Skip to main content
Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2023 Apr 26:1–13. Online ahead of print. doi: 10.1007/s10803-023-05988-7

Using Pivotal Response Treatment to Improve Language Functions of Autistic Children in Special Schools: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Leilei Wang 1,2,#, Shuting Li 3,#, Chongying Wang 1,2,
PMCID: PMC10132802  PMID: 37101061

Abstract

Language difficulties exert profound negative effects on the cognitive and social development of autistic children. Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) is a promising intervention for improving social communication in autistic children, but there is a lack of a comprehensive examination of language functions. This study aimed to investigate the effectiveness of PRT in promoting the primary language functions (requesting, labeling, repeating, responding) defined by (Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Martino Publishing.) theory of verbal behavior in autistic children. Thirty autistic children were randomly divided into the PRT group (Mage = 6.20, SDage = 1.21) and control group (Mage = 6.07, SDage = 1.49). The PRT group were provided with an 8-week training of the PRT motivation component in addition to treatment as usual (TAU) in their schools, whereas the control group only received TAU. Parents of the PRT group were also trained to practice the PRT motivation procedures at home. The PRT group demonstrated greater improvements in all four measured language functions compared to the control group. The improvement in language functions in the PRT group was generalized and maintained at the follow-up assessment. In addition, the PRT intervention enhanced untargeted social and communicative functioning, cognition, motor skills, imitation, and adaptive behaviors in the autistic children. In conclusion, language intervention using the motivation component of PRT is effective in promoting language functions as well as widespread untargeted cognitive and social functions in autistic children.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10803-023-05988-7.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder, Pivotal response treatment, Motivation procedure, Language functions


Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by social communication impairments along with restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). One of the most notable features of autistic children is deficits in language (Kwok et al., 2015; Marini et al., 2020; Reindal et al., 2021), including delayed language development (Marrus et al., 2018), impaired verbal imitation (Chen et al., 2022; Wang et al., 2021), and atypical response to speech (Chen et al., 2019; Ramezani et al., 2019). Language-related deficits in autistic children have been found to exert enormous impact on cognitive and social development (Franchini et al., 2018; Tang et al., 2022; Weismer et al., 2018), such as the acquisition of social skills (Levinson et al., 2020). Indeed, atypical language performance has been shown to be a major concern of parents of autistic children (Issarraras et al., 2019; Richards et al., 2016). Therefore, it is critical for effective therapies to be developed that can help to facilitate the development of language skills in autistic children.

Among a wide range of interventions that have attempted to improve language in autistic children, Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) has emerged as a promising approach (Fossum et al., 2018; Koegel et al., 2019; Mohammadzaheri et al., 2014, 2021; Verschuur et al., 2014). PRT is a naturalistic developmental behavioral intervention based on the principles of applied behavior analysis (ABA) (Koegel et al., 1987). At its core, PRT proposes that targeting pivotal areas of a child’s development, such as motivation and self-initiation, will lead to widespread improvements in behavior, communication, and social interactions (Koegel & Koegel, 2006, 2016). During PRT, learning opportunities are imbedded in child-led, semi-structured interactions between the child and administrators, who may be the therapists or parents (Koegel & Koegel, 2019). Compared to other strictly structured ABA approaches, such as discrete-trial training (DTT) (Lovaas et al., 1974), PRT is less time-consuming and less costly and has been shown to result in improved maintenance and generalization of the intervention outcomes (Koegel & Koegel, 2019).

Motivation is regarded as the fundamental component of PRT, with the rationale being that a child is more likely to learn when they are motivated (Koegel & Koegel, 2019; Koegel et al., 2001). To improve motivation during training sessions in PRT, evidence-based procedures have been developed (Dunlap, 1984; Dyer et al., 1990; Koegel, et al., 1998a, 1998b; Moes, 1998). Several studies using these PRT motivational procedures have found improvements in verbal initiations (Koegel et al., ; Popovic et al., 2020) and utterance length (Mohammadzaheri et al., 2014). Moreover, the improvements in the targeted linguistic skills in autistic children were found to generalize to new and unfamiliar settings (Koegel et al., ) as well as to other untargeted pragmatic skills (Mohammadzaheri et al., 2014, 2021).

Although PRT has demonstrated promising effects on language and communication in autistic children, a recent review by Forbes et al. (2020) found that most existing PRT intervention studies did not provide sufficient description or distinguishment of the targeted language outcomes. Common outcome measures have included broad communication skills (Baker-Ericzén et al., 2007; Fossum et al., 2018; Stock et al., 2013) or social facilitation and engagement (Feldman & Matos, 2013; Kim et al., 2017). The unclear and inconsistent measurement of language complicates the comparison and applicability of findings across previous studies. To resolve this issue, it is important to examine the effect of PRT using a widely used and accepted language framework.

A classic language framework has been developed by Skinner (1957), referred to as the theory of verbal behavior. According to Skinner (1957), language is similar to other operant behavior, acquired and maintained by its antecedent and consequences. Rather than emphasizing the mechanics of language (e.g., morphology, syntax, semantics), Skinner (1957) focused on the functions of language (e.g., requesting, labeling, repeating, and responding). As Skinner’s verbal behavior framework directly links the targeted communicative skills with observable context, it has been broadly applied to guide language therapies and research in ASD (Forbes et al., 2020; Johnson et al., 2017; Sundberg & Michael, 2001). It remains unclear, however, whether PRT can be applied to improve the language functions proposed by Skinner (1957).

