Abstract
Our understanding of the functions and mechanisms of sleep remains incomplete, reflecting their increasingly evident complexity1–3. Likewise, studies of interhemispheric coordination during sleep4–6 are often hard to connect precisely to known sleep circuits and mechanisms. Here, by recording from the claustra of sleeping bearded dragons (Pogona vitticeps), we show that, although the onsets and offsets of Pogona rapid-eye-movement (REMP) and slow-wave sleep are coordinated bilaterally, these two sleep states differ markedly in their inter-claustral coordination. During slow-wave sleep, the claustra produce sharp-wave ripples independently of one another, showing no coordination. By contrast, during REMP sleep, the potentials produced by the two claustra are precisely coordinated in amplitude and time. These signals, however, are not synchronous: one side leads the other by about 20 ms, with the leading side switching typically once per REMP episode or in between successive episodes. The leading claustrum expresses the stronger activity, suggesting bilateral competition. This competition does not occur directly between the two claustra or telencephalic hemispheres. Rather, it occurs in the midbrain and depends on the integrity of a GABAergic (γ-aminobutyric-acid-producing) nucleus of the isthmic complex, which exists in all vertebrates and is known in birds to underlie bottom-up attention and gaze control. These results reveal that a winner-take-all-type competition exists between the two sides of the brain of Pogona, which originates in the midbrain and has precise consequences for claustrum activity and coordination during REMP sleep.
Subject terms: Circadian rhythms and sleep, REM sleep
Electrophysiological recordings in sleeping bearded dragons (Pogona vitticeps) reveal a type of interhemispheric competition that is detected in the claustrum but generated in the midbrain, and only during rapid-eye-movement sleep.
Main
In mammals, cortical electroencephalograms during sleep can be decomposed into rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep, which is characterized by desynchronized electroencephalographic signals accompanied by rapid eye movements7–9, and non-REM (NREM) sleep, which is characterized by slow-wave activity (and hence is also called slow-wave (SW) sleep). In rodents and humans, NREM sleep has been implicated in the reactivation and consolidation of certain forms of memories10–13. The possible functions of REM, however, remain largely speculative (for example, unlearning14), although some evidence15–17 suggests a possible link with emotional memory. Biphasic sleep also exists in birds18, reptiles19–21 and fish22, suggesting the possibility of common roots (at least as old as the vertebrate lineage (500 million years)). (Biphasic sleep patterns have also been observed in some invertebrates23). If so, comparative approaches in systems that represent diverse animal lineages might help us to better understand not only the evolution of sleep, but also its functions and mechanistic underpinnings.
In Pogona vitticeps, a diurnal agamid lizard, the two phases of a regular electrophysiological sleep rhythm (as recorded in the dorsal-ventricular ridge or DVR) consist of local field potentials dominated by sharp-wave ripples that occur irregularly every 0.5–2 s for about a minute, followed by faster awake-like activity that co-occurs with rapid eye movements19, also for about a minute. These two activity modes alternate regularly throughout the night19. Because of the similarities between these two electrophysiological sleep phases and mammalian SW and REM sleep, we identify them as SW-like and REM-like (thereafter respectively named SW and REMP). (Note that our nomenclature is descriptive, and does not necessarily imply, for lack of knowledge at this point, functional or mechanistic identity with mammalian sleep states). The dominant features of SW activity in the DVR—the sharp-wave ripples—are produced in the claustrum, and are detectable in the adjacent DVR as a propagating wave21. Here we set out to examine the nature of REMP signals in Pogona, using electrophysiological recordings in vivo. Recording from the left and right claustra simultaneously, we observed differences in the nature of interhemispheric coordination between SW and REMP sleep. These differences, in turn, revealed an ongoing competition between the two hemispheres during REMP sleep—but not during SW sleep—and identified a role for a pair of midbrain pre-isthmic nuclei, which were until now not known to be involved in sleep.
Claustrum activity during REMP sleep
Recordings were made from the claustrum (mostly laterally) (Fig. 1a) or the adjacent anterior DVR, using silicon probes (Methods). During sleep, local field potential (LFP) activity alternated regularly between SW and REMP episodes (Fig. 1b, top), with each full sleep cycle lasting 1.5–2.5 min. SW sleep was characterized by the irregular generation of sharp-wave ripples, about once per second on average, as described previously19,21.
REMP activity in the claustrum consisted mainly of sharp downward (negative) extracellular potentials, occurring at irregular intervals (Fig. 1b, bottom left). Those events (which we call SNs, for sharp and negative) were quite consistent in shape but variable in both amplitude and inter-event interval (IEI; Fig. 1b, bottom). IEIs, time-to-trough and amplitude distributions over about 190,000 events are shown in Fig. 1c for one lizard (from 9 h of one night). The IEI distribution was skewed with a mode at about 40 ms (median: 60.2 ms; [25th, 75th] percentiles: [39.8, 110.5] ms). Previous results19,21 showed that REMP (in the DVR and claustrum) is dominated by LFP power in the 20-Hz (beta) band. The instantaneous power in the beta band (measured in a scrolling 10-s window) and the SN rate (Fig. 1d) were indeed well correlated, consistent with the mode of the IEI distribution. REMP activity in the claustrum is thus dominated by SNs and their interval statistics (3 animals, 9 nights; Extended Data Fig. 1a–d).
