Abstract
Objective/Background:
Co-sleeping is common practice around the globe. The relationship between early childhood co-sleeping and adolescent behavior problems remains uncertain. We aim to identify whether early childhood co-sleeping can predict behavior problems in preadolescence.
Participants:
A cohort of 1,656 Chinese preschool children were followed up in adolescence.
Methods:
Prospective cohort study design involving two waves of data collection from the China Jintan Cohort (1,656 children aged 3–5 years). Cosleeping history was collected at 3-5-years-old via parent-reported questionnaire at wave I data collection. Behavior problems were measured twice in childhood and preadolescence, respectively. Adolescent behavior problems were measured by integrating data from self-report, parent-report and teacher-report using the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment. Predictions were assessed using the general linear model with mixed effects on the inverse probability weight propensity-matched sample.
Results:
1,656 children comprising 55.6% boys aged 4.9 ± 0.6 were initially enrolled in the first wave of data collection. In the second wave of data collection, 1,274 children were 10.99 ± 0.74 (76.9%) aged 10–13 years were retained. Early childhood co-sleeping is significantly associated with increased behavior problems in childhood (Odds Ratio [OR] 1.22–2.06, ps<0.03) and preadolescence (OR 1.40–2.27, ps<0.02). Moreover, cosleeping history significantly predicted multiscale increase in internal (OR 1.63–2.61, ps<0.02) and external behavior problems in adolescence.
Conclusions:
Early childhood co-sleeping is associated with multiple behavioral problems reported by parents, teachers, and children themselves. Early childhood co-sleeping predicts preadolescent internalizing and externalizing behavior after controlling for baseline behavior problems.
Introduction
Co-sleeping, including bed-sharing and room-sharing between children and caregivers, is practiced around the world (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2017). Western societies generally have lower average co-sleeping rates, ranging from 8.6% in Australia (Mindell et al., 2010) to 39% in Denmark (Nelson et al., 2001), compared to that of Asian and African societies, ranging from 68% in Thailand (Anuntaseree et al., 2008) to 100% in Mozambique (Plucinski et al., 2015). While co-sleeping is generally more prevalent in non-Western cultures, the incidence has been increasing in Western countries (Young & Shipstone, 2018). Even within the same society, rural areas have higher rates of co-sleeping arrangements (Gupta et al., 2016) . Regarding children’s age group, co-sleeping prevalence remains steady within the infancy and preschool period and drops as children enter school-age and preadolescence. The general co-sleeping prevalence ranged from 51.6 to 62.8% in infancy (0–12 months), 59.4% at preschool age (3–5 years), and 10–23% in preadolescence (10–12) around the globe (Mindell et al., 2017, 2013; Rath & Okum, 1995).
Childhood co-sleeping practice remains a controversial topic in pediatrics (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2017). While the American Academy of Pediatrics recommends against bed-sharing during infancy (Moon, 2016), pediatricians have differing positions on the topic (Schaeffer & Asnes, 2018). The gap between health guidelines and practice also exists in China. The latest Chinese guidelines for sleep hygiene among children aged 0–5 years recommend that preschool children should sleep in their own bed, and ideally should have solitary sleeping (National Health Commission of the People Republic of China, 2017). Yet co-sleeping in the Chinese family remains common practice, with the prevalence of co-sleeping ranging from 60% in infancy and preschool age to 37.63% in school age and 25.4% in preadolescence (Huang et al., 2016; Li et al., 2009; Z. Liu et al., 2016).
Besides socioeconomic and cultural reasons, another possible reason for the inconsistency between guidelines and practice in sleeping arrangements is that the impact of co-sleeping on children’s health remains unclear. Preexisting studies examining co-sleeping have associated co-sleeping practice with both benefits and detriments. Potential benefits of co-sleeping include prolonged breastfeeding (Ball et al., 2016; Mitchell et al., 2015) and parent–child bonding (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2016), while detriments include sudden infant death syndrome (Carlin & Moon, 2017), poorer sleep quality and more night waking (Huang et al., 2016; X. Liu et al., 2003; Volkovich et al., 2015), internalizing behavior (Santos et al., 2016) and generalized anxiety (Kaymaz et al., 2015).
Currently, research that explores Chinese children’s co-sleeping arrangements mainly employs the cross-sectional design and focuses on its contributing factors and the relationship between co-sleeping and child sleep health, i.e., sleep quality, sleep disturbances, enuresis, etc. (Li et al., 2009; Z. Liu et al., 2016; Ma et al., 2014; Yue et al., 2018). Few studies have explored the long-term consequences of cosleeping on childhood and adolescent behavior. To our knowledge, only four international longitudinal studies have examined the influence of early co-sleeping on delayed manifestation of behavior problems (Barajas et al., 2011; Okami et al., 2002; Santos et al., 2016; Volkovich et al., 2015). The association between co-sleeping and children’s behavior problems remains inconsistent in these studies. Given that the long-term impact of co-sleeping practice on childhood and preadolescent behavior remains unclear, and few studies have explored this topic in China, further research in the Chinese context is warranted.
Using a large community cohort involving both rural and metropolitan areas with two waves of behavior problem follow-up, this current longitudinal study aims to investigate the relationship between co-sleeping during early childhood (3–5 y) and multi-dimensional behavior problems in preadolescence (10–13 y) reported by multiple informants including children themselves, parents, and teachers via well-established instruments (Leung et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2011). Based on previous studies (Gupta et al., 2016), we expect that co-sleeping will be more prevalent in rural areas. We hypothesized co-sleeping to be inversely related to pre-adolescent behavior problems.
Methods
Participants
The current study is derived from an on-going longitudinal project, the China Jintan Child Cohort Study (Liu et al., 2015, 2010). In fall 2004, children aged 3- to 5-years-old from four preschools in Jintan City, Jiangsu Province, China were invited to participate in the study. The four preschools were chosen due to their representativeness of the demographic, geographical, social, and economic profiles of the entire city. A total of 1656 children and their parents were invited. The response rate was 97%, from which 98% agreed to participate. To ensure the quality of the study, two research assistants were employed and overseen by a psychologist to facilitate parents and preschool teachers in the questionnaire filling process.
Data collected from the study was segmented into waves according to subjects’ developmental stage (Wave I: childhood, and Wave II: preadolescence) via collection year. At Wave I data collection, written informed consent for participation was obtained from the parents at initial enrollment. During Wave II data collection, written informed consent from both mothers and teachers and verbal consent from children were obtained. Institutional review board approval was obtained from the ethical committee for research at Jintan Hospital in China and the University of Pennsylvania. Detailed information for the study, including subjects, recruitment, assessments, and procedures are described elsewhere (Liu et al., 2015, 2010, 2011).
