Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Apr 20.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Imaging Behav. 2022 Dec 9;17(2):223–256. doi: 10.1007/s11682-022-00750-6

Altered Cerebral Blood Flow in Older Adults with Alzheimer’s Disease: A Systematic Review

Cecily G Swinford 1,2, Shannon L Risacher 1,2, Yu-Chien Wu 1,2, Liana G Apostolova 1,2,3, Sujuan Gao 2,4, Paula J Bice 1,2, Andrew J Saykin 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC10117447  NIHMSID: NIHMS1885862  PMID: 36484922

Abstract

The prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease is projected to reach 13 million in the U.S. by 2050. Although major efforts have been made to avoid this outcome, so far there are no treatments that can stop or reverse the progressive cognitive decline that defines Alzheimer’s disease. The utilization of preventative treatment before significant cognitive decline has occurred may ultimately be the solution, necessitating a reliable biomarker of preclinical/prodromal disease stages to determine which older adults are most at risk. Quantitative cerebral blood flow is a promising potential early biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease, but the spatiotemporal patterns of altered cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease are not fully understood. The current systematic review compiles the findings of 81 original studies that compared resting gray matter cerebral blood flow in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer’s disease and that of cognitively normal older adults and/or assessed the relationship between cerebral blood flow and objective cognitive function. Individuals with Alzheimer’s disease had relatively decreased cerebral blood flow in all brain regions investigated, especially the temporoparietal and posterior cingulate, while individuals with mild cognitive impairment had consistent results of decreased cerebral blood flow in the posterior cingulate but more mixed results in other regions, especially the frontal lobe. Most papers reported a positive correlation between regional cerebral blood flow and cognitive function. This review highlights the need for more studies assessing cerebral blood flow changes both spatially and temporally over the course of Alzheimer’s disease, as well as the importance of including potential confounding factors in these analyses.

Keywords: cerebral blood flow, neuroimaging, biomarker, Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment

INTRODUCTION

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the leading cause of dementia; 60–80% of dementia cases are attributable to AD (Alzheimer’s Disease Facts and Figures, 2021). Both the prevalence and mortality rate of AD are increasing in the U.S. and globally, and there are no current treatments that can stop or reverse the progressive loss of cognitive function caused by AD (Alzheimer’s Disease Facts and Figures, 2021). Clinical trials of treatments targeting pathologic forms of amyloid-β and tau proteins in the brain have successfully reduced them but have been unable to stop the progression of cognitive decline (Zhu et al., 2020). In order to effectively treat AD, a preventative treatment administered before irreversible neuronal damage has occurred may be necessary (Zhu et al., 2020). An effective early biomarker for the identification of older adults who are most likely to develop AD is critical (Blennow & Zetterberg, 2018; Counts et al., 2017). The disease process of AD can begin decades before cognitive decline is apparent and can manifest as subjective cognitive decline (SCD) or mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Petersen et al., 1999; Petersen, 2016; Jessen et al., 2014; Jessen et al., 2020). MCI is a stage of decreased cognitive functioning measured on objective cognitive tests accompanied by the preserved ability to complete day-to-day activities (Petersen et al., 1999). SCD, during which individuals still perform in the normal range on objective cognitive tests but report a subjective decline from their normal cognitive state, has been described as an even earlier stage of AD (Jessen et al., 2014). An effective early biomarker for AD that can lead to preventative treatment must be able to distinguish individuals with SCD and MCI from normally aging individuals and from older adults experiencing cognitive changes due to other reasons, such as depression (Culpepper et. al., 2017).

Cerebral blood flow (CBF) is a potential early biomarker for AD. CBF is globally decreased in patients with AD compared to age-matched non-demented counterparts. It has been shown that CBF is altered in individuals with preclinical AD as well, and that patterns of altered CBF are correlated with disease severity and progression from one diagnostic stage to the next (Duan et al., 2021). Due to the brain’s sensitivity to changes in blood pressure, chronic hypertension leads to constriction of the cerebral vasculature. Additionally, blood vessels in the brain become more rigid with age, so the combination of chronic hypertension and aging lead to a hyper-constricted cerebral vasculature and ultimately chronically reduced CBF (Iadecola & Gottesman, 2019). Although the longstanding amyloid cascade hypothesis of AD posits that amyloid-β and its aggregation ultimately lead to the disease processes and outcomes in AD (Hardy & Higgins, 1992), the more recently suggested two-hit hypothesis states that cerebrovascular dysfunction and amyloid-β pathology culminate to initiate and propagate AD (Zlokovic, 2011).

CBF has been studied in the context of AD and other dementias for several decades and has been measured by a variety of neuroimaging methods over time. The first measurements of CBF in humans were made in 1948 using the inhalation of nitrous oxide and quantification of the inert gas in venous blood (Kety & Schmidt, 1948). In the 1960s, the use of radioactive tracers including krypton-85 and xenon-133 allowed for the measurement of regional as well as global CBF (Lassen & Ingvar, 1961; Lassen et al., 1963). The development of positron emission tomography (PET) soon led to the use of positron-emitting tracers, particularly oxygen-15, for CBF measurement (Jones et al., 1976). Dynamic contrast enhanced magnetic resonance imaging (DCE MRI) and dynamic susceptibility-contrast (DSC) MRI were developed around the same time as PET and utilized gadolinium-based contrast agents that had magnetic properties to trace and quantify CBF without the need for radioactive tracers (Edelman et al., 1990; Rempp et al., 1994; Maeda et al., 1993).

The most common methods of the studies included in this review are single-photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT) and the more recently developed arterial spin labeled (ASL) MRI. SPECT requires the blood to be labeled with an injection of a radioactive tracer, such as iodine-123 or technetium-99m (Warwick, 2004). ASL MRI, on the other hand, is completely noninvasive. In ASL MRI, blood is labeled as it flows into the brain by inverting the magnetization of the water molecules in the blood and then comparing the labeled image to an unlabeled background image (Detre et al., 2012). ASL MRI can therefore be safely and comfortably used at multiple time points to monitor changes in CBF over the course of disease or treatment.

In order to utilize CBF as an early biomarker for AD, we need to better understand how CBF changes throughout the progression of AD, including in prodromal stages. The roles of other AD-related pathologies (amyloid-β, tau, gray matter atrophy, white matter hyperintensities), AD risk factors (apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 genotype, diabetes, hypertension, and other cardiovascular diseases), and demographic factors (age, sex, race/ethnicity, education, environmental risk factors) must also be taken into consideration when measuring CBF.

The purpose of this review is to consolidate the literature comparing CBF in older adults with MCI or AD and those that are cognitively normal (CN). Patterns of altered CBF are summarized using results from several brain regions. Results are also summarized for the relationship between cognitive exam scores and CBF in older adults. The aim is to compile and synthesize the results of the existing literature concerning altered CBF in MCI and AD. This is important because various methods of CBF measurement have been used over the years; standardization of results will help to clarify whether previous findings are consistent. A recently published review (Zhang et al., 2021) covers the same topic with a broader scope. It includes studies that measured CBF velocity, and it does not exclude articles based on the age of participants with AD. Zhang et al. (2021) did not include papers that report voxel-wise results in the form of z or t scores. The current review employs an age cutoff for the MCI and AD participants included because early onset AD, diagnosed before age 65, may be a different variant of AD; early onset AD patients tend to decline more rapidly and are more likely to have cognitive declines in areas other than memory as the initial symptoms. Therefore, we excluded papers where the mean age of MCI or AD participants minus two standard deviations (SDs) is less than 60, meaning that at least 97% of the individuals studied in each paper are at least 60 years old. This defines the overall population as mostly late onset. We also included papers that report voxel-wise results to assess their consistency with papers that report regional CBF values. A recent meta-analysis (Tang et al., 2022) assessed CBF measured by ASL MRI in individuals with MCI. Our review differs by including papers that measured CBF using a variety of imaging methods and by applying an age cutoff in our inclusion criteria.

The objective of this review is to assess the cumulative evidence of altered CBF in older adults with MCI and AD in multiple brain regions and of the relationship between CBF and cognitive exam scores in these individuals. We also aim to assess the consistency of findings across papers that use a variety of imaging, processing, and analytic methods. Our main question is one of correlation rather than causation between diagnosis and CBF. In reality, it is likely that AD pathologies and altered CBF exacerbate one another and have a cyclical relationship, and it is not clear which of the two is present first or whether both arise simultaneously from a common cause.

METHODS

This systematic review includes all studies in which resting gray matter CBF is compared between diagnostic groups (CN, MCI, AD) and/or correlated with cognitive exam scores. Only studies in which the mean age of MCI/AD patients minus two SDs is at least 60 years old are included. If there was not a SD listed for age of participants with MCI/AD, that paper was not eligible unless the minimum age was at least 60. We chose to use a cutoff for participant age so that a majority of the MCI/AD patients would be over the age of 60 and less likely to have early-onset variants of AD. Both observational studies and clinical trials that measured gray matter CBF at rest prior to treatment are included, as well as both cross-sectional and longitudinal designs. Only manuscripts written in English were included. All manuscripts are published as original research papers or brief reports; conference abstracts were not included. There were no restrictions on the year of publication, and papers that used any established method for the quantification of CBF were eligible for inclusion. Studies that were ineligible either did not measure gray matter CBF at rest or did not use resting gray matter CBF as an outcome measure in multiple diagnostic groups and/or in comparison with cognitive test scores.

PubMed was searched on September 12, 2022, through the National Library of Medicine and with access via Indiana University School of Medicine. 2699 potential articles resulted from a PubMed search of “brain-blood supply” or “cerebrovascular circulation” and “mild cognitive impairment” or “Alzheimer’s disease” as MeSH terms. We did not specify article type or language in the initial PubMed search in order to be inclusive of all possibly relevant articles. The search terms were chosen from PubMed MeSH terms that broadly encompassed the intended key topics of CBF, MCI, and AD.

Due to the correlational nature of the research question, the “population, intervention, control, and outcomes” (PICO) method (Richardson et al., 1995) was not used. Unlike an intervention and outcome, CBF and MCI/AD do not have a clear causal relationship. Therefore, the design used for bias and certainty assessments was a case-control research question in which the condition was MCI/AD and the variable of interest was altered CBF.

For the main synthesis, we describe increase or decrease in CBF between AD and controls in several brain regions. Altered CBF in MCI relative to controls and correlations between CBF and cognitive test scores are described as well. For papers that present relevant data only in the form of a graph, we extracted quantitative data using WebPlotDigitizer version 4.5 (automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer/). For articles that had a high potential of overlapping participant samples, the article with the higher quality rating (less risk for bias) was chosen.

The information recorded for each article includes: author(s), article title, year of publication, place of the study, number of participants, diagnostic groups, criteria by which diagnoses were defined, method used to measure and quantify CBF, ages of participants, whether the paper is included in the synthesis, and which outcomes were reported. To organize this information, we used a matrix from University of Maryland Research Guides (lib.guides.umd.edu/SR/steps).

To assess the risk of bias and the quality of each article, we used the NIH Quality Assessment of Case-Control Studies. The items on this tool are: clearly stated objective, clearly specified and defined population, sample size justification, use of same population for cases and controls, use of same inclusion and exclusion criteria for cases and controls, clearly defined and differentiated cases, random selection of sample from eligible individuals, use of concurrent controls, confirmation that exposure occurred prior to the condition, clearly defined and reliable measures of exposure/risk, blinded assessment of exposure/risk with regard to case/control, and use of matching or addition of confounding variables to statistical models. Overall judgments were “poor,” “fair,” or “good.” These decisions were made according to the guidance included in the tool, where it is explained that lack of some criteria correspond to “fatal flaws” while others do not greatly affect the overall quality of the paper. CBF was considered the “exposure,” and because the direction of causation between MCI/AD and altered CBF is outside the scope of this review, it was not relevant whether the exposure occurred prior to the condition. Therefore, this question was marked as “not applicable” and not used in the overall bias rating. Other items on the rating tool that were not used were: sample size justification, random selection of sample from eligible individuals, and use of concurrent controls, because these items were all either not present or not reported in all or nearly all papers assessed. In addition, blind assessment of the CBF was not applicable for many of the papers because the CBF was measured by fully automated methods. This item was considered for those papers in which hand-drawn regions of interest were used or in which investigators visually assessed CBF images.

For each relevant paper, we reported mean and SD of CBF in each brain region for each diagnostic group. For articles that used voxel-wise measurement of CBF and reported the z or t scores of peak voxels, these scores, along with cluster sizes (k) and p values are reported. For papers that measured the correlation between CBF and cognitive test scores, r and p values are reported. For papers with mean and SD regional CBF values, effect sizes were calculated, and syntheses were presented for multiple brain regions. To measure effect size, we used Hedge’s g, which is Cohen’s d adjusted for small sample sizes, as well as the 95% confidence interval for Hedge’s g. These effect size measures were chosen because CBF is a continuous measure that is dependent on the methods of scanning and processing, so standardized measures were necessary. Effect size was calculated for the difference in CBF between MCI/AD and control groups (control CBF minus MCI/AD CBF) in the following brain regions: frontal, temporal, parietal, temporoparietal, occipital, posterior cingulate, hippocampus, and thalamus. These regions were chosen because they were the most frequently used across all papers. Thresholds were not employed for the effect sizes; we synthesized results for each brain region across studies by reporting the average, median, and range of effect size scores in each region. All Hedge’s g values and 95% confidence intervals are presented in graphs; positive effect sizes denote relatively decreased CBF in MCI/AD. These analyses and graphs were completed using the Effect Size Calculator from the Cambridge Centre for Evaluation and Monitoring (https://www.cem.org/).

A meta-analysis was not performed. Heterogeneity is discussed where there is inconsistency in findings across papers, but statistical analyses regarding heterogeneity were not completed. Sensitivity analyses were also not completed. The scope of this paper is to compile and present the findings in the literature and to discuss overall patterns of results. Reporting bias was taken into account, and certainty ratings for each synthesis are presented in the summary of findings table. Grey literature was not included. Negative findings were included whenever they were reported. For the certainty assessment, we used the GRADE tool from the Cochrane handbook. Scores are “very low,” “low,” “moderate,” and “high.” Since this tool is designed for randomized clinical trials, it instructs the assessor to begin with “moderate” for observational or case-control studies. We rated each synthesis according to the rest of the GRADE criteria but did not strictly begin at “moderate” for each paper because, by design, most of the papers were not clinical trials.