Single units isolated from claustrum recordings (Fig. 1e, left) typically fired 0–2 action potentials per SN, aligned to the SN’s descending phase. Given the short duration of that phase, the action potentials of different units occurred within a few milliseconds of one another at most (Fig. 1e and Extended Data Fig. 1e). SN waveforms are therefore extracellular potentials that reflect a net-depolarizing current in claustrum neurons; this, in turn, probably underlies the synchronized firing of the depolarized units. We will show that this phasic field potential must result from input to the claustrum, and not solely from intrinsic and coordinated properties of claustral units themselves.
Bilateral coordination during sleep
We next recorded simultaneously from the left and the right claustra to examine the bilateral coordination of sleep-related activities. (Whereas a corpus callosum exists only in placental mammals, reptiles have several forebrain commissures24.) The regular cycling of SW and REMP was precisely synchronized across the two sides (Fig. 2a and Extended Data Fig. 2a), but the fine coordination of the two claustra differed greatly between the two phases. During SW, the sharp-wave ripples—characteristic of this phase—were not synchronized bilaterally, and their amplitudes did not covary (Fig. 2b and Extended Data Fig. 2b,c), consistent with the independent generation of sharp-wave ripples in each claustrum21 and the absence of reported contralateral projections between claustra in mammals25 and in Pogona21.
During REMP, however, SNs in each claustrum were precisely mirrored (in timing and amplitude) on the opposite side and in sweeps from one claustrum triggered on SNs produced in the other (Fig. 2c). Over hundreds of thousands of SNs analysed, this tight correspondence between left and right sides was observed in 85 to 90% of the clearly detectable events (95–100 percentiles of SN amplitudes).
Although precisely coordinated, SNs from either side were not simultaneous, but offset by a delay of about 20 ms (median: 19.3 ms; [25th, 75th] percentiles: [15.4, 24.5] ms) with one side or the other leading (Fig. 2c). This delay was consistent throughout the night, across nights and across lizards (12 nights, 7 animals; Fig. 2d). SNs on both sides showed correlated amplitude variations (Fig. 2c), suggesting a common cause. The leading side would switch from one side to the other (Fig. 2c,d), but typically not on an event-by-event basis, as shown in a scrolling cross-correlation between left and right LFPs (Fig. 2e): switches occurred between REMP episodes (as here between the first and second), or once (occasionally twice) within single REMP episodes (as here during the second, fourth or sixth). The full sleep period from which this segment is shown is in Extended Data Fig. 3a.
The leading side at a given moment could usually be predicted by comparing the left- and right-claustrum LFPs: the side with dominant beta-band power tended to lead. In the second, fourth and sixth REMP episodes (Fig. 2e), for example, the crossing of the power curves corresponds to a leading-side switch. In the first and seventh, by contrast, one side dominated and led throughout. This correspondence between leadership and LFP power dominance was corroborated by an analysis of individual SN pairs: the SN with the larger amplitude generally led its contralateral counterpart (Fig. 2f).
The combination of matching bilateral pairs of waveforms, fixed positive or negative lags with the same absolute value and temporal lead of the stronger side suggested the possibility of competition between the two claustra, in which the stronger side at a given moment imposes its output on the weaker one, with a delay. By this hypothesis, leadership would depend on instantaneous variations of the relative ‘strengths’ of activity on the two sides, and the delay could be due to signal propagation and synaptic transfer in mirror-symmetric circuits. The suppression of the weaker side’s own output—and its replacement by the stronger side’s—suggested a winner-take-all type of bilateral competition.
Interhemispheric competition during REM
To understand the nature of this hypothetical competition, we examined the transitions between leading sides. Two successive REMP episodes are shown in Fig. 3a, together with three short segments of the left and right LFP traces from the second episode: in the blue frame, left power dominates and the SNs on the left lead; in the red frame, the converse happens; in the yellow frame, leadership is not settled. The yellow segment corresponds precisely to the time when the left and right beta-band powers are balanced (top traces).
By scoring an instantaneous-leadership tendency of one side versus the other (Fig. 3b and Methods) and applying it to nine hours of sleep (around 208,000 SNs), we reveal high-density domains that correspond to identifiable states: in grey is SW, when the two sides are not correlated (see Fig. 2b); in red and blue are states in which one side leads the other (right or left, respectively); and in yellow is a state in which dominance switches (‘competition’). The trajectory shown in black (Fig. 3b) corresponds to the first REMP episode in Fig. 3a; it remains on the red side from one SW state to the next. The cyan trajectory corresponds to the second REMP episode, in which dominance starts on the left side, enters the unsettled state (yellow) and finishes on the right side before moving back to SW. Over all recordings, the competitive (yellow) state was the least common of the three REMP states—for example, over one night’s 210 REMP episodes, plotted in Fig. 3c, the average REMP episode consisted of 22 s with left dominating, 26 s with right dominating and 4 s as unsettled. When one side dominated, it tended to do so over many successive SNs. Thus, if the left and right sides do compete for dominance, they must do so with dynamics slower than individual SNs, suggesting that the drive causing left–right competition has different mechanisms than those that underlie the production of SNs.