Co-sleeping history
Co-sleeping history in childhood was established for each child during Wave I data collection, when the child was 3–5 years old. Parents were asked to answer the following question “How does your child usually (i.e. more than four times per week) sleep”, with four options: alone, with parents, with grandparents, and with other people. Co-sleeping was defined as the child sleeping with parents/grandparents/others. A total of 1,274 children with complete data on co-sleeping history and behavioral assessment were included in statistical analysis. Included children were dichotomized to cosleeping group (n = 1080) and solitary-sleeping group (n = 194).
Behavioral problem assessment
Two waves of child behavior data collection
Children’s behavior problems were assessed in both childhood (at age six) and preadolescence (at age 10 to 13) with the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) (Achenbach, 1997; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000; Ivanova et al., 2011). The original version of ASEBA contains three subscales including the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), the Teacher Report Form (TRF), and the Youth Self-Report (YSR). All the subscales were translated and validated by Liu and colleagues into Chinese with satisfactory psychometric properties (Liu et al., 2011). The internal consistencies of the CBCL, YSR, and TRF in our study were 0.966, 0.913, and 0.905, respectively, indicating good reliability of the three subscales. During Wave I data collection, childhood baseline behavior problems were assessed with the CBCL (n = 1274) and TRF (n = 1274), completed by parents and teachers, respectively (Liu et al., 2011, 2010). During Wave II data collection, behavior problems in adolescence were assessed again using the CBCL (n = 807), TRF (n = 888), and YSR (n = 924) (Liu et al., 2015, 2014).
Scoring of child behavior problems
The Chinese ASEBA was scored in three ways: two broadband-of-factor structure behavior problems (i.e., internalizing and externalizing behavior) and other behavior problems; seven syndromes (i.e. anxiety/depression, withdrawn/depression, emotional reaction, sleep problems, somatic complaints, attention problems, and aggression); and five Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (Fourth Edition) (DSM-IV)-oriented scales (anxiety, affective, oppositional defiant, attention deficit/hyperactivity, and pervasive development) (Liu et al., 2011, 2014). Subscale scores and DSM-oriented scale scores were calculated from reported raw-scores. Then, t-scores (i.e., the ratio of the behavior score’s deviation from the population mean to its standard deviation [SD]) for all raw subscale scores were calculated for analysis. Higher scores indicate more severe behavior problems.
Low cross-informant agreement
Due to reported low cross-informant agreement of the ASEBA subscales (Gomez et al., 2014; Grigorenko et al., 2010; Leung et al., 2006), Cronbach’s alpha was used to assess the agreement of childhood and preadolescent behavior reported across informants (parents, child, and teachers). The behavior problems in our sample reported by the three informants had alpha values ranging from 0.381 (withdrawal/depressed) to 0.535 (attention problems), while the DSM subscale measurements had alpha values ranging from 0.364 (anxiety problems) to 0.509 (conduct problems). The internalizing subtotal score and externalizing subtotal score had alpha values of 0.465 and 0.547, respectively, while the alpha value for the total score was 0.550. Further, the intraclass correlation coefficient (average measure) across the CBCL, TRF, and YSR in our study was 0.409 (range: 0.314, 0.491), indicating moderate cross-informant agreement. According to the ASEBA manual (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000, 2001), the ratings by different informants were based on different contextual child behaviors to tap different aspects of the same phenomenon. Thus, low agreement among parents, child, and teachers is expected. The moderate level of cross-informant agreement granted the necessity of integrating information across informants using factor scores or average scores (which are described later in the statistical analysis section) to increase specificity and sensitivity in detecting behavior problems.
Statistical analysis
Sample characteristics were summarized by descriptive statistics consisted of the mean, SD, and percentages. Sex, location, and parental education differences between childhood (3–5 y) cosleeping history groups were analyzed using 2 × 2 and k × 2 x2 tests. The student t test was performed to examine the difference in age between childhood co-sleeping history groups. Following Martel et al. (2017), factor analysis were conducted to generate factor estimators for each subscale (i.e., CBCL, TRF, and YSR), integrating all available informants’ ratings (Martel et al., 2017). Further, the average scores calculated as the unweighted average of all informants’ ratings were also generated and used for robustness analysis (Horton & Fitzmaurice, 2004).
We first used inverse probability weighting (IPW) to balance potential confounders including age, sex, residence location, and parents’ education level. Assigned stabilized weights in the main model were calculated by dividing marginal probability of co-sleeping by the propensity score for the group assigned. Re-weighting was performed for sensitivity analyses to ensure statistical robustness of results, as suggested by Nichols et al. (Nichols et al., 2007, 2008). To assess the associations between co-sleeping history and behavior problems at different development periods, weighted general linear-mixed models (GLMM) with logit linkage were performed subsequently. All baseline sample characteristics and residence locations as a mixed effect were adjusted for, and a robust variance estimator was used to increase the validity of analysis results. All questionnaire-based scores across informants as well as factor scores were explored.
To explore the marginal effect of early childhood co-sleeping history on preadolescent behavior problems, we included childhood questionnaire-based or factor scores in the corresponding weight-assigning model and GLMM model to adjust for the individual difference at the beginning of the child’s developmental trajectory. All tests in the analyses exploring the association or marginal effect were two-tailed with an alpha threshold of 0.05. Participants with missing data were excluded from the analysis of corresponding categories. In our analysis (either before or after matching), less than 5% of participants had missing data in any analyzed categories. Statistical analyses were performed using STATA 15.1 (STATA Corp, College Station, TX) and SAS software, version 9.3 (SAS institute, Cary, NC).
Results
Sample characteristics
Of the 1,274 children included in the analysis, age at preadolescent behavioral assessment was 10.99 y (standard deviation [SD] = 0.74). 54.57% were male (N = 694, mean age = 11.04, SD = 0.73), and 45.53% were female (N = 580, mean age = 10.94, SD = 0.75). 84.77% (N = 1,080) of the subjects reported a history of childhood co-sleeping, while 15.23% (N = 194) did not. In our sample, 20.33% subjects lived in rural areas, while 38.93% and 40.74% lived in suburban and city areas, respectively. Parental education levels were distributed as follows: elementary school or less (2.05%), middle school (30.94%), high school (53.07%), college, and above (13.94%) for fathers, and elementary school or less (3.38%), middle school (40.76%), high school (46.50%), college, and above (9.36%) for mothers. A total of 84.77% (1080/1274) of preschool children in Jintan City had co-sleeping arrangements. Detailed child and family characteristics are summarized in Table 1. There were significant differences in living location between children with and without childhood co-sleeping history. Rural areas had the highest co-sleeping rate, 92.28%. After reweighting, all aforementioned potential confounders were well balanced (Table S1.)