The current study aimed to examine the effectiveness of PRT on well-defined language functions based on Skinner’s (1957) theory of verbal behavior in autistic children. In this study, an 8-week PRT motivation intervention targeting verbal requesting, labeling, repeating, and responding was implemented in autistic children in autism-specialized schools. It was hypothesized that the PRT intervention relative to regular treatment would result in greater improvement in the targeted language functions and other untargeted cognitive and behavioral functions.

Methods

Participants

Participants were enrolled between May and July 2020 from two special schools and two rehabilitation centers in Ningbo, Zhejiang province, China. These institutions provided similar schooling and training services for children with ASD. Thirty-five subjects aged between 4 and 8 years were initially recruited. Participants were included only if they (1) had received a clinical diagnosis of ASD based on the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), (2) had no other genetic disorders or severe physical or psychiatric disorders, especially hearing impairment, and (3) had never received any PRT intervention. Five children were excluded as two children could not ensure completing the intervention and three parents were unable to complete the PRT parent training.

Thirty children were randomly assigned to the PRT group and control group in a 1:1 ratio using a computer-generated algorithm. The PRT and control groups were not significantly different in age, sex, or receptive vocabulary knowledge (Table 1). Based on the standardized Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Revised Edition (PPVT-R) scores, all participants had an intellectual disability (i.e., IQ ≤ 50).

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of participants

Variables PRT (n = 15) Control (n = 15) Group comparison
Age, M (SD), range 6 (1), 5–9 6 (1), 4–8 t(58) = 0.48, p = .79
Gender
 Male, n (%) 12 (80) 10 (67) χ2(1) = 0.68, p = .41
 Female, n (%) 3 (20) 5 (33)
PPVT-R, M (SD) 6 (5) 5 (5) t(28) = 0.77, p = .62

Age was measured in years. The raw score of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Revised Edition (PPVT-R) was used to compare receptive vocabulary between the two groups

PRT Pivotal Response Training

Therapists

Twenty qualified therapists were enrolled, with 10 in the PRT group and 10 in the control group. All enrolled therapists (1) had an educational background in special education, (2) had worked in special education for at least 5 years and were currently working in this sector, and (3) had received systematic ABA training. Therapists in the PRT group had obtained the PRT Level I Certificate and had conducted PRT for at least 2 years. Therapists in the control group had not received any PRT training.

Scorers

Four scorers were selected to assess the language abilities, cognition, and behavior of the participants. The selected scorers (1) had an educational background in special education, (2) had worked in special education for at least 10 years and were currently working in this sector, and (3) had qualifications for scoring the Chinese Psychoeducational Profile—Third Edition (CPEP-3) and the Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program (VB-MAPP) and had at least 5 years of experience scoring the two measures. The scorers were blind to the group of the participants. The four scorers conducted pre-tests of language functions in two autistic children using the CPEP-3 and VB-MAPP and achieved a reliable interobserver agreement (all Kendall’s W > 0.90, p < 0.001).

Materials

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Revised Edition (PPVT-R)

The Chinese version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Revised Edition (PPVT-R) was used to measure receptive vocabulary (raw score) and intelligence quotient (standardized score) (Dunn & Dunn, 1981; Sang & Miao, 1990). In the test, the child listens to a word uttered by the interviewer and then selects one of four pictures that best reflects the meaning of the word. One hundred and twenty items were included in the current test. The PPVT-R has been shown to be moderately correlated with performance in a Chinese language course for school-age children (r = 0.54; Sang & Miao, 1990) and has been shown to have high-to-very-high test–retest reliability (r = 0.82–0.92; Tillinghast et al., 1983).

Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program (VB-MAPP)

The Chinese version of the Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program (Huang & Li, 2017) was used to assess language functions based on Skinner’s (1957) Theory of Verbal Behavior. The VB-MAPP has been shown to have moderate-to-high content validity (Padilla & Akers, 2021).

Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ)

The Chinese version of the Social Communication Questionnaire (Gau et al., 2011; Rutter et al., 2003) was used to evaluate communication skills and social functioning. The SCQ contains 40 questions (yes = 1, no = 0) that is completed by the principal caregiver of the autistic individual, with higher scores indicating higher severity of autistic symptoms. The SCQ has demonstrated adequate test–retest reliability (intraclass correlation coefficient = 0.77–0.78) and moderate concurrent validity with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (r = 0.49–0.63; Gau et al., 2011).

Chinese Psychoeducational Profile—Third Edition (CPEP-3)

The performance test of the Chinese version of Psychoeducational Profile—Third Edition (CPEP-3; Schopler et al., 2005; Shek & Yu, 2014) was used to assess cognition, motor skills, and adaptive behaviors. The CPEP-3 is suitable for autistic and non-autistic children between 2 and 7.5 years old. The CPEP-3 has been reported to have high-to-very-high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.70) and test–retest reliability (r > 0.70; Yu et al., 2019).

Intervention Procedures

After initial screening and informed consent, 30 children with ASD were randomly divided into the PRT group and the control group (Fig. 1). All participants received an 8-week one-on-one language intervention at the respective special school or training center in which they were enrolled. Based on Skinner’s framework (1957), four primary types of verbal behaviors -mand, tact, echoic, and listener responding- were targeted. A mand refers to requesting an object or event from others. A tact refers to labelling or naming objects and events. An echoic refers to repeating what is heard. Listener responding occurs when the listener responds to the verbal request of another person in the form of an action. There were three 35-min intervention sessions each week. The control group received treatment as usual (TAU), while the PRT group received the motivational components of PRT in addition to TAU. During the intervention phase, parents of the PRT group were provided with a one-hour online PRT learning session relating to the theory and techniques of PRT. The parents in the PRT group were asked to administer a 30-min PRT motivational procedure with their autistic child at home each day until the conclusion of the study.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

