 | HW (18, f = 11) OW (18, f = 12) |
25.28 (0.99) 24 (0.89) |
IAT | Fat/slim | - |
Chambliss et al., 2004 | Undergraduate H (136, f = 57) Graduate in exercise science H (110, f = 53) |
Men = 22.7 (2.9) Women = 21.4 (2.9) Men = 25.79 (4.8) Women = 23.5 (2.4) |
Weight-attitude IAT; Weight- stereotype IAT | Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
- |
Degner and Wentura, 2009 | Study 1 (47, f = 24) UW (8) NW (31) OW (8) Study 2 (50, f = 28) UW (5) NW (36) OW (9) Study 3 (65, f = 41) UW (7) NW (39) OW (10) Study 4 (55, f = 55) UW (4) NW (50) OW (1) |
25 (n.r.) 21 (n.r.) 23 (n.r.) 47 (n.r.) |
AP | Normal-weight/overweight individuals | - |
Dimmock et al., 2009 | H fitness centre employees (70, f = 40) | 27 (9.53) | IAT | Overweight/thin people; Overweight/thin exerciser |
- |
Domoff et al., 2012 | Psychology students (64, f = 42) | 20.0 (2.9) | IAT | Fat/thin people | Exposition to a 40-min weight-loss reality shows |
Flint et al., 2013 | H (28, f = 11) | 22.43 (3.59) | IAT | Fat/thin | Counter-conditioning using positive images of obese members of the general public and images of obese celebrities |
Flint et al., 2015 | H (2380, f = 1767) | 27.71 (1.03) | IAT | Fat/thin | - |
Flint et al., 2016 | Employees (181, f = 74) | 38.25 (8.99) | IAT | Fat/thin | Manipulation of weight status of job candidates |
Fontana et al., 2013 | Physical education teachers (47, f = 28) | 37.07 (13.22) | IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Fontana et al., 2017 | Professors in physical education departments (94, f = 47) | 47.83 (12.18) | IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Gapinski et al., 2006 | Undergraduate students (108, f = 108) | 19.06 (1.01) | IAT | Fat/thin people | Exposition to a media-based videos intervention |
Geier et al., 2003 | H (59, f = 59) | 19.1 (0.23) | Four IATs | Fat/thin | Exposition to a ‘before and after’ diet advertisement |
Geller and Watkins, 2018 | First year medical students H cohort | No demographic data were collected | Weight-attitude IAT; Weight- stereotype IAT | Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
Ethics group session |
Glock et al., 2016 | Teachers (51, f = 48) | 21.12 (6.15) | AP | Words reflecting obesity/thinness | - |
Grover et al., 2002 | HW (42, f = 22) OW (41, f = 20) |
HW men 36.3 (12.7) OW men 32.2 (11.4) HW women 27.8 (11.2) OW women 35.1 (13.8) |
Weight-attitude IAT; Weight-identity IAT; gender attitude IAT; gender identity IAT; self-attitude IAT |
Light/heavy Self/other Female/male Self/other Self/other |
- |
Gumble and Carels, 2012 | Psychology students (85, f = 47) | 19.9 (3.7) | Weight-bias IAT; Weight-identity IAT; Self-esteem IAT; Body image IAT |
Fat/thin Self/Other Self/Other Self/Other |
- |
Halvorson et al., 2019 | Hospital attendants (10, f = 7) Pediatric residents (10, f = 7) Pediatric nurses (8, f = 8) Patients (12, f = 7) Parents/caregivers (12, f = 10) |
34 (n.r.) 29.0 (n.r.) 31.5 (n.r.) 15 (n.r.) n.r. |
IAT | Thin/obese people | - |
Hand et al., 2017 | Students (302, f = 218) | 16 (n.r.) | IAT | n.r. | - |
Hart et al., 2016 | H African American (207, f = 207) H Non-Hispanic Whites (310, f = 310) |
34.45 (10.83) 32.42 (11.17) |
Revised-IAT | Overweight/underweight, obese/underweight, overweight/obese | - |
Hilbert and Meyre, 2016 | Study 1: University students (128; f = 79) Study 2: Volunteers (128, f = 79) |
23.01 (3.24) 35.31 (12.54) |
IAT IAT |
Thin/fat Thin/fat |
Educational intervention about the multifactorial aetiology of obesity |
Hinman et al., 2015 | Students (117, f = 92) | 19.3 (1.5) | Stereotype congruent IAT; Stereotype incongruent IAT |
Fat/thin Fat/thin |
- |
Hutchison and Müller, 2018 | Children (84, f = 43) | 5.89 (1.11) | IAT | Fat/thin children | - |
Jiang et al., 2017 | 104 Asian students UW (23, f = 23) HW (67, f = 67) OW (9, f = 9) OB (5, f = 5) |
21.6 (3.3) | IAT | Fat/thin | - |
Karsay and Schmuck, 2017 | Adolescent (353, f = 176) | 17.34 (1.09) | AMP | Obese/non-obese individuals | Exposition to a reality TV show ‘The Biggest Loser Teens’ |
Lund et al., 2018 | General practitioners (240, f = 86) | 18.5 (10.2) | IAT | Fat/normal weight | - |
Lynagh et al., 2015 | H enrolled in the health and physical educational curricula for trainee teachers (62, f = 30) H nonspecialist trainee teachers (177, f = 104) |
Three age groups: 18–20 years (Specialist = 38, nonspecialist = 106) 21–23 years (Specialist = 14, nonspecialist = 24) >24 years (Specialist = 7, nonspecialist = 16) |
SC-IAT | Fat | - |
Marini et al., 2013 | Volunteers from 71 nations (338.121, f = 238.357) | 27.8 (10.64) | IAT | Thin/overweight people | - |
Miller et al., 2013 | Medical students (310, f = 132) |
<25 years = 131 (n.r.) 25–28 years = 145 (n.r.) >28 years = 29 (n.r.) |
IAT | Fat/thin | - |
O’Brien et al., 2007a | Study1 H (227, f = 140) Study2 H (134, f = 99) |
19.98 (2.91) 20.09 (4.199 |
IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
O’Brien et al., 2007b | H (344, f = 230) First (122) and third (58) year of physical education degree programme; First (95) and third (69) year of psychology programme |
First year psychology 18.49 (0.75); First year physical education 18.68 (1.3); Third year psychology 21.8 (4.1); Third year physical education 21.6 (1.9) |
Weight-attitude IAT; Weight- stereotype IAT | Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
- |
O’Brien et al., 2008 | H (104, f = 82) | 20.35 (5.04) | Weight-attitude IAT; Weight- stereotype IAT | Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
Manipulation of weight status of job candidates |
O’Brien et al., 2010 | H (159, f = 135) | 20.29 (2.32) | Weight-attitude IAT; Weight-stereotype IAT |
Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
Tutorial classes |
Penney and Lawsin, 2013 | Students (186, f = 121) | 19.7 (3.9) | IAT | Thin/obese faces | - |
Phelan et al., 2014 | Medical students (4.732, f = 2363) | 23.9 (n.r.) | IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Phelan et al., 2015a | Medical students (4687, f = 2.344) HW (3378) UW (163) OW (922) OB (224) |
23.9 (2.6) | IAT; | Fat/thin people | - |
Phelan et al., 2015b | Medical students (1795, f = 917) |
N = 575 19–22 years N = 456 23 years N = 473 24–25 years N = 281 >26 years |
IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Pryor et al., 2013 | H (100, f = 60) | 19.62 (1.42) | AMP | - | Cyberball game |
Robertson and Vohora, 2008 | Fitness professionals (57, f = 25) | 29–30 (10.239) | IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Robinson et al., 2014 | Health and non-health students (479) | 26.2 (7.6) | IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Roddy et al., 2010 | Psychology students (80, f = 58) | 21.1 (3.4) | IAT IRAP |
Overweight/average-weight people Overweight/average-weight people |
- |
Roddy et al., 2011 | Students (78, f = 5) | 20.25 (3.67) | IAT IRAP |
Overweight/average-weight people Overweight/average-weight people |
- |
Rudolph and Hilbert, 2017 | Individuals from the community (144) | n.r. | AP | Normal-weight/obese full body pictures | Exposure to health messages |
Rukavina et al., 2010 | Kinesiology pre-professionals (78, f = 26) | 21.63 (1.49) | Weight-stereotype IAT | Fat/thin | Classroom and service learning components |
Russell-Mayhew et al., 2015 | Preservice teachers (30, f = 25) | 32 | IAT | n.r. | Exposure to a professional workshop |
Sabin et al., 2012 | All test takers (3 59 261, f = 220) Medical doctors (2284, f = 1285) |
26 (10.7) 33 (12.5) |
IAT | Overweight/thin people | - |
Schwartz et al., 2003 | Professionals engaged in research and/or clinical management of obesity (389, f = 198) | n.r. | Weight-attitude IAT; Weight-stereotype IAT |
Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
- |
Schwartz et al., 2006 | UW (128) HW (1756) OW (899) OB (899) EOB (600) f = ∼3.555 of the entire sample (approximately 95% of respondents completed demographic information) |
34.6 (n.r.) | Obesity attitude IAT; Obesity stereotype IAT |
Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
- |
Scrivano et al., 2017 | Students (166) | 20.48 (2.31) | IAT | Obese/thin individuals | Exposure to an education about the uncontrollable causes of obesity |
Skinner et al., 2017 | Children (114, f = 56) | 10 | AMP | Average-weight/overweight children | - |
Solbes and Enesco, 2010 | H (120, f = 60) | 40 H 6.9 (n.r.) 40 H 8.9 (n.r.) 40 H 10.8 (n.r.) |
IAT (child-oriented version) | Fat/thin children | - |
Swift et al., 2013 | H: CG (21, f = 18) IG (22, f = 18) |
21.2 (0.8) 24.6 (7.2) |
Weight-attitude IAT; Weight-stereotype IAT |
Fat/thin Fat/thin |
Anti-stigma films |
Teachman and brownell, 2001 | Health care specialist (84, f = 24) | 48 (9.81) | Weight-attitude IAT; Weight- stereotype IAT |
Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
- |
Teachman et al., 2003 | Study 1: H (144, f = 78) Study 2A: H (90, f = 90) Study 2B: H (63, f = 32) |
35 (13.99) 21 (3.87) 42 (16.51) |
Weight-attitude IAT; Weight-stereotype IAT Weight-attitude IAT; Weight-stereotype IAT Weight-stereotype IATs |
Fat/thin people Fat/thin people Fat/thin people Overweight/underweight people Fat/thin people |
Providing information about genetic vs behavioural causes of obesity Written stories of weight discrimination Story of a severe weight discrimination |
Thomas et al., 2007 | Study 2: Children (94, f = 49) |
5 | IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Tomiyama et al., 2015 | Obesity specialists (232, f = 134) | 42.52 (12.82) | Weight-attitude IAT; Weight-stereotype IAT |
Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
- |
Vallis et al., 2007 | Healthcare providers (78, f = 69) | 37.39 (10.72) | IAT | Fat/thin people | - |
Venturini et al., 2006 | Students (45, f = 31) | n.r. | IAT | Normal-weight/fat woman | - |
Waller et al., 2012 | Nursing H (45, f = 39) Psychology H (45, f = 35) |
24.7 (n.r.) 25.60 (n.r.) |
IAT | Normal-weight/overweight man and women; normal-weight/overweight man and women in a medical setting | - |
Wang et al., 2004 | Study 1: HW (64, f = 60) Study 2: OW (48, f = 33) |
43.1 (9.4) 48.9 (9.2) |
IAT IAT |
Fat/thin people Fat/thin people |
- |
Weinsten et al., 2008 | Students (50, f = 35) | n.r. | IAT | Obese/thin | - |
Wijayatunga et al., 2019 | H (67, f = 64) CG (34, f = 22) IG (33, f = 21) |
21.76 (1.43) 21.5 (1.11) 22.03 (1.67) |
IAT | Obese/thin people | Learning about uncontrollable cause of obesity and about weight bias |
AMP = Affective Misattribution Procedure; AP = affective priming; AW = average weight; BED = binge eating disorder; CG = control group; EOB = extremely obese; f = female; H = healthy participants; HW = healthy weight; IAT = Implicit Association Test; IG = intervention group; IRAP = Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure; n.r. = not reported; OB = obese; OW = overweight; SC-IAT = Single category IAT; UW = underweight.