The fraction of REMP time spent in each one of these three states (left dominating, right dominating or competition) is plotted for one night (Fig. 3c). This plot suggests that nearby REMP episodes are not entirely independent of one another; a temporal correlation exists such that the fraction of time that either side dominated waxed and waned throughout the night (Extended Data Fig. 4). Moreover, if one side led for the longest in a REMP cycle, it tended to do so over many successive sleep cycles (sometimes more than ten) in the last two to three hours of the night (Fig. 3d). This long-lasting side dominance at the end of the night thus indicates the existence of a slow competitive process between the two sides of the brain, on a timescale longer than that of the sleep cycle, and increasingly long as the night proceeds.
The great majority of REMP episodes contained either 0 or 1 dominance switch (Fig. 3e). REMP episodes with no switch lasted about 60 s (mean, 57 s; s.d., 16.6 s; n = 59), against 90 s (mean, 88.5 s; s.d., 19.6 s; n = 103) for those with one switch. In REMP episodes with one switch, the durations of each sub-period of dominance (left-dominant and right-dominant) self-adjusted such that the duration of the REMP period remained more or less constant (diagonal trend for black points, Fig. 3f).
A midbrain nucleus active during REMP
Guided by retrograde tracing from the claustrum, we searched for circuits that might underlie the left–right claustrum competition during REMP. We found no evidence for direct connections between the two claustra, and despite anatomical evidence for ipsi- and contralateral projections from the amygdala and cortex to the claustrum (Extended Data Fig. 5a,b), lesions in or ablation of either had no effect on the bilateral switching of REMP claustrum activities (Extended Data Fig. 5c–f). We next examined areas at the mid-to-hindbrain junction, given their known involvement in REMP control in mammals1,3,17. Recordings were obtained after traversing part of the optic tectum (green; Fig. 4a). We detected, from mesencephalic recording sites ventral to the caudal end of the optic tectum (Fig. 4b–e), REMP sleep activity that matched precisely activity recorded in the ipsilateral claustrum. These recordings were from a nucleus that contained large, sparsely distributed GABAergic (Vgat+) and parvalbumin-positive (Pvalb+) somata at the edge of the tegmentum, next to a tecto-thalamic fibre tract (Fig. 4b–d and Extended Data Fig. 6). We suggest that this is likely to be the homologue of the nucleus isthmi pars magnocellularis (Imc) in avians, in which it has been intensively studied26–29.
Simultaneous recordings were made from both claustra (only the left shown) and the left Imc (Fig. 4e and Extended Data Fig. 7a–c). We noted a marked correspondence between the two (Fig. 4e–g); SNs characteristic of REMP in the claustrum aligned precisely with the Imc waveforms, which themselves corresponded to brisk firing bursts of Imc neurons (Fig. 4h). Imc field potentials led those in the claustrum by 30 ms ipsilaterally (median: −30 ms; [25th, 75th] percentiles: [−33.0, −27.0] ms) or 50 ms contralaterally (median: −45 ms; [25th, 75th] percentiles: [−51, −26] ms) (Fig. 4g,i and Extended Data Fig. 7b,c), a difference consistent with the 20 ms delay between the left and the right claustra (Fig. 2). This suggested that each claustrum receives excitatory synaptic drive coupled to Imc activity in the midbrain, and that the apparent competition between claustra (Figs. 2 and 3) might instead be the expression of bilateral competition in the midbrain.
Indeed, although the Imc was clearly active during REMP, its activity on a given side was strongest when the ipsilateral claustrum was also dominant (that is, phase-leading) (Fig. 4j). This suggested that the two Imcs compete with one another, and that claustrum dominance patterns reflect the results of Imc competition.
We recorded simultaneously from both Imcs (Fig. 4k and Extended Data Fig. 7d–j); sorted units from each side formed alternating bursts (two transitions in this REMP cycle; Fig. 4k; see also Extended Data Fig. 7g), the timing of which matched power changes in the claustrum. As predicted, the instantaneous power in the claustrum increased when the ipsilateral Imc was dominant. These results suggest that inter-claustral competition in fact results from bilateral competition within the midbrain, in which the Imc has a key role.
The Imc is required for bilateral competition
To test this hypothesis, we lesioned the Imc unilaterally using ibotenic acid (IBA) (Fig. 5a,b and Extended Data Fig. 8b) (n = 3 animals). The effects of these lesions were clear: the claustrum on the side of the lesioned Imc lost its ability to lead or dominate (Fig. 5c), and all REMP episodes were now dominated by the intact side, contralateral to the lesion, as seen in two hours of one night (Fig. 5c; see full 9 h in Extended Data Fig. 8a) and in the now unimodal claustrum phase-lag distributions (Fig. 5d) (3 animals, 8 nights). Correspondingly, the phase-leading claustrum (contralateral to the Imc lesions) was also the one with dominating beta power (blue; Extended Data Fig. 8a). Notably, lizards with unilateral Imc lesions also exhibited shorter REMP episodes (mean duration in control animals: 65 s, n = 12, versus 33 s in lesioned animals, n = 8; P = 0.000028, t = −6.992; Welch’s two-sided t-test) and an excess of SW, resulting in less time spent in REMP sleep overall (mean REMP duration: 4:40 h over 9 h of sleep in control animals, versus 1:37 h over the same duration in lesioned animals; P = 0.000062, t = −7.204), suggesting that the Imc also has a role in the SW–REMP transition.