CONSORT Flowchart of Study Procedure. PRT Pivotal Response Treatment; TAU treatment as usual

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

The PRT intervention was based on published guides of PRT motivational procedures (Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, 2012; Stahmer et al., 2011). The targeted language functions (i.e., mand, tact, echoic, and listener responding) were the same for all autistic children, but the intervention plan was individualized based on the child’s interest and language abilities (for examples, see Table 2). Therapists selected stimulus items according to the child’s preference. Parents and therapists signed a consent form to ensure that the selected stimulus items were only used them during the intervention sessions. Items and activities that were easily achieved were prioritized. The reinforcement was directly related to the targeted behaviors. For example, the therapist reinforced the spontaneous request “open the door” from a child by opening the door rather than by giving edibles. In addition to the directly related reinforcer, the therapist would reinforce the desired behaviors through generalized praise, such as “Great job!”. The child was reinforced for meaningful attempts to achieve the targeted behaviors, even if they were not able to achieve the targeted behaviors. The reinforcement was provided immediately when an attempt or behavior occurred. When a task had been learned, a more difficult task was introduced (for examples, see Table 2). The ratio of the learned to new tasks was 7:1.

Table 2.

Examples of language interventions in the PRT group

Targeted skills Examples
Mand
 Task 1: Verbally initiate four requests with language prompts The therapist demonstrated soap bubbles and other items to the child. When the child stared at the bubbles, the therapist asked, “What do you want?” The child answered, “bubbles”. Bubbles were then given to the child
 Task 2: Spontaneously verbally initiate five items in  1 hour Without any prompts from the therapist, the child said “slide”. The child would then be allowed to ride a slide
 Task 3: Verbally initiate eight requests with or without language prompts The child said “push”, and the therapist would then push the swing. The therapist might ask, “What do you want?” as a prompt for the request from the child
Tact
 Task 1: Name the reinforcing stimuli through imitation, with 90% agreement The therapist and child read the names of everyday items on picture cards. Once the child repeated the names with or after the therapist pronounced the names, the therapist would then pass the picture cards to them and say, “You are right!”
 Task 2: Independently name four items in one hour The child independently named items in a book, such as a watermelon, a door, etc. The therapist might provide a prompt, such as, “What is this?” If the child named the item correctly, the therapist would praise and repeat their answers (e.g., “You are right! This is a watermelon.”)
 Task 3: Spontaneously name ten items Without any prompts from the therapist, the child named 10 items based on their interest during play. If the child named the items correctly, the therapist would pass the items to the child
 Task 4: Name 25 items when others ask, “what is this?” The therapist pointed to a picture card and asked, “What is it?” The child named the item in the picture correctly. If the child named the item correctly, the therapist would pass the cards to the child. Once the child has consistently named the items correctly, the therapist would say, “You are right!” without passing the cards
 Task 5: Independently name five actions When the therapist instructed the child to do some exercise with music, the child independently named an action. Once the child named the action, the therapist would do the action with the child
Echoic
 Task 1: Repeat nouns with 90% agreement The therapist played a game of cutting fruits with the child and encouraged the child to imitate pronouncing the word “apple”, “banana”, “knife”, etc. If the child pronounced the words correctly, the therapist would then pass the named toys to the child
 Task 2: Repeat phrases (noun + verb) with 90% agreement In the fruit cutting game, the therapist said, “cutting a watermelon” and prompted the child to repeat. If the child repeated the phrase, the therapist would show cutting watermelon or cut the watermelon with the child
 Task 3: Repeat short sentences Based on the child’s interest, the therapist chose a suitable music video. Whilst watching the music video, the therapist said some short sentences based on the rhythm of the music and prompted the child to repeat. If the child repeated, the therapist would then demonstrate the named actions
Listener responding
 Task 1: Respond to their name five times When the therapist called the child’s name, the child looked at the therapist. Once the child looked at the therapist, the therapist would hug the child
 Task 2: Complete six gross motor movements according to other’s verbal instructions During exercise time, the child could complete a gross motor movement following the verbal instruction of the therapist. The movement need not be perfect. If the child showed the correct movement, the therapist would play the movement-related games with the child
 Task 3: Identify 20 items from pairs of four following others’ verbal instructions The therapist provided four items to the child during play. The child could identify the item based on the verbal instruction of the therapist. If the child correctly identified the items, the therapist would then pass the items to the child

The task difficulty increased with the task number. The commencing and concluding tasks were tailored to each child’s verbal and learning abilities

To enhance the fidelity of intervention implementation, all therapists participated in an online meeting halfway through and at the conclusion of the intervention phase. The meetings were to discuss the children’s behaviors and the issues of intervention and to adjust the intervention plan based on the PRT theory. For the parent-interventions, parents were asked to share a video of their intervention in turns in the weekly online parent-learning session. Then the therapists provided suggestions to ensure that parents’ intervention were in line with the PRT motivational procedure.

Treatment as Usual (TAU)

TAU targeted the same language functions as the PRT intervention, but the TAU sessions were led only by the therapists and not the parents. The choice of stimuli, the criteria for the target response, and the type of reinforcement were pre-defined and fixed. During TAU, the reinforcement was not directly related to the targeted behaviors. For example, a child was rewarded with edibles when they initiated a request to open the door. Children were only rewarded when they completed a targeted behavior, and the provision of the reinforcement might be delayed.