Looking at results, some studies proved evidence for weight stigma at both explicit and implicit level in medical students (Miller et al., 2013; Phelan et al., 2014 2015b; Baker et al., 2017). Weight bias was also found in samples of university students from different fields including psychology, nursing, business and physical education (Agerström et al., 2007; Chambliss et al., 2004; Gumble et al., 2012; Lynagh et al., 2015; Roddy et al., 2010; 2011; Waller et al., 2012). A study showed that weight bias could be transferred to arbitrary stimuli following a training, which asked to associate weight-related and neutral stimuli (Weinstein et al., 2008). The comparison between weight bias held by psychology and physical education students resulted in a stronger implicit anti-fat bias for the latter group (O’Brien et al., 2007b), whereas Robinson et al. (2014) found similar levels of implicit and explicit weight bias among samples of health and non-health discipline students. O’Brien et al. (2007a) found a correlation between the propensity of making physical-appearance-related comparisons with both explicit and implicit anti-fat attitudes. In a different sample of normal-weight and overweight students involved in a weight discrimination task, Cazzato and Makris (2019) showed a better ability to discriminate actions performed by normal-weight as compared to overweight actors. Another group of students was tested in order to explore the relationship between weight prejudice and expressed and actual behaviour. The data collected showed that prejudicial attitudes toward fatness did not necessarily predict discriminatory behaviours (Penney et al., 2013). However, the expression of weight bias was found to be influenced by the motivation to appear non-prejudiced to others and the perception of weight discrimination (Brochu et al., 2011).
Studies examining weight stigma held by youngsters found evidence for weight prejudice among adolescent (Hand et al., 2017) and children (Solbes and Enesco, 2010; Thomas et al., 2007; Hutchison et al., 2018; Skinner et al., 2017), corroborating the hypothesis that the ‘thin ideal’ occurs in an early stage of life. A study examined media’s influence on weight prejudice in young scholars, finding that more time spent watching television was associated with lower levels of anti-fat bias (Bissel and Hays, 2011).
Other studies investigated the weight bias of healthcare professionals. Some researches (Halvorson et al., 2019; Teachman and Brownell, 2001; Sabin et al., 2012; Schwartz et al., 2003; Tomiyama et al., 2015; Lund et al., 2018; Robertson and Vohora, 2008) showed preferences for thinness compared to fatness at both implicit and explicit levels in individuals working in clinical management of overweight individuals, whereas three studies (Vallis et al., 2007; Dimmock et al., 2009; Aweidah et al., 2016) found evidence for weight stigma only at the implicit level. Furthermore, a strong implicit weight bias was found in a group of physical education teachers (Fontana et al., 2013) and in a sample of professors teaching pre-service physical education (Fontana et al., 2017). However, a different research found neutral implicit attitudes toward obesity in sample of pre-service teachers (Glock et al., 2016). An implicit preference to thinness relative to fatness was found in individuals with different BMI (Anselmi et al., 2013) and a stigmatization of obesity has been found in samples of individuals with overweight or obesity and BED (Wang et al., 2004; Brauhardt et al., 2014), with an inverse relation between participants’ BMI and the extent of the bias (Schwartz et al., 2006; Brewis et al., 2016). One study found strong evidence for the influence of weight status on the automatic weight evaluation, with overweight participants showing an implicit preference for overweight over normal-weight stimuli (Degner and Wentura, 2009).
Three papers (Agerstrom et al., 2011; O’Brien et al., 2008; Flint et al., 2016) investigated discrimination of people with obesity at workplace, showing implicit and explicit stigmatization towards candidates with obesity in hiring personnel. Moreover, compared to normal-weight people, overweight individuals seem to be associated with positions characterized by restricted interactions with the public (Venturini et al., 2006). Concerning gender differences in weight bias, one study suggested that males hold more negative attitudes toward obesity (Brochu and Morrison, 2007), whereas Grover et al. (2003) showed that anti-fat bias was ubiquitously held by men and women. A different line of research (Carels et al., 2009a, 2009b, 2010, 2011) found evidence for implicit and explicit anti-fat attitudes in adults with overweight/obesity engaged in a behavioural weight-loss treatments, with higher weight stigma correlating with worse treatment outcomes (Carels et al., 2009b).
Since the prevalence of obese and overweight population is growing worldwide, some studies were interested in understanding how weight-related attitudes vary across different cultural settings. Two studies found both implicit and explicit anti-fat attitudes in a sample of UK residents (Flint et al., 2015) and in a group of individuals from 71 nations (Marini et al., 2013). Implicit anti-fat bias was detected in two samples of Asian (Jiang et al., 2017) and African American (Hart et al., 2016) females, whereas explicit anti-fat stigma has been found in a group of Paraguayan women (Brewis and Wutich, 2012). Moreover, two studies (Carels et al., 2013; Hinman et al., 2015) showed that implicit weight bias was significantly greater when obese and thin people were pictured engaging in stereotype congruent than incongruent activities. However, a behavioural weight loss programme resulted in a reduction of the stereotype consistent bias in a sample of individuals with overweight or obesity (Careles et al., 2014). Studies evaluating the impact of different manipulations on weight bias reported heterogeneous results. An exacerbation of weight stigma has been observed after the exposition to a weight-loss reality show (Domoff et al., 2012; Karsay and Schmuck, 2017) and following the administration of a ‘before and after’ advertisement, typically picturing an overweight person on the left and the new slim version of the same person on the right (Geier et al., 2003). Differently, a reduction of explicit weight bias has been found after informing participants about the uncontrollable causes of obesity (Wijayatunga et al., 2019), after educating about the multifactorial aetiology of obesity (Hilbert and Meyre, 2016), after a multi-component intervention aimed at reducing weight bias (Rukavina et al., 2010) and after showing anti-stigma films (Swift et al., 2013). Teachman et al. (2003) showed the possibility to reduce implicit weight bias by evoking empathy with stories of discrimination against individuals with obesity, depending on the weight status of participants. A different intervention based on health messages aimed at enhancing physical activity and healthy habits showed a small reduction of implicit anti-fat stigmatization (Rudolph and Hilbert, 2017). Differently, a counter-conditioning intervention (e.g. presentation of positive images of general public or celebrities with obesity) did not result in more positive perception of fatness, at both implicit and explicit levels (Flint et al., 2013). Similarly, weight bias persisted after a media-based conditioning intervention, which relied upon the presentation of videos portraying obese people struggling with their weight status and discrimination (Gapinski et al., 2006). An ethics educational training partially improved attitudes towards obesity (Geller and Watkins, 2018), whereas Russell-Mayhew et al. (2015) showed a reduction of both implicit and explicit weight bias after performing an interactive professional workshop and O’Brien et al. (2010) demonstrated that it was possible to reduce or exacerbate both anti-fat explicit and implicit attitudes, depending on the information provided about causes of obesity. In contrast, Scrivano et al. (2017) found that informing about the causes of obesity had an impact only on the explicit beliefs about the controllability of obesity. In a research that involved an interactive computer game, the Cyberball game, Pryor et al. (2013) focused on the social influence on behavioural expression of weight bias, proving that both explicit and implicit anti-fat attitudes influenced interactions with an overweight player, but only when other players ostracized the overweight subject.