Table 1.
Childhood Sleeping Arrangements History |
Statistical Test |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Solitary-sleeping (N = 194) | Co-sleeping (N = 1,080) | 2/t | p | |
Sex – no. (%) | 0.46 | 0.499 | ||
Male | 110 (15.85) | 584 (84.15) | ||
Female | 82 (14.48) | 496 (85.52) | ||
Age (Wave 1), Mean (SD) | 4.95 (0.63) | 5.00 (0.76) | 0.94 | 0.347 |
Location – no. (%) | 14.47 | 0.001 | ||
Rural | 20 (7.72) | 239 (92.28) | ||
Suburban | 82 (16.53) | 414 (83.47) | ||
City | 92 (17.73) | 427 (82.27) | ||
Father’s education – no. (%) | 1.29 | 0.277 | ||
Primary school | 2 (7.69) | 24 (92.31) | ||
Middle school | 52 (13.23) | 341 (86.77) | ||
High school | 114 (16.91) | 560 (83.09) | ||
College or above | 26 (14.69) | 151 (85.31) | ||
Mother’s education – no. (%) | 0.82 | 0.484 | ||
Primary school | 3 (6.98) | 40 (93.02) | ||
Middle school | 82 (15.83) | 436 (84.17) | ||
High school | 90 (15.23) | 501 (84.77) | ||
College or above | 19 (15.97) | 100 (84.03) |
Note: SD, Standard deviation
Associations between early childhood co-sleeping and childhood behavior problems
As shown in Table 2, early childhood co-sleeping behavior was associated with multiple concomitant behavior problems, especially as reported by parents. From parents’ reports (CBCL), early childhood co-sleeping is associated with more severe internalizing (anxious/depressed, p = .021; withdrawal/depressed, p = .006) problems, externalizing (aggressive behavior, p < .001) problems, as well as all four DSM-oriented subscale problems (affective problems, p = .048; anxiety problems, p < .001; attention deficit hyperactivity (ADH) problems, p < .001; oppositional defiant problems, p = .002). Teachers’ reports (TRF) revealed more affective problems (p = .007) in co-sleeping children.
Table 2.
CBCL, Wave 1 |
TRF, Wave 1 |
FACTOR SCORE, Wave 1 |
AVERAGE SCORE, Wave 1 |
|||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | |||||
Internalizing Problems | ||||||||||||||||
Anxious/depressed | 1.58 | 0.021* | 1.07 | 2.33 | 1.03 | 0.934 | 0.56 | 1.87 | 1.48 | <0.001*** | 1.24 | 1.77 | 1.42 | 0.071 | 0.97 | 2.09 |
Withdrawal/depressed | 1.56 | 0.006** | 1.14 | 2.14 | 1.07 | 0.629 | 0.81 | 1.42 | 1.08 | 0.791 | 0.61 | 1.92 | 1.01 | 0.959 | 0.63 | 1.63 |
Somatic Problems | 1.31 | 0.225 | 0.85 | 2.02 | 0.83 | 0.146 | 0.65 | 1.07 | 1.26 | 0.028* | 1.02 | 1.56 | 1.34 | 0.053 | 1.00 | 1.80 |
Externalizing Problems | ||||||||||||||||
Aggressive Behavior | 2.20 | <0.001*** | 1.39 | 3.49 | 1.03 | 0.936 | 0.53 | 1.98 | 1.52 | 0.005** | 1.13 | 2.05 | 1.49 | <0.001*** | 1.22 | 1.83 |
Other Behavior problems | ||||||||||||||||
Attention Problems | 1.51 | 0.078 | 0.96 | 2.37 | 1.21 | 0.283 | 0.86 | 1.71 | 1.22 | 0.018* | 1.03 | 1.45 | 1.22 | <0.001*** | 1.15 | 1.28 |
DSM-oriented Scales | ||||||||||||||||
Affective Problems | 1.57 | 0.048* | 1.00 | 2.45 | 0.81 | 0.007** | 0.70 | 0.94 | 1.09 | 0.601 | 0.79 | 1.52 | 1.21 | 0.055 | 1.00 | 1.47 |
Anxiety Problems | 3.11 | <0.001*** | 2.04 | 4.74 | 1.11 | 0.733 | 0.62 | 1.99 | 2.06 | <0.001*** | 1.47 | 2.88 | 2.31 | <0.001*** | 1.81 | 2.94 |
ADH Problems | 1.75 | <0.001*** | 1.32 | 2.34 | 1.08 | 0.647 | 0.79 | 1.47 | 1.60 | <0.001*** | 1.23 | 2.09 | 1.36 | 0.013* | 1.07 | 1.72 |
Oppositional Defiant Problems | 2.04 | 0.002** | 1.30 | 3.19 | 1.05 | 0.721 | 0.81 | 1.36 | 1.59 | <0.001*** | 1.54 | 1.64 | 1.95 | <0.001*** | 1.81 | 2.11 |
Adjusted for age, sex, parents’ education and mixed effect of location, heteroscedasticity assumed for robustness of analysis result.
CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; TRF, Teacher’s Report Form; DSM, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; ADH problems, attention deficit hyperactivity problems; OR, odds ratio.
p ≤.10;
p ≤.05;
p ≤.01.
After information from teacher and parent reports using factor score analysis was aggregated, multiple behavior problems were still associated with co-sleeping behavior. The affected behavior expanded from internalizing (anxious/depressed, p = .021; somatic problems, p = .028), externalizing (aggressive behavior, p = .005), other (attention problems, p = .018) and three DSM-oriented behavior problems (anxiety problems, p < .001; ADH problems, p < .001; oppositional defiant problems, p < .001). After varying the weighing model and/or changing to the average score method, the results (shown in Table S2.1, Table S3.1, Table S4.1S4.1, and Table S5.1) were similar, confirming the robustness of the analysis.
Associations between early childhood co-sleeping and preadolescent behavior problems
As shown in Table 3, early childhood co-sleeping continued to associate with multiple preadolescent behavior problems. Among children with co-sleeping history in early childhood, the children themselves reported higher internalizing (anxious/depressed, p < .001; subscale total, p < .001) problems and scored higher in two DSM-oriented subscales (anxiety problems, p = .003; ADH problems, p = .003); parents reported higher prevalence of internalizing (anxious/depressed, p = .036; withdrawal/depressed, p = .002; subscale total, p = .046), externalizing (aggressive behavior, p = .027), overall (total score, p = .049) and two DSM-oriented behavior problems (anxiety problems, p < .001; conduct problems, p = .011); teachers reported more internalizing (subscale total, p = .047) and other behavior problems (social problems, p = .002; thought problems, p = .039).
Table 3.
YSR Wave 2 |
CBCL Wave 2 |
TRF Wave 2 |
FACTOR SCORE Wave 2 |
AVERAGE SCORE Wave 2 |
||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | ||||||
Internalizing Problems | ||||||||||||||||||||
Anxious/depressed | 1.69 | <0.001*** | 1.43 | 2.00 | 1.37 | 0.036* | 1.02 | 1.84 | 1.50 | 0.139 | 0.88 | 2.58 | 2.08 | <0.001*** | 1.66 | 2.61 | 1.79 | <0.001*** | 1.52 | 2.11 |
Withdrawal/depressed | 1.94 | 0.116 | 0.85 | 4.44 | 1.73 | 0.002** | 1.22 | 2.47 | 1.32 | 0.174 | 0.89 | 1.96 | 2.27 | <0.001*** | 1.42 | 3.65 | 2.71 | <0.001*** | 2.12 | 3.45 |
Somatic Problems | 0.94 | 0.519 | 0.78 | 1.13 | 0.86 | 0.684 | 0.42 | 1.77 | 2.31 | 0.143 | 0.75 | 7.11 | 1.38 | 0.001** | 1.14 | 1.68 | 1.29 | 0.228 | 0.85 | 1.96 |
Subtotal: Internalizing Problems | 1.46 | <0.001*** | 1.23 | 1.73 | 1.20 | 0.046* | 1.00 | 1.44 | 1.69 | 0.047* | 1.01 | 2.83 | 2.00 | <0.001*** | 1.45 | 2.76 | 2.03 | <0.001*** | 1.48 | 2.78 |
Externalizing Problems | ||||||||||||||||||||
Rule-breaking Behavior | 1.48 | 0.071 | 0.97 | 2.26 | 1.16 | 0.448 | 0.79 | 1.72 | 1.19 | 0.505 | 0.71 | 2.01 | 1.56 | 0.129 | 0.88 | 2.79 | 1.56 | 0.129 | 0.88 | 2.79 |
Aggressive Behavior | 1.05 | 0.791 | 0.74 | 1.48 | 1.31 | 0.027* | 1.03 | 1.65 | 1.00 | 0.999 | 0.72 | 1.39 | 1.59 | 0.023* | 1.06 | 2.37 | 1.73 | 0.009** | 1.15 | 2.62 |
Subtotal: Externalizing Problems | 1.19 | 0.478 | 0.73 | 1.94 | 1.29 | 0.067 | 0.98 | 1.70 | 1.20 | 0.511 | 0.69 | 2.09 | 2.00 | <0.001*** | 1.42 | 2.82 | 1.93 | <0.001*** | 1.31 | 2.86 |
Other Behavior problems | ||||||||||||||||||||
Social Problems | 1.16 | 0.506 | 0.75 | 1.77 | 1.46 | 0.072 | 0.97 | 2.22 | 1.90 | 0.002** | 1.26 | 2.85 | 1.58 | <0.001*** | 1.21 | 2.07 | 1.51 | 0.010* | 1.10 | 2.08 |
Thought Problems | 1.27 | 0.247 | 0.85 | 1.91 | 1.15 | 0.673 | 0.61 | 2.16 | 1.29 | 0.039* | 1.01 | 1.64 | 1.34 | 0.057 | 0.99 | 1.81 | 1.68 | <0.001*** | 1.30 | 2.18 |
Attention Problems | 1.40 | 0.103 | 0.93 | 2.11 | 1.14 | 0.538 | 0.75 | 1.74 | 1.44 | 0.151 | 0.88 | 2.36 | 1.88 | <0.001*** | 1.55 | 2.29 | 1.63 | 0.029* | 1.05 | 2.54 |
Total Score | 1.30 | 0.375 | 0.73 | 2.34 | 1.47 | 0.049* | 1.00 | 2.15 | 1.36 | 0.287 | 0.77 | 2.39 | 1.71 | 0.014* | 1.11 | 2.61 | 1.80 | 0.009** | 1.16 | 2.80 |
DSM-oriented Scales | ||||||||||||||||||||
Affective Problems | 1.35 | 0.313 | 0.75 | 2.41 | 1.15 | 0.368 | 0.85 | 1.56 | 1.03 | 0.917 | 0.62 | 1.71 | 1.67 | <0.001*** | 1.34 | 2.07 | 1.77 | <0.001*** | 1.30 | 2.42 |
Anxiety Problems | 1.77 | 0.003** | 1.22 | 2.57 | 1.32 | <0.001*** | 1.16 | 1.50 | 1.62 | 0.105 | 0.90 | 2.92 | 1.68 | 0.014* | 1.11 | 2.55 | 1.68 | 0.014* | 1.11 | 2.55 |
Somatic Problems | 0.90 | 0.539 | 0.63 | 1.27 | 0.80 | 0.363 | 0.49 | 1.29 | 2.25 | 0.181 | 0.69 | 7.39 | 0.81 | 0.352 | 0.52 | 1.26 | 0.84 | 0.415 | 0.55 | 1.28 |
ADH Problems | 1.52 | 0.003** | 1.15 | 2.00 | 1.33 | 0.087 | 0.96 | 1.86 | 1.39 | 0.162 | 0.88 | 2.22 | 1.40 | 0.008** | 1.09 | 1.79 | 1.82 | <0.001*** | 1.41 | 2.34 |
Oppositional Defiant Problems | 1.34 | 0.231 | 0.83 | 2.18 | 1.29 | 0.101 | 0.95 | 1.74 | 1.55 | 0.197 | 0.80 | 3.03 | 1.50 | 0.129 | 0.89 | 2.52 | 1.90 | <0.001*** | 1.41 | 2.55 |
Conduct Problems | 1.30 | 0.361 | 0.74 | 2.30 | 1.43 | 0.011* | 1.09 | 1.89 | 0.91 | 0.741 | 0.54 | 1.56 | 1.27 | 0.415 | 0.72 | 2.23 | 1.47 | 0.153 | 0.87 | 2.49 |
Adjusted for age, sex, parents’ education and mixed effect of location, heteroscedasticity assumed for robustness of analysis result.