Data Collection

Assessments using the VB-MAPP, SCQ, and CPEP-3 were conducted prior to the language intervention (i.e., baseline test) and in the week following the completion of the intervention (i.e., intervention test). In the same week after the intervention test, to examine the generalization of language functions, children were tested with new stimuli and therapists in new environments using the generalization components of VB-MAPP. To assess the maintenance of language functions, a maintenance test was conducted three weeks following the completion of the intervention using the same VB-MAPP as the baseline and intervention tests. An 8-week intervention with a 3-week follow up test was chosen to fit into a school term to avoid missing data and confounding factors during school holidays.

Data Analyses

The data were analyzed using SPSS 21.0. To examine whether different language functions facilitated each other, Pearson’s correlation analyses were conducted to examine the relation between the measurements of different language functions (i.e., mand, tact, echoic, and listener responding) of all participants. To determine whether the PRT intervention improved the language abilities and whether the effect was maintained following the conclusion of the intervention, repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on the score of each language skill, with the treatment group (PRT versus control) and phase (baseline, intervention, generalization, maintenance) as the main factors. To explore whether the PRT intervention improved other social and cognitive functions, repeated ANOVA was performed on the SCQ score and CPEP-3 scores (i.e., cognition, fine motor, gross motor, imitation, and adaptive behaviors), with the treatment group (PRT versus control) and phase (baseline versus intervention) as the main factors. Partial eta square of 0.01, 0.06, and 0.14 were considered to represent small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively.

Results

Although two participants in the PRT group received lower language and CPEP-3 scores compared to other participants in the same group, their scores did not significantly affect the comparison between the PRT and control groups. Therefore, no data were excluded.

Effects of PRT on Language Functions

Based on Pearson’s correlation analyses, the four language functions (i.e., mand, tact, echoic, and listener responding) were correlated with each other (all rs > 0.6, ps < 0.01; see Table 3).

Table 3.

Pearson’s correlations between language functions

Mand Tact Echoic Listener responding
Mand
Tact .85*
Echoic .70* .66*
Listener Responding .68* .66* .69*

Language functions were measured using the Chinese version of the Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program (VB-MAPP)

*p < .05

Based on repeated measures ANOVAs, PRT demonstrated consistent improvements in all four language measures (see Fig. 2). The ANOVAs showed a large main effect of group (all ps < 0.05, η2 > 0.2) and phase (all ps < 0.001, η2 > 0.4) and a large interaction effect between group and phase (all ps < 0.001, η2 > 0.2) (see Table 4). According to simple effects analyses, the PRT group scored significantly higher on all language measures in the intervention, generalization, and maintenance phases compared to the baseline phase (all ps < 0.001) and showed no significant change in scores in the generalization and maintenance phases compared to the intervention phase (both ps > 0.05; see Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 1).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Effects of PRT and Regular Training on Language Functions in Autistic Children. Note. The PRT group showed significant, generalizable, and sustainable improvements in a mand, b tact, c echoic, and d listener responding, compared to the control group. The horizontal solid brackets indicate the significant results between different phases in the PRT group. The horizontal dotted brackets indicate the significant results between different phases in the control group. The vertical solid brackets indicate the significant findings between group in each phrase. Language functions were measured using the Chinese version of the VB-MAPP. The intervention and generalization assessments occurred in Week 10. The maintenance assessment occurred in Week 13. PRT Pivotal response training

Table 4.

Measures of language functions in the PRT and control groups in each phase

PRT Control Statistics
M ± SD
Mand
 Baseline 1.67 ± 0.70 1.43 ± 0.96

Group: F(1, 28) = 13.84, p = .001, η2 = 0.33

Phase: F(3, 84) = 36.12, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.56

Group × Phase: F(3, 84) = 19.14, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.41

 Intervention 3.90 ± 1.35 2.03 ± 1.30
 Generalization 3.70 ± 1.37 1.60 ± 1.55
 Maintenance 3.53 ± 1.34 1.50 ± 1.16
Tact
 Baseline 4.07 ± 1.81 2.97 ± 2.13

Group: F(1, 28) = 9.62, p = .004, η2 = 0.26

Phase: F(3, 84) = 30.63, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.52

Group × Phase: F(3, 84) = 15.56, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.36

 Intervention 6.10 ± 2.11 3.63 ± 2.18
 Generalization 5.80 ± 2.19 3.10 ± 2.14
 Maintenance 5.90 ± 2.08 3.00 ± 1.98
Echoic
 Baseline 5.27 ± 2.13 4.40 ± 2.35

Group: F(1, 28) = 8.36, p = .007, η2 = 0.23

Phase: F(3,84) = 59.40, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.68

Group × Phase: F (3, 84) = 44.34, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.61

 Intervention 7.77 ± 2.31 5.40 ± 2.80
 Generalization 7.77 ± 2.19 3.93 ± 2.48
 Maintenance 7.47 ± 2.18 4.47 ± 2.86
Listener responding
 Baseline 3.10 ± 1.56 2.37 ± 1.80

Group: F(1, 28) = 9.78, p = .004, η2 = 0.56

Phase: F(3,84) = 24.03, p < .001, η2 = 0.46

Group × Phase: F(3,84) = 7.99, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.22

 Intervention 5.43 ± 2.00 3.57 ± 2.02
 Generalization 5.43 ± 2.02 2.70 ± 1.76
 Maintenance 5.27 ± 1.96 2.83 ± 1.67

Language functions were measured using the Chinese version of the VB-MAPP. The intervention and generalization assessments occurred in Week 10. The maintenance assessment occurred in Week 13

PRT Pivotal response training

In contrast, although the control group showed improvements in tact, echoic, and listener responding in the intervention phase compared to the baseline phase (all ps < 0.05), no improvement was found in mand. A significant decrease was found in all four language measures in the maintenance phase compared to the intervention phase (all ps < 0.001; see Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 1).