Analysing the relationship between implicit and explicit measures of weight bias, studies reported heterogeneous results. High levels of implicit bias were coupled with low or completely absent explicit pro-slim/anti-fat preference in some studies (Roddy et al., 2010; Lynagh et al., 2015; Aweidah et al., 2016; Dimmock et al., 2009; Halvorson et al., 2019; Teachman and brownell, 2001; Carels et al., 2009a; Jiang et al., 2017). Conversely, other studies found similar levels of implicit and explicit weight stigma (Schwartz et al., 2003; Sabin et al., 2012; Marini et al., 2013; Phelan et al., 2014; Tomiyama et al., 2015; Flint et al., 2015 2016). A minority of data suggested high levels of explicit preference for thinness, coupled with very low levels of implicit weight bias (Brewis and Wutich, 2012).
Discussion
We reviewed studies on implicit attitudes towards food and body in healthy population and in samples of patients with ED. One hundred and eighty three papers were evaluated for bias risks and synthesized. Some main findings emerge from this review. A first evidence is that very few studies explored the neurobiological correlates of implicit attitudes related to eating behaviour. Neuroscientific investigation of eating behaviour has increased in the past decades supporting a brain-based approach to eating disorders and outlining different neurobiological models, which represent the rationale for combining psychotherapy and biological treatments (Val-Laillet et al., 2015; Frank, 2019). Neuroimaging studies reported the involvement of emotional and reward neuronal circuits in monitoring eating behaviour, and neuromodulation treatments have been tested targeting these circuits (Val-Laillet et al., 2015; Hall et al., 2018b). Therefore, understanding the relationship between neural circuits underpinning eating behaviour and brain mechanisms related to implicit attitudes, which are measures of automatic affective evaluation of food or body (Greenwald and Farnham, 2003; De Houwer et al., 2009), is of great interest. The low number of studies prevents to delineate clear conclusions on this aspect, but it is worth noting that one paper assessing reward sensitivity for food in patients with PD in dopamine replacement treatment reported discrepancies between implicit and explicit food attitudes in patients with PD and binge eating, in line with findings with other samples of disordered eating (Papies et al., 2009; Terenzi et al., 2018). One ERP study showed task-related differences in the N400 component only in patients with BN, supporting the hypothesis that implicit attitudes in EDs could be associated to anomalous brain response at an early stage (Blechert et al., 2011). Neurostimulation studies are consistent in highlighting the crucial impact of individual features in modulating implicit attitudes. Indeed, one study showed that TMS on mPFC affected food attitudes only in a subgroup of participants with low preference for tasty food (Mattavelli et al., 2015), whereas two studies with tDCS on EBA found an effect only in male participants with anti-fat bias, but not in females, and in patients with EDs, but not in control females (Cazzato et al., 2017; Mattavelli et al., 2019). Although using different IATs, these latter studies are consistent in showing the lack of modulatory effect on body images IAT in female participants applying anodal stimulation on EBA. Interestingly, one study stimulating the left dlPFC with cTBS did not find significant effect of neuromodulation on IAT, but reported different correlational patterns between IAT and food consumption in active or sham cTBS condition, supporting a role for left dlPFC in control hunger disinhibition (Hall et al., 2018).
For what concerns food attitudes many different paradigms have been used, with preferences pointing to different directions depending on the type of food categories and evaluative attributes used as stimuli. A general conclusion that can be drawn concerns the fact that most of the studies support the predictive validity of implicit measures on the actual food behaviour. Crucially, the relationship between implicit and explicit measures and behaviour seems to be mediated by individual differences in food habits and ED symptoms (Ellis et al., 2014). In particular, discrepancies between implicit and explicit preferences were more evident in restrained eaters and predicted disinhibited eating with larger validity in individuals with higher level of impulsivity (Papies et al., 2009; Goldstein et al., 2014).
Studies assessing attitudes toward body images show substantially convergent findings of negative implicit attitudes toward overweight body images or stronger preference for normal-weight compared to underweight and overweight bodies in the healthy population (Ahern and Hetherington 2006; Ahern et al.,2008; Lydecker et al., 2006; Watts et al., 2008; Moussally et al., 2015; Sabin et al., 2015; Marini, 2017; Elran-Barak and Bar-Anan, 2018; Robstad et al., 2018), whereas greater pro-thin implicit bias in patients with AN and BN compared to healthy subjects has been reported (Khan and Petróczi, 2015; Smith et al., 2018; Izquierdo et al., 2019). Nevertheless, patients with AN also showed anti-fat bias, rather than attraction to thinness (Spring and Bulik, 2014). Moreover, experimental manipulation could be effectively used to modulate body implicit attitudes in ED samples (Smith et al., 2014), or in healthy subjects (Martijn et al., 2013; Matharu et al., 2014), even though this result was not consistent across the studies (Keng and Ang, 2019). The correspondence between implicit and explicit measures on body image as well as the predictive value of implicit and explicit attitudes on personal traits and behaviours were not consistently reported by studies on both healthy and ED samples. Interestingly, implicit measures toward body and food were found to be more predictive of ED symptoms maintenance than explicit preferences across the retrieved studies (Parling et al., 2012; Khan and Petróczi, 2015). This consistent result has important clinical implications as an improvement in automatic, but not explicit, evaluation of overweight stimuli was considered a marker of treatment efficacy in patients with AN (Spring and Bulik, 2014).
Notably, results of the reviewed studies appeared consistent concerning the validity of implicit measures in discriminating between healthy and individuals with EDs. Between-group differences have been reported in different studies comparing samples of patients with AN and BN to healthy control participants, in particular when implicit attitudes toward body were assessed (Cserjési et al., 2010; Parling et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2014; Khan and Petróczi, 2015; Smith et al., 2018; Izquierdo et al., 2019). Similarly, consistent results of differences between individuals with healthy-weight and overweight/obesity have been reported (Craeynest et al., 2006, 2008b; Sartor et al., 2011; Brauhardt et al., 2014; Kemps and Tiggemann, 2015). In particular, patients with obesity, compared to healthy controls, showed larger implicit preferences for food (Kemps and Tiggemann, 2015), also when those preferences involved self-related concepts (Craeynest et al., 2006). These results could be the evidence, at an implicit level, of the craving for food that clinically characterizes patients with obesity. Moreover, samples with obesity did not associate fat-positive vs fat-negative as strongly as controls (Craeynest et al., 2006; Brauhardt et al., 2014), suggesting an ambivalent actual and ideal body images. Anomalous brain responses to body and food images are reported in patients with AN and BN as well as with obesity (Martin et al., 2010; Brooks et al., 2011; Mohr et al., 2011). Understanding whether these abnormalities are related to the automatic representation and evaluation of body and food at brain level is a challenge for future research.
A large body of literature has emerged on weight bias, and this review shows a notable consistency and pervasiveness, among different settings and populations, of weight discrimination towards individuals with overweight and obesity, typically considered worthless, lazier and less motivated than thin people (Schwartz et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2004). Indeed, in the last two decades a growing body of research has been focusing on weight stigma, due to its considerable negative impact on the social and psychological well-being of individuals with overweight and obesity. Evidence for low scores of overall health and body esteem, coupled with increased loneliness and a propensity to use alcohol or drugs to cope with stress, have been found in a sample of first-year medical students with overweight or obesity (Phelan et al., 2015a), proving that weight bias internalization can considerably affect the quality of life. Indeed, weight bias was found to exist in different population groups, even among individuals with overweight and obesity (Wang et al., 2004; Schwartz et al., 2006; Brauhardt et al., 2014), who internalize the negative attitudes toward overweight coming from the society. Moreover, since weight bias has been found even among medical students (Miller et al., 2013; Phelan et al., 2014, 2015b; Baker et al., 2017) and within the healthcare setting (Teachman and brownell, 2001; Schwartz et al., 2003; Vallis et al., 2007; Sabin et al., 2012; Tomiyama et al., 2015; Aweidah et al., 2016; Halvorson et al., 2019), the possibility that the quality of patient’s care can be negatively affected, leading people with overweight and obesity to avoid preventive healthcare, should be taken into account. Due to the potential negative implications of weight bias, some researches tried to verify the effects of different manipulations aimed to improve weight stigma (Geier et al., 2003; Teachman et al., 2003; Gapinski et al., 2006; O’Brien et al., 2010; Rukavina et al., 2010; Domoff et al., 2012; Flint et al., 2013; Swift et al., 2013; Russell-Mayhew et al., 2015; Hilbert and Meyre, 2016; Karsay and Schmuck, 2017; Rudolph and Hilbert, 2017; Scrivano et al., 2017; Geller and Watkins, 2018; Wijayatunga et al., 2019), providing mixed results, but leading hopes about the possibility that negative attitudes towards fatness can be minimized (Teachman et al., 2003; O’Brien et al., 2010; Russell-Mayhew et al., 2015; Hilbert and Meyre, 2016). A prospective research focusing on medical students (Phelan et al., 2015b) found evidence for changes in weight bias, fostered by school training and interactions with patients with obesity, suggesting that curricula and lecturers should be shaped, taking into consideration such mediating variables, in order to improve weight-related attitudes of future professionals.