YSR, Youth Self Report; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; TRF, Teacher’s Report Form; DSM, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; ADH problems, attention deficit hyperactivity problems; OR, odds ratio.
p ≤.10;
p ≤.05;
p ≤.01.
After information from all three informants via factor score was aggregated, early childhood co-sleeping history was associated with all three internalizing subscale problems (anxious/depressed, p < .001; withdrawal/depressed, p < .001; somatic problems, p = .001; subscale total, p < .001), external problem (aggressive behavior, p = .023; subscale total, p < .001), other (social problems, p < .001; attention problems, p < .001), overall (total score, p = .014), and DSM-oriented behavioral problems (affective problems, p < .001; anxiety problems, p = .014; ADH problems, p = .008). After varying the weighing model and/or changing to the average score method, the results (shown in Table S2.2, Table S3.2 TableS4.2, and Table S5.2) were similar, confirming the robustness of the analysis.
Prediction of preadolescent behavior problems among children with early childhood co-sleeping
As shown in Table 4, after childhood baseline behavior problems were controlled for, parental reports indicate that early childhood co-sleeping predicts more severe withdrawal/depressed problems in preadolescence (p < .001). After information from all available informants with the differential weighting approach was aggregated, early childhood co-sleeping history predicts increased severity in internalizing (anxious/depressed, p < .001; withdrawal/depressed, p = .008; somatic problems, p = .012), externalizing (aggressive behavior, p = .001), other (attention problem, p < .001), and two DSM-oriented behavior problems (affective problems, p = .001; anxiety problems, p = .002). After varying the weighing model and/or changing to the average score method, the results (shown in Table S2.3 Table S3.3, Table S4.3, and Table S5.3) were similar, confirming the robustness of the analysis.
Table 4.
CBCL, Wave 2 |
TRF, Wave 2 |
FACTOR SCORE, Wave 2 |
AVERAGE SCORE, Wave 2 |
|||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | OR | p | Confidence Interval | |||||
Internalizing Problems | ||||||||||||||||
Anxious/depressed | 1.08 | 0.641 | 0.78 | 1.50 | 1.52 | 0.125 | 0.89 | 2.59 | 2.03 | <0.001*** | 1.80 | 2.28 | 1.82 | <0.001*** | 1.38 | 2.39 |
Withdrawal/depressed | 1.73 | <0.001*** | 1.28 | 2.33 | 1.29 | 0.210 | 0.87 | 1.93 | 2.19 | 0.008** | 1.23 | 3.92 | 2.61 | <0.001*** | 1.92 | 3.53 |
Somatic Problems | 0.74 | 0.367 | 0.39 | 1.42 | 2.16 | 0.157 | 0.74 | 6.26 | 1.35 | 0.012* | 1.07 | 1.70 | 1.28 | 0.260 | 0.83 | 1.97 |
Externalizing Problems | ||||||||||||||||
Aggressive Behavior | 1.11 | 0.458 | 0.84 | 1.48 | 1.03 | 0.841 | 0.75 | 1.42 | 1.62 | 0.001** | 1.21 | 2.17 | 1.66 | 0.018* | 1.09 | 2.53 |
Other Behavior problems | ||||||||||||||||
Attention Problems | 0.94 | 0.840 | 0.54 | 1.66 | 1.47 | 0.085 | 0.95 | 2.29 | 1.55 | <0.001*** | 1.43 | 1.69 | 1.63 | 0.009** | 1.13 | 2.34 |
DSM-oriented Scales | ||||||||||||||||
Affective Problems | 0.86 | 0.354 | 0.63 | 1.18 | 1.05 | 0.857 | 0.61 | 1.83 | 1.47 | 0.001** | 1.16 | 1.86 | 1.65 | 0.002** | 1.19 | 2.27 |
Anxiety Problems | 1.11 | 0.502 | 0.82 | 1.49 | 1.63 | 0.107 | 0.90 | 2.95 | 1.74 | 0.002** | 1.22 | 2.48 | 1.72 | <0.001*** | 1.33 | 2.24 |
ADH Problems | 1.02 | 0.929 | 0.61 | 1.73 | 1.47 | 0.062 | 0.98 | 2.19 | 1.17 | 0.057 | 1.00 | 1.36 | 1.71 | <0.001*** | 1.28 | 2.28 |
Oppositional Defiant Problems | 1.13 | 0.440 | 0.82 | 1.56 | 1.69 | 0.088 | 0.92 | 3.09 | 1.44 | 0.223 | 0.80 | 2.59 | 1.67 | <0.001*** | 1.28 | 2.16 |
Adjusted for age, sex, parents’ education, corresponding childhood behavior problems scores and mixed effect of location, heteroscedasticity assumed for robustness of analysis result.
CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; TRF, Teacher’s Report Form; DSM, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; ADH problems, attention deficit hyperactivity problems; OR, odds ratio.
p ≤.10;
p ≤.05;
p ≤.01.
Discussion
Main findings
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal study to explore the association between early childhood co-sleeping and preadolescent behavior problems after adjusting for childhood baseline behavior. The major findings of this research include: 1) Chinese preschool children (aged 3 to 5 years) have a high prevalence (84.77%) of co-sleeping arrangements with their parents; 2) early childhood co-sleeping is significantly associated with increased behavior problems in childhood (Odds Ratio [OR] 1.22–2.06, ps<0.03) and preadolescence (OR 1.40–2.27, ps<0.02); 3) early childhood cosleeping history significantly predicts an increase in internalizing (OR 1.63–2.61, ps<0.02) and externalizing behavior problems in preadolescence.
High prevalence of co-sleeping arrangements in Chinese preschool children
In this study, we found that 84.77% (1080/1274) of preschool children in the Jintan area have co-sleeping arrangements, which is similar to the prevalence of co-sleeping among preschool children in other cities in China, including Shanghai, Guangzhou, and Nanjing (Liu, 2005; Wang, 2015; Yue et al., 2018). This suggests that co-sleeping arrangements for preschool children remain a common practice in Chinese families. Regarding residency location, we find that children living in rural areas have the highest prevalence of co-sleeping arrangements, which is in line with several studies that report children in rural areas and/or areas of lower socioeconomic status (SES) tend to have higher cosleeping rates (Gupta et al., 2016; Li et al., 2007; Norton et al., 2011; Salm Ward et al., 2016). Indeed, lower SES might indicate crowded housing space and parents’ lower awareness of children’s sleep hygiene; both can contribute to lower adherence to the recommendation for solitary-sleeping for preschool children as suggested by the latest Chinese children’s sleep hygiene guidelines (National Health Commission of the People Republic of China, 2017). Beyond these factors, however, cultural reasons may be another factor contributing to Chinese children’s sleeping arrangements, in that Chinese traditional collectivist culture and the high value placed on family unity may result in the normalization of co-sleeping by Chinese parents (Li et al., 2007; X. Liu et al., 2003; Owens, 2004).
Associations between co-sleeping and childhood and preadolescent behavior problems
Using multiple methods to balance potential variances in parent-child dyads’ baseline characteristics, we found that early childhood (3–5 y) co-sleeping is associated with internalizing (anxious/depressed, somatic complaints), externalizing (aggressive behavior), attention problems, and oppositional defiant behavior in childhood. These behaviors still exist when children reach their preadolescence (10–13 y). Furthermore, we found that positive associations between early childhood co-sleeping and preadolescent behavior problems expanded to withdrawal/depressed, social problems, and affective problems.
Existing studies exploring the relationship between co-sleeping and children’s behavior have inconsistent conclusions. While some cross-sectional studies found no associations between co-sleeping arrangements and childhood internalizing or externalizing behavior (Cortesi et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2017; Sourander, 2001), a cross-sectional study conducted in Russia found that bed-sharing infants are more likely to have negative moods and less persistence in maintaining an activity for a certain period of time (Kelmanson, 1999). Another study conducted in the US found that bed-sharing in preschool children is associated with intense temperament, less adaptability and rhythmicity (Hayes et al., 2001).
Furthermore, the impact of co-sleeping on children’s behavior remains inconclusive in existing cohort studies. A cohort study found that bed-sharing at age 1–3 years has no negative associations with children’s behavior and cognition at the age of five (Barajas et al., 2011). Yet a more recent longitudinal study conducted in Brazil with a larger sample size (n = 3583) found that bed-sharing is associated with increased odds of presenting internalizing behavior and DSM-IV diagnosed mental health issues at the age of six (Santos et al., 2016). The inconsistencies between different cohort studies may be due to potentially insufficient statistical power from small sample sizes or different assessment instruments (Barajas et al., 2011; Okami et al., 2002; Volkovich et al., 2015). However, with a much larger sample, we found that early childhood co-sleeping is associated with multiple pre-adolescent behavior problems across internalizing and externalizing behavior, and DSM-oriented mental health problems.
Prediction of preadolescent behavior problems among children with early childhood co-sleeping
After the individual differences in childhood are accounted for, early co-sleeping history remains a statistically significant predicting risk factor for aforementioned behavior problems. The predicted risk for having certain behavior problems (e.g., withdrawal/depressed) is more than doubled. Thus, our findings highlight co-sleeping in early childhood as an important predicting risk factor for persistent behavior problems. Multiple studies have identified that childhood co-sleeping is associated with children’s sleep disturbances and poorer sleep quality but not behavior problems (Kim et al., 2017; Lo, 2016). A recent longitudinal study identified a bidirectional association between childhood sleep problems and both internalizing and externalizing difficulties (Quach et al., 2018). This might suggest a potential pathway to explain the strong prediction effect identified in our study.
Strengths and limitations
The strengths of this study lie in its robust longitudinal cohort study design, which allowed for the assessment of baseline behavior problems during childhood and preadolescence using parent report and teacher reports. However, since the included children were in their early developmental stages during the first wave of data collection, their baseline behavior problems could not be collected via self-report. Another limitation is that we could not distinguish between bed-sharing and room-sharing based on one single question. Propensity score-based analysis design partially compensated for the non-randomized control study design and allowed for examination of the causal relationship between early childhood co-sleeping and preadolescent behavioral problems. However, uncontrolled confounders may have caused biased associations and predictions, as this was an epidemiological study. For example, puberty onset age and pubertal stage can potentially impact adolescent aggression/delinquent behavior (Najman et al., 2009). Since the pubertal onset and stage indicators were not collected and thus adjusted for in our analysis, our conclusions are subject to bias. Another limitation is that the co-sleeping arrangements were not collected in children’s preadolescent period. Since co-sleeping in preadolescence is associated with the increase of certain children’s behavior problems (i.e., DSM-derived anxiety problems (Palmer et al., 2018), not adjusting for this variable in our analysis may have mixed the effect of early co-sleeping with preadolescent co-sleeping on children’s preadolescent behavior. The findings should be replicated in other ethnic groups as childhood co-sleeping behavior varies vastly between different cultures with regards to prevalence and interpretation. As a result of these limitations, we acknowledge the need to interpret our conclusions with caution.
Conclusions
Overall, we found that early childhood co-sleeping is associated with multiple behavior problems reported by parents, teachers, and children themselves. We also found that childhood co-sleeping history predicts both internal and external behavior problems after controlling for baseline behavior problems. Thus, early childhood co-sleeping arrangements should be practiced with caution. Future studies are encouraged to verify our findings in other cultures as well as investigate the effect of early co-sleeping behavior in later developmental stages. Future studies can also investigate children’s sleep problems as a potential mediating factor between co-sleeping arrangements and children’s short-term and long-term behavior.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The authors thankfully acknowledge the Jintan Local Cohort Group for data collection. Special thanks are extended to the participating children and their families from Jintan City.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (R01-ES018858, K02-ES-019878, and K01-ES015877), and the University of Pennsylvania Center of Excellence in Environmental Toxicology (P30-ES013508).
Footnotes
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed on the publisher’s website
Data availability statement
Data are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
References
- Achenbach T. (1997). Guide for the caregiver-teacher report form for ages 2–5. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, & Rescorla LA (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles (Vol. 30). University of Vermont, Research center for children, youth; …. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, & Rescorla LA (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. University of Vermont. [Google Scholar]
- Anuntaseree W, Mo-Suwan L, Vasiknanonte P, Kuasirikul S, Ma ALA, & Choprapawon C (2008). Factors associated with bed sharing and sleep position in Thai neonates. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(4), 482–490. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2008.00832.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ball HL, Howel D, Bryant A, Best E, Russell C, & Ward-Platt M (2016). Bed-sharing by breastfeeding mothers: Who bed-shares and what is the relationship with breastfeeding duration? Acta paediatrica, 105(6), 628–634. 10.1111/apa.13354 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barajas RG, Martin A, Brooks-Gunn J, & Hale L (2011). Mother-child bed-sharing in toddlerhood and cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Pediatrics, 128(2), e339–e347. 10.1542/peds.2010-3300 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlin RF, & Moon RY (2017). Risk factors, protective factors, and current recommendations to reduce sudden infant death syndrome: A review. JAMA Pediatrics, 171(2), 175–180. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.3345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cortesi F, Giannotti F, Sebastiani T, & Vagnoni C (2004). Cosleeping and sleep behavior in Italian school-aged children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 25(1), 28–33. 10.1097/00004703-200402000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez R, Vance A, & Gomez RM (2014). Analysis of the convergent and discriminant validity of the CBCL, TRF, and YSR in a clinic-referred sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(8), 1413–1425. 10.1007/s10802-014-9879-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grigorenko EL, Geiser C, Slobodskaya HR, & Francis DJ (2010). Cross-informant symptoms from CBCL, TRF, and YSR: Trait and method variance in a normative sample of Russian youths. Psychological Assessment, 22(4), 893–911. 10.1037/a0020703 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gupta R, Kandpal SD, Goel D, Mittal N, Dhyani M, & Mittal M (2016). Sleep-patterns, co-sleeping and parent’s perception of sleep among school children: Comparison of domicile and gender. Sleep Science, 9(3), 192–197. 10.1016/j.slsci.2016.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes MJ, Parker KG, Sallinen B, & Davare AA (2001). Bedsharing, temperament, and sleep disturbance in early childhood. Sleep, 24(6), 657–662. 10.1093/sleep/24.6.657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horton NJ, & Fitzmaurice GM (2004). Regression analysis of multiple source and multiple informant data from complex survey samples. Statistics in Medicine, 23(18), 2911–2933. 10.1002/sim.1879 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang XN, Wang HS, Chang JJ, Wang LH, Liu XC, Jiang JX, & An L (2016). Feeding methods, sleep arrangement, and infant sleep patterns: A Chinese population-based study. World Journal of Pediatrics, 12(1), 66–75. 10.1007/s12519-015-0012-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanova MY, Rescorla LA, Bjarnadottir G, Dias P, Doepfner M, Frigerio A, Jusiene R, Lecannelier F, Lima VS, Lobel SP, Achenbach TM, Bilenberg N, Denner S, Dobrean A, Esmaeili EM, Gudmundsson HS, Kristensen S, Leung PWL, Liu J, Machado BC, … Verhulst FC (2011). Syndromes of preschool psychopathology reported by teachers and caregivers in 14 societies using the caregiver-teacher report form. Journal of Early Childhood and Infant Psychology, 7, 87–103. https://go.gale.com/ps/anonymous?id=GALE%7CA294508024&sid=googleScholar&v=2.1&it=r&linkaccess=abs&issn=15546144&p=AONE&sw=w . [Google Scholar]
- Kaymaz N, Gökten ES, Uzun ME, Yildirim Ş, Tekin M, Topaloğlu N, & Binnetoğlu FK (2015). Prolonged rooming-in in infancy is associated with generalized anxiety disorder in the adolescent period. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 27(4), 383–389. 10.1515/ijamh-2014-0045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelmanson IA (1999). Parent-infant bed sharing and behavioural features in 2–4-month-old infants. Early Child Development and Care, 149(1), 1–9. 10.1080/0300443991490101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kim E, Lee R, & Cain KC (2017). Cosleeping, sleep disturbances, childrenl features in 2–4-month-old infantsrder inefficacy among Korean American families. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 30(2), 112–120. 10.1111/jcap.12182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leung PWL, Kwong SL, Tang CP, Ho TP, Hung SF, Lee CC, Hong SL, Chiu CM, & Liu WS (2006). Test–retest reliability and criterion validity of the Chinese version of CBCL, TRF, and YSR. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(9), 970–973. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01570.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S, Jin X, Yan C, Wu S, Jiang F, & Shen X (2007). Bed- and room-sharing in Chinese school-aged children: Prevalence and association with sleep behaviors. Sleep Medicine, 9(5), 555–563. 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S, Jin X, Yan C, Wu S, Jiang F, & Shen X (2009). Factors associated with bed and room sharing in Chinese school-aged children. Child: Care, Health and Development, 35(2), 171–177. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2008.00889.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu HL (2005). A study on the situation of sleep disorders and its influencing factors in children aged 2–6 years in Guangzhou. Jinan University. [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Cao S, Chen Z, Raine A, Hanlon A, Ai Y, Zhou G, Yan C, Leung PW, McCauley L, Pinto-Martin J, Jintan Cohort Study G, & The Jintan Cohort Study, G. (2015). Cohort profile update: The China Jintan child cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 44(5), 1548, 1548a-1548l. 10.1093/ije/dyv119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Liu J, Cheng H, Cheng H, Leung PWL, & Leung PWL (2011). The application of the preschool child behavior checklist and the caregiver–teacher report form to Mainland Chinese children: Syndrome structure, gender differences, country effects, and inter-informant agreement. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(2), 251–264. 10.1007/s10802-010-9452-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Liu X, Wang W, McCauley L, Pinto-Martin J, Wang Y, Li L, Yan C, & Rogan WJ (2014). Blood lead concentrations and children’s behavioral and emotional problems: A cohort study. JAMA Pediatrics, 168(8), 737–745. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.332 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, McCauley L, Leung P, Wang B, Needleman H, Pinto-Martin J, & Jintan Cohort Group. (2011). Community-based participatory research (CBPR) approach to study children’s health in China: Experiences and reflections. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 48(7), 904–913. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2011.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, McCauley LA, Zhao Y, Zhang H, Pinto-Martin J, & Jintan Cohort Study G (2010). Cohort profile: The China Jintan child cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 39(3), 668–674. 10.1093/ije/dyp205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu X, Liu L, & Wang R (2003). Bed sharing, sleep habits, and sleep problems among Chinese school-aged children. Sleep, 26(7), 839–844. 10.1093/sleep/267.839 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Z, Wang G, Geng L, Luo J, Li N, & Owens J (2016). Sleep patterns, sleep disturbances, and associated factors among Chinese Urban Kindergarten children. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 14(1), 100–117. 10.1080/15402002.2014.963581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lo MJ (2016). Relationship between sleep habits and nighttime sleep among healthy preschool children in Taiwan. Annals of the Academy of Medicine, Singapore, 45(12), 549–556. http://www.annals.edu.sg/pdf/45VolNo12Dec2016/MemberOnly/V45N12p549.pdf. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma J, Li S, Jiang F, Jin X, Shen X, & Li F (2014). Co-sleeping and childhood enuresis in China. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 35(1), 44–49. 10.1097/01.DBP.0000439104.37513.44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martel MM, Markon K, & Smith GT (2017). Research review: Multi-informant integration in child and adolescent psychopathology diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(2), 116–128. 10.1111/jcpp.12611 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mileva-Seitz VR, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Battaini C, & Luijk MPCM (2017). Parent-child bed-sharing: The good, the bad, and the burden of evidence. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 32, 4–27. 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mileva-Seitz VR, Luijk MP, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, & Tiemeier H (2016). Association between infant nighttime-sleep location and attachement security: No easy verdict. Infant Mental Health Journal, 37(1), 5–16. 10.1002/imhj.21547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mindell JA, Leichman ES, & Walters RM (2017). Sleep location and parent-perceived sleep outcomes in older infants. Sleep Medicine, 39, 1–7. 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mindell JA, Sadeh A, Kwon R, & Goh DY (2013). Cross-cultural differences in the sleep of preschool children. Sleep Medicine, 14(12), 1283–1289. 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mindell JA, Sadeh A, Wiegand B, How TH, & Goh DY (2010). Cross-cultural differences in infant and toddler sleep. Sleep Medicine, 11(3), 274–280. 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell EA, Hutchison BL, Thompson JM, & Wouldes TA (2015). Exploratory study of bed-sharing and maternal-infant bonding. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 51(8), 820–825. 10.1111/jpc.12833 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moon RY (2016). SIDS and other sleep-related infant deaths: Evidence base for 2016 updated recommendations for a safe infant sleeping environment. Pediatrics, 138(5), e1–e36. 10.1542/peds.2016-2940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Najman JM, Hayatbakhsh MR, McGee TR, Bor W, O’Callaghan MJ, & Williams GM (2009). The impact of puberty on aggression/delinquency: Adolescence to young adulthood. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 42(3), 369–386. 10.1375/acri.42.3.369 [Record #12597 is using a reference type undefined in this output style.]. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- National Health Commission of the People Republic of China. (October 12nd, 2017).). 0岁 – 5岁Λλ8ΕΪ4指南 [Guidelines for sleep hygiene among children aged 0-5 years]. Retrieved from: http://www.nhc.gov.cn/ewebeditor/uploadfile/2017/10/20171026154305316.pdf. Accessed November 9th, 2019
- Nelson EA, Taylor BJ, Jenik A, Vance J, Walmsley K, Pollard K, Freemantle M, Ewing D, Einspieler C, Engele H, Ritter P, Hildes-Ripstein GE, Arancibia M, Ji X, Li H, Bedard C, Helweg-Larsen K, Sidenius K, Karlqvist S, Poets C, … Nepomyashchaya V. (2001). International child care practices study: Infant sleeping environment. Early Human Development, 62(1), 43–55. 10.1016/s0378-3782(01)00116-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols A (2007). Causal Inference with Observational Data. The Stata Journal: Promoting communications on statistics and Stata, 7(4), 507–541. doi: 10.1177/1536867X0800700403 4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols A (2008). Erratum and Discussion of Propensity-Score Reweighting. The Stata Journal: Promoting communications on statistics and Stata, 8(4), 532–539. doi: 10.1177/1536867X0800800405 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Norton PJ, Norton PJ, Grellner KW, & Grellner KW (2011). A retrospective study on infant bed-sharing in a clinical practice population. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 15(4), 507–513. 10.1007/s10995-010-0600-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okami P, Weisner T, & Olmstead R (2002). Outcome correlates of parent-child bed-sharing: An eighteen-year longitudinal study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 23(4), 244–253. 10.1097/00004703-200208000-00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owens JA (2004). Sleep in children: Cross-cultural perspectives. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 2(3), 165–173. 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2004.00147.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer CA, Clementi MA, Meers JM, & Alfano CA (2018). Co-sleeping among school-aged anxious and non-anxious children: Associations with sleep variability and timing. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(6), 1321–1332. 10.1007/s10802-017-0387-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plucinski MM, Chicuecue S, Macete E, Chambe GA, Muguande O, Matsinhe G, Colborn J, Yoon SS, Doyle TJ, Kachur SP, Aide P, Alonso PL, Guinovart C, & Morgan J (2015). Sleeping arrangements and mass distribution of bed nets in six districts in central and northern Mozambique. Tropical Medicine & International Health, 20(12), 1685–1695. 10.1111/tmi.12596 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quach JL, Nguyen CD, Williams KE, & Sciberras E (2018). Bidirectional associations between child sleep problems and internalizing and externalizing difficulties from preschool to early adolescence. JAMA Pediatrics, 172 (2), e174363. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.4363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rath FH, & Okum ME (1995). Parents and children sleeping together: Cosleeping prevalence and concerns. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65(3), 411–418. 10.1037/h0079654 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salm Ward TC, Robb SW, & Kanu FA (2016). Prevalence and characteristics of bed-sharing among Black and White Infants in Georgia. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 20(2), 347–362. 10.1007/s10995-015-1834-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santos IS, Barros AJD, Barros FC, Munhoz TN, Da Silva BDP, & Matijasevich A (2016). Mother-child bed-sharing trajectories and psychiatric disorders at the age of 6 years. Journal of Affective Disorders, 208, 163–169. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaeffer P, & Asnes AG (2018). What do pediatricians tell parents about bed-sharing? Maternal and Child Health Journal, 22(1), 51–58. 10.1007/s10995-017-2353-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sourander A (2001). Emotional and behavioural problems in a sample of Finnish three-year-olds. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 10(2), 98–104. 10.1007/s007870170032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkovich E, Ben-Zion H, Karny D, Meiri G, & Tikotzky L (2015). Sleep patterns of co-sleeping and solitary sleeping infants and mothers: A longitudinal study. Sleep Medicine, 16(11), 1305–1312. 10.1016/jsleep.2015.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang G (2015). Sleep disturbances in Chinese preschool children: Characteristics, influencing factors, and behavioral intervention. East China Normal University. [Google Scholar]
- Young J, & Shipstone R (2018). Shared sleeping surfaces and dangerous sleeping environments. In Duncan JR & Byard RW (Eds.), SIDS sudden infant and early childhood death: The past, the present and the future (pp. 187–216). University of Adelaide Press. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK513372/ [Google Scholar]
- Yue L, Qu H, Zhu L, Liu J, Xie H, & Mao C (2018). [Investigation of sleep arrangement and the risk factors in preschool children from Nanjing]. Military Medical Journal of Southeast China, 20(2), 122–125. 10.3969/j.1672-271X.2018.02.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.