When comparing the groups in each phase, the PRT group demonstrated similar language scores to the control group at the baseline phase (ps > 0.05) but scored significantly higher with large effect sizes in the intervention, generalization, and maintenance phases (all ps > 0.05, η2 > 0.19; see Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 2).

Effects of PRT on Other Cognitive and Behavioral Measurements

A series of repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted to assess the effects of PRT on other aspects of cognition and behaviors (see Fig. 3 and Table 5). The PRT group showed greater reduction in social and communicative difficulties, evidenced by a greater decrease in SCQ scores between the intervention and baseline phases compared to the control group (i.e., significant Group × Phase interaction; for results of the ANOVAs, see Table 5; for results of the simple effects analyses, see Supplementary Table 3 & 4). In addition, the PRT intervention also improved cognition, fine motor skills, gross motor skills, imitation, and adaptive behaviors significantly more than the usual treatment, evidenced by greater increases in all the CPEP-3 measures between the invention and baseline phases compared to the control group (i.e., significant Group × Phase; see Table 5 and Supplementary Table 3 & 4).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Effects of PRT and Regular Training on Cognition and Behaviors in Autistic Children. Note. Autistic children showed consistent improvements in a social and communicative skills, b cognition, c fine motor, d gross motor, e imitation, and f adaptive behaviors, after the PRT versus regular intervention. The horizontal solid brackets indicate the significant results between different phases in the PRT group. The horizontal dotted brackets indicate the significant results between different phases in the control group. The vertical solid brackets indicate the significant findings between the groups in each phase. Social and communicative difficulties were measured using the Chinese version of the SCQ. Cognition, fine motor skills, gross motor skills, imitation, and adaptive behaviors were measured using the CPEP-3. The intervention assessment occurred in Week 10. PRT Pivotal Response Training

Table 5.

Measures of cognition and behaviors in the PRT and control group in baseline and intervention phases

PRT Control Statistics
M ± SD
Social & communicative difficulties
 Baseline 27.93 ± 3.95 29.13 ± 3.04 Group: F(1, 28) = 2.85, p = .10, η2 = 0.92
 Intervention 23.13 ± 4.51 26.47 ± 3.23 Phase: F(1, 28) = 245.27, p < .001, η2 = 0.90
Group × Phase: F(1, 28) = 20.02, p < .001, η2 = 0.42
Cognition
 Baseline 35.60 ± 14.57 29.47 ± 14.48 Group: F(1, 28) = 4.26, p = .048, η2 = 0.13
 Intervention 46.73 ± 12.33 32.00 ± 14.86 Phase: F(1, 28) = 49.19, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.64
Group × Phase: F(1, 28) = 19.48, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.41
Fine motor skills
 Baseline 30.20 ± 8.60 31.87 ± 4.07 Group: F(1, 28) = 0.001, p = .97, η2 = 0.00
 Intervention 35.47 ± 4.82 33.67 ± 3.54 Phase: F(1, 28) = 31.46, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.53
Group × Phase: F(1, 28) = 7.58, p = .01, η2 = 0.21
Gross motor skills
 Baseline 24.33 ± 4.89 25.87 ± 4.50 Group: F(1, 28) = 0.6, p = .81, η2 = 0.002
 Intervention 28.00 ± 2.62 27.13 ± 3.29 Phase: F(1, 28) = 29.33, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.51
Group × Phase: F(1, 28) = 6.94, p = .01, η2 = 0.20
Imitation
 Baseline 13.00 ± 4.58 13.27 ± 3.71 Group: F(1, 28) = 0.81, p = .38, η2 = 0.03
 Intervention 16.67 ± 3.37 14.00 ± 3.59 Phase: F(1, 28) = 24.45, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.47
Group × Phase: F(1, 28) = 10.87, p = .003, η2 = 0.28
Adaptive behaviors
 Baseline 39.47 ± 14.06 30.47 ± 13.87 Group: F(1, 28) = 5.62, p = .03, η2 = 0.17
 Intervention 54.93 ± 14.89 39.53 ± 15.24 Phase: F(1, 28) = 90.89, p =  < .001, η2 = 0.76
Group × Phase: F(1, 28) = 6.19, p = .02, η2 = 0.42

The intervention assessment occurred in Week 10. Social and communicative difficulties were measured using the Chinese version of the SCQ. Cognition, fine motor skills, gross motor skills, imitation, and adaptive behaviors were measured using the CPEP-3

PRT Pivotal response training

Discussion

The current study investigated the effects of Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) on language functions in autistic children according to Skinner’s (1957) theory of verbal behavior. Following an 8-week intervention of a PRT motivation module conducted in autism special schools, autistic children demonstrated greater improvements in their language functions compared to autistic children who underwent treatment as usual (TAU). The improvement in language functions in the PRT group was generalized and maintained at the follow-up assessments. Moreover, the PRT intervention enhanced untargeted social and communicative functioning, cognition, fine motor skills, gross motor skills, imitation, and adaptive behaviors in autistic children. Taken together, the current study supports PRT as an effective intervention for facilitating language functions as well as general cognitive and social development in autistic children.