A clear limitation of studies included in this review is that most of research is based on female samples, as some studies included only female participants and so, in most of studies, gender is not balanced. Few studies introduced gender as factor in the analyses and all found differences between male and female participants in implicit food preferences and weight bias (Grover et al., 2003; Pechey et al., 2015; Alkozei et al., 2018). This is critically relevant considering (Striegel-Moore et al., 2009) gender differences in eating disorders and that gender differences have been reported in neuromodulation effects on implicit attitudes on weight bias and stereotype (Cattaneo et al., 2011; Cazzato et al., 2017). Thus, future research should take this issue into account in designing experiments and selecting samples.
In conclusion, implicit attitudes appear as valid tools to measure individual differences and predict behaviour in healthy population. Further research is needed to define the validity of implicit measure in distinguishing healthy individuals from patients with EDs and the advantage of using these measures in clinical settings. Neuroimaging research on brain mechanisms underpinning implicit attitudes toward food and body images is critically missing. Further research should shed light on neural mechanisms of automatic responses at brain and behavioural level, providing novel directions for the understanding of healthy and pathological eating behaviour.
Supplementary Material
Appendix 1. Descriptors of search
Search engine | Search algorithm | Alternative keywords |
---|---|---|
PubMed (May 2019) | Implicit attitudes AND eating disorder: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘attitude’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘attitude’[All Fields] OR ‘attitudes’[All Fields])) AND (‘feeding and eating disorders’[MeSH Terms] OR (‘feeding’[All Fields] AND ‘eating’[All Fields] AND ‘disorders’[All Fields]) OR ‘feeding and eating disorders’[All Fields] OR (‘eating’[All Fields] AND ‘disorder’[All Fields]) OR ‘eating disorder’[All Fields]) | Implicit association; Affective priming |
Implicit attitudes AND anorexia nervosa: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘attitude’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘attitude’[All Fields] OR ‘attitudes’[All Fields])) AND (‘anorexia nervosa’[MeSH Terms] OR (‘anorexia’[All Fields] AND ‘nervosa’[All Fields]) OR ‘anorexia nervosa’[All Fields]) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND bulimia nervosa: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘attitude’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘attitude’[All Fields] OR ‘attitudes’[All Fields])) AND (‘bulimia nervosa’[MeSH Terms] OR (‘bulimia’[All Fields] AND ‘nervosa’[All Fields]) OR ‘bulimia nervosa’[All Fields]) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND binge eating disorder: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘association’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘association’[All Fields])) AND (‘binge-eating disorder’[MeSH Terms] OR (‘binge-eating’[All Fields] AND ‘disorder’[All Fields]) OR ‘binge-eating disorder’[All Fields] OR (‘binge’[All Fields] AND ‘eating’[All Fields] AND ‘disorder’[All Fields]) OR ‘binge eating disorder’[All Fields]) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND obesity: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘attitude’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘attitude’[All Fields] OR ‘attitudes’[All Fields])) AND (‘obesity’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘obesity’[All Fields]) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND food preference: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘attitude’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘attitude’[All Fields] OR ‘attitudes’[All Fields])) AND (‘food preferences’[MeSH Terms] OR (‘food’[All Fields] AND ‘preferences’[All Fields]) OR ‘food preferences’[All Fields] OR (‘food’[All Fields] AND ‘preference’[All Fields]) OR ‘food preference’[All Fields]) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND thin idea: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘attitude’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘attitude’[All Fields] OR ‘attitudes’[All Fields])) AND (thin[All Fields] AND (‘IDEA J Law Technol’[Journal] OR ‘idea’[All Fields])) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND thin ideal: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘attitude’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘attitude’[All Fields] OR ‘attitudes’[All Fields])) AND (thin[All Fields] AND ideal[All Fields]) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND fat phobia: (implicit[All Fields] AND (‘association’[MeSH Terms] OR ‘association’[All Fields])) AND (fat[All Fields] AND (‘phobic disorders’[MeSH Terms] OR (‘phobic’[All Fields] AND ‘disorders’[All Fields]) OR ‘phobic disorders’[All Fields] OR ‘phobia’[All Fields])) | ||
EMBASE (May 2019) | Implicit attitudes AND eating disorder: implicit AND attitudes AND eating AND disorder | Implicit association; Affective priming |
Implicit attitudes AND anorexia nervosa: implicit AND attitudes AND anorexia AND nervosa | ||
Implicit attitudes AND bulimia nervosa: implicit AND attitudes AND bulimia AND nervosa | ||
Implicit attitudes AND binge eating disorder: implicit AND attitudes AND binge AND eating AND disorder | ||
Implicit attitudes AND obesity: implicit AND attitudes AND obesity | ||
Implicit attitudes AND food preference: implicit AND attitudes AND food AND preference | ||
Implicit attitudes AND thin idea: implicit AND attitudes AND thin AND idea | ||
Implicit attitudes AND thin ideal: implicit AND attitudes AND thin AND ideal | ||
mplicit attitudes AND fat phobia: implicit AND attitudes AND fat AND phobia | ||
PsychINFO (May 2019) | Implicit attitudes AND eating disorder: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘eating disorder’) | Implicit association; Affective priming |
Implicit attitudes AND anorexia nervosa: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘anorexia nervosa’) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND bulimia nervosa: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘bulimia nervosa’) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND binge eating disorder: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘binge eating disorder’) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND obesity: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘obesity’) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND food preference: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘food preference’) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND thin idea: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘thin idea’) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND thin ideal: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘thin ideal’) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND fat phobia: noft(‘implicit attitudes’) AND noft(‘fat phobia’) | ||
SCOPUS (May 2019) | Implicit attitudes AND eating disorder: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND eating AND disorder) | Implicit association; Affective priming |
Implicit attitudes AND anorexia nervosa: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND anorexia AND nervosa) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND bulimia nervosa: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND bulimia AND nervosa) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND binge eating disorder: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND binge AND eating AND disorder) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND obesity: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND obesity) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND food preference: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND food AND preference) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND thin idea: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND thin AND idea) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND thin ideal: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND thin AND ideal) | ||
Implicit attitudes AND fat phobia: TITLE-ABS-KEY (implicit AND attitudes AND fat AND phobia) |
In each search algorithm, the ‘Implicit attitudes’ keyword have been replaced with the alternatives in the right column; noft = research in all filed excluding entire manuscripts due to the exceeding number of retrieved records; TITLE-ABS-KEY = research in title, abstract and keywords (excluding entire manuscripts) due to the exceeding number of retrieved records.
Contributor Information
Alessia Gallucci, Ph.D. Program in Neuroscience, School of Medicine and Surgery, University of Milano-Bicocca, Via Cadore, 48--20900, Monza, Italy; NeuroMi (Neuroscience Center), University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1--20126, Milan, Italy.
Lilia Del Mauro, Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1--20126, Milan, Italy.
Alberto Pisoni, NeuroMi (Neuroscience Center), University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1--20126, Milan, Italy; Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1--20126, Milan, Italy.
Leonor J Romero Lauro, NeuroMi (Neuroscience Center), University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1--20126, Milan, Italy; Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1--20126, Milan, Italy.
Giulia Mattavelli, NETS, School of Advanced Studies, IUSS, Piazza della Vittoria n.15, 27100, Pavia, Italy.
Conflict of interest
Authors have no conflict of interest to declare.
Supplementary data
Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.
References
- Ackermann, C.L., Palmer, A. (2014). The contribution of implicit cognition to the Theory of Reasoned Action Model: a study of food preferences. Journal of Marketing Management, 30(5–6), 529–50. [Google Scholar]
- Adams, R.C., Lawrence, N.S., Verbruggen, F., Chambers, C.D. (2017). Training response inhibition to reduce food consumption: mechanisms, stimulus specificity and appropriate training protocols. Appetite, 109, 11–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agerström, J., Carlsson, R., Rooth, D.O. (2007). Ethnicity and obesity: evidence of implicit work performance stereotypes in Sweden. EconStor, 20.
- Agerström, J., Rooth, D.-O. (2011). The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 790–805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahern, A.L., Bennett, K.M., Hetherington, M.M. (2008). Internalization of the ultra-thin ideal: positive implicit associations with underweight fashion models are associated with drive for thinness in young women. Eating Disorders, 16(4), 294–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahern, A.L., Hetherington, M.M. (2006). The thin ideal and body image: an experimental study of implicit attitudes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(3), 338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alabduljader, K., Cliffe, M., Sartor, F., Papini, G., Cox, W.M., Kubis, H.P. (2018). Ecological momentary assessment of food perceptions and eating behavior using a novel phone application in adults with or without obesity. Eating Behaviors, 30(May), 35–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alblas, E.E., Folkvord, F., Anschütz, D.J., et al. (2018). Investigating the impact of a health game on implicit attitudes towards food and food choice behaviour of young adults. Appetite, 128, 294–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alkozei, A., Killgore, W.D.S., Smith, R., et al. (2018). Chronic sleep restriction differentially affects implicit biases toward food among men and women: preliminary evidence. Journal of Sleep Research, 27(4), 1–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anselmi, P., Vianello, M., Robusto, E. (2013). Preferring thin people does not imply derogating fat people. A Rasch analysis of the implicit weight attitude. Obesity, 21, 261–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anselmi, P., Vianello, M., Robusto, E. (2011). Positive associations primacy in the IAT. Experimental Psychology, 58, 376–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashby, C.R., Stritzke, W.G.K. (2013). Is sensitivity to reward associated with the malleability of implicit inclinations toward high-fat food? Emotion, 13(4), 711–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aweidah, L., Robinson, J., Cumming, S., Lewis, S. (2016). Australian diagnostic radiographers’ attitudes and perceptions of imaging obese patients: a study of self, peers and students. Radiography, 22, e258–63. [Google Scholar]
- Ayres, K., Conner, M.T., Prestwich, A., Smith, P. (2012). Do implicit measures of attitudes incrementally predict snacking behaviour over explicit affect-related measures? Appetite, 58(3), 835–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker, T.K., Smith, G.S., Jacobs, N.N., et al. (2017). A deeper look at implicit weight bias in medical students. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 22, 889–900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) and the relational elaboration and coherence (REC) model. Psychological Record, 60(3), 527–42. [Google Scholar]
- Becker, D., Jostmann, N.B., Wiers, R.W., Holland, R.W. (2015). Approach avoidance training in the eating domain: testing the effectiveness across three single session studies. Appetite, 85, 58–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benas, J.S., Gibb, B.E. (2011). Childhood teasing and adult implicit cognitive biases. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35(6), 491–96. [Google Scholar]
- Bissell, K., Hays, H. (2010). Understanding anti-fat bias in children: the role of media and appearance anxiety in third to sixth graders’ implicit and explicit attitudes toward obesity. Mass Communication and Society, 14(1), 113–40. [Google Scholar]
- Blechert, J., Ansorge, U., Beckmann, S., Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2011). The undue influence of shape and weight on self-evaluation in anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and restrained eaters: a combined ERP and behavioral study. Psychological Medicine, 41(1), 185–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bongers, P., Jansen, A., Houben, K., Roefs, A. (2013). Happy eating: the single target implicit association test predicts overeating after positive emotions. Eating Behaviors, 14(3), 348–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brauhardt, A., Rudolph, A., Hilbert, A. (2014). Implicit cognitive processes in binge-eating disorder and obesity. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45(2), 285–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewis, A., Brennhofer, S., van Wodern, I., Bruening, M. (2016). Weight stigma and eating behaviors on a college campus: are students immune to stigma’s effects? Preventive Medicine Reports, 4, 578–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewis, A.A., Wutich, A. (2012). Explicit versus implicit fat-stigma. American Journal of Human Biology, 24, 332–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brochu, P.M., Gawronski, B., Esses, V.M. (2011). The integrative prejudice framework and different forms of weight prejudice: an analysis and expansion. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 14(3), 429–44. [Google Scholar]
- Brochu, P.M., Morrison, M.A. (2007). Implicit and explicit prejudice toward overweight and average-weight men and women: testing their correspondence and relation to behavioral intentions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 147(6), 681–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks, S.J., O′Daly, O.G., Uher, R., et al. (2011). Differential neural responses to food images in women with bulimia versus anorexia nervosa. PLoS One, 6(7), e22259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carels, R.A., Hinman, N.G., Hoffmann, D.A., et al. (2014). Implicit bias about weight and weight loss treatment outcomes. Eating Behaviors, 15(4), 648–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carels, R.A., Hinman, N.G., Burmeister, J.M., Hoffmann, D.A., Ashrafioun, L., Koball, A.M. (2013). Stereotypical images and implicit weight bias in overweight/obese people. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 18(4), 441–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carels, R.A., Hinman, N., Koball, A., Oehlhof, M.W., Gumble, A., Young, K.M. (2011). The self-protective nature of implicit identity and its relationship to weight bias and short-term weight loss. Obesity Facts, 4, 278–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carels, R.A., Wott, C.B., Young, K.M., Gumble, A., Koball, A., Oehlof, M.W. (2010). Implicit, explicit, and internalized weight bias and psychosocial maladjustment among treatment-seeking adults. Eating Behaviors, 11, 180–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carels, R.A., Young, K.M., Wott, C.B., et al. (2009a). Internalized weight stigma and its ideological correlates among weight loss treatment seeking adults. Eating and Weight Disorders - Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 14(2–3), e92–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carels, R.A., Young, K.M., Wott, C.B., et al. (2009b). Weight bias and weight loss treatment outcomes in treatment-seeking adults. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(3), 350–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cattaneo, Z., Mattavelli, G., Platania, E., Papagno, C. (2011). The role of the prefrontal cortex in controlling gender-stereotypical associations: a TMS investigation. NeuroImage, 56, 1839–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cazzato, V., Makris, S. (2019). Implicit preference towards slim bodies and weight-stigma modulate the understanding of observed familiar actions. Psychological Research, 83, 1825–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cazzato, V., Makris, S., Urgesi, C. (2017). Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the extrastriate visual cortex modulates implicit anti-fat bias in male, but not female, participants. Neuroscience, 359, 92–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambliss, H.O., Finley, C.E., Blair, S.N. (2004). Attitudes toward obese individuals among exercise science students. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 36, 468–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chee, M.W., Sriram, N., Soon, C.S., Lee, K.M. (2000). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the implicit association of concepts and attributes. Neuroreport, 11(1), 135–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conner, M.T., Perugini, M., O’Gorman, R., Ayres, K., Prestwich, A. (2007). Relations between implicit and explicit measures of attitudes and measures of behavior: evidence of moderation by individual difference variables. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(12), 1727–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coricelli, C., Foroni, F., Osimo, S.A., Rumiati, R.I. (2019). Implicit and explicit evaluations of foods: the natural and transformed dimension. Food Quality and Preference, 73, 143–53. [Google Scholar]
- Craeynest, M., Crombez, G., Deforche, B., Tanghe, A., De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2008a). The role of implicit attitudes towards food and physical activity in the treatment of youth obesity. Eating Behaviors, 9(1), 41–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craeynest, M., Crombez, G., Koster, E.H., Haerens, L., De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2008b). Cognitive-motivational determinants of fat food consumption in overweight and obese youngsters: the implicit association between fat food and arousal. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39(3), 354–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craeynest, M., Crombez, G., Haerens, L., De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2007). Do overweight youngsters like food more than lean peers? Assessing their implicit attitudes with a personalized Implicit Association Task. Food Quality and Preference, 18(8), 1077–84. [Google Scholar]
- Craeynest, M., Crombez, G., De Houwer, J., Deforche, B., De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2006). Do children with obesity implicitly identify with sedentariness and fat food? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 37(4), 347–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craeynest, M., Crombez, G., De Houwer, J., Deforche, B., Tanghe, A., De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2005). Explicit and implicit attitudes towards food and physical activity in childhood obesity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(9), 1111–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crescentini, C., Aglioti, S.M., Fabbro, F., Urgesi, C. (2014). Virtual lesions of the inferior parietal cortex induce fast changes of implicit religiousness/spirituality. Cortex, 54, 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cserjesi, R., De Vos, I., Deroost, N. (2016). Discrepancy between implicit and explicit preferences for food portions in obesity. International Journal of Obesity, 40(9), 1464–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cserjési, R., Vermeulen, N., Luminet, O., et al. (2010). Explicit vs. implicit body image evaluation in restrictive anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research, 175(1–2), 148–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czyzewska, M., Graham, R. (2008). Implicit and explicit attitudes to high- and low-calorie food in females with different BMI status. Eating Behaviors, 9(3), 303–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dagher, A., Robbins, T.W. (2009). Personality, addiction, dopa-mine: insights from Parkinson’s disease. Neuron, 61(4), 502–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Degner, J., Wentura, D. (2009). Not everybody likes the thin and despises the fat: one’s weight matters in the automatic activation of weight-related social evaluations. Social Cognition, 27(2), 202–21. [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: a normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 347–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer, J. (2003). The Extrinsic Affective Simon Task. Experimental Psychology, 50(2), 77–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer, J. (2002). The Implicit Association Test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: strengths and limitations. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33(2), 115–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dimmock, J.A., Hallett, B.E., Grove, R.J. (2009). Attitudes toward overweight individuals among fitness center employees: an examination of contextual effects. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 80(3), 641–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domoff, S.E., Hinman, N.G., Koball, A.M., et al. (2012). The effects of reality television on weight bias: an examination of the biggest loser. Obesity, 20, 993–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, E.M., Kiviniemi, M.T., Cook-Cottone, C. (2014). Implicit affective associations predict snack choice for those with low, but not high levels of eating disorder symptomatology. Appetite, 77, 124–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elran-Barak, R., Bar-Anan, Y. (2018). Implicit and explicit anti-fat bias: the role of weight-related attitudes and beliefs. Social Science & Medicine, 204, 117–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eschenbeck, H., Heim-Dreger, U., Steinhilber, A., Kohlmann, C.-W. (2016). Self-regulation of healthy nutrition: automatic and controlled processes. BMC Psychology, 4(1), 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Expósito, P.M., López, M.H., Valverde, M.R. (2015). Assessment of implicit anti-fat and pro-slim attitudes in young women using the implicit relational assessment procedure. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 15(1), 17–32. [Google Scholar]
- Fazio, R.H., Sanbonmatsu, D.M., Powell, M.C., Kardes, F.R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferentzi, H., Scheibner, H., Wiers, R., et al. (2018). Retraining of automatic action tendencies in individuals with obesity: a randomized controlled trial. Appetite, 126, 66–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flint, S.W., Čadek, M., Codreanu, S.C., Ivić, V., Zomer, C., Gomoiu, A. (2016). Obesity discrimination in the recruitment process: “you’re not hired!”. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flint, S.W., Hudson, J., Lavallee, D. (2015). UK adults’ implicit and explicit attitudes towards obesity: a cross-sectional study. BMC Obesity, 2(1), 31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flint, S.W., Hudson, J., Lavallee, D. (2013). Counter-conditioning as an intervention to modify anti-fat attitudes. Health Psychology Research, 1, e24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fontana, F., Furtado, O.Jr, Mazzardo, O., Hong, D., de Campos, W. (2017). Anti-fat bias by professors teaching physical education majors. European Physical Education Review, 23(1), 127–38. [Google Scholar]
- Fontana, F.E., Furtado, O., Marston, R., Mazzardo, O., Gallagher, J. (2013). Anti-fat bias among physical education teachers and majors. Physical Educator, 70(1), 15. [Google Scholar]
- Forbes, C.E., Cameron, K.A., Grafman, J., et al. (2012). Identifying temporal and causal contributions of neural processes underlying the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(November 2012), 1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forhan, M., Salas, X.R. (2013). Inequities in healthcare: a review of bias and discrimination in obesity treatment. Canadian Journal of Diabetes, 37(3), 205–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jcjd.2013.03.362 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank, G.K.W. (2019). Editorial to the virtual issue highlighting neuroscience based research in eating disorders to mark the 49th Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 52(11), 1332–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friese, M., Hofmann, W., Wänke, M. (2008). When impulses take over: moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47(3), 397–419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gapinski, K.D., Schwartz, M.B., Brownell, K.D. (2006). Can television change anti-fat attitudes and behavior? Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 11(1), 1–28. [Google Scholar]
- Garner, D.M. (2004). Eating Disorder Inventory-3 (EDI-3). Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
- Gawronski, B., Bodenhausen, G.V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692–731. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geier, A.B., Schwartz, M.B., Brownell, K.D. (2003). “Before and after” diet advertisements escalate weight stigma. Eating and Weight Disorders, 8, 282–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geller, G., Watkins, P.A. (2018). Addressing medical students’ negative bias toward patients with obesity through ethics education. AMA Journal of Ethics, 20, e948–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genschow, O., Demanet, J., Hersche, L., Brass, M. (2017). An empirical comparison of different implicit measures to predict consumer choice. PLoS One, 12(8), 1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glashouwer, K.A., Bennik, E.C., de Jong, P.J., Spruyt, A. (2018). Implicit measures of actual versus ideal body image: relations with self-reported body dissatisfaction and dieting behaviors. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(5), 622–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glock, S., Beverborg, A.O.G., Müller, B.C.N. (2016). Pre-service teachers’ implicit and explicit attitudes toward obesity influence their judgments of students. Social Psychology of Education, 19(1), 97–115. [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein, S.P., Forman, E.M., Meiran, N., Herbert, J.D., Juarascio, A.S., Butryn, M.L. (2014). The discrepancy between implicit and explicit attitudes in predicting disinhibited eating. Eating Behaviors, 15(1), 164–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gozzi, M., Raymont, V., Solomon, J., Koenigs, M., Grafman, J. (2009). Dissociable effects of prefrontal and anterior temporal cortical lesions on stereotypical gender attitudes. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2125–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant, M.J., Booth, A. (2009). A typology of reviews: an analysis of 14 review types and associated methodologies. Health Information & Libraries Journal, 26(2), 91–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwald, A.G., Nosek, B.A., Banaji, M.R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwald, A.G., McGhee, D.E., Schwartz, J.L.K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grover, V.P., Keel, P.K., Mitchell, J.P. (2003). Gender differences in implicit weight identity. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 34, 125–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guidetti, M., Conner, M., Prestwich, A., Cavazza, N. (2012). The transmission of attitudes towards food: twofold specificity of similarities with parents and friends. British Journal of Health Psychology, 17(2), 346–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gumble, A., Carels, R. (2012). The harmful and beneficial impacts of weight bias on well-being: the moderating influence of weight status. Body Image, 9, 101–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall, P.A., Lowe, C.J., Safati, A.B., Li, H., Klassen, E.B., Burhan, A.M. (2018a). Effects of left dlPFC modulation on social cognitive processes following food sampling. Appetite, 126, 73–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall, P., Vincent, C.M., Burhan, A.M. (2018b). Non-invasive brain stimulation for food cravings, consumption, and disorders of eating: A review of methods, findings and controversies. Appetite, 124, 78–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halmi, K.A. (2013). Perplexities of treatment resistence in eating disorders. BMC Psychiatry, 13(1), 292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halvorson, E.E., Curley, T., Wright, M., Skelton, J.A. (2019). Weight bias in pediatric inpatient care. Academic Paediatrics, 19, 780–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hand, W.B., Robinson, J.C., Stewart, M.W., Zhang, L., Hand, S.C. (2017). The identity threat of weight stigma in adolescents. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 39(8), 991–1007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart, E.A., Sbrocco, T., Carter, M.M. (2016). Ethnic identity and implicit anti-fat bias: similarities and differences between African American and Caucasian women. Ethnicity & Disease, 26, 69–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Healy, G.F., Boran, L., Smeaton, A.F. (2015). Neural patterns of the implicit association test. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(Nov), 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heider, N., Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J. (2018). Body dissatisfaction revisited: on the importance of implicit beliefs about actual and ideal body image. PsychologicaBelgica, 57(4), 158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heider, N., Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J. (2015). Implicit beliefs about ideal body image predict body image dissatisfaction. Frontiers in Psychology, 60, 1402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hensels, I.S., Baines, S (2016). Changing ‘gut feelings’ about food: an evaluative conditioning effect on implicit food evaluations and food choice. Learning and Motivation, 55, 31–44. [Google Scholar]
- Higgins, J.P., Altman, D.G., Gøtzsche, P.C., et al. (2011). The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ, 343, d5928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilbert, A., Meyre, D. (2016). Weight stigma reduction and genetic determinism. PLoS One, 11(9), e0162993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilbert, A., Braehler, E., Haeuser, W., Zenger, M. (2014). Weight bias internalization, core self-evaluation, and health in overweight and obese persons. Obesity, 22, 79–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinman, N.G., Burmeister, J.M., Kiefner, A.E., Borushok, J., Carels, R.A. (2015). Stereotypical portrayals of obesity and the expression of implicit weight bias. Body Image, 12, 32–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoefling, A., Strack, F. (2008). The tempting effect of forbidden foods. High calorie content evokes conflicting implicit and explicit evaluations in restrained eaters. Appetite, 51(3), 681–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollands, G.J., Prestwich, A., Marteau, T.M. (2011). Using aversive images to enhance healthy food choices and implicit attitudes: an experimental test of evaluative conditioning. Health Psychology, 30(2), 195–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben, K., Roefs, A., Jansen, A. (2012). Guilty pleasures II: restrained eaters’ implicit preferences for high, moderate and low-caloric food. Eating Behaviors, 13(3), 275–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben, K., Roefs, A., Jansen, A. (2010). Guilty pleasures. Implicit preferences for high calorie food in restrained eating. Appetite, 55(1), 18–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- House, A.E., House, B.J., Campbell, M.B. (1981). Measures of interobserver agreement: calculation formulas and distribution effects. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 3(1), 37–57. [Google Scholar]
- Hutchison, S.M., Müller, U. (2018). Explicit and implicit measures of weight stigma in young children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 64(4), 427–58. [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo, A., Plessow, F., Becker, K.R., et al. (2019). Implicit attitudes toward dieting and thinness distinguish fat-phobic and non-fat-phobic anorexia nervosa from avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder in adolescents. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 52(4), 419–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang, W., Tan, J., Fassnacht, D.B. (2017). Implicit and explicit anti-fat bias among Asian females. Eating and Weight Disorders - Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 22, 457–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson, L., Ghaderi, A., Andersson, G. (2005). Stroop interference for food- and body-related words: a meta-analysis. Eating Behaviors, 6(3), 271–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juarascio, A.S., Forman, E.M., Timko, C.A., Herbert, J.D., Butryn, M., Lowe, M. (2011). Implicit internalization of the thin ideal as a predictor of increases in weight, body dissatisfaction, and disordered eating. Eating Behaviors, 12(3), 207–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kakoschke, N., Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M. (2017). The effect of combined avoidance and control training on implicit food evaluation and choice. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 55, 99–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karpinski, A., Steinman, R.B. (2006). The Single Category Implicit Association Test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karsay, K., Schmuck, D. (2019). “Weak, sad, and lazy fatties”: adolescents’ explicit and implicit weight bias following exposure to weight loss reality TV shows. Media Psychology, 22(1), 60–81. [Google Scholar]
- Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M. (2015). Approach bias for food cues in obese individuals. Psychology & Health, 30(3), 370–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keng, S.-L., Ang, Q. (2019). Effects of mindfulness on negative affect, body dissatisfaction, and disordered eating urges. Mindfulness, 10(9), 1779–91. [Google Scholar]
- Khan, S., Petróczi, A. (2015). Stimulus-response compatibility tests of implicit preference for food and body image to identify people at risk for disordered eating: a validation study. Eating Behaviors, 16, 54–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kottner, J., Streiner, D.L. (2011). The difference between reliability and agreement. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 64(6), 701–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraus, A.A., Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2016). Sandwich or sweets? An assessment of two novel implicit association tasks to capture dynamic motivational tendencies and stable evaluations towards foods. Food Quality and Preference, 49, 11–9. [Google Scholar]
- Lamote, S., Hermans, D., Baeyens, F., Eelen, P. (2004). An exploration of affective priming as an indirect measure of food attitudes. Appetite, 42(3), 279–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lebens, H., Roefs, A., Martijn, C., Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., Jansen, A. (2011). Making implicit measures of associations with snack foods more negative through evaluative conditioning. Eating Behaviors, 12(4), 249–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lund, T.B., Brodersen, J., Sandøe, P. (2018). A study of anti-fat bias among Danish general practitioners and whether this bias and general practitioners’ lifestyle can affect treatment of tension headache in patients with obesity. Obesity Facts, 11(6), 501–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lydecker, J.A., O’Brien, E., Grilo, C.M. (2018). Parents have both implicit and explicit biases against children with obesity. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 41(6), 784–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynagh, M., Cliff, K., Morgan, P.J. (2015). Attitudes and beliefs of nonspecialist and specialist trainee health and physical education teachers toward obese children: evidence for “anti-fat” bias. The Journal of School Health, 9, 595–603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maas, J., Woud, M.L., Keijsers, G.P.J., Rinck, M., Becker, E.S., Wiers, R.W. (2017). The attraction of sugar: an association between body mass index and impaired avoidance of sweet snacks. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 8(1), 40–54. [Google Scholar]
- Mai, R., Hoffmann, S. (2015). How to combat the unhealthy = tasty intuition: the influencing role of health consciousness. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 34(1), 63–83. [Google Scholar]
- Marini, M. (2017). Underweight vs. Overweight/obese: which weight category do we prefer? Dissociation of weight-related preferences at the explicit and implicit level. Obesity Science & Practice, 3(4), 390–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marini, M., Sriram, N., Schnabel, K., et al. (2013). Overweight people have low levels of implicit weight bias, but overweight nations have high levels of implicit weight bias. PLoS One, 8, e83543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martijn, C., Sheeran, P., Wesseldijk, L.W., et al. (2013). Evaluative conditioning makes slim models less desirable as standards for comparison and increases body satisfaction. Health Psychology, 32(4), 433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin, L.E., Holsen, L.M., Chambers, R.J., et al. (2010). Neural mechanisms associated with food motivation in obese and healthy weight adults. Obesity, 18(2), 254–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matharu, K., Shapiro, J.F., Hammer, R.R., Kravitz, R.L., Wilson, M.D., Fitzgerald, F.T. (2014). Reducing obesity prejudice in medical education. Education for Health: Change in Learning and Practice, 27(3), 231–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mattavelli, G., Gallucci, A., Schiena, G., et al. (2019). Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates implicit attitudes towards food in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 52(5), 576–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mattavelli, G., Zuglian, P., Dabroi, E., Gaslini, G., Clerici, M., Papagno, C. (2015). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex modulates implicit attitudes towards food. Appetite, 89, 70–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer, B., Bos, A.E.R., Muris, P., Huijding, J., Vlielander, M. (2008). Does disgust enhance eating disorder symptoms? Eating Behaviors, 9(1), 124–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McConnell, A.R., Dunn, E.W., Austin, S.N., Rawn, C.D. (2011). Blind spots in the search for happiness: implicit attitudes and nonverbal leakage predict affective forecasting errors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 628–34. [Google Scholar]
- McKenna, I., Hughes, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., De Schryver, M., Yoder, R., O’Shea, D. (2016). Obesity, food restriction, and implicit attitudes to healthy and unhealthy foods: lessons learned from the implicit relational assessment procedure. Appetite, 100, 41–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, D.P., Spangler, J.G., Vitolins, M.Z., et al. (2013). Are medical students aware of their anti-obesity bias? Academic Medicine, 88(7), 978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Misener, K., Libben, M. (2017). Risk for eating disorders modulates interpretation bias in a semantic priming task. Body Image, 21, 103–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews, 4(1), 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohr, H.M., Röder, C., Zimmermann, J., Hummel, D., Negele, A., Grabhorn, R. (2011). Body image distortions in bulimia nervosa: investigating body size overestimation and body size satisfaction by fMRI. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1822–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moussally, J.M., Billieux, J., Mobbs, O., Rothen, S., Van der Linden, M. (2015). Implicitly assessed attitudes toward body shape and food: the moderating roles of dietary restraint and disinhibition. Journal of Eating Disorders, 3(1), 47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nederkoorn, C., Houben, K., Hofmann, W., Roefs, A., Jansen, A. (2010). Control yourself or just eat what you like? Weight gain over a year is predicted by an interactive effect of response inhibition and implicit preference for snack foods. Health Psychology, 29(4), 389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nurjannah, I., Siwi, S.M. (2017). Guidelines for analysis on measuring interrater reliability of nursing outcome classification. International Journal of Research in Medical Sciences, 5(4), 1169–75. [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien, K.S., Puhl, R.M., Latner, J.D., Mir, A.S., Hunter, J.A. (2010). Reducing anti-fat prejudice in preservice health students: a randomized trial. Obesity, 18, 2138–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien, K.S., Latner, J.D., Halberstadt, J., Hunter, J.A., Anderson, J., Caputi, P. (2008). Do antifat attitudes predict anti-fat behaviors? Obesity, 16, s87–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien, K.S., Hunter, J.A., Halberstadt, J., Anderson, J. (2007a). Body image and explicit and implicit anti-fat attitudes: the mediating role of physical appearance comparisons. Body Image, 4, 249–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien, K.S., Hunter, J.A., Banks, M. (2007b). Implicit anti-fat bias in physical educators: physical attributes, ideology and socialization. International Journal of Obesity, 31, 308–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ouzzani, M., Hammady, H., Fedorowicz, Z., Elmagarmid, A. (2016). Rayyan--a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Systematic reviews, 5(1), 210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne, B.K., Cheng, C.M., Govorun, O., Stewart, B.D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 277–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papies, E.K., Stroebe, W., Aarts, H. (2009). Who likes it more? Restrained eaters’ implicit attitudes towards food. Appetite, 53(3), 279–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parling, T., Cernvall, M., Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., Ghaderi, A. (2012). Using the implicit relational assessment procedure to compare implicit pro-thin/anti-fat attitudes of patients with anorexia nervosa and non-clinical controls. Eating Disorders, 20(2), 127–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlović, M., Žeželj, I., Marinković, M., Sučević, J. (2016). Implicit preference of sweets over fruit as a predictor of their actual consumption. British Food Journal, 118(10), 2567–80. [Google Scholar]
- Pearl, R.L., White, M.A., Grilo, C.M. (2013). Weight bias internalization, depression, and self-reported health among overweight binge eating disorder patients. Obesity, 22, 142–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pechey, R., Monsivais, P., Ng, Y.-L., Marteau, T.M. (2015). Why don’t poor men eat fruit? Socioeconomic differences in motivations for fruit consumption. Appetite, 84, 271–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penney, E., Lawsin, C. (2013). Application of the MODE model to implicit weight prejudice and its influence on expressed and actual behavior among college students. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, E229–36. [Google Scholar]
- Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of implicit and explicit attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44(1), 29–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phelan, S.M., Dovidio, J.F., Puhl, R.M., et al. (2014). Implicit and explicit weight bias in a national sample of 4,732 medical students: the medical student CHANGES study. Obesity, 4, 1201–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phelan, S.M., Burgess, D.J., Puhl, R., et al. (2015a). The adverse effect of weight stigma on the well-being of medical students with overweight or obesity: findings from a national survey. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 30(9), 1251–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phelan, S.M., Puhl, R.M., Burke, S.E., et al. (2015b). The mixed impact of medical school on medical students’ implicit and explicit weight bias. Medical Education, 49, 983–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phelan, S.M., Burgess, D.J., Yeazel, M.W., Hellerstedt, W.L., Griffin, J.M., van Ryn, M. (2015c). Impact of weight bias and stigma on quality of care and outcomes for patients with obesity. Obesity Reviews, 16(4), 319–26. doi: 10.1111/obr.12266 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pryor, J.B., Reeder, G.D., Wesselmann, E.D., Williams, K.D., Wirth, J.H. (2013). The influence of social norms upon behavioral expressions of implicit and explicit weight-related stigma in an interactive game. The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 86, 189–201. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raghunathan, R., Naylor, R.W., Hoyer, W.D. (2006). The unhea-lthy = tasty intuition and its effects on taste inferences, enjoyment, and choice of food products. Journal of Marketing, 70(4), 170–84. [Google Scholar]
- Richard, A., Meule, A., Blechert, J. (2019). Implicit evaluation of chocolate and motivational need states interact in predicting chocolate intake in everyday life. Eating Behaviors, 33, 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ritzert, T.R., Anderson, L.M., Reilly, E.E., Gorrell, S., Forsyth, J.P., Anderson, D.A. (2016). Assessment of weight/shape implicit bias related to attractiveness, fear, and disgust. The Psychological Record, 66(3), 405–17. [Google Scholar]
- Robertson, N., Vohora, R. (2008). Fitness vs. fatness: implicit bias towards obesity among fitness professionals and regular exercisers. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9(4), 547–57. [Google Scholar]
- Robinson, E.L., Ball, L.E., Leveritt, M.D. (2014). Obesity bias among health and non-health students attending an Australian university and their perceived obesity education. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 46(5), 390–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roddy, S., Stewart, I., Barnes‐Holmes, D. (2011). Facial reactions reveal that slim is good but fat is not bad: implicit and explicit measures of body‐size bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(6), 688–94. [Google Scholar]
- Roddy, S., Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D. (2010). Anti-fat, pro-slim, or both? Using two reaction-time based measures to assess implicit attitudes to the slim and overweight. Journal of Health Psychology, 15(3), 416–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roefs, A., Quaedackers, L., Werrij, M.Q., et al. (2006). The environment influences whether high-fat foods are associated with palatable or with unhealthy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(5), 715–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roefs, A., Herman, C.P., MacLeod, C.M., Smulders, F.T.Y., Jansen, A. (2005a). At first sight: how do restrained eaters evaluate high-fat palatable foods? Appetite, 44(1), 103–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roefs, A., Stapert, D., Isabella, L.A.S., Wolters, G., Wojciechowski, F., Jansen, A. (2005b). Early associations with food in anorexia nervosa patients and obese people assessed in the affective priming paradigm. Eating Behaviors, 6(2), 151–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roefs, A., Jansen, A. (2002). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward high-fat foods in obesity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(3), 517–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robstad, N., Siebler, F., Söderhamn, U., Westergren, T., Fegran, L. (2018). Design and psychometric testing of instruments to measure qualified intensive care nurses’ attitudes toward obese intensive care patients. Research in Nursing & Health, 41(6), 525–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph, A., Hilbert, A. (2017). The effects of obesity-related health messages on explicit and implicit weight bias. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 2064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rukavina, P.B., Li, W., Shen, B., Sun, H. (2010). A service learning based project to change implicit and explicit bias toward obese individuals in kinesiology pre-professionals. Obesity Facts, 3, 117–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell-Mayhew, S., Nutter, S., Ireland, A., et al. (2015). Pilot testing a professional development model for preservice teachers in the area of health and weight: feasibility, utility, and efficacy. Advances in School Mental Health Promotion, 8(3), 176–86. [Google Scholar]
- Sabin, J.A., Moore, K., Noonan, C., Lallemand, O., Buchwald, D. (2015). Clinicians’ implicit and explicit attitudes about weight and race and treatment approaches to overweight for American Indian children. Childhood Obesity, 11(4), 456–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabin, J.A., Marini, M., Nosek, B.A. (2012). Implicit and explicit anti-fat bias among a large sample of medical doctors by BMI, race/ethnicity and gender. PLoS One, 7, e48448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sartor, F., Donaldson, L.F., Markland, D.A., Loveday, H., Jackson, M.J., Kubis, H.P. (2011). Taste perception and implicit attitude toward sweet related to body mass index and soft drink supplementation. Appetite, 57(1), 237–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato, W., Sawada, R., Kubota, Y., Toichi, M., Fushiki, T. (2017). Homeostatic modulation on unconscious hedonic responses to food. BMC Research Notes, 10(1), 1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato, W., Sawada, R., Kubota, Y., Toichi, M., Fushiki, T. (2016). Unconscious affective responses to food. PLoS One, 11(8), e0160956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schakel, L., Veldhuijzen, D.S., Van Middendorp, H., et al. (2018). The effects of a gamified approach avoidance training and verbal suggestions on food outcomes. PLoS One, 13(7), 7–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz, M.B., Vartanian, L.R., Nosek, B.A., Brownell, K.D. (2006). The influence of one’s own body weight on implicit and explicit anti-fat bias. Obesity, 14, 440–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz, M.B., Chambliss, H.O., Brownell, K.D., Blair, S.N., Billington, C. (2003). Weight bias among health professionals specializing in obesity. Obesity Research, 11, 1033–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scrivano, R.M., Scisco, J.L., Giumetti, G.W. (2017). The impact of applicants’ weight and education about obesity on applicant ratings. Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research, 22(4), 278–85. [Google Scholar]
- Seibt, B., Häfner, M., Deutsch, R. (2007). Prepared to eat: how immediate affective and motivational responses to food cues are influenced by food deprivation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(2), 359–79. [Google Scholar]
- Shamseer, L., Moher, D., Clarke, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ, 349; g7647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner, A.C., Payne, K., Perrin, A.J., et al. (2017). Implicit weight bias in children age 9 to 11 years. Pediatrics, 140(1), e20163936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, A.R., Forrest, L.N., Velkoff, E.A., Ribeiro, J.D., Franklin, J. (2018). Implicit attitudes toward eating stimuli differentiate eating disorder and non‐eating disorder groups and predict eating disorder behaviors. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 51(4), 343–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, A.R., Joiner, T.E.Jr, Dodd, D.R. (2014). Examining implicit attitudes toward emaciation and thinness in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47(2), 138–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solbes, I., Enesco, I. (2010). Explicit and implicit anti-fat attitudes in children and their relationships with their body images. Obesity Facts, 3, 23–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Songa, G., Russo, V. (2018). IAT, consumer behaviour and the moderating role of decision-making style: an empirical study on food products. Food Quality and Preference, 64, 205–20. [Google Scholar]
- Spring, V.L., Bulik, C.M. (2014). Implicit and explicit affect toward food and weight stimuli in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 15(1), 91–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stafford, L.D., Scheffler, G. (2008). Hunger inhibits negative associations to food but not auditory biases in attention. Appetite, 51(3), 731–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storr, S.M., Sparks, P. (2016). Does self-affirmation following ego depletion moderate restrained eaters’ explicit preferences for, and implicit associations with, high-calorie foods? Psychology and Health, 31(7), 840–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Striegel-Moore, R.H., Rosselli, F., Perrin, N., et al. (2009). Gender difference in the prevalence of eating disorder symptoms. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 42(5), 471–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swift, J.A., Tischler, V., Markham, S., et al. (2013). Are anti-stigma films a useful strategy for reducing weight bias among trainee healthcare professionals? Results of a pilot randomized control trial. Obesity Facts, 6, 101–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teachman, B.A., Jeyaram, S., Gapinski, K.D., Brownell, K.D., Rawlins, M. (2003). Demonstrations of implicit anti-fat bias: the impact of providing causal information and evoking empathy. Health Psychology, 22, 68–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teachman, B.A., Brownell, K.D. (2001). Implicit anti-fat bias among health professionals: is anyone immune? International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 25, 1525–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terenzi, D., Rumiati, R.I., Catalan, M., et al. (2018). Reward sensitivity in Parkinson’s patients with binge eating. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 51, 79–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, S.R., Burton Smith, R., Ball, P.J. (2007). Implicit attitudes in very young children: an adaptation of the IAT. Current Research in Social Psychology, 13(7), 75–85. [Google Scholar]
- Tomiyama, A.J., Finch, L.E., Incollingo Belsky, A.C., et al. (2015). Weight bias in 2001 versus 2013: contradictory attitudes among obesity researchers and health professionals. Obesity, 23, 46–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trendel, O., Werle, C.O.C. (2016). Distinguishing the affective and cognitive bases of implicit attitudes to improve prediction of food choices. Appetite, 104, 33–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Dessel, P., Hughes, S., De Houwer, J. (2018). Consequence-based approach-avoidance training: a new and improved method for changing behavior. Psychological Science, 29(12), 1899–910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Val-Laillet, D., Aarts, E., Weber, B., et al. (2015). Neuroimaging and neuromodulation approaches to study eating behavior and prevent and treat eating disorders and obesity. NeuroImage: Clinical, 8, 1–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallis, T.M., Currie, B., Lawlor, D., Ransom, T. (2007). Healthcare professional bias against the obese: how do we know if we have a problem? Canadian Journal of Diabetes, 31(4), 365–70. [Google Scholar]
- Veenstra, E.M., de Jong, P.J. (2010). Restrained eaters show enhanced automatic approach tendencies towards food. Appetite, 55(1), 30–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venturini, B., Castelli, L., Tomelleri, S. (2006). Not all jobs are suitable for fat people: experimental evidence of a link between being fat and “out-of-sight” jobs. Social Behavior and Personality, 34(4), 389–98. [Google Scholar]
- Verbeken, S., Boendermaker, W.J., Loeys, T., et al. (2018). Feasibility and effectiveness of adding an approach avoidance training with game elements to a residential childhood obesity treatment-a pilot study. Behaviour Change, 35(2), 91–107. [Google Scholar]
- Waller, T., Lampman, C., Lupfer‐Johnson, G. (2012). Assessing bias against overweight individuals among nursing and psychology students: an implicit association test. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 21(23–24), 3504–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, Y., Zhu, J., Hu, Y., et al. (2016). The effect of implicit preferences on food consumption: moderating role of ego depletion and impulsivity. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, S.S., Brownell, K.D., Wadden, T.A. (2004). The influence of the stigma of obesity on overweight individuals. International Journal of Obesity, 28, 1333–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warschburger, P., Gmeiner, M., Morawietz, M., Rinck, M. (2018). Battle of plates: a pilot study of an approach-avoidance training for overweight children and adolescents. Public Health Nutrition, 21(2), 426–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watts, K., Cranney, J., Gleitzman, M. (2008). Automatic evaluation of body-related images. Body Image, 5(4), 352–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstein, J.H., Wilson, K.G., Drake, C.E., Kellum, K.K. (2008). A relational frame theory contribution to social categorization. Behavior and Social Issues, 17(1), 40–65. [Google Scholar]
- Werle, C.O., Trendel, O., Ardito, G. (2013). Unhealthy food is not tastier for everybody: the “healthy = tasty” French intuition. Food Quality and Preference, 28(1), 116–21. [Google Scholar]
- Werntz, A.J., Steinman, S.A., Glenn, J.J., Nock, M.K., Teachman, B.A. (2016). Characterizing implicit mental health associations across clinical domains. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 52, 17–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werrij, M.Q., Roefs, A., Janssen, I., et al. (2009). Early associations with palatable foods in overweight and obesity are not disinhibition related but restraint related. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40(1), 136–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wijayatunga, N.N., Kim, Y., Butsch, W.S., Dhurandhar, E.J. (2019). The effects of a teaching intervention on weight bias among kinesiology undergraduate students. International Journal of Obesity, 43, 2273–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodward, H.E., Treat, T.A., Cameron, C.D., Yegorova, V. (2017). Valence and arousal-based affective evaluations of foods. Eating Behaviors, 24, 26–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodward, H.E., Treat, T.A. (2015). Unhealthy how?: Implicit and explicit affective evaluations of different types of unhealthy foods. Eating Behaviors, 17, 27–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yen, J.Y., Chang, S.J., Ko, C.H., et al. (2010). The high-sweet-fat food craving among women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder: emotional response, implicit attitude and rewards sensitivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(8), 1203–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.