RESULTS

Of 2699 potential articles resulting from a PubMed search of “brain-blood supply” or “cerebrovascular circulation” and “mild cognitive impairment” or “Alzheimer’s disease,” 1309 were retained for further assessment and 1390 were excluded based on their abstracts. Of those excluded, 745 were reviews, editorials, commentaries, case studies, and other types of articles that did not match the eligibility criteria. 381 used animal models or cell cultures rather than human participants, 101 were not in English, and access was not available for 163 articles. Of the 1309 retained, 81 articles were ultimately chosen for inclusion in this review, and 1228 were excluded. Of these, 285 sampled MCI/AD patients who were too young or whose ages were not clearly defined, 542 did not use resting CBF as an outcome, 207 did not compare CBF between diagnostic groups or correlate it with cognitive function, 166 did not include MCI/late onset AD or had mixed patient groups, and 28 were likely to have used the same sample as other articles included in this review. The process of choosing relevant articles is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

PRISMA flow chart of articles identified, assessed for eligibility, and included in systematic review. Reasons for exclusion are listed. Included papers compare resting CBF between AD/MCI and CN, include an AD sample where over 95% are 60 years old or older, and are written in English.

The excluded papers with potentially overlapping samples were: Hanyu et al. (2003a), Hanyu et al. (2003b), Hanyu et al. (2004), Hanyu et al. (2008a), Hanyu et al. (2008b), Sakai et al. (2013), Sakurai et al. (2013), and Hanyu et al. (2010; with Shimizu et al. [2005]), Haji et al. (2015) and Kimura et al. (2012; with Kimura et al. [2011]), Brown et al. (1997; with Brown et al. [1996]), Colloby et al. (2002; with Firbank et al. [2011]), Kawamura et al. (1991) and Mortel et al. (1994; with Obara et al. [1994]), Pearlson et al. (1992) and Harris et al. (1998; with Harris et al. [1991]), Hoyer (1991) and Hoyer (1993; with Hoyer, Nitsch and Oesterreich [1991]), Alegret et al. (2012; with Alegret et al. [2010]), Brugnolo et al. (2010; with Nobili et al. [2005]), Lacalle-Aurioles et al. (2014; with Lacalle-Aurioles et al. [2013]), Sase et al. (2018; with Sase et al. [2017]), Ishii et al. (1999; with Yamaji et al. [1997]), Dai et al. (2009) and Duan et. al. (2021; with Duan et al. [2020]), Yew and Nation (2017) and Camargo and Wang (2021; with Mattsson et al. [2014]), and Albrecht et. al. (2020; with Rubinski et al. [2021]).

Papers included in this review are listed and characterized in Supplementary Table 1. They include: Alegret et al. (2010), Alfini et al. (2019), Ashford et al. (2000), Bangen et al. (2012), Benoit et al. (2002), Brayet et al. (2017), Brown et al. (1996), Chaudhary et al. (2013), Claus et al. (1994), Colloby et al. (2016), DeKosky et al. (1990), Deo et al. (2014), Ding et al. (2014), Duan et al. (2020), Duran et al. (2007), Encinas et al. (2003), Firbank et al. (2003), Firbank et al. (2011), Germán et al. (2021), Habert et al. (2011), Hanyu et al. (2008), Harris et al. (1991), Hayashi et al. (2011), Hayashi et al. (2021), Houston et al. (1998), Hoyer (1991), Iizuka and Kameyama (2017), Imran et al. (1998), Ishiwata et al. (2006), Ito et al. (2013), Jagust et al. (1987), Johnson et al. (1988), Johnson et al. (2007), Kaneta et al. (2017), Keller et al. (2011), Kimura et al. (2011), Knapp et al. (1996), Komatani et al. (1988), Lacalle-Aurioles et al. (2013), Lajoie et al. (2017), Lee et al. (2003), Liu et al. (2015), Lobotesis et al. (2001), Lövblad et al. (2015), Mattsson et al. (2014), Mielke et al. (1994), Mitsumoto et al. (2009), Mubrin et al. (1989), Nagahama et al. (2003), Nobili et al. (2005), O’Mahony et al. (1996), Obara et al. (1994), Ottoy et al. (2019), Pappatà et al. (2010), Prohovnik et al. (1989), Quattrini et al. (2021), Rane et al. (2018), Rubinski et al. (2021), Sase et al. (2017), Schuff et al. (2009), Shimizu et al. (2005), Song et al. (2014), Sundström et al. (2006), Takahashi et al. (2019), Takano et al. (2020), Takenoshita et al. (2020), Tanaka et al. (2002), Tateno et al. (2008), Terada et al. (2013), Tokumitsu et al. (2021), Tosun et al. (2016), Tu et al. (2022), van de Haar et al. (2016), Wang et al. (2009), Warkentin et al. (2004), Xie et al. (2016), Yamaji et al. (1997), Yamashita et al. (2014), Yoshida et al. (2011), Yoshida et al. (2017), and Zurynski et al. (1989). Supplementary Table 1 provides the following information for each included article: author, title, year of publication, location of the study (or study authors), sample size by diagnostic group, criteria used to define diagnostic groups, type of scan used to measure CBF, ages of participants in each diagnostic group, whether the paper was included in the synthesis, and which outcomes of interest were included. Notes are included to clarify special circumstances as necessary. Of the 81 papers, 49 measured CBF using SPECT, 18 using ASL MRI, one using both, and 13 using other methods. Publication dates range from 1987 to 2022.

Table 1 reports the component scores and overall quality rating on the risk of bias assessment for each paper. Justification for each score is given in the “Overall Quality Rating and Notes” column. Of the 81 papers, 36 were rated as “good,” 31 were rated as “fair,” and 14 were rated as “poor.” All of the papers rated “poor” either lacked cognitive test scores for the CN group, without which it is not clear that the CN group is free of cognitive decline, or had a CN group that was much younger than the MCI/AD group and thus not part of the same population.

Table 1.

Risk of bias assessment scores for included articles.

Authors Clearly Stated Objective Clearly Defined Sample Controls and Cases from same Population Same Inclusion Criteria for Controls and Cases Clear Distinction of Cases and Controls Reliable Methods to Measure CBF Blind Assessment of CBF Confounding Variables Score and Justification
Alegret et al. yes no yes no yes yes not applicable yes fair; little information about samples; AD group were all taking AChEIs while the other groups were not, and most CN did not have CT to check for exclusions
Alfini et. al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Ashford et. al. yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes not reported no fair; it appears that scan images were assembled into 3D surface pictures partly by visualizing brain landmarks, and it is not mentioned whether any manual visualization was done by blinded investigators
Bangen et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not reported no fair; image processing included manual editing of regions of interest, and it is not mentioned whether investigators were blinded to diagnosis
Benoit et al. yes yes no not reported not reported yes for AD; not reported for CN not applicable no poor; CN are from a different population, scanned separately but “similarly”, not clear whether the inclusion criteria is the same, and cognitive scores for the CN group are not reported
Brayet et. al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable no good
Brown et al. yes no yes yes yes yes yes no good
Chaudhary et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not reported yes fair; image processing included manual editing of regions of interest, and it is not mentioned whether investigators were blinded to diagnosis
Claus et al. yes yes no yes yes yes yes yes good
Colloby et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
DeKosky et al. yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes yes no good
Deo et al. yes yes yes no yes yes not applicable yes fair; all AD are taking cognitive enhancing medications
Ding et al. yes yes not reported yes yes yes not applicable no fair; it is not reported whether the groups are from the same population; only CN are excluded for substance abuse
Duan et. al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Duran et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable no good
Encinas et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes yes no good
Firbank et al. 2003 yes yes yes yes yes yes not reported no fair; image processing included manual editing of regions of interest, and it is not mentioned whether investigators were blinded to diagnosis
Firbank et al. 2011 yes no not reported not reported yes yes yes no fair; little information about sample; unclear whether population and/or inclusion criteria are the same between groups
Germán et al. yes yes yes no yes yes not applicable no fair; all AD have positive PIB PET scans and show neurodegeneration on FDG PET or CT
Habert et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable no good
Hanyu et al. yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes yes no good
Harris et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes yes no good
Hayashi et. al. 2011 yes yes yes no yes yes not applicable yes fair; the only criteria listed for the control group is a normal MMSE score
Hayashi et al. 2021 yes yes no no yes yes not applicable no fair; the controls were already incorporated into the eZIS SPECT analysis program; all AD showed MTL atrophy and were amyloid beta positive on PIB PET
Houston et al. yes no no no yes yes not applicable no poor; the control groups are much younger than AD and one is all men; this is because that control group was matched to the group of boxers rather than AD. It is not clear where any of the participant groups were recruited from.
Hoyer, Nitsch & Oesterreich yes yes no no yes yes not applicable no poor; the controls are much younger than the advanced late onset AD group
Iizuka & Kameyama yes yes for cases, no for controls not reported not reported yes yes not applicable no fair; it is not clear whether the groups are from the same population; only AD are excluded for taking anti-anxiety or antidepressant medications
Imran et al. yes no not reported no no yes not applicable no poor; it is not clear whether cases and controls are from the same population; only AD have scores reported on MMS and CDR
Ishiwata et al. yes yes yes yes no yes yes yes fair; NINCDS/ADRDA was used to exclude MCI, but the paper did not specifically say that it was used to diagnose AD.
Ito et. al. yes yes yes yes yes yes yes yes good
Jagust et al. yes no yes yes not reported yes not reported no poor; while not explicitly clear, it can be deduced that groups are from the same population; inclusion criteria appears to be the same; cognitive exam scores are not reported for CN
Johnson et al. 1988 yes no not reported yes yes yes no yes fair; it is not clear where the participants were recruited from or whether AD and CN came from the same population; ROIs may have been manually placed, and it is not clear whether this was done by blinded investigators
Johnson et al. 2007 yes no yes for the MCI at baseline; unknown for AD yes yes yes not applicable no fair; not clear whether the AD are from the same population as the MCI at baseline. Inclusion criteria is also not applied to several MCI, who are younger than the reported 65 year cutoff
Kaneta et. al. yes no yes yes yes for AD, not reported for MCI yes not applicable no fair; all patients are MCI or AD, but it isn’t clear where they were recruited from or how MCI is defined here
Keller et. al. yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes not applicable no good
Kimura et al. yes yes for cases, no for controls not reported yes yes yes not applicable no fair; little information about the CN group; not clear if they are from the same population as AD
Knapp et. al. yes no not reported no yes yes yes no fair; it is not clear whether the cases and controls are from the same population, and they have slightly different exclusion criteria
Komatani et al. yes yes for controls, no for AD not reported not reported not reported yes not reported no poor; the criteria used for diagnostic classification is not explicitly stated, and it is not stated how the CN group was confirmed to be CN; image processing may have included manually placing ROIs, and it is not stated whether investigators were blinded
Lacalle-Aurioles et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes yes no good
Lajoie et. al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Lee et al. yes no not reported not reported yes yes not applicable no fair; little information given about the sample, not clear whether the groups are from the same population or if they have the same inclusion criteria
Liu et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Lobotesis et al. yes yes yes not reported yes yes yes no fair; the exclusion criteria for the AD group is not reported
Lövblad et. al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable no good
Mattsson et. al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Mielke et al. yes no not reported no yes yes not applicable no fair; it is not clear whether AD and CN are from the same population, and only AD are tested/ excluded for ischemic infarcts or brain tumors
Mitsumoto et al. yes yes yes not reported yes yes yes no fair; the exclusion criteria is not reported
Mubrin et al. yes no not reported not reported not reported yes for imaging; not reported for processing not reported no poor; very little information about samples, especially CN; CN does not have cognitive scores reported
Nagahama et al. yes yes for cases, no for controls not reported yes not reported yes not applicable no poor; little information about CN, not sure if they are from the same population as AD; cognitive scores not reported for CN
Nobili et al. yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes not applicable yes good
O’Mahony et al. yes yes yes no yes yes no no fair; only AD had screening with CT scans; it appears that ROIs were manually placed, and it is not stated whether investigators were blinded to diagnosis
Obara et al. yes no not reported no yes yes not reported yes fair; little information about sample; not sure whether groups are from the same population; inclusion criteria differs slightly between groups
Ottoy et. al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Pappatà et al. yes yes not reported yes yes yes no yes fair; it is not clear where participants are recruited from; ROIs are manually adjusted, but it is not stated whether investigators were blinded to diagnosis
Prohovnik et al. yes yes yes not reported no yes no yes poor; inclusion criteria is not fully reported, CN do not have cognitive scores reported, and it is not stated whether the investigators were blinded during image processing
Quattrini et al. yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes not applicable yes good
Rane et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Rubinski et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Sase et al. yes yes yes no yes yes not applicable no fair; inclusion criteria differs slightly between groups
Schuff et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Shimizu et al. yes yes for cases, no for controls not reported no not reported yes not applicable no poor; not reported whether the groups are from the same population; only AD are excluded for taking medication that affects the CNS; cognitive exam scores are not reported for CN
Song et al. yes yes yes no yes yes not applicable no fair; only controls are excluded for history of head trauma, epilepsy, stroke, or brain surgery
Sundström et al. yes yes no yes yes yes not applicable no poor; CN are younger than AD
Takahashi et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes yes no good
Takano et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable no good
Takenoshita et al. yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes not applicable no good
Tanaka et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Tateno et al. yes no not reported no yes yes not applicable no fair; little information given about the sample, not clear whether the groups are from the same population; only CN have abnormalities on scans as exclusion criteria
Terada et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable no good
Tokumitsu et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Tosun et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Tu et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes no yes fair; ROIs were manually placed, and it is not stated whether investigators were blinded to diagnosis
van de Haar et al. yes yes yes no not reported yes yes yes poor; only AD are excluded for history of stroke; CN are said to complete cognitive exams, but their scores are not reported
Wang et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes yes no good
Warkentin et al. yes yes no yes yes yes not applicable no poor; CN are much younger than AD
Xie et al. yes yes yes yes yes yes not applicable yes good
Yamaji et al. yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes not applicable no good
Yamashita et al. yes yes not reported yes yes yes not applicable no fair; it is not clear where the controls were recruited from
Yoshida et al. 2011 yes yes yes no yes yes yes no fair; inclusion criteria are not the same: all AD have APOE ε4, but this is not reported for CN
Yoshida et al. 2017 yes yes not applicable not applicable yes yes not applicable yes good
Zurynski et al. yes yes Yes yes no yes not applicable yes poor; it is not clear whether CN completed cognitive testing

AD: Alzheimer’s disease, AchEI: acetyl cholinesterase inhibitor, APOE: apolipoprotein E, CDR: Clinical Dementia Rating, CBF: cerebral blood flow, CN: cognitively normal, CNS: central nervous system, CT: computed tomography, eZIS: easy Z-score imaging system, FDG: fluorodeoxyglucose, MCI: mild cognitive impairment, MMS/ MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination, MTL: medial temporal lobe, NINCDS/ADRDA: National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke/Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association, PET: positron emission tomography, PIB: Pittsburgh Compound-B, ROI: region of interest, SPECT: single-photon emission computerized tomography

Table 2 and Supplementary Tables 2 and 3 report the results of interest for each study. Table 2 reports the relative CBF in AD, Supplementary Table 2 reports relative CBF in MCI, and Supplementary Table 3 presents correlations between CBF and cognitive exam scores. In Table 2 and Supplementary Table 2, regional CBF data is given as mean (SD) for each diagnostic group, when available. For regional CBF in these tables and further analyses, values for left and right regions were averaged, with SDs pooled; this was also done across smaller brain regions within those of interest. When bilateral or subregional averaging was done, this is clarified in the “Notes” column. For voxel-wise results, cluster size (k), p value, and z or t value are reported, and the specific region where the peak voxel is located is given in the “Notes” column. The “Notes” column also mentions if the data were extracted from a graph rather than reported numerically in the paper. All results are reported, regardless of whether they were statistically significant. Papers with qualitative results are included in these tables as well, for completeness.

Table 2.

CBF in CN and AD in each paper.

Author Sample Size Frontal Parietal Temporal Temporoparietal Occipital Posterior Cingulate Hippocampus Thalamus Notes
Alegret et al. CN: 42
AD: 42
AD < CN

CN: 667 (25)
AD: 504 (26)
AD < CN

CN: 1028 (25)
AD: 802 (24)
AD < CN

CN: 885 (20)
AD: 696 (26)
AD < CN

CN: 716 (23)
AD: 535 (22)
Values are eigenvariates from peak voxels where CBF differs between groups; SDs are adjusted for age; data extracted from bar graph. Regions are temporal: left temporal lobe and right temporal pole, temporoparietal: right angular gyrus, and right hippocampus.
Benoit et al. CN: 11
AD: 30
AD < CN

z = 3.79
k = 1322
p = 0.008
AD < CN

z = 3.25
k = 1431
p = 0.005
Results are from comparison of the whole AD group (with and without apathy) and CN; voxels correspond to right medial frontal and right posterior cingulate regions.
Brown et al. CN: 14
AD: 24
AD < CN

CN: 0.854 (0.040)
AD: 0.796 (0.086)
AD < CN

CN: 0.890 (0.046)
AD: 0.764 (0.104)
AD < CN

CN: 0.917 (0.050)
AD: 0.839 (0.077)
Values are regional CBF normalized to calcarine cortex; regions are frontal: low frontal & high frontal; temporal: temporal & posterior temporal; data extracted from bar graph; significance reported with Bonferroni correction
Chaudhary et al. CN:20
AD: 25
AD < CN

CN: 48.56 (+10.14, −5.53)
AD: 44.64 (+10.31, −11.34)
AD < CN

CN: 54.95 (+9.44, −6.70)
AD: 50.93 (+9.71, −9.55)
AD < CN

CN: 59.28 (+18.44, −15.67)
AD: 51.24 (+13.41, −10.31)
The paper presented results in a graph in which error bars were not symmetrical about the mean. The temporal region combines lateral and medial temporal.

Global GM CBF AD < CN

CN: 55.36 (+10.14, −8.30)
AD: 51.24 (+11.34, −9.28)

AD < CN in anterior cingulate as well.

CN: 51.03 (+12.91, −12.91)
AD: 43.81 (+13.41, −12.37)
Claus et al. CN: 60
AD: 48
AD < CN
CN: 84.3 (5.6)
AD: 80.8 (6.3)
AD < CN

CN: 83.0 (5.2)
AD: 76.0 (7.1)
AD < CN

CN: 78.4 (6.8)
AD: 70.5 (9.8)
AD < CN

CN: 89.2 (6.1)
AD: 86.5 (6.4)
Values are percentages of regional CBF relative to cerebellar CBF; anatomical ROI multi-slice values used.
Colloby et al. CN: 16
AD: 13
Mixed results; some frontal regions AD < CN and others AD > CN

Middle frontal z = −2.1; medial frontal z = 2.5
AD < CN

z = −2.9
AD < CN

z = −1.9
AD > CN

z = 2.0
AD < CN

z = −2.6
AD < CN

z = −2.4
This paper measured the degree to which each participant exhibited the spatial distribution of altered CBF that differentiated AD from CN. AD exhibited the pattern more than CN and thus scored higher on this measure.

CN: 3.2 (1.5)
AD: 10.0 (1.9)

Regional differences reported contribute to the overall pattern and are the peak voxels in the region. Negative z scores denote decreased CBF in AD relative to CN. Specific peak regions are frontal: medial and middle frontal; inferior parietal, middle temporal, and lingual gyrus (occipital).
Deo et al. CN: 9
AD: 9
AD < CN AD < CN

CN: 0.85 (0.08)
AD: 0.77 (0.11)
AD < CN

CN: 0.91 (0.06)
AD: 0.74 (0.11)
AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN Values are ratio of regional CBF to cerebellar CBF. Regions without values come from the comparison of averaged CBF maps from AD and CN.
Ding et al. CN: 21
AD: 24
AD > CN

t = −3.95
k = 312
p < 0.01
AD < CN

t = 3.34
k = 391
p < 0.01
AD < CN

t = 3.33
k = 340
p < 0.01
AD < CN

t = 4.77
k = 2569
p < 0.01
AD > CN

t = −3.32
k = 355
p < 0.01
Voxels are: right paracentral, right superior parietal, right middle & inferior temporal, left superior, middle & inferior occipital & cuneus, and left thalamus
Duan et al. CN: 58
MCI: 50
AD: 40
AD < CN
AD < MCI

MCI: 1.547 (0.788)
AD: −1.500 (0.471)
AD < CN
AD < MCI

MCI: −0.264 (0.245)
AD: −1.698 (0.208)
AD < CN
AD < MCI

MCI: 1.481 (0.440)
AD: −1.510 (0.395)
AD < CN
AD < MCI

MCI: −0.604 (0.264)
AD: −2.226 (0.265)
AD < CN
AD < MCI

MCI: −0.010 (1.058)
AD: −1.293 (0.670)
Global CBF AD < MCI and AD < CN

CN: 38.41 (5.15)
MCI: 39.66 (5.49)
AD: 37.21 (3.98)

Regional values are normalized to global CBF and transformed into z scores relative to CN. Negative z values denote decreased CBF relative to CN. Regions are averaged bilaterally and include: right and left inferior frontal and insular and bilateral superior medial frontal, right inferior parietal, left and right temporoparietal, bilateral posterior cingulate and precuneus, right and left hippocampus.
Duran et al. CN: 15
AD: 19
AD < CN

z = 2.93
k = 197
p > 0.05
AD < CN

z = 3.31
k = 882
p=0.04
AD < CN

z = 3.16
k = 932
p=0.04
AD < CN

z = 3.63
k = 617
p = 0.04
Peak voxels are reported for each region and include: left precuneus, left insula, left middle temporal gyrus, and right posterior cingulate; AD > CN in bilateral cerebellum and right pre- and post-central gyri.
Encinas et al. stable MCI: 21
MCI progressing to AD: 21
AD < MCI

MCI: 89.8 (10)
AD: 80.8 (7.8)
AD < MCI

MCI: 90.0 (8.6)
AD: 82.0 (8.5)
AD < MCI

MCI: 85.0 (9.4)
AD: 76.0 (9.0)
AD < MCI

MCI: 87.5 (9.1)
AD: 79.5 (7.5)
Values are percentage of regional CBF over cerebellar CBF; comparisons are stable MCI vs. MCI that progress to AD, at baseline; frontal: right and left prefrontal, right and left frontal; parietal: right and left parietal; temporal: right and left temporal, left posterior lateral temporal; temporoparietal: right and left frontoparietotemporal
Firbank et al. 2003 CN: 37
AD: 32
AD < CN

p = 0.006
AD < CN Voxels for parietal region are in left precuneus. There is no p value listed for the posterior cingulate.
Firbank et al. 2011 CN: 29
AD: 17
AD < CN

CN: 1.82 (0.27)
AD: 1.58 (0.28)
AD < CN

CN: 1.70 (0.32)
AD: 1.34 (0.31)
AD < CN

CN: 1.55 (0.27)
AD: 1.47 (0.29)
AD < CN

CN: 2.68 (1.08)
AD: 2.28 (0.71)
Values are GM:WM ratio; one control does not have data for thalamus; statistics are Gabriel post hoc tests; frontal: prefrontal.
Germán et al. CN: 22
AD: 18
AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN All differences are present at a level of p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Frontal regions are: lateral orbitofrontal, medial orbitofrontal, and superior frontal. Parietal regions are inferior parietal and superior parietal. Temporal regions are: inferior temporal, middle temporal, superior temporal, and transverse temporal. All regions are bilateral.
Habert et al. Stable MCI: 72
MCI convert to AD: 11
Converters < non-converters

z = 3.86
k = 2679
p = 0.041
Converters < non-converters

z = 4.10
k = 749
p = 0.041
CBF values were measured at baseline, before any participants converted to AD. Regions are temporoparietal: right supramarginal, right angular, and right inferior parietal; and right hippocampus.
Harris et al. CN: 8
AD: 15
AD < CN

CN: 101.6 (8.8)
AD: 91.7 (10.7)
AD < CN

CN: 105.8 (8.2)
AD: 89.4 (11.9)
AD < CN

CN: 106.0 (8.9)
AD: 97.6 (11.3)
All regions are combined from two slices: at basal ganglia level and superior to it
Hayashi et al. 2021 AD: 24 AD < CN AD < CN Values are z-scores, where positive z-scores denote a reduction in CBF.

Posterior cingulate, precuneus, and parietal lobe CBF AD < CN

AD: 1.55 (0.57)
Houston et al. CN1: 50
CN2: 40
AD: 21
CN1: 0.05
CN2: 0.33
AD: 2.70
CN1: 0.09
CN2: 0.22
AD: 2.13
CN1: 0.10
CN2: 0.51
AD: 2.07
CN1: 0.05
CN2: 0.83
AD: 3.05
Values are percentages of abnormal (outside 3 SDs) voxels in each region. Regions are averaged as follows: frontal (left and right frontal and left and right inferior frontal), parietal (left and right), temporal (left and right inferior temporal).
Hoyer, Nitsch & Oesterreich CN: 11
AD: 7
Global CBF
AD < CN

CN: 55.7 (3.0)
AD: 24.3 (3.9)
Iizuka & Kameyama. CN: 9
AD: 36
AD < CN AD <CN AD < CN AD < CN Results were qualitative in the text and presented in a brain surface figure with a color scale, but it was not feasible to accurately extract values based on color. Comparisons were at baseline and were present in CN vs. responding and/or nonresponding AD before treatment began.
Imran et al. CN: 10
AD: 10
AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN in bilateral superior and inferior parietal lobules, bilateral inferior temporal gyri, and bilateral superior frontal gyri at t = 2.1 and p < 0.05.
Ishiwata et al. CN: 10
AD: 6
AD > CN

1%

* voxel-wise results show a pattern of AD < CN in the frontal lobe
AD < CN

−8%
AD < CN

−3%
AD < CN

−2%
AD < CN

−5%
AD < CN

−3%
Values are percentage differences (AD minus CN) in regional CBF normalized to global CBF. Regions are lateral frontal, lateral parietal, lateral temporal, and lateral occipital.
Ito et al. MCI who do not convert: 113
MCI who convert to AD: 99
Converters < non-converters Converters < non-converters Converters < non-converters Regions are bilateral; temporal is medial temporal.
Jagust et al. CN: 5
AD: 9
AD > CN

CN: 1.11 (0.09)
AD: 1.16 (0.08)
AD < CN

CN: 1.10 (0.05)
AD: 0.93 (0.04)
Values are regional CBF over whole tomographic slice
Johnson et al. 1988 CN: 9
AD: 33
AD < CN

CN: 0.69 (0.10)
AD: 0.62 (0.10)
AD < CN

CN: 0.73 (0.10)
AD: 0.57 (0.11)
AD < CN

CN: 0.75 (0.09)
AD: 0.67 (0.08)
AD < CN

CN: 0.90 (0.10)
AD: 0.81 (0.11)
Values are regional CBF to cerebellar CBF ratios. The AD averages are calculated without the four most severe cases, who were too young for this review, but the SDs for the AD group were calculated with the 4, which is not expected to have a large effect on the SDs.
Johnson et al. 2007 CN: 19
AD: 34
AD < CN
z = 3.59
k = 214
corr. p < 0.05
AD < CN
z = 5.20
k = 6629
corr. p < 0.05
AD < CN
z = 4.01
k = 2117
corr. p < 0.05
AD < CN

z = 4.87
k = 862
corr. p < 0.05
AD > CN

z = −4.42
k = 1184
corr. p < 0.05
Voxels are located in: left inferior frontal, left inferior parietal, right superior temporal, right posterior cingulate, and left thalamus & striatum; p values are corrected for multiple comparisons.
Kimura et al. CN: 27
AD: 62
AD < CN

CN: 35.4 (4.3)
AD: 30.5 (5.2)
AD < CN

CN: 35.3 (3.6)
AD: 30.8 (4.7)
AD < CN

CN: 40.4 (4.6)
AD: 34.5 (5.9)
AD < CN

CN: 41.3 (5.1)
AD: 35.9 (5.5)
Regions are: left superior and inferior parietal, left and right middle and inferior temporal, right angular gyrus, and left and right posterior cingulate
Knapp et al. CN: 14
AD: 30
AD < CN

CN: 84.28 (6.08)
AD: 78.00 (9.65)
AD < CN

CN: 87.65 (6.30)
AD: 77.20 (10.24)
AD < CN

CN: 80.4 (9.65)
AD: 73.85 (9.51)
AD < CN

CN: 83.95 (8.30)
AD: 79.55 (7.66)
AD < CN

CN: 89.50 (6.44)
AD: 85.30 (11.66)
Values are mean count density (%) relative to cerebellum. All regions were averaged bilaterally. Frontal region includes prefrontal and frontal. Temporal region includes inferior temporal, lateral temporal, and temporal.
Komatani et al. CN: 7
AD: 9
AD > CN

