Abstract
Research on teacher goals has primarily followed a variable-centered approach, although person-centered approaches have inspired achievement goal research in other domains. The multiple goal perspective posits that individuals pursue different combinations of goals—goal profiles—that might be differentially adaptive or maladaptive. We investigate how beneficial goal profiles may be for research on teacher motivation, using data from three study sets (total N = 3,681) from different countries (Israel, Germany) and institution types (schools, universities). We analyzed whether psychologically meaningful, coherent, and generalizable goal profiles could be identified and compared the explanatory power of profiles and individual goals as predictors of teachers’ self-efficacy and work-related distress. Results showed six psychologically meaningful and largely generalizable goal profiles. Compared to individual goals, profiles only explained little differences in self-efficacy and work-related distress. Given these findings, we critically evaluate achievement goal profiles as a means to study effects of teacher goals.
1. Introduction
Research into teacher motivation has shown that teachers’ achievement goal pursuit matters, for teachers themselves as well as for their teaching outcomes. Studies conducted in different countries and in different types of institutions ranging from primary schools to universities have consistently documented that different achievement goals are differentially associated with teachers’ coping strategies [e.g., 1], emotional experiences [e.g., 2, 3], well-being at work [e.g., 4–6], learning behaviors [e.g., 7], and instructional practices [e.g., 8–11]. Nevertheless, the by now substantial body of empirical research on this subject has primarily relied on a variable-centered approach, which assumes population-homogeneity and considers linear associations between discrete goals and one or more theoretically relevant outcomes. Besides this approach, a multiple goal perspective has emerged within research on achievement goals [see 12, 13] postulating that different subgroups of individuals can be identified that pursue different combinations of achievement goals, or goal profiles, that might in turn be particularly adaptive or maladaptive. This perspective has inspired a large body of person-centered research on elementary [e.g., 14], secondary [e.g., 15, 16], and higher education students [e.g., 17].
Yet when it comes to teachers, achievement goal profiles have rarely been investigated and little is known about the power of such an approach for describing their achievement motivations and explaining differences in their cognitions and experiences. Besides studying the main effects of achievement goals, considering achievement goal profiles holds the potential to provide complimentary information for improving our understanding of how teachers pursue different goals and how these different combinations of goals matter. To this end, investigating differences in teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs and work-related distress can be considered as particularly insightful, given that these constructs are central, important, and well-researched aspects of teachers’ work [18] and are often considered in profile-based investigations in students [13]. Furthermore, knowledge on teachers’ achievement goal profiles is also essential for practical implications, as information on different subgroups of (differentially motivated) teachers is essential for group-specific interventions and training.
Given the lack of research in the teaching domain, the merits of person-centered approaches besides variable-based approaches for modeling achievement goal configurations within teachers are unclear. Therefore, we address the applicability of such an approach to teacher motivation by investigating the suitability of goal profiles for describing teacher motivation in a psychologically meaningful way and for predicting differences in teachers’ cognitions and experiences compared to variable-centered approach.
1.1 Achievement goal profiles as an approach to achievement motivation
The cornerstone of our reflections on teacher motivation constitutes the achievement goal approach, which provides an influential framework for explaining what motivates individuals in achievement situations. It has been applied to a broad range of populations such as students [e.g., 19], athletes [e.g., 20], employees [e.g., 21], and teachers [e.g., 22]. Within this, achievement motivation is defined in terms of qualitatively distinct types of goals characterized by the different end-states that individuals pursue in achievement situations [23]. In research on teacher motivation, four distinct types of achievement goals are commonly distinguished [e.g., 11, 22, 24, 25]: mastery goals (i.e., striving to develop one’s professional competence). In the present study, we focus on learning (approach) goals (defined by intraindividual competence gains) as a central component of the superordinate mastery goal construct [that also comprises task standards and avoidance valence, see 26], performance approach goals (i.e., striving to show high ability and outperform others), performance avoidance goals (i.e., striving to avoid appearing incompetent and performing worse than others), and work avoidance goals (i.e., striving to get through the day with little effort).
Several goal theorists have further differentiated teachers’ achievement goals, for example by considering relational goals (i.e., striving to create positive relationships with students; [10], learning avoidance goals (i.e., striving to not do worse than before; [27]), and task goals (i.e., striving to master a task, [27]; see [28], for an overview model). In the present study, we focus on the original four goals (mastery, performance-approach, performance-avoidance, and work-avoidance) proposed by Butler [22] as this framework has guided most of the research on teachers’ achievement goals to date and thus has a strong empirical foundation [2]. Importantly, these goals also align best with those most frequently used in person-centered studies on students’ achievement goal profiles [13], enabling us to compare the identified goal profiles to those found in prior research.
Research on goal profiles among students has largely stemmed from debates as to whether certain goals, or alternatively goal combinations, lead to (or at least are associated with) more effective learning [13]. The position that certain goals are superior to others is commonly labeled as the “mastery-goal perspective”. Researchers adhering to this perspective state that purely pursuing mastery goals elicits the most beneficial outcome pattern [29, 30].
In contrast, advocates of the “multiple-goal perspective” share the notion that achievement goals are not mutually exclusive [see 31]. Moreover, they argue that—besides for example specialized effects—a certain combination of goals may lead to the most beneficial outcome pattern. This perspective has primarily emerged from the mixed pattern of results for performance approach goals [13], which correlate with outcomes deemed desirable such as high achievement, as well as with undesirable outcomes such as increased test anxiety [see also 32, for an overview]. Advocates of the multiple goal perspective often claim that performance approach goals are especially beneficial when adopted alongside mastery goals, such that the strong endorsement of both goals is equally or even more beneficial than the endorsement of strong mastery goals alone [12, 33]. While this specific assumption can be tested with interaction terms within the General Linear Model, the multiple goal approach also sparked a more general debate as to whether variable-centered approaches are appropriate in the first place, given the number and complexity of possible goal patterns with potentially differential consequences [13]. These considerations led to the growing popularity of person-centered investigations into students’ goal profiles.
1.2 Merits and limitations of person-centered analyses into achievement goals
While variable-centered analyses allow for investigations into associations between variables within certain populations, person-centered analyses such as Cluster Analyses or Latent Profile Analyses (LPAs) enable researchers to identify subgroups of individuals that differ from one another regarding the investigated variables [13]. In line with prior research on achievement goal profiles, we use the term person-centered to refer to research that identifies and compares subgroups of individuals based on mean differences where individuals with similar expressions in a bundle of variables are grouped together. Person-centered analyses into achievement goals allow for insight into potential goal configurations (i.e., goal profiles) that differentiate groups of individuals based on how the various goals combine within individuals, while also maintaining the power of subsequent analyses into correlates of these goal profiles. This can be illustrated by comparing variable- versus person-centered approaches for the four goals that are typically used to characterize teachers’ achievement goals. Using a variable-centered approach, it would be necessary to consider four main effects, six two-way interactions, four three-way interactions, and one four-way interaction to investigate the impact of every possible goal composition [e.g. see 34]. Analyses into so many complex interactions on top of main effects likely face problems regarding statistical power [35] and interpretability [36]. Furthermore, variable-centered approaches rely on the assumption of population-homogeneity and linear interactions for all strengths to which goals are pursued. In contrast, person-centered approaches relax the assumption of population-homogeneity and allow for the identification of goal profiles that are potentially suitable to characterize groups within a certain population, usually resulting in a much smaller number of profiles (compared to the number of possible interactions). Further, Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13] noted in their meta-analysis on goal profiles that, compared to variable-centered approaches, person-centered approaches may be more useful to model the effects of performance approach and performance avoidance goals [which are often very strongly associated in both students as well as teachers, see 2, 32, 37]. Finally, person-centered approaches afford comparisons between individuals characterized by a certain goal profile and individuals characterized by another goal profile, allowing for a better understanding of how goal compositions relate to adaptive patterns of cognitions and experiences. This advantage is particularly relevant for examining the contrasting predictions of the “mastery goal” and “multiple goals” perspectives as the latter assumes that the combination of strong mastery and performance goals will be beneficial mainly to the extent that they combine also with weak performance-avoidance goals.
Despite these potential benefits, an important premise of using person-centered analyses to investigate achievement goal pursuit is that similar motivational profiles can be found across different contexts. While different contexts, such as different types of educational institutions that differently value certain standards or different countries with varying educational systems and emphases can go along with differences in the motivations of teachers active in these contexts [38, 39], the general psychological mechanisms motivating teachers should apply equally [40, 41]. If this is not the case, findings from different samples cannot be sufficiently compared [see 13, 42]. Further, person-centered analyses into achievement goal pursuit can only provide substantial insights into differences in cognitions and experiences if goal profiles do indeed explain meaningful amounts of variance compared to the main effects of the single goals, as suggested by advocates of the multiple goal perspective [see also 43 for a similar argument]. This notion, however, is rarely reflected in the methodology of studies examining goal profiles [15–17], as the necessary investigations would require combining and comparing person-centered and variable-centered analyses [see 44, for a comparison of person and variable centered approaches to explain differences in students’ changes in conceptual understanding]. Besides the extent to which profiles are generalizable and theoretically reasonable, a strong case for the usefulness of the multiple goal perspective in explaining differences in educational outcomes requires that the profiles explain substantial portions of variance compared to the main effects of the individual goals.
In their meta-analytical review including a large set of studies of students’ goal profiles, Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13] found that almost half of around 15,000 students (49.1%) were not characterized by qualitatively different configurations of stronger and weaker goals. Rather, there were quantitative differences in the general levels of achievement motivation such that the levels of all goals were average, high, or low. Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13] interpreted this finding to indicate that these different general levels of achievement goal pursuit may be as important as the relative strength of the individual goals for describing students’ goal pursuit. Using other motivational frameworks such as Self-determination Theory, person-centered approaches often document similar findings about the prevalence of qualitative and quantitative differences in motivational profiles [e.g., 45]. Taking a critical stance, one might interpret a large proportion of profiles only differing by quantitative differences as challenging a person-centered approach, as it highlights the necessity to compare mean values, which can be achieved more directly with variable-centered approaches. Moreover, as the quantitative differences in motivational profiles align primarily with older approaches that often defined achievement motivation as a one-dimensional, quantitative motivational force, one might even argue that such findings do not align well with the fundamental assumptions of the achievement goal approach and Self-determination Theory on the quality of motivation mattering [i.e., “which kind” of motivation, and not "how strong" motivation is, see 12].
In sum, a person-centered approach could advance research into teachers’ achievement goals—if it helps to differentiate teachers alongside goal profiles that are meaningful (i.e., characterized by qualitative and theoretically reasonable differences rather than quantitative differences in achievement motivation), generalizable (e.g., found in different groups of teachers), and explain substantial variance in other variables when compared to the main effects of the individual achievement goals.
1.3 Potential goal profiles within teacher populations
Beyond the finding that about half of the students in the reviewed studies could be characterized by the general strength of their achievement goals, Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13] found two profiles to frequently (but not always) emerge that were characterized to a stronger degree by qualitative rather than only quantitative differences regarding goal pursuit. In particular, these goal profiles allow for deeper comparison of the mastery goal perspective versus the multiple goal perspective. The first qualitatively differentiated goal profile identified in 62.5% of all investigated studies was indicated by strong mastery goals. According to the mastery-goal perspective, individuals that are characterized by this goal profile should show the most beneficial learning behavior when compared to any other goal profile. The second goal profile identified in 50% of all investigated studies was indicated by strong mastery and strong performance approach goals. This goal profile is of particular interest for scholars expecting interactive goal patterns following the multiple-goal perspective, as comparisons between individuals with this goal profile to other groups (especially with individuals characterized by exclusively strong mastery goals) could indicate whether the combination of strong performance approach goals and strong mastery goals is particularly beneficial. Replicating these two goal profiles within teacher populations would be of great interest in order to test the applicability of the mastery- versus multiple-goal perspectives within the population of teachers, as it is not clear without specific research on this topic whether this finding holds and can be extended to this population [see also 46 on the general relevance of replication in educational research].
When writing this manuscript, Subsequently, another study has investigated goal profiles in teachers [for a preprint, see 47]. While not directly relatable to the presented findings as the profiles also included relational goals, the authors identified profiles characterized by (a) high levels of approach goals, (b) high levels of mastery and relational goals, (c) high levels of performance goals, and (d) low levels of all goals the only published study on goal profiles in the teaching profession that we were aware of was reported by Kunst et al. [48], who analyzed achievement goals of 984 vocational training instructors from the Netherlands. The authors found that 50.1% were classified into a “diffuse” profile with average levels of all goals, 10.7% into a high mastery and performance approach goal profile, and 12.3% into a low performance approach and performance avoidance goal profile. These profiles are conceptually similar to the previously described profiles reported by Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13]. In line with the multiple goal perspective, Kunst et al. [48] found that teachers who were classified into the high mastery and performance approach goal profiles reported more information acquisition and feedback asking than those in the other profiles. However, it is also worth noting that Kunst et al. [48] did not identify a high mastery profile, but only a profile with moderate mastery goals and weak performance goals. Nevertheless, this affirms the notion of investigating goal profiles also in teachers and expecting, in principle, similar patterns as in students [see 42, for qualitative differences in motivational profiles in teachers using a SDT framework].
1.4 Relevant correlates for research into teachers’ goal profiles
Beyond the existence of achievement goal profiles in students, Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13] investigated whether students who were characterized by certain goal profiles also differed in other motivational variables and their social and emotional well-being. In the reviewed articles, they found the strongest support for the view that a high-mastery goal profile was most beneficial with regard to these outcomes when compared to any other goal profile. While this provides some support for the mastery-goals perspective within students, a conceptual replication in samples of teachers would support the generalizability of findings and the usefulness of person-centered approaches on achievement-motivation. With that being said, studies on teachers’ achievement goals that have applied the variable-centered approach frequently showed positive associations between mastery goals and other aspects of teachers’ motivation such as self-efficacy beliefs [49, 50] and teachers’ well-being [6, 11].
In order to relate our research to the broader picture of research into teachers’ goals, we chose to investigate two variables that are central to teachers’ cognitions and experiences, that matter for their work, and that have often been investigated in prior (variable-centered) studies of teachers’ achievement goals: teachers’ self-efficacy (reflecting an expectancy-based aspect of motivation aside from the goals that are grounded in the value-component of motivation) and work-related distress (as proxy for impaired well-being).
Teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs describe teachers’ self-ascribed capabilities to influence student learning [51]. Entailing the core aspects of teacher agency, visible as effort and perseverance when fulfilling instructional goals, self-efficacy beliefs are considered one of the key personal beliefs influencing teachers’ professional behaviors and student learning [52]. Teachers’ self-efficacy is positively related to both mastery and performance approach goals, while being negatively associated with performance avoidance goals and especially with work avoidance goals [e.g., 2, 24, 49, 53]. As far as we are aware, no empirical studies to date have investigated whether there is any effect of the goal composition beyond the additive influence of both goals. Fundamentally, we want to investigate whether teachers who are characterized by strong mastery and performance approach goals (multiple goals) experience substantially higher self-efficacy than those who merely report strong mastery goals or strong performance approach goals.
Besides self-efficacy, teacher well-being is a critical construct for schools and society that is linked to the effective functioning of schools, school improvement and implementation of educational reforms, staff commitment, and teacher absenteeism [54]. We understand well-being as a multifaceted and broad construct, entailing satisfaction as well as positive and negative affect [54, 55]. An important component that threatens the effective work of teachers are experiences of work-related distress [56]. These have often been operationalized as burnout experiences defined by feelings of exhaustion at work, reduced personal accomplishment, and depersonalization [57]. Research has confirmed strong links between achievement goals and both burnout [6, 9] and other measures of work-related distress such as occupational strain [58]. Mastery goals were negatively associated with experiences of work-related distress, and performance avoidance goals and work avoidance goals (the latter to an even greater extent) were positively related to such experiences. The pattern of results was more inconclusive for performance approach goals, which showed either positive associations [58] or, more typically, null effects [e.g., 9]. This leaves room for speculation whether associations may vary depending on feelings of distress can differently emerge from a combination of different (performance) goals. For example, performance approach goals might be associated with distress if combined with strong performance avoidance or work avoidance goals, while no such association, or possibly even a negative association, may be found if instead accompanied by strong mastery goals. A person-centered approach could shed light on these complex relations.
2. Research questions
Our overarching objective was to investigate achievement goal profiles of teachers and their meaningfulness compared to the insights provided by variable-centered approaches. Our first research question was whether it is possible to consistently identify achievement goal profiles, which indicate qualitative rather than quantitative differences in achievement goal pursuit, between subgroups of teachers (particularly in terms of strong mastery goals but not strong performance goals, and strong mastery goals combined with strong performance goals). One serious problem of the person-centered approach is that the emergence of some goal profiles might strongly depend on the respective sample. For instance, Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13] pointed out that half of the goal profiles identified in their meta-analysis only emerged in less than 25 percent of the investigated student samples. This means that we have to be cautious in over-interpreting results from single samples of teachers. To overcome this problem, we included a set of samples from different countries (Israel, Germany) and different teaching contexts (primary/secondary versus higher education) to investigate the robustness of the observed goal profiles. Specifically, we examined whether the profile patterns varied depending on systematic differences between these teacher populations (country, institution type) or whether we would find generalizable profiles across different populations.
As a second research question, we investigated whether teachers with different goal profiles also differed in their self-efficacy beliefs and experiences of work-related distress. Building on this, our third research question was whether the person-centered latent profile approach can yield insights into associations of teachers’ achievement goal pursuit with self-efficacy beliefs and experiences of work-related distress that cannot easily be obtained using a variable-centered approach. Specifically, we investigated the explanatory power of goal profiles and set this in relation to the additive main effects of the individual achievement goals.
3. Method
3.1 Procedure and sample
To examine the research questions, we used existing datasets from two different countries and two different educational contexts. In total, we used six datasets from studies that were conceptually similar regarding their focus on teachers, their cross-sectional design, and the measures used. Two studies were conducted with primary and secondary school teachers in Israel (n = 950 and n = 408), two studies with university teachers in Germany (n = 933 and n = 832), and two studies with secondary school teachers in Germany (n = 224 and n = 334). Table 1 provides an overview of all included datasets, reference publications, and details about their sample. As the two data sets from each of the three different educational contexts are very similar (not only regarding the sampled population, but also the measures; see Table 1), we combined them to yield three larger datasets (Israel-school, German-university, German-school) that were used to conduct the LPAs. Unlike some of the single datasets, these three combined datasets met the rule of thumb criteria of N = 500 that is frequently considered as a minimum sample size necessary for sufficiently high accuracy in identifying the correct number of latent profiles [59, 60].
Table 1. Overview of the included datasets.
Studies used | N | Population | Achievement goal measure | Other measures |
---|---|---|---|---|
Israel-school study set | ||||
Study 1 [10] | 950 | Israeli inservice primary and secondary school teachers | Butler [22] scale (“I would feel that I had a successful day in school if …”): Mastery (4 items, ωh = .74, e.g., „I saw that I was developing as a teacher and teaching more effectively than in the past”); Performance approach (4 items, ωh = .83, e.g., “my classes did better on an exam than those of other teachers”); Performance avoidance (4 items, ωh = .64, e.g., “no one asked a question in class that I couldn’t answer”); Work avoidance (4 items, ωh = .79, e.g., “some of my classes were cancelled”). |
Self-efficacy (11 items, ωh = .89, e.g., insert sample items here); Work-related distress (10 items, ωh = .85, e.g., “I feel emotionally drained by my work”; [61]). |
Study 2: [62] | 408 | Israeli inservice school teachers | Butler’s [22] scale (see above): Mastery (4 items, ωh = .73); Performance approach (4 items, ωh = .82); Performance avoidance (4 items, ωh = .65); Work avoidance (4 items, ωh = .74). |
Work-related distress (4 items, ωh = .93, e.g., “I feel emotionally drained by my work”; [61]). |
German-university study set | ||||
Study 3 [63] | 933 | German university teachers | Daumiller et al. [28] scale (“In my current teaching activities …”: Mastery (4 items, ωh = .88, e.g., “my goal is to expand my professional and methodological knowledge as much as possible”); Performance approacha (8 items, ωh = .92, e.g., “my goal is to teach better than my colleagues”); Performance avoidancea (8 items, ωh = .93, e.g., “my goal is to not teach worse than my colleagues do”); Work avoidance (4 items, ωh = .94, e.g., “it is my goal to have the least amount of work as possible”). |
Self-efficacyb (12 items, ωh = .86, e.g., “How well can you respond to difficult questions from your students?”; adaption from Nie et al. [64]); Work-related distressc (17 items, ωh = .92, e.g., “I feel emotionally drained by my work”; MBI, [65]). |
Study 4: [38] | 832 | German university teachers | Daumiller et al. [28] scale (see above): Mastery (4 items, ωh = .92); Performance approacha (8 items, ωh = .91); Performance avoidancea (8 items, ωh = .94); Work avoidance (4 items, ωh = .93). |
Self-efficacy b (12 items, ωh = .82, same scale as in Study 3); Work-related distressc (22 items, ωh = .85, same scale as in Study 3). |
German-school study set | ||||
Study 5: [24] | 224 | German inservice secondary school teachers | Nitsche et al. [24] scale (“In my vocation, I aspire to …): Mastery (18 items, ωh = .92, e.g., “improve my pedagogical knowledge and competence.”); Performance approach (6 items, ωh = .90, e.g., “show my colleagues that I deal better with critical lessons than other teachers”); Performance avoidance (6 items, ωh = .91, e.g., “conceal from my colleagues when I do something less satisfying than other teachers”); Work avoidance (6 items, ωh = .85, e.g., ”get through the day with little effort.”). |
Self-efficacy (7 items, ωh = .73, e.g., “When I try really hard, I am able to reach even the most difficult students”; [66]); Work-related distress (15 items, ωh = .88, e.g., ”The pressure under which I am working is too big”; [67]). |
Study 6: [68] | 334 | German inservice secondary school teachers | Nitsche et al. [24] scale (see above): Mastery (9 items, ωh = .88); Performance approach (3 items, ωh = .84); Performance avoidance (3 items, ωh = .84); Work avoidance (3 items, ωh = .80). |
Self-efficacyd (5 items, ωh = .74, “Considering the challenges that my occupation poses to me, I get along well in my job”; [69]); Work-related distresse (6 items, ωh = .84, same scale as in Study 5). |
Note.
a This scale distinguishes between appearance and normative aspects of performance and assesses them separately; for the investigation at hand, we combined both aspects to correspond to the measures of the other studies.
b This scale distinguishes self-efficacy for instruction, classroom management, and motivation; again, we combined all three aspects as a general measure of teaching self-efficacy.
c This scale distinguishes emotional exhaustion, depersonalization in the workplace, and reduced personal accomplishment; we again combined all three aspects.
d In this dataset, self-efficacy in a narrow sense was not measured, but rather core-efficacy beliefs in the form of self-concept. We conducted the analyses twice, once with and once without this measure to ensure that our findings were not affected by the use of this measure that is slightly different to the other self-efficacy scales, albeit strongly related to self-efficacy beliefs.
3.2 Measures
An overview of the measures in the different studies including sample items and internal consistencies can be found in Table 1. All McDonalds Omega values were > .60 which is typically deemed as a minimum value for evaluating acceptable internal consistency [see 70].
Achievement goals
Regarding teachers’ achievement goals, mastery approach goals (measured by learning approach goals as a core aspect of the superordinate mastery goal construct), performance approach goals, performance avoidance goals, and work avoidance goals were measured using three established scales for the three study sets, namely scales by Butler [22], Daumiller et al. [28], and Nitsche et al. [24]. These scales are conceptually similar, however differ slightly in their measures. Most notably, Daumiller et al. [28] distinguished performance goals further, as to whether they focus on appearance aspects or normative aspects. As the other two scales include items that address both of these aspects, we aggregated both appearance and normative goals for the studies that used the Daumiller et al. [28] measure to enhance comparability with the other two scales. In addition, the Nitsche et al. [24] scale distinguishes three facets of the content of mastery goals as well as four facets of different addresses of performance goals. These were also aggregated on the overall goal level [see 28, for a similar approach].
Self-efficacy beliefs
To assess teachers’ self-efficacy, the Israel-school studies used a Hebrew translation of Tschannen-Moran & Hoy’s [71] short measure of teacher self-efficacy, the German-university studies an adaption of the scale by Nie, Lau, and Liau [64] and the German-school studies a German version of the teacher self-efficacy scale by Schwarzer and Hallum [66]. To enhance comparability of all scales across the different study sets, we aggregated the subscales that distinguished between different aspects of self-efficacy beliefs on overall scale level.
Work-related distress
Regarding teachers’ work-related distress, the Israel-school studies used measures based on the Maslach Burnout Inventory for teachers [61, 65] that were developed and validated for teachers in Israel by Friedman and Farber [72]. The German-university studies also used a scale based on the MBI, and the German-school studies used the perceived overload subscale by Enzmann and Kleiber [67]. We aggregated the MBI measures that distinguished different components of work-related distress (emotional exhaustion, cynicism, reduced personal accomplishment) as a composite measure to ensure comparability of all scales between study sets.
3.3 Missing values
Overall, there was relatively little missing data (< 2.1% for each variable, for each dataset) that was handled using the full-information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML) and the EM-algorithm for all analyses [73].
3.4 Analyses
As the individual studies used different scales that did not always have the same number of answer options (see Table 1), we first z-standardized all variables (except for gender) for each individual dataset. For the LPAs, we followed the procedure outlined by Asparouhov and Muthén [74] using Mplus 8.1 [75]. Based on the teachers’ answers to their mastery, performance approach, performance avoidance, and work avoidance goals, we first specified two latent profiles. We subsequently increased the number of profiles until the increase in model fit no longer indicated specifying another profile and losing model parsimony. Based on the range of profiles found in prior research, we considered up to 9 latent profiles. Models were estimated using 5,000 random sets of start values, 100 iterations for each random start, and 200 best solutions retained for final stage optimization [76]. All solutions converged on well-replicated loglikelihood values. Changes in model fit were investigated with the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) and the sample-size-adjusted BIC (SSBIC). We also report the Akaike information criterion (AIC) for informational purposes but do not use it for model evaluation as it has a tendency for overextraction [see e.g., 77]. The exact fit values for a particular model were not relevant for deciding how many profiles to select. Instead, we analyzed the progressions of these model fit indices across the different number of profiles by visually inspecting the development of these fit indices analogously to the interpretation of scree plots [78] and selecting the profile after which the decrease in model fit was substantially lower compared to the previous profiles [79]. In addition, we used the Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio test (VLMR) as well as the Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR) to compare models with adjacent numbers of profiles. A significant p-value of these two tests indicates that a particular profile solution fits the data better than a model with one profile less. Finally, we also considered model parsimony (preferring simpler solutions over more complex ones), the statistical adequacy of the solution [e.g., absence of negative variance estimates; 80], and ease of interpretation (preferring solutions that yield profiles with qualitative differences and not solely mean levels, as well as profiles that can be sensibly interpreted). Subsequently, we used the posterior distributions to determine the most likely profile membership for each teacher (i.e., the profile to which an individual most likely belongs). The respective classification error of this procedure is reflected in the entropy values, with high values indicating small error.
We conducted this procedure for each for the three study sets (Israel-school, Germany-university, Germany-school). We then compared the number of profiles found and their configuration between these three study sets.
Next, we used the profile membership obtained from these analyses to examine the association of goal profiles with self-efficacy and work-related distress. To this end, we conducted equality tests of means across profiles using the BCH procedure [81]. Subsequently, we compared the exploratory power of goal profiles with the individual goals through structural equation modelling. Model 1 investigated goal profiles as predictors and Model 2 investigated the individual achievement goals. In each model, self-efficacy and work-related distress were regressed on these predictors. Achievement goals, self-efficacy, and work-related distress were estimated as latent variables based on item parcels as indicators [using the item-to-construct method, two parcels were used for each construct; see 82]. Again, we conducted these analyses separately on the level of the three study sets in order to allow for further insights into the generalizability of our findings.
We provide all code and data underlying our results in an open repository (https://osf.io/vrtsf/?view_only=e1eb3e81ab9b4c39a16530b354e74df5).
4. Results
Descriptive statistics for all analyzed datasets are presented in Table 2.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for all included datasets (displayed are percentages for the proportion of female and means and standard deviations for the remaining characteristics).
% Female | Years of teaching experience | Mastery (learning approach) goals | Performance approach goals | Performance avoidance goals | Work avoidance goals | Self-efficacy | Work-related distress | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Israel school teachers | ||||||||
Study 1: [10] | 91 | 15.90 (9.51) | 4.13 (0.62) | 3.43 (0.91) | 2.94 (0.77) | 2.56 (0.88) | 3.89 (0.56) | 2.63 (0.63) |
Study 2: [83] | 88 | 14.04 (9.63) | 4.17 (0.61) | 2.07 (1.97) | 2.73 (0.88) | 2.18 (0.93) | – | 2.65 (0.87) |
German university teachers | ||||||||
Study 3: [84] | 49 | – | 4.23 (0.71) | 3.36 (0.93) | 3.69 (0.99) | 1.70 (1.02) | 3.67 (0.57) | 1.82 (0.73) |
Study 4: [85] | 36 | 11.54 (9.13) | 4.29 (0.76) | 2.89 (0.99) | 3.44 (1.15) | 1.58 (1.01) | 3.70 (0.51) | 2.14 (0.50) |
German school teachers | ||||||||
Study 5: [24] | 67 | 13.7 (11.8) | 4.17 (0.51) | 2.42 (0.91) | 2.39 (0.95) | 2.33 (0.87) | 3.78 (0.52) | 2.21 (0.61) |
Study 6: [77] | 72 | 11.13 (11.6) | 4.34 (0.48) | 1.99 (0.90) | 2.28 (0.98) | 2.34 (0.96) | 4.08 (0.50) | 2.85 (0.78) |
Note. N = 3,681 teachers. For comparability of the descriptive statistics, goals, self-efficacy, and work-related distress have been rescaled to 1–5. “–” indicates that this variable was not assessed in the respective study.
4.1 Number of profiles
The results of the LPAs for the three study sets are presented in Table 3. We estimated and compared models comprising between two and nine profiles.
Table 3. Overview of LPA profile solutions.
Number of profiles | Fit indices | Likelihood ratio tests (p values) | Entropy | Number of teachers in profiles | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BIC | SSBIC | AIC | VLMR | LMR | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | ||
Israel-school study set | |||||||||||||||
2 | 14,831 | 14,790 | 14,763 | < .001 | < .001 | .66 | 789 | 569 | |||||||
3 | 14,721 | 14,664 | 14,627 | < .001 | < .001 | .63 | 754 | 312 | 292 | ||||||
4 | 14,705 | 14,632 | 14,585 | .08 | .08 | .55 | 409 | 379 | 302 | 268 | |||||
5 | 14,684 | 14,595 | 14,538 | .56 | .56 | .62 | 467 | 294 | 290 | 285 | 22 | ||||
6 | 14,692 | 14,587 | 14,519 | .03 | .04 | .64 | 476 | 304 | 256 | 188 | 100 | 34 | |||
7 | 14,699 | 14,578 | 14,501 | .19 | .19 | .65 | 374 | 353 | 254 | 208 | 92 | 74 | 3 | ||
8 | 14,713 | 14,576 | 14,489 | .21 | .21 | .67 | 362 | 355 | 234 | 185 | 117 | 53 | 49 | 3 | |
9 | 14,726 | 14,574 | 14,476 | .61 | .61 | .70 | 359 | 354 | 181 | 170 | 79 | 74 | 71 | 67 | 3 |
German-university study set | |||||||||||||||
2 | 17,880 | 17,839 | 17,809 | < .001 | < .001 | .79 | 1,254 | 511 | |||||||
3 | 17,486 | 17,429 | 17,387 | .001 | .002 | .83 | 1,217 | 370 | 178 | ||||||
4 | 17,200 | 17,127 | 17,074 | .002 | .002 | .80 | 1,019 | 356 | 245 | 145 | |||||
5 | 17,020 | 16,931 | 16,867 | .003 | .003 | .74 | 651 | 556 | 236 | 203 | 119 | ||||
6 | 16,899 | 16,795 | 16,719 | .04 | .04 | .76 | 657 | 524 | 200 | 189 | 158 | 37 | |||
7 | 16,871 | 16,750 | 16,663 | .35 | .36 | .77 | 646 | 515 | 191 | 177 | 172 | 52 | 12 | ||
8 | 16,820 | 16,683 | 16,584 | .59 | .59 | .78 | 647 | 484 | 195 | 179 | 106 | 86 | 53 | 15 | |
9 | 16,782 | 16,629 | 16,519 | .18 | .19 | .78 | 661 | 396 | 186 | 168 | 152 | 73 | 72 | 45 | 12 |
German-school study set | |||||||||||||||
2 | 6,095 | 6,053 | 6,038 | < .001 | < .001 | .73 | 286 | 271 | |||||||
3 | 6,058 | 6,001 | 5,980 | .11 | .12 | .74 | 289 | 207 | 61 | ||||||
4 | 6,027 | 5,954 | 5,928 | .56 | .57 | .80 | 288 | 209 | 57 | 3 | |||||
5 | 6,026 | 5,937 | 5,905 | .07 | .07 | .80 | 195 | 190 | 144 | 27 | 2 | ||||
6 | 6,030 | 5,925 | 5,887 | .52 | .53 | .81 | 195 | 189 | 35 | 28 | 8 | 3 | |||
7 | 6,034 | 5,914 | 5,870 | .35 | .36 | .81 | 189 | 175 | 128 | 38 | 19 | 6 | 3 | ||
8 | 6,041 | 5,904 | 5,855 | .54 | .54 | .79 | 180 | 154 | 125 | 50 | 27 | 14 | 6 | 2 | |
9 | 6,052 | 5,900 | 5,844 | .01 | .01 | .82 | 185 | 154 | 118 | 50 | 27 | 16 | 6 | 1 | 1 |
Note. BIC = Bayesian information criterion. SSA-BIC = sample-size-adjusted BIC. AIC = Akaike information criterion. VLMR = Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio test. LMR = Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test.
For the Israel-school study set, the differences in AIC, BIC, and SSBIC fit values were small after a three-profile solution and negligible after the six-profile model (see S1 Fig in S1 File for a visualization). For subsequent (i.e., seven and more profile solutions) models, we did not observe a good class discrimination, meaning that the new profiles were of low frequency and primarily characterized by differences in the general strength of goal pursuit but not so much by qualitative differences in the identified profiles. The three-profile solution was composed by three profiles that indicated whether mastery goals were endorsed alongside weak, moderate, or strong performance approach, performance avoidance, and work avoidance goals. The six-profile solution, however, indicated qualitatively more complex goal profiles that provided deeper insight into fluctuations within mastery goals as well. This is why we retained the six-profile solution, which provided an entropy of .64.
For the German-university study set, the fit differences pointed to a six-profile solution (see “knee” points for 2 and 6 profiles in S1 Fig in S1 File), as did the likelihood ratio tests. We did not observe a good class discrimination for more goal profiles, and therefore we retained the six-profile solution, which provided an entropy of .76.
For the German-school study set, the fit differences were small after a six-profile model, while the likelihood ratio suggested a two-profile model that primarily reflected mean level differences in performance goals (see S3 Fig in S1 File). The changes of the fit indices gradually became smaller after the two-profile solution, while the emerging profiles were still primarily characterized by mean level differences in performance goals (see S3 Fig in S1 File). Beginning with the six-profile solution, BIC values increased, while the changes in SSBIC and AIC values were small. In terms of content, the six-profile solution was the most meaningful; further profile solutions did not provide a good class discrimination. Although only a few teachers were classified into some of the profiles beyond a three-profile solution, we therefore again retained the six-profile solution to also allow for comparability with the other two study sets. The selected six-profile solution provided an entropy of .81.
In addition to these analyses, we also estimated the latent profiles on the level of an overall dataset that integrated the three datasets. The results from these additional findings are presented as supplementary materials (see S1 and S2 Tables, and S2 Fig in S1 File) and are to be interpreted with caution, as the three study sets contribute unequally to it with regard to sample size (with the German-university study sets being over- and the German-school study sets being underrepresented). Also on this level, both the decrease in fit indices and the likelihood ratio tests clearly spoke for the six-profile solution. This was further supported by low class discrimination in seven or more profile solutions and meaningful mean levels for the six-profile solution.
4.2 Configuration of profiles
Fig 1 shows the means of the achievement goals for the six-profile solutions for the different study sets. The profiles were similar for the different study sets, particularly between the Israeli-school and the German-university study sets. We observed a group of teachers that pursued as strong mastery goals as most of the other teachers and average levels of the other three goals (♢). Overall, most teachers were classified into this profile. In the second profile, the teachers exhibited similar levels of mastery goals, but low levels of the other three goals (●). This profile was also similar across all three study sets and contained a substantial number of teachers. Another profile that was very similar across all three study sets entailed weak mastery and weak performance goals and average levels of work avoidance goals (x). Overall, only relatively few teachers were classified into this profile.
Fig 1. Goal means for the six latent profiles (with 95% confidence intervals).
(a) Israel school teachers. (b) German university teachers. (c) German school teachers.
The configurations of the remaining three profiles varied a bit more across the study sets: First, there were two profiles with strong performance approach and performance avoidance goals. In the German-university study set, these had similarly strong levels of mastery goals but differed in the level of work avoidance goals, while in the Israel-school study set they differed in the level of mastery goals while work avoidance goals were similar between both profiles. In the German-school study set however, these two profiles also differed in the quantitative level of performance goals. Finally, the last profile was characterized by rather weak mastery goals and average to strong performance and work avoidance goals. This profile was more pronounced in the German-school study set than the other two.
On the overall dataset level, we found very similar profiles as in the three study sets, with average goal levels (♢; average all goals; overall: 39% of participants), strong mastery and weak performance and work avoidance goals (●; mastery high; 15%), weak mastery, weak performance, and average work avoidance goals (x; mastery/performance low; 2%), strong mastery, performance, and work avoidance goals (▲; high all; 13%), strong mastery and performance, but weak work avoidance goals (◻; mastery/performance high; 25%), as well as average mastery and performance goals combined with strong work avoidance goals (◼; work avoidance high; 6%).
It is worth pointing out that there were profiles that were characterized to the same extent by mastery (and work avoidance) goals, but had either strong, average, or weak performance goals. Indeed, these quantitative differences in performance goals characterized the three most prevalent profiles that around three fourths of the overall sample was classified into. Interestingly, we did not identify any profiles that were characterized by different levels of performance approach and performance avoidance goals. In somewhat similar vein, the high all and mastery/performance high profiles primarily differed by the extent to which work avoidance goals were pursued.
4.3 Associations of goal profiles with teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs and work-related distress
To determine the explanatory power of the profiles, we conducted equality tests of the means of self-efficacy and work-related distress across the latent profiles. Fig 2 displays standardized latent means and their 95%-confidence intervals, the results of all individual tests are provided in S3 Table in S1 File. The results indicated small differences in self-efficacy between teachers who were classified into different profiles (χ2 = 18.3–31.4, p < .003). Specifically, teachers in the work avoidance high profile reported lower self-efficacy beliefs than teachers in the average, high all, and mastery high profiles (χ2 = 4.3–38.3, p < .05). In addition, but only in the German-university study set, participants in the mastery/performance high profile in turn had stronger self-efficacy beliefs than the average and high all profile (χ2 = 4.9–16.0, p < .05). For work-related distress, we found a medium goal profile membership effect (χ2 = 31.1–95.3, p < .001). Teachers in the mastery high profile reported least work-related distress compared to teachers from the other profiles (χ2 > 4.2, p < .05), however, not all differences tests in the German-school study were statistically significant due to the small number of teachers in some profiles. Further, in the German-university study set, teachers in the high mastery profile did not differ statistically significantly from the mastery/performance high profile that was generally characterized by lower levels of work-related distress than the respective profile in the other two study sets. The highest levels of work-related distress were observed in the high all profile.
Fig 2. z-standardized latent means and 95% confidence intervals for self-efficacy and work-related distress levels in the six profiles per study set.
These differences in self-efficacy beliefs and work-related distress were also evident in structural equation models (see Table 4); profile membership explained 2–9% of the variance in self-efficacy and 7–8% of the variance in work-related distress across the three study sets. Conversely, taking a variable-centered approach by investigating the effect of the individual goals instead of the goal profiles, the individual achievement goals explained 9–25% of the variance in self-efficacy and 12–19% of the variance in work-related distress. Self-efficacy was positively related to mastery approach and performance approach goals and negatively related to performance avoidance and work avoidance goals, while work-related distress was negatively related to mastery goals, and positively to performance avoidance and work avoidance goals. This pattern of results was similar across the three study sets, with two exceptions: work avoidance goals did not reach statistical significance in the Israel-school study set for self-efficacy, and performance avoidance goals were statistically significantly related to increased work-related distress in the German-school study set but not the other two. Comparisons of the confidence intervals showed that the additive main effects of the achievement goals explained substantially more variance in the outcome variables than the goal profiles.
Table 4. Results of structural equation models analyzing the relations between goals profiles and individual goals with self-effiacy and work-related distress.
Israel-school study set (N = 1,358) | German-university study set (N = 1,765) | German-school study set (N = 558) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Self-efficacy | Work-related Distress | Self-efficacy | Work-related Distress | Self-efficacy | Work-related Distress | |
Model 1: Goal profile membership | ||||||
Average all goals (♢) | –a | –a | –a | –a | –a | –a |
Mastery high (●) | .01 (p = .97) | –.11 (p < .01) | .06 (p = .05) | –.11 (p < .01) | .07 (p = .18) | –.10 (p < .05) |
Mastery/performance low (x) | –.03 (p = .26) | .04 (p = .16) | –.02 (p = .48) | –.04 (p = .14) | .02 (p = .71) | .07 (p = .16) |
High all (▲) | –.05 (p = .19) | .21 (p < .01) | .02 (p = .57) | .14 (p < .01) | –.08 (p = .20) | .16 (p < .01) |
Mastery/performance high (◻) | –.09 (p = .02) | .11 (p < .01) | .14 (p < .01) | –.10 (p < .01) | –.09 (p = .09) | .15 (p < .01) |
Work avoidance high (◼) | –.14 (p < .01) | .11 (p < .01) | –.15 (p < .01) | .12 (p < .01) | –.25 (p < .01) | .02 (p = .75) |
R2 with 95% confidence interval | .02 [.01; .04] | .08 [.05; .11] | .05 [.03; .08] | .07 [.04; .09] | .09 [.01; .17] | .07 [.02; .12] |
Model 2: Individual achievement goals | ||||||
Mastery (learning approach) | .19 (p < .01) | –.16 (p < .01) | .27 (p < .01) | –.14 (p < .01) | .29 (p < .01) | –.10 (p = .20) |
Performance approach | .22 (p < .01) | –.04 (p = .56) | .18 (p < .01) | .06 (p = .20) | .43 (p < .01) | –.13 (p = .18) |
Performance avoidance | –.22 (p = .03) | .01 (p = .94) | –.11 (p = .02) | –.02 (p = .71) | –.53 (p < .01) | .32 (p < .01) |
Work avoidance | –.11 (p = .08) | .41 (p < .01) | –.13 (p < .01) | .27 (p < .01) | –.12 (p = .04) | .17 (p = .01) |
R2 with 95% confidence interval | .09 [.04; .13] | .19 [.13; .24] | .14 [.10; .19] | .12 [.08; .15] | .25 [.14; .36] | .12 [.04; .19] |
Note. Presented are standardized regression weights. Individual achievement goals, self-efficacy, and work-related distress were estimated as latent variables based on item parcels, χ2 ≤ 132.6, CFA ≥ .991, TLI ≥ .985, RMSEA ≤ .051, SRMR ≤ .028.
a The average all profile was used as a reference group.
5. Discussion
We analyzed achievement goal profiles of teachers and their meaningfulness by investigating whether psychologically reasonable, coherent, and generalizable goal profiles can be found in different teacher populations, how such profiles are associated with teachers’ self-efficacy and work-related distress, and how powerful such an approach can be compared to a more commonly used variable-centered approach. Strengths of the work include the consideration of multiple large datasets with similar measures from different educational settings, the investigation of goal profiles in a hitherto uninvestigated population, and the explicit comparison of exploratory power between profiles and individual goals.
Using large teacher samples from Israel and Germany as well as schools and universities and data about their mastery, performance approach, performance avoidance, and work avoidance goals, we found psychologically sensible and largely generalizable goal profiles that can be helpful in describing teachers’ achievement goal pursuit. However, most profiles primarily differed by strength of performance goals and had only little explanatory value compared to the individual achievement goals. As such, a more critical picture results from the present study towards using achievement goal profiles in teachers.
5.1 Is it possible to identify meaningful achievement goal profiles in teachers?
Regarding our first question, we identified six achievement goal profiles in each of the different study sets. In order to overcome limitations of person-centered approaches, we also investigated the generalizability of the profiles across countries and educational settings. In doing so, we found profiles to be largely comparable across the study sets. While the detected profiles were quite similar across the first two study sets (Israel school teachers, German university teachers), one should note that a less clear pattern emerged for the smallest study set of German school teachers. Only a few teachers were classified into some of the profiles, and goal means in the high all versus work avoidance high profiles differed somewhat more than in the other two study sets. Additionally, selection of the number of profiles was less clear in this sample. It is possible that the smaller sample size might have played some role. There is currently no consensus regarding the minimum required sample sizes for LPAs [for overviews, see 60, 77]. Although the sample sizes of the study sets were above commonly used rules of thumbs of N = 500 [59, 60], there is evidence that required sample sizes can vary depending on many aspects such as the number of indicators, the structure of the latent profiles, and covariates [59, 83, 86]. Furthermore, small samples might enable detection of the correct number of profiles [84], but not yield clear patterns for some of the profiles. This generally emphasizes that requirements of LPAs may not always be fulfilled and that one needs to be cautious when running and interpreting such analyses.
The goal profiles that we consistently found showed quantitative as well as qualitative differences. Similar to the meta-analysis of students’ achievement goal profiles by Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13] and the LPA of vocational training instructors by Kunst et al. [48], we found that the majority of participants were classified into an average all goals profile (39 percent overall) or a high all goals profile (13 percent overall). Further, our analyses documented that the identified profiles mainly differed in mean levels of performance goals but not so much in qualitatively different compositions of the other goals. In particular, the group with strong mastery and strong performance goals and the group with similarly strong mastery goals but much less strongly pursued performance goals were very similar to the respective profiles reported in Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13], and line up with initial theorizing into the nature of achievement goal pursuit from a multiple goal perspective (pursuit of mastery and performance goals; e.g., [12]). Finding such profiles in different populations strongly speaks to their existence and to their sensibility in describing differences in achievement goal pursuit.
However, comparing our findings to these previous works, it also needs to be noted that the profiles that we found mostly did not strongly differ regarding mastery goals. In fact, nearly all participants were in a profile with similarly strong mastery goals, with no profile with stronger mastery goals standing out having been identified. This finding may be well in line with the generally strong mastery goals reported for the teacher population and could indicate differences in goal pursuit between student and teacher populations [with teachers strongly valuing learning already; see also 10, 29]. Investigation of our sample characteristics also showed indications of such a general ceiling effect of mastery goals (reflected in large mean values and small standard deviations, see Table 2). Furthermore, while Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13] conflated performance avoidance and work avoidance goals, we investigated them separately from each other. This proved to be very helpful in the present work, as we found substantial differences in goal profiles based on teachers’ work avoidance goals (particularly regarding the high all vs. the mastery/performance high profiles). From a psychological perspective, it makes sense that teachers can pursue strong mastery and performance and either strong and little work avoidance goals. Our findings also point to the distinctiveness of work avoidance goals from other goals (particularly performance avoidance goals; see [85]) as well as the merits of including these goals for describing the pursuit of the different self-related aims that teachers may hold in achievement situations at work. Finally, as in Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13], we did not find goal profiles that differed between performance approach and performance avoidance goals, which is in line with the strong correlation typically reported for this type of goal, particularly in research on teachers [see 28, 32]. While it stands to reason that different groups of people should be able to be identified that differ in the extent to which they pursue performance approach and avoidance goals, these differences are likely much smaller than the differences regarding mastery and work avoidance goals, which is why they may not be readily found in latent profile analyses when also including these goals. Taken together, this suggests that combining performance approach and performance avoidance goals in profile analyses might not be particularly useful—at least when other achievement goals (such as mastery or work avoidance goals) are also considered.
In sum, goal profiles can theoretically be considered as sensible to describe how individuals actually pursue goals (multiple goals at the same time and to different strengths), however, the large proportion of profile differences due to mean level differences in performance goals might question the usefulness of the LPA approach (opposed to merely describing the strength to which they pursued the different achievement goals) for describing differences between different groups of people in the quality of their motivations.
5.2 How useful are teachers’ achievement goal profiles in explaining differences in teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs and work-related distress compared to individual goals?
Regarding our second and third research questions, we did find differences in self-efficacy beliefs and work-related distress in teachers based on the goal profiles they were assigned to. However, these differences were primarily found for those profiles such as mastery high or work avoidance high in which one type of goal was substantially stronger than the others. Moreover, results for these profiles were in line with the positive associations typically reported under a variable-centered view for mastery goals, as well as with the negative associations for work-avoidance goals, with favorable levels of self-efficacy beliefs and work-related distress. It should also be noted that while the general pattern of results regarding the differences in self-efficacy and work-related distress were largely similar across the three study sets, this was not the case for the mastery/performance high profile. Participants that were classified into this profile in the German-university sample exhibited higher levels of self-efficacy and lower levels of work-related distress than participants classified into the same profile in the other two study sets. This might be a function of the mastery/performance high profile that was not characterized by the same level of mastery goal across the three study sets (but primarily so within the German-university sample). As such, for the purpose of explaining differences in self-efficacy and work-related distress, knowledge of a teacher being classified into this profile proved not to be particularly helpful without further information on which study set the profile belonged to—or, more specifically, the exact levels of the mastery goals.
This finding was also evident in general, when comparing the profile approach to the main effects of the individual goals. Importantly, we found here that the inter-individual differences between teachers in their self-efficacy and work-related distress could be primarily traced back to individual goals and their additive effects. Compared to these effects, the goal profiles yielded hardly any explanatory value for teachers’ self-efficacy and work-related distress. Such direct comparisons between person-centered and variable-centered analyses are rare and an important source of information for evaluating the merits of both approaches to the study of achievement goals [43, 44, 87]. However, it should be noted that the amount of explained variance may not be a fair comparison criterion for the two methods, as goal profile membership contains restricted variance compared to the strength to which the individual goals are pursued. Nevertheless, as researchers typically use these approaches with the ultimate aim of explaining differences in outcome variables, the amount of explained variance is a highly relevant criterion with regard to application purposes. Furthermore, the small amounts of explained variances raises concerns about using them as a basis for profile-specific interventions. Taken together, our findings on the comparison of these two approaches imply that asking “which profile is more beneficial” may not be a very beneficial question—especially when this question is asked with a pure exploratory focus and the analysis is not guided by justified hypotheses on the effects of these profiles (as is frequently the case in general applications of person-centered approaches). Instead, the individual achievement goals seem to be more useful in explaining differences in teachers’ cognitions and experiences.
Regarding the multiple-goal perspective, this means that such an approach is likely adequate for describing actual goal pursuit, the core premise that functionality and effects of achievement goals would change substantially depending on their composition [see 12, 33] cannot be confirmed with the present work. Instead, the main effects that we found are rather in favor of a mastery-goal perspective—similar to the most favorable effects having been reported for the high-mastery goal profiles by Wormington and Linnenbrink-Garcia [13]. While we only investigated self-efficacy beliefs and work-related distress in the teacher population, this finding may also hold true for other variables and populations, and thus warrants a more skeptical view on the merits of achievement goal profiles in explaining cognitions and experiences of individuals compared to the main effects of the individual goals.
5.3 Limitations and future research
Interpreting these findings, there are limitations of the current work that need to be borne in mind. First, while we used data from different studies and different institutions, not all of the measures were identical (albeit very similar). As such, it is likely that with the measures tapping on slightly different aspects, we could not have found perfectly identical profiles across the different study sets in the first place. This means that our findings on the generalizability of the different goal profiles across the different study sets can be expected to underestimate the actual similarities.
Second, we only investigated teachers from Israel and Germany. While the inclusion of different countries and types of institutions is a great strength of the present investigation, future research could profit from including even more different (e.g., Eastern) countries or other Western countries that place greater emphasis on teacher evaluation through high-stakes testing (e.g., the US). This could offer a very interesting perspective regarding the generalizability of different goal profiles.
Third, our findings are limited in that we only included mastery, performance approach, performance avoidance, and work avoidance goals. While this lines up well with the majority of past research on teachers’ goals, roots in the fundamental work by Butler [22], and allows sensible comparisons to investigations into goal profiles in students, it should be noted that the profiles found might strongly depend on the different goals used (as indicated by our results for work avoidance goals). Future research might therefore also consider other goals, such as relational goals that have proven powerful in describing the motivations for teaching [see 88] and have also been used successfully in first investigations into academic and social goal profiles in students [see 87, 89].
Fourth, we used LPAs as a person-centered approach to studying teacher motivation. It should be considered that the background of person-centered analyses is to focus on the individual rather than on the population [thus the term “person-centered”; e.g., 13, 90]. However, in most studies, the “person-centered” approach is applied to compare subgroups of individuals on outcome variables [this is particularly true for research on goal profiles; e.g., 15, 17]. In this case, the term “person-centered approach” is strictly speaking misleading and it might be more appropriate to speak of “pattern-centered or categorical latent variable” analyses that allow for the identification and comparison of subgroups within a given population. In contrast, it may be argued that person-centered analyses in a strict sense require variance within individuals and as such, multiple measurement occasions [91] to investigate the interplay of variables within persons or their shift between identified subgroups. Even though this terminology might therefore be considered as somewhat inaccurate, we used the term “person-centered approach” in the present work given that it is commonly used in the achievement goal literature when referring to analyses into goal profiles.
5.4 Conclusions
Taken together, our findings indicate that results from latent profile analyses may be fuzzier than often thought. Our findings imply that goal profiles make sense and can be a good approach to describing teachers’ actual goal pursuit, with psychologically meaningful profiles that we consistently found across different study sets (albeit heavily relying on differences in the strength of performance goals instead of further qualitative differences in motivations). Nevertheless, finding goal profiles may not always be easy and depend on relevant methodological features such as the sample size. Moreover, the usefulness of this approach for explaining differences in teachers’ cognitions and experiences may be limited as goal profiles had little explanatory value compared to individual goals. As such, we consider goal profile analyses valuable for describing goal pursuit, but we also see danger in the unreflective use of such analyses and are skeptical about their added value for research on the correlates of teachers’ achievement motivation and achievement goal research in general.
Supporting information
(PDF)
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files. The data underlying the results presented in the study are available in an open repository (https://osf.io/vrtsf/).
Funding Statement
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Parker PD, Martin AJ. Coping and buoyancy in the workplace: Understanding their effects on teachers’ work-related well-being and engagement. Teaching and Teacher Education. 2009;25(1):68–75. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Janke S, Bardach L, Oczlon S, Lüftenegger M. Enhancing feasibility when measuring teachers’ motivation: A brief scale for teachers’ achievement goal orientations. Teaching and Teacher Education. 2019;83:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2019.04.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wang H, Hall NC, Goetz T, Frenzel AC. Teachers’ goal orientations: Effects on classroom goal structures and emotions. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2017;87(1):90–107. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fasching MS, Dresel M, Dickhäuser O, Nitsche S. Goal orientations of teacher trainees: Longitudinal analysis of magnitude, change and relevance. Journal for Educational Research Online. 2010;2(2):9–33. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Collie RJ, Martin AJ. Teachers’ sense of adaptability: Examining links with perceived autonomy support, teachers’ psychological functioning, and students’ numeracy achievement. Learning and Individual Differences. 2017;55:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Parker PD, Martin AJ, Colmar S, Liem GA. Teachers’ workplace well-being: Exploring a process model of goal orientation, coping behavior, engagement, and burnout. Teaching and Teacher Education. 2012;28(4):503–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2012.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Daumiller M, Rinas R, Olden D, Dresel M. Academics’ motivations in professional training courses: Effects on learning engagement and learning gains. International Journal for Academic Development. 2021;26(1):7–23. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Butler R, Shibaz L. Achievement goals for teaching as predictors of students’ perceptions of instructional practices and students’ help seeking and cheating. Learning and Instruction. 2008;18(5):453–67. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.06.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Retelsdorf J, Butler R, Streblow L, Schiefele U. Teachers’ goal orientations for teaching: Associations with instructional practices, interest in teaching, and burnout. Learning and Instruction. 2010;20(1):30–46. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Butler R. Striving to connect: Extending an achievement goal approach to teacher motivation to include relational goals for teaching. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012;104(3):726–42. doi: 10.1037/a0028613 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Retelsdorf J, Günther C. Achievement goals for teaching and teachers’ reference norms: Relations with instructional practices. Teaching and Teacher Education. 2011;27(7):1111–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2011.05.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pintrich PR. Multiple goals, multiple pathways: The role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2000;92(3):544–55. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.544 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wormington SV, Linnenbrink-Garcia L. A new look at multiple goal pursuit: The promise of a person-centered approach. Educational Psychology Review. 2016;29(3):407–45. doi: 10.1007/s10648-016-9358-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Veermans M, Tapola A. Primary school students’ motivational profiles in longitudinal settings. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. 2004;48(4):373–95. doi: 10.1080/0031383042000245780 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Jang LY, Liu WC. 2 × 2 Achievement goals and achievement emotions: A cluster analysis of students’ motivation. European Journal of Psychology of Education. 2011;27(1):59–76. doi: 10.1007/s10212-011-0066-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Tuominen-Soini H, Salmela-Aro K, Niemivirta M. Stability and change in achievement goal orientations: A person-centered approach. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2011;36(2):82–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.08.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pastor DA, Barron KE, Miller BJ, Davis SL. A latent profile analysis of college students’ achievement goal orientation. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2007;32(1):8–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Richardson PW, Karabenick SA, Watt HM, editors. Teacher motivation: Routledge; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ames C, Archer J. Achievement goals in the classroom: Students’ learning strategies and motivation processes. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1988;80(3):260–7. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.260 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Duda JL, Nicholls JG. Dimensions of achievement motivation in schoolwork and sport. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1992;84(3):290–9. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.290 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Janssen O, Van Yperen NW. Employees’ goal orientations, the quality of leader-member exchange, and the outcomes of job performance and job satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal. 2004;47(3):368–84. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Butler R. Teachers’ achievement goal orientations and associations with teachers’ help seeking: Examination of a novel approach to teacher motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2007;99(2):241–52. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.241 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Elliot AJ. A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. Elliot AJ, Dweck CS, editors: Guilford; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nitsche S, Dickhäuser O, Fasching MS, Dresel M. Rethinking teachers’ goal orientations: Conceptual and methodological enhancements. Learning and Instruction. 2011;21(4):574–86. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wang J, Shen B, Luo X, Hu Q, Garn AC. Validation of a teachers’ achievement goal instrument for teaching physical education. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education. 2018;37(1):91–100. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.2016-0210 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Elliot AJ, Murayama K, Pekrun R. A 3 × 2 achievement goal model. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2011;103(3):632–48. doi: 10.1037/a0023952 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mascret N, Elliot AJ, Cury F. The 3 × 2 Achievement Goal Questionnaire for teachers. Educational Psychology. 2015;37(3):346–61. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2015.1096324 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Daumiller M, Dickhäuser O, Dresel M. University instructors’ achievement goals for teaching. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2019;111(1):131–48. doi: 10.1037/edu0000271 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kaplan A, Middleton MJ. Should childhood be a journey of a race? Response to Harackiewicz et al. (2002). Journal of Educational Psychology. 2002;94(3):646–8. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.3.646 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Midgley C, Kaplan A, Middleton M. Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for whom, under what circumstances, and at what cost? Journal of Educational Psychology. 2001;93(1):77–86. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.77 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Harackiewicz JM, Barron KE, Pintrich PR, Elliot AJ, Thrash TM. Revision of achievement goal theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2002;94(3):638–45. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.3.638 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Janke S, Nitsche S, Praetorius A-K, Benning K, Fasching M, Dresel M, et al. Deconstructing performance goal orientations: The merit of a dimensional approach. Learning and Individual Differences. 2016;50:133–46. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.08.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Senko C, Hulleman CS, Harackiewicz JM. Achievement Goal Theory at the crossroads: Old controversies, current challenges, and new directions. Educational Psychologist. 2011;46(1):26–47. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2011.538646 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Elliot AJ, McGregor HA. A 2 × 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80(3):501–19. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Shieh G. Detecting interaction effects in moderated multiple regression with continuous variables power and sample size considerations. Organizational Research Methods. 2008;12(3):510–28. doi: 10.1177/1094428108320370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Morin AJS. Person-centered research strategies in commitment research. Handbook of Employee Commitment: Edward Elgar Publishing. p. 490–508. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Murayama K, Elliot AJ, Yamagata S. Separation of performance-approach and performance-avoidance achievement goals: A broader analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2011;103(1):238. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Daumiller M, Janke S, Rinas R, Dickhäuser O, Dresel M. Need satisfaction and achievement goals of university faculty: an international study of their interplay and relevance. Higher Education. 2021;83(6):1183–206. doi: 10.1007/s10734-021-00736-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Watt HM, Richardson PW. An introduction to teaching motivations in different countries: Comparisons using the FIT-Choice scale. Asia-Pacific Journal of Teacher Education. 2012;40(3):185–97. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Dekker S, Fischer R. Cultural differences in academic motivation goals: A meta-analysis across 13 societies. The Journal of Educational Research. 2008;102(2):99–110. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Maehr ML. Culture and achievement motivation. American Psychologist. 1974;29(12):887–96. doi: 10.1037/h0037521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Litalien D, Morin AJS, McInerney DM. Generalizability of achievement goal profiles across five cultural groups: More similarities than differences. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2017;51:267–83. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.08.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Marsh HW, Lüdtke O, Trautwein U, Morin AJS. Classical latent profile analysis of academic self-concept dimensions: Synergy of person- and variable-centered approaches to theoretical models of self-concept. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2009;16(2):191–225. doi: 10.1080/10705510902751010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Linnenbrink-Garcia L, Pugh KJ, Koskey KLK, Stewart VC. Developing conceptual understanding of natural selection: The role of interest, efficacy, and basic prior knowledge. The Journal of Experimental Education. 2012;80(1):45–68. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2011.559491 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Vansteenkiste M, Sierens E, Soenens B, Luyckx K, Lens W. Motivational profiles from a self-determination perspective: The quality of motivation matters. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2009;101(3):671–88. doi: 10.1037/a0015083 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Plucker JA, Makel MC. Replication is important for educational psychology: Recent developments and key issues. Educational Psychologist. 2021;56(2):90–100. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Watt HM, Butler R, Richardson PW. Antecedents and consequences of teachers’ goal profiles in Australia and Israel. Learning and Instruction. 2021;76:101491. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kunst EM, van Woerkom M, Poell RF. Teachers’ goal orientation profiles and participation in professional development activities. Vocations and Learning. 2017;11(1):91–111. doi: 10.1007/s12186-017-9182-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cho Y, Shim SS. Predicting teachers’ achievement goals for teaching: The role of perceived school goal structure and teachers’ sense of efficacy. Teaching and Teacher Education. 2013;32:12–21. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2012.12.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Gorozidis G, Papaioannou A. Teachers’ self-efficacy, achievement goals, attitudes and intentions to implement the new Greek physical education curriculum. European Physical Education Review. 2011;17(2):231–53. doi: 10.1177/1356336x11413654 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Klassen RM, Chiu MM. Effects on teachers’ self-efficacy and job satisfaction: Teacher gender, years of experience, and job stress. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2010;102(3):741–56. doi: 10.1037/a0019237 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Klassen RM, Tze V, Betts SM, Gordon KA. Teacher efficacy research 1998–2009: Signs of progress or unfulfilled promise? Educational psychology review. 2011;23(1):21–43. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Daumiller M, Grassinger R, Dickhäuser O, Dresel M. Structure and relationships of university instructors’ achievement goals. Front Psychol. 2016;7:375-. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00375 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hascher T, Waber J. Teacher well-being: A systematic review of the research literature from the year 2000–2019. Educational research review. 2021;34:100411. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Diener E. Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist. 2000;55(1):34–43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Maslach C, Leiter MP. Teacher burnout: A research agenda. 1999.
- 57.Hakanen JJ, Bakker AB, Schaufeli WB. Burnout and work engagement among teachers. Journal of School Psychology. 2006;43(6):495–513. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2005.11.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Nitsche S, Dickhäuser O, Fasching MS, Dresel M. Teachers’ professional goal orientations: Importance for further training and sick leave. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013;23:272–8. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.07.017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in Latent Class Analysis and Growth Mixture Modeling: A Monte Carlo Simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2007;14(4):535–69. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Spurk D, Hirschi A, Wang M, Valero D, Kauffeld S. Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. Journal of Vocational Behavior. 2020;120:103445. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2020.103445 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Maslach C, Jackson SE, Leiter MP. Maslach burnout inventory: Scarecrow Education; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Butler R, Shibaz L, Levontin L. Contextual and motivational antecedents of teachers’ social support for students. 2020 AERA Annual Meeting; San Francisco, CA2020.
- 63.Daumiller M, Dresel M. Teaching and research: Specificity and congruence of university faculty achievement goals. International Journal of Educational Research. 2020;99:101460. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2019.08.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Nie Y, Lau S, Liau A. The teacher efficacy scale: A reliability and validity study. The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher. 2012;21(2):414–21. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Büssing A, Glaser J. Managerial stress und burnout. A collaborative international study (CISMS). Die deutsche Untersuchung. Technical University Munich, 1998.
- 66.Schwarzer R, Hallum S. Perceived teacher self-efficacy as a predictor of job stress and burnout: mediation analyses. Applied Psychology. 2008;57(s1):152–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00359.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Enzmann D, Kleiber D. Helfer-Leiden. Heidelberg: Asanger; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Janke S, Nitsche S, Dickhäuser O. The role of perceived need satisfaction at work for teachers’ work-related learning goal orientation. Teaching and Teacher Education. 2015;47:184–94. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2015.01.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Dickhäuser O, Schöne C, Spinath B, Stiensmeier-Pelster J. Die Skalen zum akademischen Selbstkonzept. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie. 2002;23(4):393–405. doi: 10.1024//0170-1789.23.4.393 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Ponterotto JG, Ruckdeschel DE. An overview of coefficient alpha and a reliability matrix for estimating adequacy of internal consistency coefficients with psychological research measures. Perceptual and motor skills. 2007;105(3):997–1014. doi: 10.2466/pms.105.3.997-1014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Tschannen-Moran M, Hoy AW. Teacher efficacy: Capturing an elusive construct. Teaching and Teacher Education. 2001;17(7):783–805. doi: 10.1016/s0742-051x(01)00036-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Friedman IA, Farber BA. Professional self-concept as a predictor of teacher burnout. The Journal of Educational Research. 1992;86(1):28–35. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1992.9941824 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Peugh JL, Enders CK. Missing data in educational research: A review of reporting practices and suggestions for improvement. Review of Educational Research. 2004;74(4):525–56. doi: 10.3102/00346543074004525 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-sStep approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2014;21(3):329–41. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.915181 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Muthén L, Muthén B. Mplus. Version 8.1 2017.
- 76.Hipp JR, Bauer DJ. Local solutions in the estimation of growth mixture models. Psychological Methods. 2006;11(1):36–53. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.1.36 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Park J, Yu H-T. Recommendations on the sample szes for multilevel Latent Class Models. Educ Psychol Meas. 2018;78(5):737–61. Epub 2017/07/09. doi: 10.1177/0013164417719111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Cattell RB. The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1966;1(2):245–76. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr0102_10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Morin AJS, Morizot J, Boudrias J-S, Madore I. A multifoci person-centered perspective on workplace affective commitment: A latent profile/factor mixture analysis. Organizational Research Methods. 2010;14(1):58–90. doi: 10.1177/1094428109356476 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Bauer DJ, Curran PJ. The Integration of continuous and dscrete latent variable models: Potential problems and promising opportunities. Psychological Methods. 2004;9(1):3–29. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.9.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Using the BCH method in Mplus to estimate a distal outcome model and an arbitrary secondary model. Mplus web notes. 2014;21(2):1–22. [Google Scholar]
- 82.Little TD, Cunningham WA, Shahar G, Widaman KF. To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2002;9(2):151–73. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem0902_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Wurpts IC, Geiser C. Is adding more indicators to a latent class analysis beneficial or detrimental? Results of a Monte-Carlo study. Front Psychol. 2014;5:920-. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00920 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Tein J-Y, Coxe S, Cham H. Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Struct Equ Modeling. 2013;20(4):640–57. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2013.824781 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.King RB, McInerney DM. The work avoidance goal construct: Examining its structure, antecedents, and consequences. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2014;39(1):42–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.12.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Henson JM, Reise SP, Kim KH. Detecting mixtures from structural model differences using latent variable mixture modeling: A comparison of relative model fit statistics. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2007;14(2):202–26. doi: 10.1080/10705510709336744 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Bardach L, Daumiller M, Lüftenegger M. Multiple social and academic achievement goals: Students’ goal profiles and their linkages. The Journal of Experimental Education. 2022:1–21. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2022.2081959 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Butler R. What teachers want to achieve and why it matters. Teacher Motivation: Routledge; 2014. p. 20–35. [Google Scholar]
- 89.Shim SS, Finch WH. Academic and social achievement goals and early adolescents’ adjustment: A latent class approach. Learning and Individual Differences. 2014;30:98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.10.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Bergman LR, Trost K. The person-oriented versus the variable-oriented approach: Are they complementary, opposites, or exploring different worlds? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2006;52(3):601–32. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0023 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Voelkle MC, Brose A, Schmiedek F, Lindenberger U. Toward a unified framework for the study of between-person and within-person structures: building a bridge between two research paradigms. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2014;49(3):193–213. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.889593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
(PDF)
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files. The data underlying the results presented in the study are available in an open repository (https://osf.io/vrtsf/).