Discussion
We have described competitive dynamics between the two sides of the brain, which are detectable in the claustrum during REMP but not during SW sleep. Claustrum activity during REMP sleep consisted of very brief synchronized firing bursts coincident with sharp negative LFP events (SNs). Contrary to sharp-wave ripples during SW sleep, SNs were tightly correlated across the two claustra, with precise time delays and rapid switches of leadership correlated with reversals of amplitude dominance, suggesting a winner-take-all type of competition between the two claustra. This competition, however, did not have a telencephalic origin: the dominance of either claustrum required the integrity of the ipsilateral member of a bilateral pair of GABAergic mesencephalic nuclei (Imc). Hence, the apparent claustral competition during REMP sleep is the expression of competition in the midbrain, transferred bilaterally to the two claustra. The proposed functional circuits that underlie this relationship are schematized in Fig. 5e: in this model, the two sides of the midbrain compete during REMP, and side dominance depends on Imc integrity (Figs. 4 and 5). Each side of the midbrain projects to both claustra (possibly, but not necessarily or exclusively, via the thalamic nucleus rotundus30,31) such that, as one side dominates, left and right claustrum LFPs covary (owing to their common source) but with a time lag (owing to a longer decussating path). The smaller SN amplitude on the contralateral (lagging) side also suggests a lesser gain or reliability of the midline-crossing pathway. During the brief transitions when no midbrain side dominates, the left and right claustra are coactive, which results in disordered SN patterns in both claustra (Fig. 3a).
The Imc is part of a complex of cholinergic, glutamatergic and GABAergic isthmic nuclei that has been studied mainly in birds26,28,29,32 and is implicated in bottom-up attention and gaze control. The Imc, which is embryologically a pre-isthmic nucleus33, mediates a type of broad lateral inhibition of excitatory neurons in both the ipsilateral optic tectum and the nucleus isthmi pars parvocellularis (Ipc)—a companion cholinergic–glutamatergic nucleus that forms a positive feedback loop with the ipsilateral tectum27,29,31,32,34. When two stimuli fall onto one retina, these circuits undergo a winner-take-all-type competitive interaction such that responses to the stronger stimulus are selected, and attention and gaze are directed towards it, rather than to a weighted mean of the two stimuli. The Imc, by virtue of its heterotypic connectivity with the optic tectum and the Ipc, underlies this competition27,28. This work was at first concerned with the selection of competing visual stimuli falling on the same retina, but experiments have now established that competition also takes place between contralateral stimuli, and even between sensory modalities35, although the underlying circuits are not well understood. Likely homologues of these avian (reptilian) isthmic nuclei are found in fish36–38, amphibians39 and non-avian reptiles40,41, as well as in mammals (parabigeminal nucleus), in which a role in visual attention has been hypothesized42.
The isthmo–claustral connection uncovered here is notable in part because some of the claustrum’s hypothesized functions are attentional43,44. But our results raise many new questions. First, what pathways underlie the bilateral mesencephalic competition (Fig. 5e)? Although the Imc is inhibitory (Fig. 4c) and necessary for this competition (Fig. 5), we are not aware of contralateral Imc projections. Second, what pathways link Imc, a GABAergic nucleus, to the activation of both claustra? The high correlation between isthmic and claustral LFPs during REMP suggests high-gain excitatory pathways between those areas, or drive from a common source, the dominance of which depends on the integrity of the ipsilateral Imc. Third, what is the functional importance of the tight correlation and ±20-ms delays between the claustra during REMP? Although there is no evidence for direct projections between the claustra, bilateral claustral projections to the telencephalon exist25,44, such that convergence on common targets is plausible. If so, the delays might, in conjunction with the activation of spike-timing-dependent plasticity rules, have a role in synaptic homeostasis16,45,46 or plasticity during REMP. These hypotheses are especially intriguing when considering that the temporal profiles of dominance are different early and late in the night (Fig. 3), and that sharp-wave ripples are uncorrelated bilaterally during SW (Fig. 2 and Extended Data Fig. 2). Fourth, several species of birds and marine mammals show unilateral sleep47,48. Could the same mesencephalic circuits underlie this (slower) alternation? Fifth, side dominance is temporary and balanced between the two sides over each night. What is the cause of the recurrent transfer of side dominance (Fig. 3), presumably located upstream of the Imc? Because the duration of one-sided dominance varies between a fraction of one sleep cycle and many cycles, it probably does not originate in the circuits that cause SW–REMP alternation. Sixth, what role does the isthmic complex have in SW–REMP transitions? Imc lesions revealed a decrease in REMP and a concurrent increase in SW sleep, indicating an interaction with circuits that control the ultradian sleep cycle. Finally, how are activities between the isthmi and the claustra coordinated in the awake state, given that brain activity during REMP sleep most resembles that in the awake state9,17?
Hence, the claustrum—a well-described44,49 but functionally poorly understood area of the telencephalon—seems to have a role in SW sleep in reptiles and mammals21,50, and reflects competitive brain dynamics during REMP sleep (this paper), with origins in the midbrain. Antiphase activity between the two hemispheres has been identified in running rodents and during REM with fast rhythms (140-Hz ‘splines’) in superficial retrosplenial cortex6, but the mechanisms that underlie this relationship are unknown. The relative simplicity of the reptilian model may thus help to reveal not only some mechanisms of sleep, but also some of the functions of the claustrum and of the midbrain in sleep.