CN: 1.04 (0.02)
AD: 1.08 (0.07)
AD < CN

CN: 0.98 (0.05)
AD: 0.84 (0.11)
Whole brain CBF is AD < CN, CN: 50.9 (6.9); AD: 39.7 (5.2); frontal and temporoparietal values are regional: whole brain CBF ratios.
Lacalle-Aurioles et al. CN: 20
AD: 28
AD < CN

CN: 0.849 (0.032)
AD: 0.812 (0.051)
AD > CN

CN: 0.778 (0.063)
AD: 0.828 (0.069)
Values are regional CBF over whole cortical GM CBF; data was extracted from graphs; regions are: left and right parietal lobe, right medial temporal lobe.
Lajoie et al. CN: 37
AD: 34
AD < CN

CN: 39.5 (10.8)
AD: 37.4 (9.0)
AD < CN

CN: 39.3 (13.1)
AD: 32.7 (9.8)
AD < CN

CN: 41.2 (10.8)
AD: 36.1 (8.3)
AD < CN

CN: 43.7 (11.4)
AD: 39.9 (8.6)
Lee et al. CN: 20
AD: 20
AD < CN

z = 4.64
corr. p = 0.007
AD < CN

z = 6.87
corr. p < 0.001
AD < CN

z = 6.94
corr. p < 0.001
Used moderate AD as the AD group; voxels come from: left middle frontal, right superior parietal, and right superior temporal; cluster sizes not reported; p values corrected for multiple testing.
Liu et al. CN: 19
AD: 16
AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN Regions are left superior frontal, left inferior parietal, bilateral temporal, left angular (temporoparietal), and bilateral posterior cingulate. t = 5.32, p < 0.001 for overall voxel-wise comparison.
Lobotesis et al. CN: 20
AD: 50
AD < CN

CN: 0.90 (0.05)
AD: 0.85 (0.07)
AD < CN

CN: 0.92 (0.06)
AD: 0.87 (0.09)
AD < CN

CN: 0.89 (0.06)
AD: 0.83 (0.09)
AD < CN

CN: 1.00 (0.04)
AD: 0.97 (0.08)
AD < CN

CN: 0.98 (0.06)
AD: 0.93 (0.11)
Values are ratio of regional CBF: cerebellar CBF using the cerebellar lobe with the highest CBF. All regions were averaged across hemispheres and the temporal region includes both temporal and medial temporal measurements.
Mattsson et al. CN: 51
AD: 24
AD < CN

CN: 23.67 (5.58)
AD: 21.37 (6.31)
AD < CN

CN: 32.5 (5.85)
AD: 26.28 (4.88)
AD < CN

CN: 26.08 (6.41)
AD: 19.96 (6.26)
AD < CN

CN: 31.22 (6.74)
AD: 27.79 (9.17)
AD < CN

CN: 29.47 (6.32)
AD: 25.41 (6.34)
Frontal region is medial orbitofrontal, temporal is inferior temporal, and parietal is inferior parietal.
Mielke et al. CN: 13
AD: 20
AD = CN

CN: 0.94 (0.03)
AD: 0.94 (0.04)
AD < CN

CN: 0.98 (0.01)
AD: 0.96 (0.03)
AD < CN

CN: 1.06 (0.04)
AD: 1.04 (0.07)
AD > CN

CN: 1.21 (0.05)
AD: 1.27 (0.09)
Values are ratio of regional CBF: global CBF. The paper also reports values with cerebellum as the reference region. Regions are frontal association cortex, temporoparietal association cortex, and occipital association cortex.
Mitsumoto et al. CN: 7
AD: 13
AD < CN

CN: 0.93 (0.07)
AD: 0.85 (0.06)
AD < CN

CN: 1.01 (0.07)
AD: 0.88 (0.05)
Values are ratio of regional CBF to cerebellar CBF. Parietal region is the combination of superior and inferior parietal lobules.
Mubrin et al. CN: not clear
AD: 31
AD < CN AD < CN Results are qualitative only; both regions are bilateral.
Obara et al. CN: 18
AD: 17
AD < CN

CN: 42.3 (9.4)
AD: 37.5 (4.4)
AD < CN

CN: 43.1 (6.9)
AD: 37.7 (4.9)
AD < CN

CN: 43.1 (7.6)
AD: 38.2 (6.1)
AD < CN

CN: 39.5 (8.8)
AD: 35.9 (4.7)
AD < CN

CN: 51.8 (10.7)
AD: 47.0 (9.4)
O’Mahony et al. CN: 12
AD: 18
AD < CN

CN: 0.788 (0.053)
AD: 0.756 (0.068)
AD < CN

CN: 0.863 (0.048)
AD: 0.758 (0.051)
AD < CN

CN: 0.785 (0.043)
AD: 0.719 (0.054)
AD < CN

CN: 0.929 (0.055)
AD: 0.889 (0.093)
Values are ratio of regional CBF to cerebellar CBF.
Ottoy et al. CN: 10
AD: 10
AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN

CN: 1.26 (0.1)
AD: 0.98 (0.2)
AD > CN
CN: 0.86 (0.1)
AD: 0.87 (0.1)
Values are ratio of regional CBF to cerebellar CBF. This paper validates several imaging methods; values reported here are from the established 15O-H2O PET method. Frontal and parietotemporal CBF reductions were only described from a voxel-wise image (k =. 50, p < 0.05, corrected for age and education).
Prohovnik et al. CN: 12
AD: 18
Total gray matter CBF AD < CN
CN: 50.31 (6.17)
AD: 42.04 (5.33).
Rubinski et al. CN: 84
AD: 21
Global gray matter CBF AD < CN

CN: 42.39 (9.73)
AD: 33.70 (12.31)

Partial volume effect correction was used.
Sase et al. CN: 15
AD: 27
This paper measures CBF in 5 mm layers from the surface of the brain. AD < CN in cingulate cortex and in every layer measured; measurements of layers were taken from lateral, superior, anterior, and posterior views. The greatest differences were found in the left lateral view of the third and fourth layers.
Schuff et al. CN: 18
AD: 14
AD < CN
CN: 56.4 (7.9)
AD: 41.2 (10.9)
AD < CN
CN: 58.7 (9.8)
AD: 43.6 (11.7)
AD < CN
CN: 52.7 (19.1)
AD: 45.5 (15.2)
Shimizu et al. CN: 28
AD: 75
AD < CN
AD: 1.08 (0.35)
AD < CN
AD: 1.39 (0.55)
AD < CN
AD: 1.40 (0.65)
AD < CN
AD: 0.90 (0.43)
Values are z scores normalized to CN values
Song et al. CN: 33
AD: 32
AD < CN

z = 6.96
k = 49, 544
AD < CN

z = 5.63
k = 49, 544
AD < CN

z = 4.42
k = 49, 544
AD < CN

z = 5.46
k = 49, 544
Regions are bilateral; frontal: inferior and medial frontal; inferior parietal, inferior temporal, and cingulate gyri. Z and expected k are reported for the peak cluster in each region.
Sundström et al. CN: 18
AD: 18
AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN Voxel-wise comparison are presented graphically but not numerically; frontal region is mainly middle frontal
Takahashi et al. AD: 9 Three nuclear medicine experts blinded to diagnosis visually assessed SPECT images based on a typical AD pattern of hypoperfusion. They rated the likelihood of this pattern as follows: very likely in 2 AD, likely in 4 AD, unlikely in 3 AD. None were given the rating of very unlikely.
Takano et al. CN: 12
AD: 19
AD < CN

CN: 1.68 (0.53)
AD: 1.83 (0.56)
AD < CN

CN: 1.89 (0.83)
AD: 2.16 (0.88)
AD < CN

CN: 1.78 (0.62)
AD: 1.85 (0.71)
AD < CN

CN: 1.26 (0.54)
AD: 1.28 (0.65)
AD < CN

CN: 1.80 (0.59)
AD: 2.25 (0.65)
AD < CN

CN: 1.70 (0.72)
AD: 2.00 (0.78)
Values are z scores relative to the global mean of a normal brain database. Positive z scores denote hypoperfusion. Frontal region includes: superior frontal, middle frontal, inferior frontal, medial frontal, orbital gyrus, rectal gyrus, paracentral lobule, precentral gyrus, and subcallosal gyrus. Parietal region includes: superior parietal lobule, inferior parietal lobule, angular gyrus, postcentral gyrus, precuneus, and supramarginal gyrus. Temporal includes: superior temporal, middle temporal, inferior temporal, and transverse temporal. Occipital includes: superior occipital, middle occipital, inferior occipital, cuneus, fusiform, and lingual gyrus.
Tateno et al. CN: 16
AD: 38
AD < CN
CN: 42.2 (3.5)
AD: 37.2 (3.9)
AD < CN
CN: 36.8 (5.3)
AD: 33.7 (4.1)
AD < CN
CN: 45.2 (4.0)
AD: 38.5 (4.9)
AD < CN
CN: 30.4 (5.2)
AD: 26.3 (3.1)
AD < CN
CN: 39.3 (7.2)
AD: 34.4 (5.7)
Values were extracted from bar graphs; the temporoparietal data is from the angular gyrus
Tokumitsu et al. AD: 128 This paper calculates z scores normalized to a control database for the severity of CBF decrease in bilateral posterior cingulate, precunei, and parietal cortices. Positive z-scores denote reduced CBF.
AD < CN
AD: 1.29 (0.35).
Tu et al. CN: 30
AD: 50
AD < CN

CN: 53.0 (9.9)
AD: 48.7 (9.9)
AD < CN

CN: 53.6 (11.2)
AD: 51.4 (11.1)
Regions are averaged bilaterally.The frontal region includes superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri. The temporal region includes superior, middle, and inferior temporal gyri.
van de Haar et al. CN: 16
MCI/AD: 14
MCI/AD < CN

CN: 38.9 (7.2)
MCI/AD: 33.3 (7.6)
MCI/AD < CN

CN: 39.6 (6.7)
MCI/AD: 32.4 (7.4)
MCI/AD < CN

CN: 36.8 (6.2)
MCI/AD: 31.1 (7.6)
MCI/AD < CN

CN: 37.9 (8.3)
MCI/AD: 30.7 (9.1)
The AD group includes MCI
Warkentin et al. CN: 92
AD: 132
AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN AD < CN Global CBF AD < CN

CN: 45.9 (7.2)
AD: 36.9 (5.5)

A cluster analysis was used to group AD participants according to their hypoperfusion patterns. All 132 had decreased CBF in temporal and temporoparietal regions. In addition to this hypoperfusion, 67 AD had hypoperfusion in mostly parietal regions, and 65 AD had hypoperfusion in mostly frontal regions.
Yoshida et al. 2011 CN: 8
AD: 8
AD > CN
CN: 0.80 (0.06)
AD: 0.82 (0.09)
AD < CN
CN: 0.84 (0.08)
AD: 0.79 (0.11)
AD < CN
CN: 0.76 (0.03)
AD: 0.63 (0.11)
AD < CN
CN: 0.84 (0.11)
AD: 0.60 (0.11)
Values are regional CBF: occipital cortex CBF ratios; the temporal region is the medial temporal lobe.
Zurynski et al. CN: 15
AD: 15
Global CBF AD < CN

CN: 44.7 (7.4)
AD: 31.2 (6.4)

AD: Alzheimer’s disease, CBF: cerebral blood flow, CN: cognitively normal, GM: grey matter, MCI: mild cognitive impairment, PET: positron emission tomography, ROI: region of interest, SD: standard deviation, SPECT: single-photon emission computerized tomography, WM: white matter

With respect to reported values regardless of statistical significance, most of the 57 included papers reported relatively decreased CBF in AD in all brain regions we included. Relatively increased CBF in AD was reported in some papers in the following regions: frontal, temporal and occipital lobes, hippocampus and thalamus. Most of the 16 papers that assessed MCI reported relatively decreased CBF as well, but there was a greater proportion of relatively increased CBF in MCI than in AD. Relatively decreased CBF in MCI was reported in all included regions, and relatively increased CBF in MCI was reported in the frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes and the temporoparietal region. The following paragraphs describe the frequency of each finding. These findings are listed for each paper in Table 2 for AD and Supplementary Table 2 for MCI.

Decreased relative CBF in AD was reported in the frontal lobe in 31 papers, in the parietal lobe in 37 papers, in the temporal lobe in 34 papers, in temporoparietal regions in 17 papers, in the occipital lobe in 14 papers, in the posterior cingulate in 22 papers, in the hippocampus in five papers, and in the thalamus in seven papers. Relative increases in CBF in AD were reported in the frontal lobe in five papers, in the temporal lobe in one paper, in the occipital lobe in one paper, in the hippocampus in one paper, and in the thalamus in three papers. One paper reported regions of both increased and decreased relative CBF in the frontal lobe, and another paper reported no relative difference of CBF in the frontal lobe. One paper reported a higher percentage of abnormal CBF voxels in AD in frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital regions, and one paper reported relatively decreased CBF in AD in a combined posterior cingulate, precuneus, and parietal region. Of the papers that did not report relative CBF by the included regions, five papers reported lower global CBF in AD compared to CN and one paper reported that 6 of 9 AD participants showed AD-typical patterns of decreased CBF. Additionally, individuals with MCI who converted to AD had decreased CBF compared to those who did not convert in the temporoparietal region in two papers and in the temporal lobe, posterior cingulate, and hippocampus in one paper each.

Relative decreases in CBF in MCI were reported in the frontal lobe in two papers, in the parietal lobe in five papers, in the temporal lobe in six papers, in the temporoparietal region in one paper, in the occipital lobe in two papers, in the posterior cingulate in seven papers, in the hippocampus in five papers, and in the thalamus in three papers. One paper reported decreased relative CBF in a combined region including posterior cingulate, precuneus, and parietal lobe. Relative increases in CBF in MCI were reported in the frontal lobe in three papers, in the temporal lobe in one paper, and in the temporoparietal region in one paper. One paper reported equal CBF between MCI and CN in parietal and occipital lobes. One paper found relative decreases in CBF in progressive MCI but relative increases in CBF in stable MCI in parietal and occipital lobes. This paper also found a relative increase in CBF in progressive MCI but unaltered CBF in stable MCI in the frontal lobe. Of the papers that did not report relative CBF by the included regions, one paper reported lower global CBF in MCI compared to CN, one paper reported decreased relative CBF in the anterior cingulate, and one paper reported that 23 of 39 individuals with MCI had an AD-like pattern of decreased CBF. Finally, one paper reported that CN who later declined to MCI had relatively decreased CBF in the posterior cingulate compared to those who did not decline.

Supplementary Table 3 reports r and p values for the correlation of regional CBF and cognitive test scores with details in the “Notes” column. Thirty-six papers were included, and a majority of them reported a positive correlation (better cognitive function associated with higher CBF), but there were also reports of negative correlations and of no correlation. Results were included in the table regardless of statistical significance. A positive association between regional CBF and cognitive function was reported in 30 papers. The cognitive tests used in these papers include the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), 15- Objects Test, 15 item Boston Naming Test, Poppelreuter test, California Verbal Learning Test, delayed recall portion of the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test, Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS), cognitive portion of the ADAS, modified MMSE, Kanji writing test, Cognitive Capacity Screening Examination, Cambridge Cognition Examination (CAMCOG; language, praxis, and abstraction sub-scores), Mini Mental State Questionnaire, Trail Making Test Part B, episodic memory, visuospatial, and attention z scores, Hasegawa’s Dementia Scale, Montreal Cognitive Assessment, paired associate learning on the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB), Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices, Sally-Anne test, Clinical Dementia Rating scale, logical memory portion of the Wechsler Memory Scale 3rd Edition, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease neuropsychological battery, and selective reminding, constructional praxis, and visual search tests. Negative correlations between regional CBF and cognitive function were reported in two papers, using the Trail Making Test Part B and Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Finally, eleven papers reported no relationship between CBF and cognitive function. The cognitive tests used in these papers include: Trail Making Test Part B, Clinical Dementia Rating scale, MMSE, cognitive portion of the ADAS, memory and executive portions of the CAMCOG, revised CAMCOG, revised Wechsler Memory Scale, five-choice reaction time portion of the CANTAB, California Verbal Learning Test, self-ordering test, category fluency, and logical memory test.

Figure 2 and Supplementary Figure 1 graphically present the Hedge’s g score and 95% confidence interval for the difference in CBF between AD and CN (Figure 2) and between MCI and CN (Supplementary Figure 1) for each brain region of interest. For all effect sizes, a positive value represents a decrease in CBF in patients relative to CN, as this was how the results were reported in nearly all of the papers.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals of difference in CBF between AD and CN in frontal lobe (A), parietal lobe (B), temporal lobe (C), temporoparietal region (D), occipital lobe (E), posterior cingulate (F), hippocampus (G), and thalamus (H). Positive effect sizes indicate that CBF is decreased in AD relative to CN.

Table 3 is a Summary of Findings table for the syntheses of results. Results are presented for CBF in CN vs. AD in papers that reported regional CBF values as mean (SD). Twenty-eight papers were included. Alegret et al. (2010) was not included because the results were reported as eigenvariates and are not comparable to the measurements in the other papers. Table 3 reports combined sample size, number of studies included, authors of included studies, mean, median, and range of Hedge’s g scores, certainty, and comments for syntheses of CBF in AD relative to controls in each brain region. The region with the greatest mean Hedge’s g (greatest relative decrease in CBF in AD) is the temporoparietal (g = 1.47), followed by the posterior cingulate (g = 1.30). All regions had positive mean and median Hedge’s g scores (decreased relative CBF in AD). At least one paper reported an increase in CBF in AD compared to CN in the frontal lobe, temporal lobe, hippocampus, and thalamus.

Table 3.

Syntheses of CBF in AD compared to CN by brain region.

Brain Region Individuals Studies Hedge’s G; Mean, Median and Range Certainty and average risk of bias score (1 to 3) Comments
Frontal 593 cases; 487 controls 22: Brown et al., Chaudhary et al., Claus et al., Duan et al., Firbank et al. 2011, Harris et al., Jagust et al., Johnson et al. 1988, Knapp et al., Komatani et al., Lajoie et al., Lobotesis et al., Mattsson et al., Mielke et al., Obara et al., O’Mahony et al., Schuff et al., Shimizu et al., Takano et al., Tu et al., van de Haar et al., Yoshida et al. 2011 0.57; 0.61 (−0.70, 1.80) moderate 2.29 Three papers reported increased CBF in AD relative to CN (Jagust, Yoshida, and Komatani) and one study reported no difference in CBF between AD and CN (Mielke). Data was extracted from graphs in three papers (Brown, Chaudhary, and Duan). Subregions were combined in six papers (Brown, Duan, Harris, Knapp, Takano, and Tu). Subgroups of AD severity were combined in Johnson, and the AD group included MCI in van de Haar.
Parietal 567 cases; 423 controls 20: Brown et al., Deo et al., Duan et al., Firbank et al. 2011, Johnson et al. 1988, Kimura et al., Knapp et al., Lacalle-Aurioles et al., Lajoie et al., Lobotesis et al., Mattsson et al., Mitsumoto et al., Obara et al., O’Mahony et al., Schuff et al., Shimizu et al., Takano et al., Tateno et al., van de Haar et al., Yoshida et al. 2011 1.13; 1.11 (0.31, 2.15) moderate 2.26 Data was extracted from graphs in five papers (Brown, Deo, Duan, Lacalle-Aurioles, and Tateno). Subregions were combined in three papers (Kimura, Mitsumoto, and Takano). Subgroups of AD severity were combined in Johnson, and the AD group included MCI in van de Haar.
Temporal 620 cases; 439 controls 20: Brown et al., Chaudhary et al., Claus et al., Deo et al., Johnson et al. 1988, Kimura et al., Knapp et al., Lacalle-Aurioles et al., Lajoie et al., Lobotesis et al., Mattsson et al., Obara et al., O’Mahony et al., Schuff et al., Shimizu et al., Takano et al., Tateno et al., Tu et al., van de Haar et al., Yoshida et al. 2011 0.80; 0.76 (−0.74, 1.83) moderate 2.25 One paper reported increased CBF in AD relative to CN (Lacalle-Aurioles). Data was extracted from graphs in five papers (Brown, Chaudhary, Deo, Lacalle-Aurioles, and Tateno). Subregions were combined in seven papers (Brown, Chaudhary, Kimura, Knapp, Lobotesis, Takano, and Tu). Subgroups of AD severity were combined in Johnson, and the AD group included MCI in van de Haar.
Temporoparietal 271 cases; 208 controls 9: Claus et al., Duan et al., Harris et al., Jagust et al., Kimura et al., Knapp et al., Komatani et al., Mielke et al., Tateno et al. 1.47; 1.42 (0.55, 3.63) moderate 2.42 Data was extracted from graphs in two papers (Duan and Tateno). Subregions were combined in one paper (Harris).
Occipital 356 cases; 239 controls 12: Claus et al., Firbank et al. 2011, Harris et al., Johnson et al. 1988, Knapp et al., Lobotesis et al, Mielke et al., Obara et al., O’Mahony et al., Shimizu et al., Takano et al., van de Haar et al. 0.56; 0.46 (0.03, 1.41) moderate 2.05 Subregions were combined in two papers (Harris and Takano). Subgroups of AD severity were combined in Johnson, and the AD group included MCI in van de Haar.
Posterior Cingulate 235 cases; 230 controls 9:Chaudhary et al., Duan et al., Kimura et al., Lajoie et al., Mattsson et al., Mitsumoto et al., Ottoy et al., Takano et al., Yoshida et al. 2011 1.30; 0.99 (0.37, 2.80) moderate 2.63 Data was extracted from graphs in two papers (Chaudhary and Duan). Subregions were combined in one paper (Duan).
Hippocampus 112 cases; 135 controls 4: Duan et al., Mattsson et al., Ottoy et al., Tateno et al. 0.76; 0.85 (−0.10, 1.46) moderate 2.78 One paper reported increased CBF in AD relative to CN (Ottoy). Data was extracted from graphs in two papers (Duan and Tateno).
Thalamus 161 cases; 108 controls 6: Firbank et al. 2011, Lobotesis et al., Mielke et al., Obara et al., Takano et al., Tateno et al. 0.30; 0.44 (−0.76, 0.78) moderate 2.12 One paper reported increased CBF in AD relative to CN (Mielke). Data was extracted from a graph in one paper (Tateno).

AD: Alzheimer’s disease, CBF: cerebral blood flow, CN: cognitively normal, MCI: mild cognitive impairment

Reporting bias was assessed by rating the completeness of each paper in the syntheses. All papers that reported mean and SD CBF values for AD and CN were included except for Alegret et al. (2010) due to this paper’s use of voxel-wise eigenvariates. Of the included papers, Firbank et al. (2011) was missing CBF data from the thalamus for one CN participant, and Schuff et al. (2009) did not analyze or report occipital CBF data because too few voxels were present after selecting only “pure” gray or white matter voxels. Otherwise, results were fully reported in all papers. Papers were also included in regional syntheses if they reported CBF values for relevant subregions, some of which we combined for an average value within the synthesis region. This review only includes published articles from PubMed that were in English and fully accessible. These restrictions contribute to a moderate level of reporting bias, but reporting bias is likely not increased from other sources or for other reasons.

Certainty of syntheses was rated based on cumulative risk of bias scores of the constituent papers. Papers rated “good” were given three points, papers rated “fair” were given two points, and papers rated “poor” were given one point. A weighted average of this point system based on sample size was calculated for each synthesis region, and if the weighted average risk of bias score (based on sample size) was less than two, the overall certainty was lowered. All regions had average risk of bias scores of at least two; these scores are included in the “certainty” column of Table 3. Certainty was also lowered due to inconsistency (scores were higher if all results were in the same direction), and indirectness and/or imprecision (including papers where we combined subregions’ values, subgroups, or where we extracted data from graphs). Large effect sizes, evidence of dose-response relationships (MCI values intermediate between CN and AD values), and confounding factors that would have likely resulted in an effect of the opposite direction were considered as reasons to raise or maintain the certainty score. Certainty scores could be “very low,” “low,” “moderate,” or “high.” All regions were given a certainty score of “moderate.” Justifications for each certainty score are included in detail in Table 3.

DISCUSSION

In this systematic review, we compiled findings of altered CBF and the relationship between CBF and cognitive test scores in MCI/AD from previously published literature. Fifty-seven papers were included for relative CBF in AD, 17 papers were included for relative CBF in MCI, and 36 papers were included for the relationship between CBF and cognition. There were consistent reports of decreased CBF in AD compared to CN in parietal, temporoparietal, and posterior cingulate regions. Frontal, temporal, occipital, hippocampal and thalamic regions each had either relatively increased or unaltered CBF in AD reported in at least one paper. The greatest relative decrease in CBF in AD was in the temporoparietal and posterior cingulate regions according to the 28 papers included in the synthesis. There were consistent reports of decreased CBF in MCI compared to CN in posterior cingulate, hippocampal and thalamic regions, with mixed results in all other included regions. Across regions, 94% of the results reported relatively decreased CBF in AD, whereas 86% reported relatively decreased CBF in MCI.

The higher percentage of brain regions with relatively increased CBF in MCI may be because there were fewer papers that compared CBF between MCI and CN. However, it has been suggested that increased CBF in MCI could be a compensatory response to early AD pathology, in which increased blood flow is necessary in order to function at a normal level. Tang et al. (2022) completed a meta-analysis of CBF findings in MCI with ASL MRI; they found that MCI had decreased CBF in precuneus, inferior parietal, superior occipital, middle temporal and middle occipital regions and increased CBF in the lentiform nucleus. Here, we found the most consistent relatively decreased CBF in MCI in the posterior cingulate and the most frequently reported relatively increased CBF in MCI in the frontal lobe. It appears that decreased regional CBF is common in MCI, and future meta-analyses will allow us to more thoroughly describe the spatial and temporal patterns of CBF changes in MCI. In Zhang et al. (2021), the authors concluded that decreased CBF begins in the posterior cingulate, temporoparietal, and thalamic regions during MCI and that decreased CBF becomes more widespread in AD. We also found evidence of decreased CBF in MCI and reported MCI values that were intermediate between CN and AD values (Alegret et al., 2010). Additionally, we found that the greatest relative decreases in CBF in AD were located in the temporoparietal region and the posterior cingulate; CBF in the posterior cingulate was consistently relatively decreased in MCI as well. Both of these regions have been reported to have decreased CBF and decreased glucose metabolism in previous studies and reviews, and these alterations are considered characteristic of AD (Matsuda, 2001). The reasons for this pattern may be due to the spatial spread of tau pathology during AD, to vulnerability of these areas to vascular and/or neuronal damage, or to damage of functionally connected brain regions. However, several papers reported relatively increased CBF in MCI and even in AD, suggesting that alterations in CBF leading up to and during the course of AD are not linear in nature. Like Zhang et al. (2021), we found quite consistent findings of positive correlations between regional CBF and cognitive functioning, which supports the idea that decreased CBF is associated with cognitive impairment in AD. It will be necessary for future studies to elucidate whether transient increases in CBF during MCI/AD are characteristic of the disease course or if this only occurs in a particular subset of individuals with MCI/AD.

One of the limitations of this review is that we included only accessible articles from PubMed that were published in English. Another limitation inherent to this review is the inclusion of articles across various decades, in which several different imaging and processing methods were used to measure CBF. Papers also used different reference regions to normalize the CBF values. Despite this, our results were generally consistent. Additionally, this review only includes papers with MCI/AD samples in a specific age range, and while this should make our results more generalizable to the population with late-onset AD, it also significantly limited the number of papers that met criteria for inclusion. We included all papers that used an established imaging method to measure CBF, but we did not require papers to have accounted for potential atrophy. A number of papers did account for group or individual differences in gray matter volume, but not all of them. Thus, some results could partially reflect tissue loss and decreased demand for blood flow rather than pathologically decreased blood flow alone. Finally, there are various subtypes of AD according to the rate at which various cognitive functions decline and the patterns of AD-typical brain pathologies (Murray et al., 2011), so these subtypes may exhibit distinct patterns of altered CBF as well. Nearly all papers included in this review defined MCI/AD participants according to diagnostic criteria that does not distinguish between these subtypes. Thus, the samples within and certainly between included papers are likely to be somewhat heterogeneous, perhaps explaining the mixed results in both MCI and AD. Due to these limitations and to the observational and correlational nature of the research question, the results and syntheses presented here are meant to summarize and consolidate the existing evidence of altered CBF in MCI/AD and to characterize general patterns in these findings, rather than to make conclusive statements about the degree of these changes.

CONCLUSIONS

The results of this review suggest that more research is needed to better understand the spatial and temporal nature of changes in CBF throughout the course of AD, including in early stages of disease such as MCI. In addition, standardization of methods to measure and process CBF would allow results from different studies to be more easily consolidated or compared. We believe that to best characterize CBF for its potential use as an early noninvasive biomarker for AD, large and diverse samples of participants must be employed so that analyses can be completed in multiple brain regions and with multiple factors included as covariates. Longitudinal studies that meet these criteria and include individuals from different disease stages will likely help to clarify the specific patterns of altered CBF that are usually present during the years leading up to a diagnosis of AD, as well as determine how alterations in CBF relate to AD pathologies and cognitive decline.

Supplementary Material

Swinford_tables

Supplementary Table 1. Characteristics of included articles.

Supplementary Table 2. CBF in CN and MCI in each paper.

Supplementary Table 3. Relationships between CBF and cognition in each paper.

Swinford_figures

Supplementary Figure 1. Effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals of difference in CBF between MCI and CN in frontal lobe (A), parietal lobe (B), temporal lobe (C), temporoparietal region (D), occipital lobe (E), posterior cingulate (F), hippocampus (G), and thalamus (H). Positive effect sizes indicate that CBF is decreased in MCI relative to CN.

Funding Sources

Dr. Risacher receives support from the National Institute on Aging (K01 AG049050, R01 AG061788) and the New Vision Award. Dr. Wu receives support from the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG053993). Dr. Apostolova receives support from NIH, Alzheimer Association, AVID Pharmaceuticals, Life Molecular Imaging, and Roche Diagnostics. Dr. Saykin receives support from multiple NIH grants (P30 AG010133, P30 AG072976, R01 AG019771, R01 AG057739, U01 AG024904, R01 LM013463, R01 AG068193, T32 AG071444, and U01 AG068057 and U01 AG072177). He has also received support from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, a subsidiary of Eli Lilly (in kind contribution of PET tracer precursor).

Abbreviations:

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

ADAS

Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale

ASL

arterial spin labeled

APOE

apolipoprotein E

CAMCOG

Cambridge Cognition Examination

CANTAB

Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery

CBF

cerebral blood flow

CN

cognitively normal

DCE

dynamic contrast enhanced

DSC

dynamic susceptibility contrast

GRADE

Grading of Recommendations Assessment Development and Education

MCI

mild cognitive impairment

MeSH

Medical Subject Headings

MMSE

Mini-Mental State Examination

MRI

magnetic resonance imaging

PET

positron emission tomography

PICO

population intervention comparison and outcomes

SCD

subjective cognitive decline

SD

standard deviation

SPECT

single-photon emission computerized tomography

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

Dr. Apostolova receives support in the form of consulting fees from Biogen, Two Labs, IQVIA, NIH, Florida Department of Health, NIH Biobank, Eli Lilly, GE Healthcare, and Eisai; in the form of payment for lectures, etc. from AAN, MillerMed, AiSM, and Health and Hospitality; and in the form of travel and meeting support from Alzheimer’s Association. She also participates on Data Safety Monitoring or Advisory boards for IQVIA, NIA R01 AG061111, UAB Nathan Shock Center, and New Mexico Exploratory ADRC; in leadership roles for Medical Science Council Alzheimer Association Greater IN Chapter, Alzheimer Association Science Program Committee, and FDA PCNS Advisory Committee; stock or stock options Cassava Neurosciences and Golden Seeds, and receipt of materials, etc. from AVID Pharmaceuticals, Life Molecular Imaging, and Roche Diagnostics. Dr. Saykin serves or served on the following: Bayer Oncology (Scientific Advisory Board), Eisai (Scientific Advisory Board), Siemens Medical Solutions USA, Inc. (Dementia Advisory Board), and Springer-Nature Publishing (Editorial Office Support as Editor-in-Chief, Brain Imaging and Behavior). All other authors report no disclosures.

Data Availability Statement

We did not analyze or generate datasets because this work involved the compilation and synthesis of previously published results, all of which are cited and available to view.

References

  1. 2021 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. (2021). Alzheimers Dement, 17(3), 327–406. 10.1002/alz.12328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Albrecht D, Isenberg AL, Stradford J, Monreal T, Sagare A, Pachicano M, Sweeney M, Toga A, Zlokovic B, Chui H, Joe E, Schneider L, Conti P, Jann K, & Pa J (2020). Associations between Vascular Function and Tau PET Are Associated with Global Cognition and Amyloid. J Neurosci, 40(44), 8573–8586. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1230-20.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alegret M, Vinyes-Junqué G, Boada M, Martínez-Lage P, Cuberas G, Espinosa A, Roca I, Hernández I, Valero S, Rosende-Roca M, Mauleón A, Becker Jt, & Tárraga L. (2010). Brain perfusion correlates of visuoperceptual deficits in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis, 21(2), 557–567. 10.3233/jad-2010-091069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alegret M, Cuberas-Borros G, Vinyes-Junque G, Espinosa A, Valero S, Hernandez I, Roca I, Ruiz A, Rosende-Roca M, Mauleon A, Becker JT, Castell-Conesa J, Tarraga L, & Boada M (2012). A two-year follow-up of cognitive deficits and brain perfusion in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis, 30(1), 109–120. 10.3233/JAD-2012-111850 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alfini AJ, Weiss LR, Nielson KA, Verber MD, & Smith JC (2019). Resting Cerebral Blood Flow After Exercise Training in Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Alzheimers Dis, 67(2), 671–684. 10.3233/JAD-180728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ashford JW, Shih WJ, Coupal J, Shetty R, Schneider A, Cool C, Aleem A, Kiefer VH, Mendiondo MS, & Schmitt FA (2000). Single SPECT measures of cerebral cortical perfusion reflect time-index estimation of dementia severity in Alzheimer’s disease. J Nucl Med, 41(1), 57–64. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10647605 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bangen KJ, Restom K, Liu TT, Wierenga CE, Jak AJ, Salmon DP, & Bondi MW (2012). Assessment of Alzheimer’s disease risk with functional magnetic resonance imaging: an arterial spin labeling study. J Alzheimers Dis, 31 Suppl 3, S59–74. 10.3233/JAD-2012-120292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benoit M, Koulibaly PM, Migneco O, Darcourt J, Pringuey DJ, & Robert PH (2002). Brain perfusion in Alzheimer’s disease with and without apathy: a SPECT study with statistical parametric mapping analysis. Psychiatry Res, 114(2), 103–111. 10.1016/s0925-4927(02)00003-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blennow K, & Zetterberg H (2018). The Past and the Future of Alzheimer’s Disease Fluid Biomarkers. J Alzheimers Dis, 62(3), 1125–1140. 10.3233/JAD-170773 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brayet P, Petit D, Baril AA, Gosselin N, Gagnon JF, Soucy JP, Gauthier S, Kergoat MJ, Carrier J, Rouleau I, & Montplaisir J (2017). Brain perfusion during rapid-eye-movement sleep successfully identifies amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Sleep Med, 34, 134–140. 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.01.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown DR, Hunter R, Wyper DJ, Patterson J, Kelly RC, Montaldi D, & McCullouch J (1996). Longitudinal changes in cognitive function and regional cerebral function in Alzheimer’s disease: a SPECT blood flow study. J Psychiatr Res, 30(2), 109–126. 10.1016/0022-3956(95)00032-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown DR, Wyper DJ, Owens J, Patterson J, Kelly RC, Hunter R, & McCulloch J (1997). 123Iodo-MK-801: a spect agent for imaging the pattern and extent of glutamate (NMDA) receptor activation in Alzheimer’s disease. J Psychiatr Res, 31(6), 605–619. 10.1016/s0022-3956(97)00031-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brugnolo A, Morbelli S, Dessi B, Girtler N, Mazzei D, Fama F, Barbieri P, Cabassi G, Koulibaly PM, Sambuceti G, Rodriguez G, & Nobili F (2010). The reversed clock drawing test phenomenon in Alzheimer’s disease: A perfusion SPECT study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 29(1), 1–10. 10.1159/000270898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Camargo A, Wang Z, & Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging I (2021). Longitudinal Cerebral Blood Flow Changes in Normal Aging and the Alzheimer’s Disease Continuum Identified by Arterial Spin Labeling MRI. J Alzheimers Dis, 81(4), 1727–1735. 10.3233/JAD-210116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chaudhary S, Scouten A, Schwindt G, Janik R, Lee W, Sled JG, Black SE, & Stefanovic B (2013). Hemodynamic effects of cholinesterase inhibition in mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Magn Reson Imaging, 38(1), 26–35. 10.1002/jmri.23967 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Claus JJ, van Harskamp F, Breteler MM, Krenning EP, van der Cammen TJ, Hofman A, & Hasan D (1994). Assessment of cerebral perfusion with single-photon emission tomography in normal subjects and in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: effects of region of interest selection. Eur J Nucl Med, 21(10), 1044–1051. 10.1007/bf00181058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Colloby SJ, Fenwick JD, Williams ED, Paling SM, Lobotesis K, Ballard C, McKeith I, & O’Brien JT (2002). A comparison of (99m)Tc-HMPAO SPET changes in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease using statistical parametric mapping. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging, 29(5), 615–622. 10.1007/s00259-002-0778-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Colloby SJ, Field RH, Wyper DJ, O’Brien JT, & Taylor JP (2016). A spatial covariance (123)I-5IA-85380 SPECT study of alpha4beta2 nicotinic receptors in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging, 47, 83–90. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.07.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Counts SE, Ikonomovic MD, Mercado N, Vega IE, & Mufson EJ (2017). Biomarkers for the Early Detection and Progression of Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurotherapeutics, 14(1), 35–53. 10.1007/s13311-016-0481-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Culpepper L, Lam RW, & McIntyre RS (2017). Cognitive Impairment in Patients With Depression: Awareness, Assessment, and Management. J Clin Psychiatry, 78(9), 1383–1394. 10.4088/JCP.tk16043ah5c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dai W, Lopez OL, Carmichael OT, Becker JT, Kuller LH, & Gach HM (2009). Mild cognitive impairment and alzheimer disease: patterns of altered cerebral blood flow at MR imaging. Radiology, 250(3), 856–866. 10.1148/radiol.2503080751 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. DeKosky ST, Shih WJ, Schmitt FA, Coupal J, & Kirkpatrick C (1990). Assessing utility of single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) scan in Alzheimer disease: correlation with cognitive severity. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord, 4(1), 14–23. 10.1097/00002093-199040100-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Deo AK, Borson S, Link JM, Domino K, Eary JF, Ke B, Richards TL, Mankoff DA, Minoshima S, O’Sullivan F, Eyal S, Hsiao P, Maravilla K, & Unadkat JD (2014). Activity of P-Glycoprotein, a beta-Amyloid Transporter at the Blood-Brain Barrier, Is Compromised in Patients with Mild Alzheimer Disease. J Nucl Med, 55(7), 1106–1111. 10.2967/jnumed.113.130161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Detre JA, Rao H, Wang DJ, Chen YF, & Wang Z (2012). Applications of arterial spin labeled MRI in the brain. J Magn Reson Imaging, 35(5), 1026–1037. 10.1002/jmri.23581 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ding B, Ling HW, Zhang Y, Huang J, Zhang H, Wang T, & Yan FH (2014). Pattern of cerebral hyperperfusion in Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment using voxel-based analysis of 3D arterial spin-labeling imaging: initial experience. Clin Interv Aging, 9, 493–500. 10.2147/cia.S58879 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Duan W, Sehrawat P, Balachandrasekaran A, Bhumkar AB, Boraste PB, Becker JT, Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Gach HM, & Dai W (2020). Cerebral Blood Flow Is Associated with Diagnostic Class and Cognitive Decline in Alzheimer’s Disease. J Alzheimers Dis, 76(3), 1103–1120. 10.3233/JAD-200034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Duan W, Zhou GD, Balachandrasekaran A, Bhumkar AB, Boraste PB, Becker JT, Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Gach HM, & Dai W (2021). Cerebral Blood Flow Predicts Conversion of Mild Cognitive Impairment into Alzheimer’s Disease and Cognitive Decline: An Arterial Spin Labeling Follow-up Study. J Alzheimers Dis, 82(1), 293–305. 10.3233/JAD-210199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Duran FL, Zampieri FG, Bottino CC, Buchpiguel CA, & Busatto GF (2007). Voxel-based investigations of regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease using a single-detector SPECT system. Clinics (Sao Paulo), 62(4), 377–384. 10.1590/s1807-59322007000400002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Edelman RR, Mattle HP, Atkinson DJ, Hill T, Finn JP, Mayman C, Ronthal M, Hoogewoud HM, & Kleefield J (1990). Cerebral blood flow: assessment with dynamic contrast-enhanced T2*-weighted MR imaging at 1.5 T. Radiology, 176(1), 211–220. 10.1148/radiology.176.1.2353094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Encinas M, De Juan R, Marcos A, Gil P, Barabash A, Fernández C, De Ugarte C, & Cabranes JA (2003). Regional cerebral blood flow assessed with 99mTc-ECD SPET as a marker of progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging, 30(11), 1473–1480. 10.1007/s00259-003-1277-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Firbank MJ, Colloby SJ, Burn DJ, McKeith IG, & O’Brien JT (2003). Regional cerebral blood flow in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. NeuroImage, 20(2), 1309–1319. 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00364-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Firbank MJ, He J, Blamire AM, Singh B, Danson P, Kalaria RN, & O’Brien JT (2011). Cerebral blood flow by arterial spin labeling in poststroke dementia. Neurology, 76(17), 1478–1484. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318217e76a [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. German F, Andres D, Leandro U, Nicolas N, Graciela L, Yanina B, Patricio C, Adriana Q, Cecilia B, Ismael C, Ismael C, de Leon MP, Valeria C, Feuerstein V, Sergio D, Ricardo A, Henry E, & Silvia V (2021). Connectivity and Patterns of Regional Cerebral Blood Flow, Cerebral Glucose Uptake, and Abeta-Amyloid Deposition in Alzheimer’s Disease (Early and Late-Onset) Compared to Normal Ageing. Curr Alzheimer Res, 18(8), 646–655. 10.2174/1567205018666211116095035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Habert MO, Horn JF, Sarazin M, Lotterie JA, Puel M, Onen F, Zanca M, Portet F, Touchon J, Verny M, Mahieux F, Giron A, Fertil B, & Dubois B (2011). Brain perfusion SPECT with an automated quantitative tool can identify prodromal Alzheimer’s disease among patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging, 32(1), 15–23. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Haji M, Kimura N, Hanaoka T, Aso Y, Takemaru M, Hirano T, & Matsubara E (2015). Evaluation of regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease patients with subclinical hypothyroidism. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 39(5–6), 360–367. 10.1159/000375298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hanyu H, Shimuzu T, Tanaka Y, Takasaki M, Koizumi K, & Abe K (2003). Effect of age on regional cerebral blood flow patterns in Alzheimer’s disease patients. J Neurol Sci, 209(1–2), 25–30. 10.1016/s0022-510x(02)00456-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hanyu H, Shimizu T, Tanaka Y, Takasaki M, Koizumi K, & Abe K (2003). Regional cerebral blood flow patterns and response to donepezil treatment in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 15(4), 177–182. 10.1159/000068785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hanyu H, Shimizu S, Tanaka Y, Takasaki M, Koizumi K, & Abe K (2004). Differences in regional cerebral blood flow patterns in male versus female patients with Alzheimer disease. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol, 25(7), 1199–1204. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hanyu H, Sakurai H, Hirao K, Sato T, & Iwamoto T (2008). Difference in practice effects depending on cerebral perfusion pattern in mild cognitive impairment. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 23(1), 111–112. 10.1002/gps.1834 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hanyu H, Sato T, Shimizu S, Kanetaka H, Iwamoto T, & Koizumi K (2008). The effect of education on rCBF changes in Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal SPECT study. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging, 35(12), 2182–2190. 10.1007/s00259-008-0848-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hanyu H, Sato T, Hirao K, Kanetaka H, Iwamoto T, & Koizumi K (2010). The progression of cognitive deterioration and regional cerebral blood flow patterns in Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal SPECT study. J Neurol Sci, 290(1–2), 96–101. 10.1016/j.jns.2009.10.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hardy JA, & Higgins GA (1992). Alzheimer’s disease: the amyloid cascade hypothesis. Science, 256(5054), 184–185. 10.1126/science.1566067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Harris GJ, Links JM, Pearlson GD, & Camargo EE (1991). Cortical circumferential profile of SPECT cerebral perfusion in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res, 40(3), 167–180. 10.1016/0925-4927(91)90008-e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Harris GJ, Lewis RF, Satlin A, English CD, Scott TM, Yurgelun-Todd DA, & Renshaw PF (1998). Dynamic susceptibility contrast MR imaging of regional cerebral blood volume in Alzheimer disease: a promising alternative to nuclear medicine. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol, 19(9), 1727–1732. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9802497 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hayashi A, Nomura H, Mochizuki R, Ohnuma A, Kimpara T, Ootomo K, Hosokai Y, Ishioka T, Suzuki K, & Mori E (2011). Neural substrates for writing impairments in Japanese patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease: a SPECT study. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1962–1968. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hayashi H, Kobayashi R, Kawakatsu S, Ohba M, Morioka D, & Otani K (2021). Comparison of the decreases in regional cerebral blood flow in the posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus, and parietal lobe between suspected non-Alzheimer’s disease pathophysiology and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychogeriatrics, 21(5), 716–721. 10.1111/psyg.12729 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Houston AS, Kemp PM, Macleod MA, Francis JR, Colohan HA, & Matthews HP (1998). Use of significance image to determine patterns of cortical blood flow abnormality in pathological and at-risk groups. J Nucl Med, 39(3), 425–430. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9529286 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hoyer S (1991). Abnormalities of glucose metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 640, 53–58. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1991.tb00190.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hoyer S, Nitsch R, & Oesterreich K (1991). Predominant abnormality in cerebral glucose utilization in late-onset dementia of the Alzheimer type: a cross-sectional comparison against advanced late-onset and incipient early-onset cases. J Neural Transm Park Dis Dement Sect, 3(1), 1–14. 10.1007/BF02251132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hoyer S (1993). Intermediary metabolism disturbance in AD/SDAT and its relation to molecular events. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, 17(2), 199–228. 10.1016/0278-5846(93)90043-r [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Iadecola C, & Gottesman RF (2019). Neurovascular and Cognitive Dysfunction in Hypertension. Circ Res, 124(7), 1025–1044. 10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.118.313260 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Iizuka T, & Kameyama M (2017). Cholinergic enhancement increases regional cerebral blood flow to the posterior cingulate cortex in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Geriatr Gerontol Int, 17(6), 951–958. 10.1111/ggi.12818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Imran MB, Kawashima R, Awata S, Sato K, Kinomura S, Ito H, Ono S, Yoshioka S, Sato M, & Fukuda H (1998). Use of automated image registration to generate mean brain SPECT image of Alzheimer’s patients. Ann Nucl Med, 12(3), 127–132. 10.1007/BF03164776 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ishii K, Sasaki M, Sakamoto S, Yamaji S, Kitagaki H, & Mori E (1999). Tc-99m ethyl cysteinate dimer SPECT and 2-[F-18]fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose PET in Alzheimer’s disease. Comparison of perfusion and metabolic patterns. Clin Nucl Med, 24(8), 572–575. 10.1097/00003072-199908000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ishiwata A, Sakayori O, Minoshima S, Mizumura S, Kitamura S, & Katayama Y (2006). Preclinical evidence of Alzheimer changes in progressive mild cognitive impairment: a qualitative and quantitative SPECT study. Acta Neurol Scand, 114(2), 91–96. 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2006.00661.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ito K, Mori E, Fukuyama H, Ishii K, Washimi Y, Asada T, Mori S, Meguro K, Kitamura S, Hanyu H, Nakano S, Matsuda H, Kuwabara Y, Hashikawa K, Momose T, Uchida Y, Hatazawa J, Minoshima S, Kosaka K, Yamada T, Yonekura Y, & Group JCS (2013). Prediction of outcomes in MCI with (123)I-IMP-CBF SPECT: a multicenter prospective cohort study. Ann Nucl Med, 27(10), 898–906. 10.1007/s12149-013-0768-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Jagust WJ, Budinger TF, & Reed BR (1987). The diagnosis of dementia with single photon emission computed tomography. Arch Neurol, 44(3), 258–262. 10.1001/archneur.1987.00520150014011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, van Boxtel M, Breteler M, Ceccaldi M, Chetelat G, Dubois B, Dufouil C, Ellis KA, van der Flier WM, Glodzik L, van Harten AC, de Leon MJ, McHugh P, Mielke MM, Molinuevo JL, Mosconi L, Osorio RS, Perrotin A, Petersen RC, Rabin LA, Rami L, Reisberg B, Rentz DM, Sachdev PS, de la Sayette V, Saykin AJ, Scheltens P, Shulman MB, Slavin MJ, Sperling RA, Stewart R, Uspenskaya O, Vellas B, Visser PJ, Wagner M, & Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative Working G. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement, 10(6), 844–852. 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, Buckley RF, van der Flier WM, Han Y, Molinuevo JL, Rabin L, Rentz DM, Rodriguez-Gomez O, Saykin AJ, Sikkes SAM, Smart CM, Wolfsgruber S, & Wagner M (2020). The characterisation of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol, 19(3), 271–278. 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30368-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Johnson KA, Holman BL, Mueller SP, Rosen TJ, English R, Nagel JS, & Growdon JH (1988). Single photon emission computed tomography in Alzheimer’s disease. Abnormal iofetamine I 123 uptake reflects dementia severity. Arch Neurol, 45(4), 392–396. 10.1001/archneur.1988.00520280038013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Johnson KA, Moran EK, Becker JA, Blacker D, Fischman AJ, & Albert MS (2007). Single photon emission computed tomography perfusion differences in mild cognitive impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 78(3), 240–247. 10.1136/jnnp.2006.096800 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Jones T, Chesler DA, & Ter-Pogossian MM (1976). The continuous inhalation of oxygen-15 for assessing regional oxygen extraction in the brain of man. Br J Radiol, 49(580), 339–343. 10.1259/0007-1285-49-580-339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kaneta T, Katsuse O, Hirano T, Ogawa M, Shihikura-Hino A, Yoshida K, Odawara T, Hirayasu Y, & Inoue T (2017). Voxel-wise correlations between cognition and cerebral blood flow using arterial spin-labeled perfusion MRI in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: a cross-sectional study. BMC Neurol, 17(1), 91. 10.1186/s12883-017-0870-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kawamura J, Meyer JS, Terayama Y, & Weathers S (1991). Cerebral white matter perfusion in dementia of Alzheimer type. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord, 5(4), 231–239. 10.1097/00002093-199100540-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Keller C, Kadir A, Forsberg A, Porras O, & Nordberg A (2011). Long-term effects of galantamine treatment on brain functional activities as measured by PET in Alzheimer’s disease patients. J Alzheimers Dis, 24(1), 109–123. 10.3233/JAD-2010-101290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kety SS, & Schmidt CF (1948). The Nitrous Oxide Method for the Quantitative Determination of Cerebral Blood Flow in Man: Theory, Procedure and Normal Values. J Clin Invest, 27(4), 476–483. 10.1172/JCI101994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kimura N, Kumamoto T, Masuda H, Hanaoka T, Hazama Y, Okazaki T, & Arakawa R (2011). Relationship between thyroid hormone levels and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord, 25(2), 138–143. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181f9aff2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kimura N, Kumamoto T, Masuda T, Hanaoka T, Okazaki T, & Arakawa R (2012). Evaluation of the regional cerebral blood flow changes during long-term donepezil therapy in patients with Alzheimer’s disease using 3DSRT. J Neuroimaging, 22(3), 299–304. 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2011.00612.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Knapp WH, Dannenberg C, Marschall B, Zedlick D, Loschmann K, Bettin S, Barthel H, & Seese A (1996). Changes in local cerebral blood flow by neuroactivation and vasoactivation in patients with impaired cognitive function. Eur J Nucl Med, 23(8), 878–888. 10.1007/BF01084360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Komatani A, Yamaguchi K, Sugai Y, Takanashi T, Kera M, Shinohara M, & Kawakatsu S (1988). Assessment of demented patients by dynamic SPECT of inhaled xenon-133. J Nucl Med, 29(10), 1621–1626. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3262723 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lacalle-Aurioles M, Alemán-Gómez Y, Guzmán-De-Villoria JA, Cruz-Orduña I, Olazarán J, Mateos-Pérez JM, Martino ME, & Desco M (2013). Is the cerebellum the optimal reference region for intensity normalization of perfusion MR studies in early Alzheimer’s disease? PLoS One, 8(12), e81548. 10.1371/journal.pone.0081548 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lacalle-Aurioles M, Mateos-Perez JM, Guzman-De-Villoria JA, Olazaran J, Cruz-Orduna I, Aleman-Gomez Y, Martino ME, & Desco M (2014). Cerebral blood flow is an earlier indicator of perfusion abnormalities than cerebral blood volume in Alzheimer’s disease. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab, 34(4), 654–659. 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lajoie I, Nugent S, Debacker C, Dyson K, Tancredi FB, Badhwar A, Belleville S, Deschaintre Y, Bellec P, Doyon J, Bocti C, Gauthier S, Arnold D, Kergoat MJ, Chertkow H, Monchi O, & Hoge RD (2017). Application of calibrated fMRI in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage Clin, 15, 348–358. 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.05.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Lassen NA, & Ingvar DH (1961). The blood flow of the cerebral cortex determined by radioactive krypton. Experientia, 17, 42–43. 10.1007/BF02157946 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Lassen NA, Hoedt-Rasmussen K, Sorensen SC, Skinhoj E, Cronquist S, Bodforss B, & Ingvar DH (1963). Regional Cerebral Blood Flow in Man Determined by Krypton. Neurology, 13, 719–727. 10.1212/wnl.13.9.719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lee YC, Liu RS, Liao YC, Sun CM, Wang PS, Wang PN, & Liu HC (2003). Statistical parametric mapping of brain SPECT perfusion abnormalities in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Eur Neurol, 49(3), 142–145. 10.1159/000069086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Liu Y, Zeng X, Wang Z, Zhang N, Fan D, & Yuan H (2015). Different post label delay cerebral blood flow measurements in patients with Alzheimer’s disease using 3D arterial spin labeling. Magn Reson Imaging, 33(9), 1019–1025. 10.1016/j.mri.2015.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Lobotesis K, Fenwick JD, Phipps A, Ryman A, Swann A, Ballard C, McKeith IG, & O’Brien JT (2001). Occipital hypoperfusion on SPECT in dementia with Lewy bodies but not AD. Neurology, 56(5), 643–649. 10.1212/wnl.56.5.643 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lovblad KO, Montandon ML, Viallon M, Rodriguez C, Toma S, Golay X, Giannakopoulos P, & Haller S (2015). Arterial Spin-Labeling Parameters Influence Signal Variability and Estimated Regional Relative Cerebral Blood Flow in Normal Aging and Mild Cognitive Impairment: FAIR versus PICORE Techniques. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol, 36(7), 1231–1236. 10.3174/ajnr.A4291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Maeda M, Itoh S, Kimura H, Iwasaki T, Hayashi N, Yamamoto K, Ishii Y, & Kubota T (1993). Tumor vascularity in the brain: evaluation with dynamic susceptibility-contrast MR imaging. Radiology, 189(1), 233–238. 10.1148/radiology.189.1.8372199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Matsuda H (2001). Cerebral blood flow and metabolic abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Nucl Med, 15(2), 85–92. 10.1007/BF02988596 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Mattsson N, Tosun D, Insel PS, Simonson A, Jack CR Jr., Beckett LA, Donohue M, Jagust W, Schuff N, Weiner MW, & Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging I (2014). Association of brain amyloid-beta with cerebral perfusion and structure in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Brain, 137(Pt 5), 1550–1561. 10.1093/brain/awu043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mielke R, Pietrzyk U, Jacobs A, Fink GR, Ichimiya A, Kessler J, Herholz K, & Heiss WD (1994). HMPAO SPET and FDG PET in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: comparison of perfusion and metabolic pattern. Eur J Nucl Med, 21(10), 1052–1060. 10.1007/BF00181059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mitsumoto T, Ohya N, Ichimiya A, Sakaguchi Y, Kiyota A, Abe K, Morishita J, & Sasaki M (2009). Diagnostic performance of Tc-99m HMPAO SPECT for early and late onset Alzheimer’s disease: a clinical evaluation of linearization correction. Ann Nucl Med, 23(5), 487–495. 10.1007/s12149-009-0266-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mortel KF, Pavol MA, Wood S, Meyer JS, Terayama Y, Rexer JL, & Herod B (1994). Prospective studies of cerebral perfusion and cognitive testing among elderly normal volunteers and patients with ischemic vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Angiology, 45(3), 171–180. 10.1177/000331979404500301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Mubrin Z, Knezevic S, Spilich G, Risberg J, Gubarev N, Wannenmacher W, & Vucinic G (1989). Normalization of rCBF pattern in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Psychiatry Res, 29(3), 303–306. 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90072-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Murray ME, Graff-Radford NR, Ross OA, Petersen RC, Duara R, & Dickson DW (2011). Neuropathologically defined subtypes of Alzheimer’s disease with distinct clinical characteristics: a retrospective study. Lancet Neurol, 10(9), 785–796. 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70156-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Nagahama Y, Nabatame H, Okina T, Yamauchi H, Narita M, Fujimoto N, Murakami M, Fukuyama H, & Matsuda M (2003). Cerebral correlates of the progression rate of the cognitive decline in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Eur Neurol, 50(1), 1–9. 10.1159/000070851 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Nobili F, Brugnolo A, Calvini P, Copello F, De Leo C, Girtler N, Morbelli S, Piccardo A, Vitali P, & Rodriguez G (2005). Resting SPECT-neuropsychology correlation in very mild Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol, 116(2), 364–375. 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Obara K, Meyer JS, Mortel KF, & Muramatsu K (1994). Cognitive declines correlate with decreased cortical volume and perfusion in dementia of Alzheimer type. J Neurol Sci, 127(1), 96–102. 10.1016/0022-510x(94)90141-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. O’Mahony D, Coffey J, Murphy J, O’Hare N, Hamilton D, Rowan M, Freyne P, Walsh JB, & Coakley D (1996). Event-related potential prolongation in Alzheimer’s disease signifies frontal lobe impairment: evidence from SPECT imaging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 51(3), M102–107. 10.1093/gerona/51a.3.m102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Ottoy J, Verhaeghe J, Niemantsverdriet E, De Roeck E, Wyffels L, Ceyssens S, Van Broeckhoven C, Engelborghs S, Stroobants S, & Staelens S (2019). (18)F-FDG PET, the early phases and the delivery rate of (18)F-AV45 PET as proxies of cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation against (15)O-H2O PET. Alzheimers Dement, 15(9), 1172–1182. 10.1016/j.jalz.2019.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Pappata S, Varrone A, Vicidomini C, Milan G, De Falco C, Sansone V, Iavarone A, Comerci M, Lore E, Panico MR, Quarantelli M, Postiglione A, & Salvatore M (2010). SPECT imaging of GABA(A)/benzodiazepine receptors and cerebral perfusion in mild cognitive impairment. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging, 37(6), 1156–1163. 10.1007/s00259-010-1409-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Pearlson GD, Harris GJ, Powers RE, Barta PE, Camargo EE, Chase GA, Noga JT, & Tune LE (1992). Quantitative changes in mesial temporal volume, regional cerebral blood flow, and cognition in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 49(5), 402–408. 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820050066012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, & Kokmen E (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol, 56(3), 303–308. 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Petersen RC (2016). Mild Cognitive Impairment. Continuum (Minneap Minn), 22(2 Dementia), 404–418. 10.1212/CON.0000000000000313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Prohovnik I, Smith G, Sackeim HA, Mayeux R, & Stern Y (1989). Gray-matter degeneration in presenile Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol, 25(2), 117–124. 10.1002/ana.410250203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Quattrini G, Marizzoni M, Pizzini FB, Galazzo IB, Aiello M, Didic M, Soricelli A, Albani D, Romano M, Blin O, Forloni G, Golay X, Jovicich J, Nathan PJ, Richardson JC, Salvatore M, Frisoni GB, Pievani M, & PharmaCog C (2021). Convergent and Discriminant Validity of Default Mode Network and Limbic Network Perfusion in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment Patients. J Alzheimers Dis, 82(4), 1797–1808. 10.3233/JAD-210531 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Rane S, Koh N, Boord P, Madhyastha T, Askren MK, Jayadev S, Cholerton B, Larson E, & Grabowski TJ (2018). Quantitative cerebrovascular pathology in a community-based cohort of older adults. Neurobiol Aging, 65, 77–85. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.01.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Rempp KA, Brix G, Wenz F, Becker CR, Guckel F, & Lorenz WJ (1994). Quantification of regional cerebral blood flow and volume with dynamic susceptibility contrast-enhanced MR imaging. Radiology, 193(3), 637–641. 10.1148/radiology.193.3.7972800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Richardson WS, Wilson MC, Nishikawa J, & Hayward RS (1995). The well-built clinical question: a key to evidence-based decisions. ACP J Club, 123(3), A12–13. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7582737 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Rubinski A, Tosun D, Franzmeier N, Neitzel J, Frontzkowski L, Weiner M, & Ewers M (2021). Lower cerebral perfusion is associated with tau-PET in the entorhinal cortex across the Alzheimer’s continuum. Neurobiol Aging, 102, 111–118. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Sakai M, Hanyu H, Kume K, Sato T, Hirao K, Kanetaka H, Abe S, Kanaya K, Sakurai H, & Iwamoto T (2013). Rate of progression of Alzheimer’s disease in younger versus older patients: a longitudinal single photon emission computed tomography study. Geriatr Gerontol Int, 13(3), 555–562. 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2012.00934.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Sakurai H, Hanyu H, Sato T, Kume K, Hirao K, Kanetaka H, & Iwamoto T (2013). Effects of cilostazol on cognition and regional cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and cerebrovascular disease: a pilot study. Geriatr Gerontol Int, 13(1), 90–97. 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2012.00866.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Sase S, Yamamoto H, Kawashima E, Tan X, & Sawa Y (2017). Discrimination Between Patients With Alzheimer Disease and Healthy Subjects Using Layer Analysis of Cerebral Blood Flow and Xenon Solubility Coefficient in Xenon-Enhanced Computed Tomography. J Comput Assist Tomogr, 41(3), 477–483. 10.1097/rct.0000000000000525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Sase S, Yamamoto H, Kawashima E, Tan X, & Sawa Y (2018). Discrimination between patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease and healthy subjects based on cerebral blood flow images of the lateral views in xenon-enhanced computed tomography. Psychogeriatrics, 18(1), 3–12. 10.1111/psyg.12281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Schuff N, Matsumoto S, Kmiecik J, Studholme C, Du A, Ezekiel F, Miller BL, Kramer JH, Jagust WJ, Chui HC, & Weiner MW (2009). Cerebral blood flow in ischemic vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease, measured by arterial spin-labeling magnetic resonance imaging. Alzheimers Dement, 5(6), 454–462. 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.04.1233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Shimizu S, Hanyu H, Kanetaka H, Iwamoto T, Koizumi K, & Abe K (2005). Differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer’s disease using brain SPECT. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 20(1), 25–30. 10.1159/000085070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Song IU, Chung YA, Chung SW, & Jeong J (2014). Early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease associated with dementia using cerebral perfusion SPECT. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 37(5–6), 276–285. 10.1159/000357128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Sundström T, Elgh E, Larsson A, Näsman B, Nyberg L, & Riklund KA (2006). Memory-provoked rCBF-SPECT as a diagnostic tool in Alzheimer’s disease? Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging, 33(1), 73–80. 10.1007/s00259-005-1874-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Takahashi M, Tada T, Nakamura T, Koyama K, & Momose T (2019). Efficacy and Limitations of rCBF-SPECT in the Diagnosis of Alzheimer’s Disease With Amyloid-PET. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen, 34(5), 314–321. 10.1177/1533317519841192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Takano Y, Mutoh T, Tatewaki Y, Seki T, Yamamoto S, Odagiri H, Arai H, & Taki Y (2020). Hypoperfusion in the posterior cingulate cortex is associated with lower bone mass density in elderly women with osteopenia and Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol, 47(3), 365–371. 10.1111/1440-1681.13217 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Takenoshita S, Hayashi S, Shinya T, Miki T, Yokota O, Maki Y, Hattori H, Yamada N, & Terada S (2020). Sally-Anne test and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Psychogeriatrics, 20(5), 549–556. 10.1111/psyg.12533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Tanaka M, Fukuyama H, Yamauchi H, Narita M, Nabatame H, Yokode M, Fujimoto N, Kita T, & Murakami M (2002). Regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in nondemented patients with memory impairment. J Neuroimaging, 12(2), 112–118. 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2002.tb00106.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Tang T, Huang L, Zhang Y, Li Z, & Liang S (2022). Aberrant pattern of regional cerebral blood flow in mild cognitive impairment: A meta-analysis of arterial spin labeling magnetic resonance imaging. Front Aging Neurosci, 14, 961344. 10.3389/fnagi.2022.961344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Tateno M, Kobayashi S, Shirasaka T, Furukawa Y, Fujii K, Morii H, Yasumura S, Utsumi K, & Saito T (2008). Comparison of the usefulness of brain perfusion SPECT and MIBG myocardial scintigraphy for the diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 26(5), 453–457. 10.1159/000165918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Terada S, Sato S, Nagao S, Ikeda C, Shindo A, Hayashi S, Oshima E, Yokota O, & Uchitomi Y (2013). Trail making test B and brain perfusion imaging in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res, 213(3), 249–255. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Tokumitsu K, Yasui-Furukori N, Takeuchi J, Yachimori K, Sugawara N, Terayama Y, Tanaka N, Naraoka T, & Shimoda K (2021). The combination of MMSE with VSRAD and eZIS has greater accuracy for discriminating mild cognitive impairment from early Alzheimer’s disease than MMSE alone. PLoS One, 16(2), e0247427. 10.1371/journal.pone.0247427 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Tosun D, Schuff N, Jagust W, Weiner MW, & Alzheimer”s Disease Neuroimaging I (2016). Discriminative Power of Arterial Spin Labeling Magnetic Resonance Imaging and 18F-Fluorodeoxyglucose Positron Emission Tomography Changes for Amyloid-beta-Positive Subjects in the Alzheimer’s Disease Continuum. Neurodegener Dis, 16(1–2), 87–94. 10.1159/000439257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Tu MC, Chung HW, Hsu YH, Yang JJ, & Wu WC (2022). Stage-Dependent Cerebral Blood Flow and Leukoaraiosis Couplings in Subcortical Ischemic Vascular Disease and Alzheimer’s Disease. J Alzheimers Dis, 86(2), 729–739. 10.3233/JAD-215405 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. van de Haar HJ, Jansen JFA, van Osch MJP, van Buchem MA, Muller M, Wong SM, Hofman PAM, Burgmans S, Verhey FRJ, & Backes WH (2016). Neurovascular unit impairment in early Alzheimer’s disease measured with magnetic resonance imaging. Neurobiol Aging, 45, 190–196. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Wang H, Golob E, Bert A, Nie K, Chu Y, Dick MB, Mandelkern M, & Su MY (2009). Alterations in regional brain volume and individual MRI-guided perfusion in normal control, stable mild cognitive impairment, and MCI-AD converter. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol, 22(1), 35–45. 10.1177/0891988708328212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Warkentin S, Ohlsson M, Wollmer P, Edenbrandt L, & Minthon L (2004). Regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease: classification and analysis of heterogeneity. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 17(3), 207–214. 10.1159/000076358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Warwick JM (2004). Imaging of brain function using SPECT. Metab Brain Dis, 19(1–2), 113–123. 10.1023/b:mebr.0000027422.48744.a3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Xie L, Dolui S, Das SR, Stockbower GE, Daffner M, Rao H, Yushkevich PA, Detre JA, & Wolk DA (2016). A brain stress test: Cerebral perfusion during memory encoding in mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage Clin, 11, 388–397. 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.03.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Yamaji S, Ishii K, Sasaki M, Imamura T, Kitagaki H, Sakamoto S, & Mori E (1997). Changes in cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism related to magnetic resonance imaging white matter hyperintensities in Alzheimer’s disease. J Nucl Med, 38(9), 1471–1474. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9293811 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Yamashita KI, Taniwaki Y, Utsunomiya H, & Taniwaki T (2014). Cerebral blood flow reduction associated with orientation for time in amnesic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease patients. J Neuroimaging, 24(6), 590–594. 10.1111/jon.12096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Yew B, & Nation DA (2017). Cerebrovascular resistance: effects on cognitive decline, cortical atrophy, and progression to dementia. Brain, 140(7), 1987–2001. 10.1093/brain/awx112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Yoshida T, Kazui H, Tokunaga H, Kito Y, Kubo Y, Kimura N, Morihara T, Shimosegawa E, Hatazawa J, & Takeda M (2011). Protein synthesis in the posterior cingulate cortex in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychogeriatrics, 11(1), 40–45. 10.1111/j.1479-8301.2010.00350.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Yoshida T, Mori T, Shimizu H, Yoshino Y, Sonobe N, Matsumoto T, Kikuchi K, Miyagawa M, Iga J, Mochizuki T, & Ueno SI (2017). Neural basis of visual perception and reasoning ability in Alzheimer’s disease: correlation between Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices test and (123) I-IMP SPECT imaging results. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 32(4), 407–413. 10.1002/gps.4481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Zhang H, Wang Y, Lyu D, Li Y, Li W, Wang Q, Li Y, Qin Q, Wang X, Gong M, Jiao H, Liu W, & Jia J (2021). Cerebral blood flow in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev, 71, 101450. 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Zhu CC, Fu SY, Chen YX, Li L, Mao RL, Wang JZ, Liu R, Liu Y, & Wang XC (2020). Advances in Drug Therapy for Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr Med Sci, 40(6), 999–1008. 10.1007/s11596-020-2281-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Zlokovic BV (2011). Neurovascular pathways to neurodegeneration in Alzheimer’s disease and other disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci, 12(12), 723–738. 10.1038/nrn3114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Zurynski Y, Singer A, Kraiuhin C, Gordon E, Dorsch N, Landau P, Howson A, & Meares R (1989). Regional cerebral blood flow measurements in the diagnosis of dementia. Aust N Z J Med, 19(5), 436–442. 10.1111/j.1445-5994.1989.tb00301.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Swinford_tables

Supplementary Table 1. Characteristics of included articles.

Supplementary Table 2. CBF in CN and MCI in each paper.

Supplementary Table 3. Relationships between CBF and cognition in each paper.

Swinford_figures

Supplementary Figure 1. Effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals of difference in CBF between MCI and CN in frontal lobe (A), parietal lobe (B), temporal lobe (C), temporoparietal region (D), occipital lobe (E), posterior cingulate (F), hippocampus (G), and thalamus (H). Positive effect sizes indicate that CBF is decreased in MCI relative to CN.

Data Availability Statement

We did not analyze or generate datasets because this work involved the compilation and synthesis of previously published results, all of which are cited and available to view.

RESOURCES