By incorporating Skinner’s (1957) theory of verbal behavior, the current study provided new evidence supporting the effectiveness of PRT on improving language functions in autistic children. A recent systematic review showed that despite the promising evidence of PRT on facilitating language development in autistic children, the majority of existing studies lacked clear definitions of the forms and functions of the assessed language outcomes, hindering the comparison between research and clinical applications of PRT (Forbes et al., 2020). In the current study, autistic children scored significantly higher on mands (i.e., verbal requesting), tacts (i.e., labeling), echoics (i.e., repeating), and listener responding (i.e., responding to verbal requests) following the PRT intervention, and the improvements in the PRT group was significantly larger than the control group that received TAU. The therapeutic effects of PRT were generalized even when administrator and environment were altered and were maintained at the 3-week follow-up assessment. Given that previous PRT intervention studies on language have primarily focused on requesting and initiating questions (Koegel et al., 1998a, b; Mohammadzaheri et al., 2021; Popovic et al., 2020), the current study provides new evidence that PRT can exert a consistent, generalizable, and stable effect across a range of well-defined language functions in autistic children.

The positive outcomes of PRT on language functions in the current study may have been due to several factors. One reason may be that autistic children were more engaged in the training in the PRT intervention compared to the traditional structured ABA intervention. During the PRT sessions, the stimulus items were chosen based on the children’s interests and the training of desired behaviors was imbedded in the natural interactions between the child and the therapist. Due to the child-led semi-structured nature of the PRT intervention, autistic children may have been more likely to generate and maintain interest in the activities. The motivation to participate in the activities may then subsequently stimulate the use of different functions of language in autistic children (e.g., saying “bubbles” to request the therapist to blow more bubbles and listening to the therapist’s instruction when learning to dance). Indeed, our therapists noticed fewer disruptive and avoidance behaviors in the autistic children during the PRT sessions. Similar findings were also reported in previous research where the PRT intervention improved learning and decreased avoidance and escape behaviors in autistic children (Koegel et al., 1992; Mohammadzaheri et al., 2014, 2015).

The cooperation between therapists and parents is likely to contribute to the effectiveness of PRT on improving language outcomes. In the current study, although the PRT intervention was primarily administered by therapists in schools, parents of the children in the PRT group were trained to practice the PRT techniques at home for 12 weeks. This setting may have assisted in generalizing the PRT-induced language improvements to different natural contexts and in maintaining the achieved language functions after the completion of PRT in school. Consistent with these results, a previous study showed that a 12-week PRT parent intervention improved the frequency of utterances and adaptive communication skills in autistic children (Gengoux et al., 2015). Another study also found that the incorporating teacher- and parent-delivered PRT lead to an increase in socio-communicative skills and a reduction in behavioral and emotional problems (de Korte et al., 2021). These findings converge to suggest that the combination of therapist- and parent-delivered PRT should be considered in order to facilitate language and behavioral improvements in autistic children.

In addition to the targeted language functions, PRT demonstrated generalized effects to untargeted behaviors, including social communication, cognition, motor skills, imitation and adaptive behaviors in the autistic participants. This is consistent with the fundamental idea of PRT that increased motivation would elicit widespread improvements (Koegel & Koegel, 2019; Koegel et al., 2001). Similarly, previous studies found that PRT intervention targeting initiating questions resulted in collateral gains in overall communicative skills (Mohammadzaheri et al., 2021), happiness and interest (Popovic et al., 2020). The current findings extend the existing literature by showing that incorporating the motivation component of PRT into a language intervention not only improves language and emotions but also benefits general cognitive and social development. It is possible that with increased motivation to practice language skills, autistic children create more play and interaction opportunities, leading to the learning of a variety of untargeted functions. Therefore, this study suggests that language functions should be considered an important intervention target in PRT intervention to facilitate general development in autistic children.

While the results of the current study are promising, several limitations should be noted. First, a 3-week follow-up assessment might not be sufficiently long to assess the long-term benefits of PRT. This timing was chosen to align the end of the testing with the end of the school term as school holidays might have introduced confounding factors and resulted in data loss. Previous studies have found that the therapeutic effects of PRT on communication were maintained 3 months following the conclusion of the intervention (Gengoux et al., 2015). To further understand the long-term benefits of PRT, multiple follow-up tests over a period of at least 3 months should be considered in future studies. Second, although the current study demonstrated the benefits of a combined therapist- and parent-delivered PRT intervention, it did not compare the effects of therapist- versus parent-administered PRT. To maximize the benefits of PRT, future investigation into the optimal parameters for intervention, such as frequency, intensity, duration, and administrator, is required. Third, although the current study recruited qualified and experienced therapists and hosted regular meetings to ensure fidelity of the treatment, more rigorous and consistent integrity checks should be conducted across the PRT and TAU groups.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the current study conducted the first experimental examination of the effects of PRT on language functions in autistic children in the context of Skinner’s (1957) theory of verbal behavior. The results indicated that combining PRT motivational components with language intervention is helpful to facilitate the development of different language functions in autistic children. These functions included verbal requesting, labeling, repeating, and responding to the speech of others. The PRT-induced improvements in language functions were maintained when the testing environment changed and when the intervention concluded. In line with PRT theory, the PRT intervention also showed generalized effects on untargeted cognitive and social skills. While further research is needed to investigate the long-term effects of PRT on language development and the impact of the administrator (e.g., therapists versus parents), this naturalistic intervention appears to be a promising option for promoting language development in autistic children.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all children and their families, and teachers for their participation. Part of this study has been previously presented at the 17th Annual Autism Conference in San Diego, California, February 19–21, 2023.

Author contributions

LW and CW developed the research questions. LW was responsible for data collection and analyzed the data with assistance from SL. LW, SL and CW were involved in interpreting the results. SL drafted the manuscript based on information provided by LW. All authors read, edited and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the grants from China Disabled Persons Federation for the project titled as “The research and development of social skills assistive technology for children with autism”, the collaborative project of industry and academia by Ministry of Education titled “The intervention for the social behaviors of ASD children” (220600287161903), Tianjin Social Sciences Planning Project (TJQYWT2004-05) titled "Psychological Adjustment of Children during COVID-19 Pandemic" and the National Social Science Foundation of China for the project titled “Social Mechanism of Vitality of NGOs for the People with Disabilities” (Grant No. 21&ZD182).