Finally, our data emphasize the value of investigating non-mammalian sleep. Comparing stages of sleep (for example, SW and REM), electrophysiological waveforms (for example, SWRs) or even brain areas (for example, the claustrum) across distant species can be difficult; indeed, the terminology of sleep originated with data from humans and cats, the phenomena to be compared are usually multiparametric and sleep is polymorphic even among mammals. Matches across species are thus often only partial. But by comparing data using exhaustive and increasingly precise approaches, hypotheses about structural homologies can now be tested more accurately, at least among vertebrates. This should in turn help us to determine which phenotypic and mechanistic attributes of sleep are common across species, how they came to diverge or converge, and eventually whether they subserve similar functions.
Methods
Animals
Lizards (P. vitticeps) of either sex, weighing 150–250 g, were obtained from our institute colony and selected for size, weight and health status. The lizards were housed in our state-of-the-art animal facility. All experimental procedures were approved by the relevant animal welfare authority (Regierungspräsidium Darmstadt, Germany) and conducted following the strict federal guidelines for the use and care of laboratory animals (permit numbers V54-19c20/15-F126/1005_1011 and 2006).
Lizard surgery for chronic recordings
Twenty-four hours before surgery, lizards were administered analgesics (butorphanol, 0.5 mg kg−1 subcutaneously; meloxicam, 0.2 mg kg−1 subcutaneously) and antibiotics (marbofloxacin, marbocyl, 2 mg kg−1). On the day of surgery, anaesthesia was initiated with 5% isoflurane, and maintained with isoflurane (1–4 vol%) after intubation. The lizards were placed in a stereotactic apparatus after ensuring deep anaesthesia (absence of corneal reflex). Body temperature during surgery was maintained at 30 °C using a heating pad and an oesophageal temperature probe. Heart rate was continuously monitored using a Doppler flow detector. The skin covering the skull was disinfected using 10% povidone-iodine solution (Betai-NE) before removal with a scalpel. A cranial window was made to reach the various regions of interest; the dura and arachnoid were then removed with fine forceps and scissors. The pia was carefully removed over the area of electrode insertion (dorsal cortex for claustrum and optic tectum for Imc recordings). The exposed skull was covered with a layer of ultraviolet (UV)-hardening glue, and the stripped ends of insulated stainless steel wires were secured in place subdurally with UV-hardening glue, to serve as reference and ground.
Silicon probes were mounted on a Nanodrive (Cambridge Neurotech) and secured to a stereotactic adaptor for insertion. On the day after the surgery, probes were slowly lowered into the tissue (claustrum: 0.8–1.2 mm; Imc: 2.5–3.0 mm). For Imc recordings, the probe was advanced in small steps over two to three days until the signal began to show the area-typical signature.
The brain was covered with Duragel (Cambridge Neurotech) followed by Vaseline, and the probes were secured with UV-hardening glue. After surgery, lizards were released from the stereotactic apparatus and left on a heating pad set to 30 °C until full recovery from anaesthesia.
In vivo electrophysiology
Two to three days before surgery, lizards were habituated to a sleep arena, which was itself placed in a 3 × 3 × 3-m electromagnetic-shielded room. One hour before lights off (toff = 18:00/19:00, winter/summer), lizards were placed into the arena and left to sleep and behave naturally overnight. They were returned to their home terrarium 3 to 4 hours after lights on (ton = 06:00/07:00, winter/summer). The lizards then received food and water. Experiments were performed at a constant room temperature of around 21.5 °C.
Electrodes were either 32-channel silicon probes (NeuroNexus; 50-μm pitch, 177-μm2 surface area for each site; in 2 rows of 16 contacts), or Neuropixels 1.0 probes51. Recordings using 32-channel probes were performed with a Cheetah Digital Lynx SX system and HS-36 headstages as previously described21. Signals were sampled at 32 kHz, and IronClust with manual curation was used for spike sorting (https://github.com/flatironinstitute/ironclust#readme). Neuropixels data were acquired nominally at 30 kHz using SpikeGLX software (http://billkarsh.github.io/SpikeGLX/). The true sampling frequency varied slightly with different headstages and could drift throughout a recording. Data acquired from multiple probes were synchronized by linear interpolation, using as a reference a common 1-Hz squarewave signal recorded with all probes.
Action potentials were sorted using Kilosort2 (ref. 52; https://github.com/MouseLand/Kilosort) and the ecephys pipeline (https://github.com/jenniferColonell/ecephys_spike_sorting); clusters were curated manually in Phy (https://github.com/cortex-lab/phy).
Lesion experiments
In preparation for Imc lesion experiments, we carefully removed the pia overlaying the optic tectum and inserted a bevelled quartz micropipette at an angle of 6–8° from the vertical axis to depths of 2,700–2,900 μm. IBA (200–350 nl; 10 μg μl−1 in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS), pH 7.2) was injected at a rate of 50–100 nl min−1 (UMP3, World Precision Instruments). The injection pipette was retracted 3–5 min after the end of injection.
The cortical sheets of Pogona overlay the lateral ventricles and can be removed using surgical scissors. This operation was preceded by removing the overlaying pia and vasculature, staunching bleeding using forceps. For surgical lesions of the amygdala, we first removed the cortical sheet and then the amygdala (together with caudal parts of the DVR), using fine forceps and scissors.
Silicon probes were positioned bilaterally above the two claustra and recordings were performed each night from one to six days after surgery. The effects of lesioning Imc could be observed 24 h after surgery and were stable thereafter. By the end of the experiment, the lizard was euthanized and its brain sectioned and Nissl-stained for histological confirmation of the lesion.
Cross-correlations
To compute lagged cross-correlations between 2 channels (for example, Fig. 2e) the signals were first downsampled to 1 kHz, low-pass-filtered to 40 Hz and z-scored. Lagged cross-correlations were computed on the first derivative of the resulting time series with a sliding window of 10 s scrolled in 100-ms steps. Cross-correlations were normalized to the value of their 99.9% percentile.
To estimate the lags that best represent the relationship between two signals, we extracted distributions of peak-correlation lags (for example, Fig. 2d). We took, for every time point in the lagged cross-correlation, the lag corresponding to the maximum value, producing a time series of lags. To isolate periods of high correlation, we kept only lags corresponding to correlation values in the top 75% percentile. To avoid boundary artefacts resulting from a finite exploration of lags, we removed those at the extremes of the range (for example, −51 ms and +51 ms in Fig. 2d).
To score the instantaneous-leadership between 2 channels, we extracted their cross-correlation using the lags corresponding to the modes of the distribution of peak-correlation lags (−20 ms and +20 ms in Fig. 3b). Before extracting the cross-correlation, we processed the signals as described above and we then clipped their first derivative to be less than or equal to zero. Note that the resulting cross-correlation must be greater than or equal to zero. We then took the base-10 logarithm of the resulting cross-correlation values
where w is the 10-s sliding window and c0 and c1 are the first derivatives of the 2 channels as described above. To define periods of dominance (Fig. 3), we selected linear thresholds of the −20 ms score and the +20 ms score manually. To avoid over-fragmentation resulting from the noisy crossing of these thresholds, we ignored detours that left and re-entered a single state for a short duration (less than 3 s).
REMP detection
To define periods of REMP and SW sleep, we extracted the power of the LFP signal in the beta band (12–30 Hz) with a 10-s sliding window at 1-s steps. We considered periods with a beta power above its 15th percentile as REMP sleep periods. To avoid over-fragmentation resulting from the noisy crossing of this threshold, we ignored detours that left and re-entered the same state for a short duration (less than 15 s).
SN detection and matching
To detect the SNs typical of claustrum REMP activity, we first low-pass-filtered the signal to 40 Hz and extracted its first and second derivatives. We then extracted peaks in the resulting time series. We considered a potential SN as a triplet of peaks: a negative second derivative peak followed by a negative first derivative peak and a positive second derivative peak. The first and last peaks correspond to the beginning and end of the downward phase of an SN; we used them to calculate amplitude and duration (Fig. 1b,c). To remove false positives from this set of potential SNs, we estimated the distribution of the noise and took only those SNs with a low probability (P < 0.025) on the corresponding cumulative distribution function (CDF) of negative amplitude and duration. To estimate the distribution of noise—that is, of small LFP deflections wrongly identified by our method as SNs—we multiplied the signal by −1 and repeated the same process of triplet peak detection. This was equivalent to attempting to detect sharp positive deflections, which did not exist in the signal. Consequently, any positive events detected by our triplet peak detection were the result of LFP noise. We then used those wrongly identified positive events to establish minimum thresholds of amplitude and duration on the originally detected SNs.
To match bilateral pairs of potential SNs, we scored each pair by the value of the lagged cross-correlation at their corresponding time and lag. The lagged cross-correlation was computed with a window of 100 ms (see ‘Cross-correlations’) and multiplied by an exponential kernel with a time constant of 10 ms. We then algorithmically detected the optimal combination of potential SN pairs that maximized the total sum of this score. Note that an SN could have only one match and that some could be left without a matching partner. Pairs of potential SNs were evaluated against the noise CDF and were accepted if at least one of the two potential SNs had a low null probability (P < 0.05).
The SN detection and matching process was performed in consecutive sections of 1 h duration.
Tract tracing
For ex vivo tracing experiments (Extended Data Fig. 6), lizards were deeply anaesthetized with isoflurane, ketamine (60 mg kg−1) and midazolam (2 mg kg−1). After loss of the corneal reflex, the lizards were decapitated and their heads immediately submerged in ice-cooled artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) (126 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 1.8 mM CaCl2, 1.2 mM MgCl2, 24 mM NaHCO3, 0.72 mM NaH2PO4 and 20 mM glucose). We then perfused the brains with cooled aCSF, extracted them and carefully removed the pia above injection sites. Neurobiotin (5–10% dissolved in phosphate buffer) was delivered through glass micropipettes and iontophoresis, applying 5-µA current pulses (5 s on, 5 s off) for 2–10 min. After injection, the brains were kept submerged in aCSF at room temperature for 15–20 h to allow for transport of the tracer, before being transferred to 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS for 24–48 h at 4 °C. After fixation, the brains were immersed in 30% sucrose for at least 48 h at 4 °C. Transverse or horizontal sections were obtained using a cryostat, at a thickness of 70 μm, and neurobiotin was detected with streptavidin, Alexa Fluor 568.
Statistics
Statistical tests were performed using the standard Python package scipy (v.1.6.2). P values equal to zero indicate values too small (<5 × 10−324) to be computed with a standard float64 and result from the large number of samples in our recordings.
Data reporting
No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample size. The experiments were not randomized and the investigators were not blinded to allocation during experiments and outcome assessment.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Online content
Any methods, additional references, Nature Portfolio reporting summaries, source data, extended data, supplementary information, acknowledgements, peer review information; details of author contributions and competing interests; and statements of data and code availability are available at 10.1038/s41586-023-05827-w.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank E. Northrup, N. Vogt and G. Wexel for veterinary care; E. Joesten, T. Klappich, M. Lange and M. de Vries for reptile care; A. Arends, M. Klinkmann, J. Knop, A. Macias Pardo and C. Thum for technical assistance; T. Gallego-Flores for contributing in situ stains; S. Weiss for help in setting up Neuropixels recordings; L. Puelles for feedback on the identity and mesencephalic origin of the Imc; L. Faraggiana, J. Gjorgjieva and H. Norimoto for discussions; and D. Evans and H. Ito for their comments on the manuscript. This work was funded by the Max Planck Society (G.L.), the European Research Council under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement no. 834446) (G.L.) and the DFG (CRC1080) (G.L.). L.A.F. was supported by an EMBO long-term fellowship (ALTF 421-2017).
Extended data figures and tables
Author contributions
L.A.F. and G.L. designed the project. L.A.F. performed the experiments. All authors discussed and interpreted the results. J.L.R. and L.A.F. analysed the data and prepared the figures. G.L. wrote the manuscript, with contributions from J.L.R. and L.A.F., and supervised the project.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature thanks Paul-Antoine Libourel and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Max Planck Society.
Data availability
Data will be made available upon reasonable request.
Code availability
Analysis code is available at: https://brain.mpg.de/research/laurent-department/software-techniques.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Lorenz A. Fenk, Email: lorenz.fenk@brain.mpg.de
Gilles Laurent, Email: gilles.laurent@brain.mpg.de.
Extended data
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41586-023-05827-w.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41586-023-05827-w.
References
- 1.Weber F, Dan Y. Circuit-based interrogation of sleep control. Nature. 2016;538:51–59. doi: 10.1038/nature19773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Scammell TE, Arrigoni E, Lipton JO. Neural circuitry of wakefulness and sleep. Neuron. 2017;93:747–765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Saper CB, Fuller PM. Wake-sleep circuitry: an overview. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2017;44:186–192. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Simor P, Gombos F, Blaskovich B, Bódizs R. Long-range alpha and beta and short-range gamma EEG synchronization distinguishes phasic and tonic REM periods. Sleep. 2017;41:zsx210. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Imbach LL, et al. Inter-hemispheric oscillations in human sleep. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e48660. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ghosh M, et al. Running speed and REM sleep control two distinct modes of rapid interhemispheric communication. Cell Rep. 2022;40:111028. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Aserinsky E, Kleitman N. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science. 1953;118:273–274. doi: 10.1126/science.118.3062.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Dement W. The occurrence of low voltage, fast, electroencephalogram patterns during behavioral sleep in the cat. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1958;10:291–296. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(58)90037-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jouvet M, Michel F, Courjon J. On a stage of rapid cerebral electrical activity in the course of physiological sleep [French] C. R. Seances Soc. Biol. Fil. 1959;153:1024–1028. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. 1994;265:676–679. doi: 10.1126/science.8036517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rasch B, Buchel C, Gais S, Born J. Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science. 2007;315:1426–1429. doi: 10.1126/science.1138581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Girardeau G, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Buzsaki G, Zugaro MB. Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:1222–1223. doi: 10.1038/nn.2384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ego-Stengel V, Wilson MA. Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat. Hippocampus. 2010;20:1–10. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Crick F, Mitchison G. The function of dream sleep. Nature. 1983;304:111–114. doi: 10.1038/304111a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wagner U, Gais S, Born J. Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learn. Mem. 2001;8:112–119. doi: 10.1101/lm.36801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Klinzing JG, Niethard N, Born J. Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 2019;22:1598–1610. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0467-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Peever J, Fuller PM. The biology of REM sleep. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:R1237–R1248. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ookawa T, Gotoh J. Electroencephalographs study of chickens: periodic recurrence of low voltage and fast waves during behavioral sleep. Poultry Sci. 1964;43:1603–1604. doi: 10.3382/ps.0431603. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Shein-Idelson M, Ondracek JM, Liaw HP, Reiter S, Laurent G. Slow waves, sharp waves, ripples, and REM in sleeping dragons. Science. 2016;352:590–595. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf3621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Libourel PA, et al. Partial homologies between sleep states in lizards, mammals, and birds suggest a complex evolution of sleep states in amniotes. PLoS Biol. 2018;16:e2005982. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2005982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Norimoto H, et al. A claustrum in reptiles and its role in slow-wave sleep. Nature. 2020;578:413–418. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-1993-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Leung LC, et al. Neural signatures of sleep in zebrafish. Nature. 2019;571:198–204. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1336-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Frank MG, Waldrop RH, Dumoulin M, Aton S, Boal JG. A preliminary analysis of sleep-like states in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e38125. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Suarez R, Gobius I, Richards LJ. Evolution and development of interhemispheric connections in the vertebrate forebrain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:497. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wang Q, et al. Organization of the connections between claustrum and cortex in the mouse. J. Comp. Neurol. 2017;525:1317–1346. doi: 10.1002/cne.24047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wang Y, Major DE, Karten HJ. Morphology and connections of nucleus isthmi pars magnocellularis in chicks (Gallus gallus) J. Comp. Neurol. 2004;469:275–297. doi: 10.1002/cne.11007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Marin G, et al. A cholinergic gating mechanism controlled by competitive interactions in the optic tectum of the pigeon. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:8112–8121. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1420-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mysore SP, Knudsen EI. A shared inhibitory circuit for both exogenous and endogenous control of stimulus selection. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:473–478. doi: 10.1038/nn.3352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Knudsen EI. Neural circuits that mediate selective attention: a comparative perspective. Trends Neurosci. 2018;41:789–805. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Guirado S, Real MA, Davila JC. The ascending tectofugal visual system in amniotes: new insights. Brain Res. Bull. 2005;66:290–296. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gruberg E, et al. Influencing and interpreting visual input: the role of a visual feedback system. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:10368–10371. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3288-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Marin G, Mpodozis J, Sentis E, Ossandon T, Letelier JC. Oscillatory bursts in the optic tectum of birds represent re-entrant signals from the nucleus isthmi pars parvocellularis. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:7081–7089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1379-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hidalgo-Sanchez M, Martinez-de-la-Torre M, Alvarado-Mallart RM, Puelles L. A distinct preisthmic histogenetic domain is defined by overlap of Otx2 and Pax2 gene expression in the avian caudal midbrain. J. Comp. Neurol. 2005;483:17–29. doi: 10.1002/cne.20402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Asadollahi A, Knudsen EI. Spatially precise visual gain control mediated by a cholinergic circuit in the midbrain attention network. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:13472. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schryver HM, Mysore SP. Spatial dependence of stimulus competition in the avian nucleus isthmi pars magnocellularis. Brain Behav. Evol. 2019;93:137–151. doi: 10.1159/000500192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Zottoli SJ, Rhodes KJ, Corrodi JG, Mufson EJ. Putative cholinergic projections from the nucleus isthmi and the nucleus reticularis mesencephali to the optic tectum in the goldfish (Carassius auratus) J. Comp. Neurol. 1988;273:385–398. doi: 10.1002/cne.902730309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Henriques PM, Rahman N, Jackson SE, Bianco IH. Nucleus isthmi is required to sustain target pursuit during visually guided prey-catching. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:1771–1786. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Fernandes AM, et al. Neural circuitry for stimulus selection in the zebrafish visual system. Neuron. 2021;109:805–822. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gruberg ER, Udin SB. Topographic projections between the nucleus isthmi and the tectum of the frog Rana pipiens. J. Comp. Neurol. 1978;179:487–500. doi: 10.1002/cne.901790303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sereno MI, Ulinski PS. Caudal topographic nucleus isthmi and the rostral nontopographic nucleus isthmi in the turtle, Pseudemys scripta. J. Comp. Neurol. 1987;261:319–346. doi: 10.1002/cne.902610302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Belekhova MG, Kenigfest NB. Turtle isthmic complex of visual nuclei: immunohistochemistry of gamma-aminobutyric acid, choline acetyltransferase, calcium-binding proteins and histochemistry of cytochrome oxidase activity. J. Evol. Biochem. Phys. 2014;50:435–447. doi: 10.1134/S0022093014050081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Graybiel AM. A satellite system of the superior colliculus: the parabigeminal nucleus and its projections to the superficial collicular layers. Brain Res. 1978;145:365–374. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90870-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Goll Y, Atlan G, Citri A. Attention: the claustrum. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38:486–495. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jackson J, Smith JB, Lee AK. The anatomy and physiology of claustrum–cortex interactions. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2020;43:231–247. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-092519-101637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Abbott LF, Nelson SB. Synaptic plasticity: taming the beast. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:1178–1183. doi: 10.1038/81453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron. 2014;81:12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Rattenborg NC, Amlaner CJ, Lima SL. Behavioral, neurophysiological and evolutionary perspectives on unihemispheric sleep. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2000;24:817–842. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00039-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mascetti GG. Unihemispheric sleep and asymmetrical sleep: behavioral, neurophysiological, and functional perspectives. Nat. Sci. Sleep. 2016;8:221–238. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S71970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Crick FC, Koch C. What is the function of the claustrum? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2005;360:1271–1279. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Narikiyo K, et al. The claustrum coordinates cortical slow-wave activity. Nat. Neurosci. 2020;23:741–753. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0625-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Jun JJ, et al. Fully integrated silicon probes for high-density recording of neural activity. Nature. 2017;551:232–236. doi: 10.1038/nature24636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Stringer C, et al. Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science. 2019;364:255. doi: 10.1126/science.aav7893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ramón y Cajal, S. Histologie du Système Nerveux de l’Homme & des Vertébrés [French] Vol. 2 (Maloine, 1911).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data will be made available upon reasonable request.
Analysis code is available at: https://brain.mpg.de/research/laurent-department/software-techniques.