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect of the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committees and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This study was reviewed and approved by the ethics committee of Nankai University.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from the caregivers of all individual participants before they joined the current study.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Leilei Wang and Shuting Li have contributed equally to this work and should be considered co-first authors.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5. American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baker-Ericzén MJ, Stahmer AC, Burns A. Child demographics associated with outcomes in a community-based pivotal response training program. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions. 2007;9(1):52–60. doi: 10.1177/10983007070090010601. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chen F, Cheung CCH, Peng G. Linguistic tone and non-linguistic pitch imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders: A cross-linguistic investigation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2022;52(5):2325–2343. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05123-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chen J, Liang C, Wei Z, Cui Z, Kong X, Dong C, Lai Y, Peng Z, Wan G. Atypical longitudinal development of speech-evoked auditory brainstem response in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research. 2019;12(7):1022–1031. doi: 10.1002/aur.2110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. de Korte MWP, van den Berk-Smeekens I, Buitelaar JK, Staal WG, van Dongen-Boomsma M. Pivotal response treatment for school-aged children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2021;51(12):4506–4519. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-04886-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Dunlap G. The influence of task variation and maintenance tasks on the learning and affect of autistic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1984;37(1):41–64. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90057-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. American Guidance Service, Inc; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  8. Dyer K, Dunlap G, Winterling V. Effects of choice making on the serious problem behaviors of students with severe handicaps. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1990;23(4):515–524. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1990.23-515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Feldman EK, Matos R. Training paraprofessionals to facilitate social interactions between children with autism and their typically developing peers. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions. 2013;15(3):169–179. doi: 10.1177/1098300712457421. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Forbes HJ, Travers JC, Vickers Johnson J. A systematic review of linguistic and verbal behavior outcomes of pivotal response treatment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2020;50(3):766–778. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04307-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fossum KL, Williams L, Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Pivotal response treatment for preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder: Defining a predictor profile. Autism Research. 2018;11(1):153–165. doi: 10.1002/aur.1859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Franchini M, Duku E, Armstrong V, Brian J, Bryson SE, Garon N, Roberts W, Roncadin C, Zwaigenbaum L, Smith IM. Variability in verbal and nonverbal communication in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: predictors and outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2018;48(10):3417–3431. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3607-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gau SSF, Lee CM, Lai MC, Chiu YN, Huang YF, Kao JD, Wu YY. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the social communication questionnaire. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011;5(2):809–818. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.09.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gengoux GW, Berquist KL, Salzman E, Schapp S, Phillips JM, Frazier TW, Minjarez MB, Hardan AY. Pivotal response treatment parent training for autism: Findings from a 3-month follow-up evaluation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;45(9):2889–2898. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2452-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Huang WH, Li D. Yu yan xing wei li cheng bei ping gu yu an zhi cheng xu [Tran: The verbal behavior milestones assessment and placement program: The VB-MAPP] Peking University Medical Press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  16. Issarraras A, Matson JL, Matheis M, Burns CO. Differences in developmental concerns of young children with autism spectrum disorder across racial/ethnic groups. Developmental Neurorehabilitation. 2019;22(3):174–179. doi: 10.1080/17518423.2018.1504828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Johnson G, Kohler K, Ross D. Contributions of Skinner’s theory of verbal behaviour to language interventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Early Child Development and Care. 2017;187(3–4):436–446. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1236255. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kim S, Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Training paraprofessionals to target socialization in students with ASD: Fidelity of implementation and social validity. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions. 2017;19(2):102–114. doi: 10.1177/1098300716669813. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Koegel LK, Bryan KM, Su P, Vaidya M, Camarata S. Intervention for non-verbal and minimally verbal individuals with autism: A systematic review. International Journal of Pediatric Research. 2019 doi: 10.23937/2469-5769/1510056. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Koegel LK, Camarata SM, Valdez-Menchaca M, Koegel RL. Setting generalization of question-asking by children with autism. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1998;102(4):346–357. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(1998)102&#x0003c;0346:SGOQBC&#x0003e;2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Carter CM. Pivotal responses and the natural language teaching paradigm. Seminars in Speech and Language. 1998;19(04):355–372. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1064054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Green-Hopkins I, Barnes CC. Brief report: Question-Asking and collateral language acquisition in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40(4):509–515. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0896-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Koegel RL, Bradshaw JL, Ashbaugh K, Koegel LK. Improving question-asking initiations in young children with autism using pivotal response treatment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;44(4):816–827. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1932-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Pivotal response treatments for autism: Communication, social, & academic development. Brookes Publishing Co; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  25. Koegel RL, Koegel LK. The PRT pocket guide: Pivotal response treatment for autism spectrum disorders. Brookes Publishing Co; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  26. Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Pivotal response treatments for autism: Communication, social, and academic development. Brookes Publishing Co; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  27. Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Pivotal response treatment for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) 2. Brookes Publishing Company; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  28. Koegel RL, Koegel LK, McNerney EK. Pivotal areas in intervention for autism. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2001;30(1):19–32. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3001_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Koegel RL, Koegel LK, Surratt A. Language intervention and disruptive behavior in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1992;22(2):141–153. doi: 10.1007/BF01058147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Koegel RL, O’Dell MC, Koegel LK. A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1987;17(2):187–200. doi: 10.1007/BF01495055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kwok EYL, Brown HM, Smyth RE, Oram Cardy J. Meta-analysis of receptive and expressive language skills in autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2015;9:202–222. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.10.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Levinson S, Eisenhower A, Bush HH, Carter AS, Blacher J. Brief report: Predicting social skills from semantic, syntactic, and pragmatic language among young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2020;50(11):4165–4175. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04445-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lovaas OI, Schreibman L, Koegel RL. A behavior modification approach to the treatment of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia. 1974;4(2):111–129. doi: 10.1007/BF02105365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Marini A, Ozbič M, Magni R, Valeri G. Toward a definition of the linguistic profile of children with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Marrus N, Hall LP, Paterson SJ, Elison JT, Wolff JJ, Swanson MR, Parish-Morris J, Eggebrecht AT, Pruett JR, Hazlett HC, Zwaigenbaum L, Dager S, Estes AM, Schultz RT, Botteron KN, Piven J, Constantino JN, Piven J, Hazlett HC, Gu H. Language delay aggregates in toddler siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2018;10(1):1–16. doi: 10.1186/s11689-018-9247-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Moes DR. Integrating choice-making opportunities within teacher-assigned academic tasks to facilitate the performance of children with autism. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps. 1998;23(4):319–328. doi: 10.2511/rpsd.23.4.319. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mohammadzaheri F, Koegel LK, Bakhshi E, Khosrowabadi R, Soleymani Z. The effect of teaching initiations on the communication of children with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2021;52(6):2598–2609. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05153-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mohammadzaheri F, Koegel LK, Rezaee M, Rafiee SM. A randomized clinical trial comparison between pivotal response treatment (PRT) and structured applied behavior analysis (ABA) intervention for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;44(11):2769–2777. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2137-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mohammadzaheri F, Koegel LK, Rezaei M, Bakhshi E. A Randomized clinical trial comparison between pivotal response treatment (PRT) and adult-driven applied behavior analysis (ABA) intervention on disruptive behaviors in public school children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;45(9):2899–2907. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2451-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Padilla KL, Akers JS. Content validity evidence for the verbal behavior milestones assessment and placement program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2021;51(11):4054–4066. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04864-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Popovic SC, Starr EM, Koegel LK. Teaching initiated question asking to children with autism spectrum disorder through a short-term parent-mediated program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2020;50(10):3728–3738. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04426-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ramezani M, Lotfi Y, Moossavi A, Bakhshi E. Auditory brainstem response to speech in children with high functional autism spectrum disorder. Neurological Sciences. 2019;40(1):121–125. doi: 10.1007/s10072-018-3594-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Reindal L, Nærland T, Weidle B, Lydersen S, Andreassen OA, Sund AM. Structural and pragmatic language impairments in children evaluated for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04853-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Richards M, Mossey J, Robins DL. Parents’ concerns as they relate to their child’s development and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2016;37(7):532–540. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C. The social communication questionnaire: Manual. Western Psychological Services; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  46. Sang B, Miao X. The revision of PPVT–R trial norm in Shanghai. Psychological Science. 1990;5:20–25. [Google Scholar]
  47. Schopler E, Lansing MD, Reichler RJ, Marcus LM. Psychoeducational Profile: TEACCH individualized assessment for children with autism spectrum disorders. 3. Austin; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  48. Shek DTL, Yu L. Construct validity of the Chinese version of the psycho-educational profile-3rd edition (CPEP-3) Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;44(11):2832–2843. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2143-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Skinner BF. Verbal behavior. Martino Publishing; 1957. [Google Scholar]
  50. Stahmer, A. C., Suhrheinrich, J., Reed, S., Schreibman, L., & Bolduc, C. (2011). Classroom Pivotal Response Teaching for Children with Autism. Guilford Publications. https://books.google.com.au/books?id=fluK_xLWND0C
  51. Stock R, Mirenda P, Smith IM. Comparison of community-based verbal behavior and pivotal response treatment programs for young children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2013;7(9):1168–1181. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.06.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sundberg ML, Michael J. The benefits of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior for children with autism. Behavior Modification. 2001;25(5):698–724. doi: 10.1177/0145445501255003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Tang X, Hua Z, Xing J, Yi L, Ji Z, Zhao L, Su X, Yin T, Wei R, Li X, Liu J. Verbal fluency as a predictor of autism spectrum disorder diagnosis and co-occurring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Reading and Writing. 2022 doi: 10.1007/s11145-022-10319-w. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tillinghast BS, Morrow JE, Uhlig GE. Retest and alternate-form reliabilities of the ppvt-r with fourth, fifth, and sixth grade pupils. Journal of Educational Research. 1983;76(4):243–244. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1983.10885458. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Verschuur R, Didden R, Lang R, Sigafoos J, Huskens B. Pivotal response treatment for children with autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;1(1):34–61. doi: 10.1007/s40489-013-0008-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wang L, Pfordresher PQ, Jiang C, Liu F. Individuals with autism spectrum disorder are impaired in absolute but not relative pitch and duration matching in speech and song imitation. Autism Research. 2021;14(11):2355–2372. doi: 10.1002/aur.2569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Weismer SE, Kaushanskaya M, Larson C, Mathée J, Bolt D. Executive function skills in school-age children with autism spectrum disorder: Association with language abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2018;61(11):2641–2658. doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-RSAUT-18-0026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Yu L, Zhu X, Shek DTL, Zou XB, Deng HZ, Au Yeung PWH. Validation of the simplified Chinese psychoeducational profile third edition in mainland China. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2019;49(4):1599–1612. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3827-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES