Skip to main content
Bulletin of the World Health Organization logoLink to Bulletin of the World Health Organization
. 2023 Mar 2;101(5):307–316C. doi: 10.2471/BLT.22.288888

Life-course inequalities in intrinsic capacity and healthy ageing, China

Inégalités au cours de la vie en matière de capacités intrinsèques et vieillissement en bonne santé en Chine

Desigualdades vitales en la capacidad intrínseca y el envejecimiento saludable en China

جوانب عدم المساواة في مسار الحياة من حيث القدرة الذاتية والتقدم في العمر بصحة جيدة في الصين

中国生命历程中内在能力与健康老龄化的不平等关系

Неравенство в течение жизни в отношении индивидуальной жизнеспособности и здорового старения, Китай

Yafei Si a, Katja Hanewald a, Shu Chen a, Bingqin Li b, Hazel Bateman a, John R Beard c,
PMCID: PMC10140694  PMID: 37131938

Abstract

Objective

To investigate the contribution of early-life factors on intrinsic capacity of Chinese adults older than 45 years.

Methods

We used data on 21 783 participants from waves 1 (2011) and 2 (2013) of the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), who also participated in the 2014 CHARLS Life History Survey to calculate a previously validated measure of intrinsic capacity. We considered 11 early-life factors and investigated their direct association with participants’ intrinsic capacity later in life, as well as their indirect association through four current socioeconomic factors. We used multivariable linear regression and the decomposition of the concentration index to investigate the contribution of each determinant to intrinsic capacity inequalities.

Findings

Participants with a favourable environment in early life (that is, parental education, childhood health and neighbourhood environment) had a significantly higher intrinsic capacity score in later life. For example, participants with a literate father recorded a 0.040 (95% confidence interval, CI: 0.020 to 0.051) higher intrinsic capacity score than those with an illiterate father. This inequality was greater for cognitive, sensory and psychological capacities than locomotion and vitality. Overall, early-life factors directly explained 13.92% (95% CI: 12.07 to 15.77) of intrinsic capacity inequalities, and a further 28.57% (95% CI: 28.19 to 28.95) of these inequalities through their influence on current socioeconomic inequalities.

Conclusion

Unfavourable early-life factors appear to decrease late-life health status in China, particularly cognitive, sensory and psychological capacities, and these effects are exacerbated by cumulative socioeconomic inequalities over a person’s life course.

Introduction

A growing number of researchers consider traditional disease-based conceptualizations of health or health metrics based on single domains to be inadequate proxies for the health of older adults.1 Recognizing the importance of healthy ageing, the World Health Organization (WHO) used a capabilities approach to frame healthy ageing in the broadest possible sense in its 2015 World report on ageing and health.2 In the framework presented in the report, healthy ageing is experienced when the functional ability to be and do the things a person values is developed and maintained across a person’s life course. Functional ability has many dimensions, including being able to meet one’s daily basic needs; to learn, grow and make critical decisions; to be actively mobile; to build and maintain meaningful relationships; and to contribute to society.3 This ability is determined by a person’s intrinsic capacity, the environment they live in, and the interaction between the individual and their environment.1,2

Intrinsic capacity comprises all of the individual-level capacities that a person can draw on. Developing and maintaining these capacities across a person’s life course is critical to healthy ageing.4 Building on gerontological and disability theory,5 intrinsic capacity reflects the biological capacities of an individual rather than its impairments or deficits. This strengths-based approach is distinct from, but complementary to, concepts such as frailty and resilience.6 Clinicians and researchers have increasingly adopted intrinsic capacity to measure healthy ageing,7,8 and the construct has been successfully validated and empirically examined in longitudinal analyses of English9 and Chinese cohorts.10

Intrinsic capacity differs in several ways from other ageing concepts. First, intrinsic capacity potentially provides a continuous, fine-grained and comprehensive measure of overall health that can be assessed across much of the life course, rather than focusing on late-life impairments or impairment of specific body functions.11 Second, it adopts a functioning-oriented approach rather than a traditional disease-centred approach to framing health states.12 Third, apart from other comprehensive measures of overall health,13,14 it aims to distinguish individual attributes of functioning from the influence of the environment a person lives in. These characteristics facilitate cross-time, cross-country and cross-culture comparisons of healthy ageing and enable the investigation of the contextual attributes that may either affect intrinsic capacity or, in conjunction with intrinsic capacity, contribute to an individual’s functional ability. Researchers have suggested that regular monitoring of intrinsic capacity could provide an early warning for reduced functioning or adverse outcomes, and inform preventive interventions.11,15

In functional phenotype research, researchers are increasingly recognizing life-course analyses, which conceptualize an accumulation of risk as the incremental burden of different exposures with age, highlighting the importance of timing and cumulative exposure.16 These exposures may play a role in determining health through overt physiological change (e.g. weight loss due to famine) or through internal changes (e.g. regulation of the growth hormone insulin in relation to dietary exposure). Intrinsic capacity is likely to be shaped by many life-course factors, including underlying age-related biological changes, health-related behaviours and the presence of disease.3 Intrinsic capacity can also be influenced by socioeconomic and physical determinants that occur at different life stages. Moreover, disadvantages in nutrition, health, wealth, education, employment and income that arise early in life can reinforce each other and accumulate over the life course.17 The large heterogeneity in intrinsic capacity observed in older adults is thus likely to strongly reflect the accumulated impact of these determinants,18,19 inextricably linking poorer healthy ageing trajectories to early-life inequalities. However, the cumulative contribution of health and socioeconomic inequalities over time to intrinsic capacity levels in late life has not been well studied. To fill the gap, this study aims to longitudinally examine the relationships between life-course factors and intrinsic capacity in a representative sample of China’s older adult population.

Methods

Data and study participants

We used publicly available de-identified data from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) to measure the intrinsic capacity construct and to examine life-course inequalities within it.20 The study aims to collect high-quality data from a nationally representative sample of older Chinese adults aged 45 years or older.21 The first CHARLS cohort was recruited in 2011, with follow-up surveys conducted every two years. The 2011 nationally representative baseline sampling frame included 150 counties or districts and 450 villages or urban communities across China. All living respondents from the first two waves (2011 and 2013) were invited to participate in the 2014 CHARLS Life History Survey, which included a series of questions about health and health-care history, residential history, education history and important childhood events. We included all individuals who participated in the 2011 and 2013 waves and the 2014 CHARLS Life History Survey.

Intrinsic capacity

We have previously validated and described prognostic value of intrinsic capacity in this cohort.10 In summary, we calculated the intrinsic capacity score using structural equation models and confirmatory factor analysis of all objective characteristics that might be associated with the intrinsic capacity construct, including forced expiratory volume; chair-stand test; grip strength; balance; walking speed; haemoglobin; episodic memory test; affective, hearing and vision impairments; intact mental status; and sleep quantity and quality. The final construct includes an overarching domain, that is, intrinsic capacity, and five subdomains: locomotion; cognitive capacity; vitality; sensory capacity; and psychological capacity, with a mean standardized score of 0 and a standard deviation of 1. Higher scores indicate a higher capacity to do the things that individuals have reason to value.

Life-course factors

We included 11 early-life factors that happened in critical periods of individuals’ lives (generally before the age of 17 years) based on the existing literature:2224 (i) parental education; (ii) self-reported health status during childhood; (iii) access to health-care services; (iv) healthy behaviour; (v) nutritional status; (vi) family economic status during childhood; (vii) neighbourhood quality; (viii) friendships; (ix) parental mental health; (x) domestic violence; and (xi) receiving mentorship and support. Early-life factors can directly affect late-life intrinsic capacity, indirectly influence intrinsic capacity via their influence on the subsequent accumulation of socioeconomic inequalities, or both. We used four measures to account for current socioeconomic factors: (i) current family economic status; (ii) educational achievement; (iii) urban residence; and (iv) urban hukou registration. Hukou is a registration in the Chinese household registration system. More details on the definitions and measurements of these factors are available in the online repository.25

Statistical analysis

First, we examined the unadjusted association of intrinsic capacity with each life-course factor of interest in a bivariate analysis.26 We then examined the correlation between early-life factors and current socioeconomic status using a Spearman correlation matrix, since early-life factors may affect late-life intrinsic capacity via accumulative health and socioeconomic inequalities.

Second, we used multivariable linear regressions to examine how life-course factors affect late-life intrinsic capacity, adjusting for current socioeconomic, demographic and lifestyle factors (online repository).25 We expressed associations as marginal effects measured at the mean of each covariate. As biases may arise in the estimated relationship between intrinsic capacity and factors of interest when the outcome variable and error term are correlated, our primary econometric strategy was to exploit the comprehensive information in the survey data to directly account for as many potential confounding factors as possible.27

Third, we used the concentration index to quantify inequalities in late-life intrinsic capacity.28 As a rank-dependent method, the concentration index was calculated based on family economic status.29,30 Moreover, we decomposed the concentration index of intrinsic capacity to quantify each determinant’s specific contribution and summarized them by categories (further details available in the online repository).25 Additionally, by excluding current socioeconomic factors from our models, we performed mediation analysis to identify the cumulative contribution of early-life factors to intrinsic capacity inequalities through health and socioeconomic inequalities over a person’s life course.

Finally, we performed sensitivity analyses to ensure the reliability of our findings (online repository).25 First, since a moderate proportion of participants was excluded, we used an incidental truncation model by introducing the inverse Mills ratio to adjust for potential selection bias.31 Second, we used multiple imputations with chained equations to impute missing values,32 assuming observations were missing at random. We combined estimates across 20 imputed data sets based on Rubin’s rules.33 Third, to ensure the relationships were consistent in both older and younger participants, we used a chronologically pre-old group (younger than 60 years) and an older group (aged 60 years or older) to perform heterogeneity analysis. Fourth, to assess whether recall bias among participants affected our results, we performed heterogeneity analysis by splitting the sample into a low-cognition and a high-cognition subgroup. Fifth, we excluded duplicated participants by prioritizing data in 2013, and used family income per capita rather than family consumption. Sixth, we replicated our analysis using the latest wave of CHARLS in 2018. We did not include this wave in our main analysis since, by design, several key intrinsic capacity measures were not collected, and participants added in the 2015 and 2018 waves did not complete the 2014 CHARLS Life History Survey. Seventh, we examined the validity of early-life factors using data from the 2011 CHARLS community questionnaire, which was completed by a respondent from a village committee office or a community committee.34

We reported two-sided P-values (unadjusted for multiple comparisons) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). We considered a P-value < 0.05 to be statistically significant, and we used the Holm-Bonferroni method to adjust for multiple comparisons. All analyses were performed in Stata version 16.0 (Stata Corp LLP, College Station, United States of America).

Ethical considerations

The Biomedical Ethics Review Committee of Peking University approved the CHARLS study (IRB00001052–11015), and all the data is publicly available. Furthermore, the current study received approval from the University of New South Wales (UNSW) Ethics Committee (HC210472).

Results

After excluding 117 individuals who did not answer the intrinsic capacity questions, and 7651 and 941 who had missing information on life history or socioeconomic factors, respectively, we included 21 783 survey participants. The mean age of the participants was 59.07 years (standard deviation, SD: 9.16) and 10 600 participants (48.66%) were male. The sample included 9997 participants in the older age group (range: 60–109 years; mean: 67.15; SD: 6.01) and 11 786 participants in the younger age group (range:19–59 years; mean: 52.22; SD: 4.71). Table 1 shows the characteristics of the included participants. The 8709 excluded participants were more likely to have a lower family economic status, a lower education level and not having urban hukou. Also, the excluded participants were more likely to be older, single, female, more likely to consume tobacco and alcohol, and more likely to have chronic diseases (online repository).25

Table 1. Characteristics of participants included in the study on intrinsic capacity and healthy ageing, China, 2011–2013.

Characteristic No. (%) (n = 21 783)
Gendera
Female 11 183 (51.3)
Male 10 600 (48.7)
Age group, years
< 45 385 (1.8)
45–60 11 401 (52.3)
60–70 6 951 (31.9)
70–80 2 674 (12.3)
≥ 80 372 (1.7)
Marital status
Married 18 310 (84.1)
Not married 3 473 (15.9)
Residence
Rural 15 455 (70.9)
Urban 6 328 (29.1)
Education level
Illiterate 5 385 (24.7)
Primary 8 911 (40.9)
Middle school 4 773 (21.9)
High school 2 586 (11.9)
College and above 128 (0.6)
Urban hukoub
Agriculture 17 194 (78.9)
Non-agriculture 4 589 (21.1)
Family economic status, wealth quintile
1: poorest 4 387 (20.1)
2: poor 4 539 (20.8)
3: average 4 410 (20.2)
4: richer 4 309 (19.8)
5: richest 4 138 (19.0)
Tobacco consumption
No 13 875 (63.7)
Yes 7 908 (36.3)
Alcohol consumption
No 14 105 (64.8)
Yes 7 678 (35.2)
Presence of chronic diseases
No 7 285 (33.4)
Yes 14 498 (66.6)

a The interviewer recorded respondent’s gender as either female or male.

b Hukou is an individual registered in the Chinese household registration system.

Note: Inconsistencies arise in some values due to rounding.

We first plotted Kernel density distributions to explore the association between intrinsic capacity and early-life factors (Fig. 1). The plots showed that participants with lower parental education and worse neighbourhood environment had lower intrinsic capacity scores than more advantaged participants. Other early-life factors and the subdomains of intrinsic capacity showed similar patterns (P-value: < 0.001; online repository).25 The multivariate analysis using all early-life factors also supported these results (online repository).25 Intrinsic capacity was also significantly associated with current socioeconomic, demographic and lifestyle factors (P-value: < 0.001; online repository).25 The Spearman correlation matrix showed reasonably moderate and strong correlations for factors across life-course stages (Fig. 2).

Fig. 1.

Distribution of intrinsic capacity and the association of life-course factors, China, 2011–2013

Notes: We included 21 783 participants from the CHARLS study, waves 1 and 2. The plot shows Kernel density distribution of intrinsic capacity. Neighbourhood quality was assessed by four questions asking participants whether the neighbourhood where they lived as a child was safe, willing to help, close-knit, and clean and attractive. Each measure was dichotomised as low (not very or not at all) and high (somewhat or very) leading to a sum measure of adversity ranging from 0 (lowest) to 4 (highest).

Fig. 1

Fig. 2.

Pairwise Spearman correlation matrix between life-course factors, China, 2011–2013

Notes: We included 21 783 participants from the CHARLS study, waves 1 and 2. Variables in bold denote positive measures with higher socioeconomic position or childhood advantages. Hukou is an individual registered in the Chinese household registration system. Definitions of the variables are available in the online repository.25

Fig. 2

We further investigated the association between intrinsic capacity and life-course factors. In the regression analysis, early-life factors were significantly associated with intrinsic capacity later in life, even after eliminating the effect of current socioeconomic, demographic and lifestyle factors (Fig. 3). For example, people with a literate father recorded a 0.040 (95% CI: 0.020 to 0.051) higher intrinsic capacity score than those with an illiterate father (Table 2; available at: https://www.who.int/publications/journals/bulletin/). People living in neighbourhoods with worse environment and who were lacking friends during childhood recorded significantly lower intrinsic capacity scores. Moreover, higher current socioeconomic positions were significantly associated with higher intrinsic capacity scores for participants with a higher family economic status, living in urban areas, having an urban hukou or having attained higher education attainments (Table 2).

Fig. 3.

Linking early-life factors with late-life intrinsic, cognitive and sensory capacities, China, 2011–2013

CI: confidence interval.

Notes: We included 21 783 participants from the CHARLS study, waves 1 and 2. Multivariate linear regressions were performed by adjusting for current socioeconomic factors, lifestyle factors and demographic factors. Intrinsic capacity represents the overall level of health and capacities to be and do things the participant has reasons to value cognitive capacity includes memory, intelligence and problem-solving; sensory capacity includes vision and hearing; psychological capacity involves mood and emotional resilience. Table 2 presents all the marginal effects and their associated 95% CIs.

Fig. 3

Table 2. Association between life-course factors and intrinsic capacity and its subdomains, China, 2011–2013.

Variable Marginal effect (95% CI)
Intrinsic capacity Locomotion Cognitive Vitality Sensory Psychological
Gendera
Female Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Male 0.236 (0.222 to 0.250) 0.031 (−0.022 to 0.083) 0.119 (0.095 to 0.144) 0.424 (0.407 to 0.440) 0.079 (0.063 to 0.096) 0.427 (0.367 to 0.487)
Age group, years
< 45 Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
45–60 −0.124 (−0.159 to −0.088) −0.089 (−0.148 to −0.029) −0.095 (−0.157 to −0.033) −0.049 (−0.084 to −0.014) −0.187 (−0.233 to −0.140) −0.109 (−0.249 to 0.032)
60–70 −0.222 (−0.259 to −0.185) −0.216 (−0.284 to −0.148) −0.142 (−0.206 to −0.078) −0.190 (−0.227 to −0.154) −0.255 (−0.303 to −0.207) −0.105 (−0.251 to 0.040)
70–80 −0.346 (−0.386 to −0.307) −0.513 (−0.606 to −0.419) −0.253 (−0.321 to −0.185) −0.347 (−0.385 to −0.308) −0.303 (−0.353 to −0.252) −0.126 (−0.281 to 0.029)
≥ 80 −0.525 (−0.585 to −0.466) −1.411 (−1.714 to −1.108) −0.424 (−0.516 to −0.332) −0.458 (−0.520 to −0.396) −0.351 (−0.418 to −0.285) −0.236 (−0.460 to −0.012)
Marital status
Married Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Single −0.056 (−0.070 to −0.042) −0.045 (−0.095 to 0.005) −0.073 (−0.097 to −0.048) −0.044 (−0.058 to −0.030) −0.003 (−0.019 to 0.013) −0.165 (−0.223 to −0.108)
Education level
No education Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Primary school 0.193 (0.178 to 0.208) 0.151 (0.101 to 0.201) 0.563 (0.537 to 0.590) 0.025 (0.011 to 0.040) −0.004 (−0.020 to 0.013) 0.025 (−0.036 to 0.085)
Middle school 0.302 (0.285 to 0.320) 0.168 (0.111 to 0.226) 0.815 (0.784 to 0.846) 0.072 (0.053 to 0.091) 0.027 (0.006 to 0.047) 0.030 (−0.044 to 0.103)
High school 0.361 (0.339 to 0.382) 0.182 (0.113 to 0.252) 0.897 (0.860 to 0.933) 0.086 (0.060 to 0.111) 0.065 (0.039 to 0.092) 0.119 (0.028 to 0.209)
College and above 0.412 (0.353 to 0.472) 0.183 (−0.028 to 0.394) 0.952 (0.874 to 1.030) 0.038 (−0.036 to 0.111) 0.097 (0.011 to 0.182) 0.242 (0.006 to 0.478)
Residence
Rural Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Urban 0.048 (0.034 to 0.062) 0.001 (−0.047 to 0.049) 0.093 (0.069 to 0.117) 0.019 (0.002 to 0.036) 0.027 (0.010 to 0.045) −0.062 (−0.119 to −0.004)
Urban hukoub
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes 0.062 (0.046 to 0.079) 0.006 (−0.048 to 0.061) 0.114 (0.087 to 0.142) 0.012 (−0.009 to 0.032) 0.032 (0.012 to 0.052) 0.048 (−0.020 to 0.116)
Family economic status
Poorest Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Poorer 0.032 (0.017 to 0.047) 0.060 (0.006 to 0.113) 0.055 (0.028 to 0.081) 0.032 (0.016 to 0.049) 0.013 (−0.005 to 0.030) −0.035 (−0.095 to 0.025)
Average 0.044 (0.029 to 0.059) 0.033 (−0.021 to 0.086) 0.064 (0.037 to 0.091) 0.037 (0.020 to 0.053) 0.032 (0.014 to 0.050) −0.006 (−0.067 to 0.055)
Richer 0.048 (0.032 to 0.063) 0.007 (−0.049 to 0.063) 0.087 (0.059 to 0.114) 0.037 (0.020 to 0.054) 0.032 (0.014 to 0.050) −0.017 (−0.080 to 0.045)
Richest 0.066 (0.049 to 0.082) −0.003 (−0.065 to 0.058) 0.124 (0.095 to 0.152) 0.037 (0.019 to 0.055) 0.053 (0.034 to 0.073) −0.030 (−0.095 to 0.036)
Literacy
Mother illiterate Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Mother literate 0.027 (0.009 to 0.044) 0.040 (−0.005 to 0.084) 0.007 (−0.021 to 0.035) 0.020 (−0.002 to 0.042) 0.040 (0.019 to 0.061) −0.010 (−0.083 to 0.063)
Father illiterate Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Father literate 0.040 (0.029 to 0.051) 0.035 (0.001 to 0.069) 0.077 (0.057 to 0.096) 0.024 (0.011 to 0.036) 0.012 (−0.001 to 0.025) 0.043 (−0.003 to 0.089)
Childhood health
General
High Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Average −0.043 (−0.054 to −0.032) −0.003 (−0.038 to 0.032) −0.026 (−0.045 to −0.007) −0.031 (−0.044 to −0.019) −0.061 (−0.074 to −0.047) −0.059 (−0.106 to −0.012)
Low −0.082 (−0.099 to −0.065) −0.032 (−0.090 to 0.027) −0.046 (−0.076 to −0.016) −0.074 (−0.093 to −0.055) −0.077 (−0.098 to −0.057) −0.218 (−0.291 to −0.145)
Severely ill before the age of 16 years
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes −0.022 (−0.046 to 0.001) −0.031 (−0.106 to 0.043) −0.021 (−0.062 to 0.019) −0.006 (−0.030 to 0.018) −0.008 (−0.033 to 0.017) −0.061 (−0.158 to 0.035)
Severely ill after the age of 16 years
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes −0.064 (−0.079 to −0.050) −0.201 (−0.258 to −0.144) −0.034 (−0.059 to −0.010) −0.046 (−0.062 to −0.030) −0.035 (−0.052 to −0.019) −0.199 (−0.258 to −0.139)
Access to health-care services
Vaccination deficiencyc
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes −0.027 (−0.042 to −0.012) 0.014 (−0.037 to 0.065) −0.063 (−0.090 to −0.035) −0.019 (−0.035 to −0.004) −0.006 (−0.023 to 0.011) −0.008 (−0.072 to 0.057)
Physician deficiencyd
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes −0.030 (−0.048 to −0.012) 0.026 (−0.033 to 0.084) −0.026 (−0.057 to 0.005) −0.005 (−0.024 to 0.014) −0.038 (−0.058 to −0.018) −0.069 (−0.143 to 0.004)
Healthy behaviour
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes 0.027 (0.016 to 0.039) 0.018 (−0.018 to 0.053) 0.064 (0.044 to 0.084) 0.010 (−0.003 to 0.023) 0.006 (−0.008 to 0.019) 0.041 (−0.005 to 0.088)
Starvation
None Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Low −0.006 (−0.020 to 0.007) 0.031 (−0.014 to 0.076) 0.016 (−0.008 to 0.040) 0.000 (−0.016 to 0.016) −0.021 (−0.038 to −0.005) −0.088 (−0.145 to −0.030)
Moderate 0.007 (−0.009 to 0.024) 0.038 (−0.015 to 0.091) 0.049 (0.019 to 0.078) 0.009 (−0.010 to 0.028) −0.024 (−0.044 to −0.004) −0.042 (−0.111 to 0.028)
Severe −0.033 (−0.050 to −0.016) 0.019 (−0.037 to 0.076) −0.030 (−0.059 to −0.000) −0.010 (−0.028 to 0.008) −0.035 (−0.054 to −0.015) −0.153 (−0.224 to −0.082)
Famine
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes 0.020 (0.006 to 0.035) 0.036 (−0.014 to 0.087) 0.047 (0.022 to 0.072) 0.031 (0.016 to 0.047) −0.012 (−0.030 to 0.006) 0.006 (−0.055 to 0.068)
Famine death in family
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes −0.008 (−0.025 to 0.009) 0.047 (−0.006 to 0.100) −0.008 (−0.038 to 0.021) 0.013 (−0.005 to 0.032) −0.009 (−0.028 to 0.010) −0.094 (−0.169 to −0.020)
Economic status in childhood
High Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Average 0.010 (−0.001 to 0.022) 0.012 (−0.025 to 0.049) 0.006 (−0.014 to 0.026) 0.005 (−0.008 to 0.018) 0.008 (−0.005 to 0.021) 0.033 (−0.015 to 0.081)
Low 0.007 (−0.012 to 0.025) 0.003 (−0.057 to 0.064) −0.019 (−0.051 to 0.014) −0.017 (−0.039 to 0.005) 0.024 (0.001 to 0.047) 0.075 (−0.005 to 0.155)
Neighbourhood quality
Highest Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
High −0.012 (−0.023 to −0.000) −0.020 (−0.056 to 0.017) −0.013 (−0.032 to 0.007) 0.003 (−0.010 to 0.016) −0.018 (−0.031 to −0.004) −0.026 (−0.074 to 0.021)
Poor −0.039 (−0.059 to −0.020) 0.004 (−0.059 to 0.067) −0.075 (−0.111 to −0.038) −0.021 (−0.043 to −0.000) −0.020 (−0.043 to 0.003) −0.024 (−0.108 to 0.060)
Poorest −0.056 (−0.092 to −0.019) −0.111 (−0.255 to 0.032) −0.056 (−0.115 to 0.002) −0.048 (−0.082 to −0.015) −0.027 (−0.065 to 0.011) −0.052 (−0.181 to 0.077)
Friendship deficiency
Low Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Lower −0.045 (−0.057 to −0.033) −0.057 (−0.096 to −0.017) −0.061 (−0.082 to −0.040) −0.024 (−0.038 to −0.011) −0.027 (−0.040 to −0.013) −0.049 (−0.099 to 0.001)
Higher −0.073 (−0.094 to −0.053) −0.016 (−0.087 to 0.056) −0.118 (−0.156 to −0.081) −0.027 (−0.050 to −0.003) −0.037 (−0.061 to −0.013) −0.151 (−0.240 to −0.062)
Highest −0.109 (−0.159 to −0.059) −0.031 (−0.186 to 0.123) −0.105 (−0.190 to −0.019) −0.028 (−0.072 to 0.017) −0.064 (−0.121 to −0.007) −0.321 (−0.511 to −0.131)
Parental mental health
Mother
Good Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Poor −0.004 (−0.020 to 0.011) 0.021 (−0.024 to 0.067) −0.003 (−0.030 to 0.023) 0.018 (0.001 to 0.034) 0.001 (−0.017 to 0.019) −0.107 (−0.173 to −0.041)
Father
Good Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Poor −0.038 (−0.055 to −0.021) −0.026 (−0.079 to 0.026) −0.045 (−0.075 to −0.016) −0.023 (−0.041 to −0.005) −0.020 (−0.040 to −0.000) −0.062 (−0.133 to 0.009)
Domestic violence
Low Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Lower 0.003 (−0.011 to 0.017) −0.007 (−0.052 to 0.038) 0.005 (−0.018 to 0.029) 0.014 (−0.001 to 0.029) −0.007 (−0.024 to 0.009) −0.050 (−0.109 to 0.008)
Higher 0.001 (−0.016 to 0.018) 0.012 (−0.038 to 0.061) 0.018 (−0.010 to 0.047) 0.018 (−0.003 to 0.040) −0.019 (−0.038 to 0.000) −0.109 (−0.179 to −0.039)
Highest −0.009 (−0.045 to 0.026) 0.016 (−0.091 to 0.123) −0.029 (−0.086 to 0.028) 0.041 (0.009 to 0.074) 0.003 (−0.039 to 0.045) −0.080 (−0.229 to 0.068)
Mentoring and support
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes 0.019 (0.007 to 0.031) 0.038 (0.004 to 0.073) 0.040 (0.019 to 0.060) 0.012 (−0.002 to 0.026) 0.011 (−0.003 to 0.026) −0.014 (−0.064 to 0.035)
Tobacco consumption
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes −0.027 (−0.040 to −0.014) 0.017 (−0.029 to 0.063) −0.040 (−0.062 to −0.018) −0.009 (−0.024 to 0.007) −0.016 (−0.032 to −0.001) −0.053 (−0.107 to 0.001)
Alcohol consumption
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes 0.025 (0.014 to 0.036) 0.053 (0.016 to 0.090) 0.006 (−0.013 to 0.026) 0.046 (0.033 to 0.059) 0.024 (0.010 to 0.038) −0.006 (−0.054 to 0.041)
Chronic disease
No Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference
Yes −0.085 (−0.096 to −0.074) −0.097 (−0.127 to −0.066) −0.022 (−0.041 to −0.003) −0.034 (−0.046 to −0.022) −0.108 (−0.121 to −0.094) −0.326 (−0.371 to −0.281)

a The interviewer recorded respondent’s gender as either female or male.

b Hukou is an individual registered in the Chinese household registration system.

c Vaccination deficiency was assessed by the question: before or when you were 15 years old, did you receive any vaccinations?

d Physician deficiency was assessed by the question: have you always had a usual source of care, that is, a particular person or a place that you went to when you were sick or needed advice about your health?

Note: Further information on the measurement of variables is available in the online repository.25

The strongest associations of childhood disadvantages were with lower cognitive, sensory and psychological capacities, although we also identified negative effects for locomotion and vitality (Table 2, Fig. 3 and Fig. 4). Most estimated marginal effects for cognitive, sensory and psychological capacities were statistically significant. For example, persons with a literate father recorded a 0.077 (95% CI: 0.057 to 0.096) higher cognitive capacity score than those with an illiterate father (Table 2 and Fig. 3). However, the estimated marginal effects for vitality were small, if statistically significant, and most estimated marginal effects for locomotion were not statistically significant.

Fig. 4.

Linking early-life factors with late-life locomotion, vitality and psychological capacity, China, 2011–2013

CI: confidence interval.

Notes: We included 21 783 participants from the CHARLS study, waves 1 and 2. Multivariate linear regressions were performed by adjusting for current socioeconomic factors, lifestyle factors and demographic factors. Locomotion includes balance, muscle strength and gait; vitality is defined as underlying a person’s resilience to challenges, vigour and stamina, to maintain homeostasis in the face of daily stressors; psychological capacity involves mood and emotional resilience. Table 2 presents all the marginal effects and their associated 95% CIs.

Fig. 4

We quantified the extent of intrinsic capacity inequalities using the concentration index method. The concentration index was 0.024 (95% CI: 0.021 to 0.027) for intrinsic capacity (Table 3). The positive value indicates that the richer wealth groups had higher intrinsic capacity scores. Cognitive and sensory capacities had the highest concentration indices, indicating more inequalities in these capacities.

Table 3. Extent of inequalities in intrinsic capacity, China, 2011–2013 .

Capacity Modified concentration index (95% CI)
Intrinsic 0.024 (0.021 to 0.027)
Locomotion 0.002 (−0.002 to 0.006)
Cognitive 0.043 (0.036 to 0.050)
Vitality 0.012 (0.011 to 0.013)
Sensory 0.032 (0.025 to 0.039)
Psychological 0.005 (−0.006 to 0.016)

CI: confidence interval.

Notes: We included 21 783 participants from the CHARLS study, waves 1 and 2. The concentration index ranges from −1 to 1, with positive values indicating that people with a higher intrinsic capacity are more concentrated among high-income groups and negative values indicating that people with a higher intrinsic capacity are more concentrated among low-income groups.

We decomposed the concentration index into the contribution of each life-course factor to identify the comparative importance of these factors. Overall, early-life factors directly accounted for 13.92% (95% CI: 13.57 to 14.27) of intrinsic capacity inequalities (Fig. 5). The contribution of each early-life factor was similar across subdomains, with psychological capacity as the only exception (online repository).25 The decomposition for locomotion and psychological capacity was less precise, since the two subdomains only had small absolute values of the concentration index. In our mediation analysis, the marginal effect of early-life factors on intrinsic capacity generally became larger (online repository),25 leading early-life factors to account for 28.57% (95% CI: 28.19 to 28.95) of intrinsic capacity inequalities (Fig. 5). The details about each determinant’s contribution are reported in the online repository.25

Fig. 5.

Decomposing the life-course inequalities in intrinsic capacity, China, 2011–2013

Notes: We included 21 783 participants from the CHARLS study, waves 1 and 2. The percentages show the contribution of each category factor to the overall intrinsic capacity inequalities by decomposing the modified concentration index. For bars showing direct contributions, all life course factors were included, and thus the graph shows the direct contribution of early-life factors to inequalities in intrinsic capacity. For bars showing cumulative contributions, current socioeconomic position was excluded from the model of mediation analysis, and thus the graph shows the cumulative contribution of early-life factors to inequalities in intrinsic capacity. Current socioeconomic position excludes the family economic status factor. Theoretically, the contribution of observable determinants to inequalities in intrinsic capacity can be more than 100% since the contribution of the error term is not included and can be negative. Further details are available in the online repository.25

Fig. 5

We performed six theory-based sensitivity analyses. None of these substantially changed our results, suggesting that our results are robust and reliable (online repository).25

Discussion

Using a nationally representative sample of the Chinese population, we find that unfavourable early-life factors were strongly linked to lower late-life intrinsic capacity scores. Approximately 14% of late-life intrinsic capacity inequalities could be explained by the direct effect of early-life factors, and a further 29% of intrinsic capacity inequalities through early-life factors’ influence on current socioeconomic inequalities. Unfavourable early-life factors were more likely to be associated with lower cognitive, sensory and psychological capacities and vitality than inequalities in locomotion.

Previous research has linked early-life factors to late-life health outcomes, including frailty, depression and biological ageing.24,35,36 However, this study represents a comprehensive life-course inequalities analysis of intrinsic capacity, a continuous and holistic construct of human health that is being increasingly applied in research and clinical practice.

The association of early-life factors with cognitive capacity are particularly interesting. There is growing evidence that, at least in western Europe, cognitive capacity in older adults is higher than in previous generations of the same age,37 although the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. Early brain development,38 such as child brain structural alterations of the cerebral cortex39 and the volume of the hippocampus,40 can often determine cognitive capacity in older age. The associations we found between many early-life factors (i.e. parental education and childhood health) and cognitive inequalities in late life can support this cohort difference.

The association of events early in life with vitality is consistent, with the suggestion that ambient stress caused by negative exposures results in biological ageing throughout a person’s life course.41 We have previously hypothesized that vitality is a marker of underlying biological changes associated with ageing and of an individual’s physiologic resilience.10

The main early-life factors associated with poor sensory capacity in late life concerned starvation and illness which may physiologically have affected the development of hearing and vision.

Our measure of psychological capacity was dominated by a depression score,42 which itself is heavily influenced by current life events and genetic predisposition.43 However, psychological capacity was partly dominated by early-life factors, although the marginal effects were seldom significant, suggesting an influence of childhood development on the capacity to manage stressors.

The smaller negative association of early-life adversities with locomotion are somewhat surprising. Locomotion is influenced by many factors, having modest associations with childhood socioeconomic position.44 Moreover, the dysregulation derived from stressors in childhood could be masked by the growth of organs since locomotion generally covers skeletal muscle physiology, cardiovascular system, energy homeostasis, osteoarticular and neurophysiology.45 While diminished locomotor capacity was also associated with adverse early exposures, the impact was less than for cognitive capacity.

Overall, our findings are consistent with extensive research across countries pointing to the important influence of socioeconomic determinants across the life course on different health inequalities.46 However, the outcomes considered by these studies have often been limited to mortality or specific conditions.46 Where functioning has been considered more broadly as an ageing-related outcome, it is often in relation to severe losses of functioning identified by a diagnosis of frailty or the inability to undertake basic activities without assistance.24,47 Most of these analyses did not study the contribution of life-course inequalities to health. In contrast, the intrinsic capacity construct allows us to explore the impact of life-course factors on the overall functioning of older adults, and to understand the different domains of functioning that are most affected.24,35,48

Decomposing intrinsic capacity inequalities into life-course factors may help identify key opportunities for proactive interventions to preserve intrinsic capacity in older age. Our findings emphasize the importance of equitable early-life opportunities on people’s subsequent capacity to make choices, contribute to society and receive support when needed.3 Almost all the determinants of health disparities can be reshaped by public policies across an individual’s life course. Our findings emphasize the importance of early-life events, which are largely experienced without choice soon after birth. This fact is critical for the individual and the society, since outcome inequalities are considered unfair if they are rooted in factors that are beyond individual control.49

Our analysis has many strengths. We used a representative sample of the world’s largest ageing population, the wealth of outcome data enabled us to explore health inequalities through a broad continuous measure of functioning, and the associated life events survey allowed us to distinguish between the impact of early- and late-life events. Moreover, the approach to measuring intrinsic capacity is consistent with a similar analysis of a large English cohort, suggesting the approach is robust across different cultural settings.9 However, the study has several limitations. First, we used individual recall data for early-life factors with potential recall bias, although our sensitivity analysis suggests that cognitive decline does not seem to significantly affect our findings. Second, this study could not distinguish whether intrinsic capacity variations were caused by higher or lower peaks of intrinsic capacity or the rate of declining speeds of such capacity. Accordingly, we will further examine the longitudinal nature of intrinsic capacity when new data becomes available. Third, potential cohort effects may limit the generalization of our findings, and this effect should be further explored in an analysis regarding the trajectories of intrinsic capacity. Fourth, genetic analysis was not included in the study; instead, we included a comprehensive set of variables to try to counteract individual heterogeneity. Finally, the study design precludes the interpretation of our results as causal relationships.

Despite these limitations, the results highlight the important and cumulative impact early-life advantages and disadvantages have on people’s experience of health in older age. Our findings also provide support for the Chinese government’s latest initiatives, such as common prosperity and rural revitalization, designed to reduce rural–urban disparities and wealth inequalities.

Acknowledgements

We thank the CHARLS research and field team. YS, KH, SC, and HB are also affiliated with ARC Centre of Excellence in Population Ageing Research, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia.

Funding:

This study was funded by the ARC Centre of Excellence in Population Ageing Research (project CE170100005) and the University of New South Wales.

Competing interests:

None declared.

References

  • 1.Beard JR, Officer A, de Carvalho IA, Sadana R, Pot AM, Michel JP, et al. The world report on ageing and health: a policy framework for healthy ageing. Lancet. 2016. May 21;387(10033):2145–54. 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00516-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.World report on ageing and health. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2015. Available from: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/186463 [cited 2021 Nov 1].
  • 3.Decade of healthy ageing: baseline report. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2020. Available from: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240017900 [cited 2021 Nov1].
  • 4.Belloni G, Cesari M. Frailty and intrinsic capacity: two distinct but related constructs. Front Med (Lausanne). 2019. Jun 18;6:133. 10.3389/fmed.2019.00133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cesari M, Araujo de Carvalho I, Amuthavalli Thiyagarajan J, Cooper C, Martin FC, Reginster JY, et al. Evidence for the domains supporting the construct of intrinsic capacity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2018. Nov 10;73(12):1653–60. 10.1093/gerona/gly011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Morley JE, Vellas B, van Kan GA, Anker SD, Bauer JM, Bernabei R, et al. Frailty consensus: a call to action. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2013. Jun;14(6):392–7. 10.1016/j.jamda.2013.03.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Charles A, Buckinx F, Locquet M, Reginster JY, Petermans J, Gruslin B, et al. Prediction of adverse outcomes in nursing home residents according to intrinsic capacity proposed by the World Health Organization. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2020. Jul 13;75(8):1594–9. 10.1093/gerona/glz218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Huang CH, Umegaki H, Makino T, Uemura K, Hayashi T, Kitada T, et al. Effect of various exercises on intrinsic capacity in older adults with subjective cognitive concerns. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2021. Apr;22(4):780–6.e2. 10.1016/j.jamda.2020.06.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Beard JR, Jotheeswaran AT, Cesari M, Araujo de Carvalho I. The structure and predictive value of intrinsic capacity in a longitudinal study of ageing. BMJ Open. 2019. Nov 2;9(11):e026119. 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-026119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Beard JR, Si Y, Liu Z, Chenoweth L, Hanewald K. Intrinsic capacity: validation of a new WHO concept for healthy ageing in a longitudinal Chinese study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2022. Jan 7;77(1):94–100. 10.1093/gerona/glab226 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stolz E, Mayerl H, Freidl W, Roller-Wirnsberger R, Gill TM. Intrinsic capacity predicts negative health outcomes in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2022. Jan 7;77(1):101–5. 10.1093/gerona/glab279 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cesari M, Marzetti E, Thiem U, Pérez-Zepeda MU, Abellan Van Kan G, Landi F, et al. The geriatric management of frailty as paradigm of “The end of the disease era”. Eur J Intern Med. 2016. Jun;31:11–4. 10.1016/j.ejim.2016.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wu C, Newman AB, Dong BR, Odden MC. Index of healthy ageing in Chinese older adults: China health and retirement longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018. Jul;66(7):1303–10. 10.1111/jgs.15390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Xue DD, Cheng Y, Wu M, Zhang Y. Comprehensive geriatric assessment prediction of postoperative complications in gastrointestinal cancer patients: a meta-analysis. Clin Interv Aging. 2018. Apr 24;13:723–36. 10.2147/CIA.S155409 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Prince MJ, Acosta D, Guerra M, Huang Y, Jacob KS, Jimenez-Velazquez IZ, et al. Intrinsic capacity and its associations with incident dependence and mortality in 10/66 Dementia Research Group studies in Latin America, India, and China: a population-based cohort study. PLoS Med. 2021. Sep 14;18(9):e1003097. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ben-Shlomo Y, Cooper R, Kuh D. The last two decades of life course epidemiology, and its relevance for research on ageing. Int J Epidemiol. 2016. Aug;45(4):973–88. 10.1093/ije/dyw096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sarfati H. OECD. Preventing ageing unequally. Paris, Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2017. 253 pp. ISBN 978-92-64-27907-0. Int Soc Secur Rev. 2018;71(3):125–7. 10.1111/issr.12183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Carrieri V, Davillas A, Jones AM. A latent class approach to inequity in health using biomarker data. Health Econ. 2020. Jul;29(7):808–26. 10.1002/hec.4022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Li H, Hanewald K, Wu S. Healthy life expectancy in China: modelling and implications for public and private insurance. Annals of Actuarial Science. 2021;15(1):40–56. 10.1017/S1748499520000135 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). Beijing: Peking University; 2021. Available from: http://charls.pku.edu.cn/en [cited 2023 Jan 17].
  • 21.Zhao Y, Hu Y, Smith JP, Strauss J, Yang G. Cohort profile: the China health and retirement longitudinal study (CHARLS). Int J Epidemiol. 2014. Feb;43(1):61–8. 10.1093/ije/dys203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hu H, Si Y, Li B. Decomposing inequality in long-term care need among older adults with chronic diseases in China: a life course perspective. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020. Apr 8;17(7):2559. 10.3390/ijerph17072559 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kuruvilla S, Sadana R, Montesinos EV, Beard J, Vasdeki JF, Araujo de Carvalho I, et al. A life-course approach to health: synergy with sustainable development goals. Bull World Health Organ. 2018. Jan 1;96(1):42–50. 10.2471/BLT.17.198358 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Li Y, Xue QL, Odden MC, Chen X, Wu C. Linking early life risk factors to frailty in old age: evidence from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study. Age and Ageing. 2020. Feb 27;49(2):208–17. 10.1093/ageing/afz160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Si Y, Hanewald K, Chen S, Li B, Bateman H, Beard JR. Life-course inequalities in intrinsic capacity and healthy ageing: retrospective evidence from pre-old and older populations in China [online repository]. Online: Zenodo; 2023. 10.5281/zenodo.7588511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 26.Szklo M, Nieto FJ. Epidemiology: beyond the basics. 3rd ed. Burlington: Jones & Bartlett Publishers; 2014. [cited 2021 Nov 1]. Available from: https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/resource/pt/int-4623 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Violato M, Petrou S, Gray R, Redshaw M. Family income and child cognitive and behavioural development in the United Kingdom: does money matter? Health Econ. 2011. Oct;20(10):1201–25. 10.1002/hec.1665 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.O’Donnell O, Van Doorslaer E, Wagstaff A, Lindelow M. Analyzing health equity using household survey data: a guide to techniques and their implementation. Washington, DC: The World Bank; 2007. 10.1596/978-0-8213-6933-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Erreygers G, Van Ourti T. Putting the cart before the horse. A comment on Wagstaff on inequality measurement in the presence of binary variables. Health Econ. 2011. Oct;20(10):1161–5. 10.1002/hec.1754 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Erreygers G, Van Ourti T. Measuring socioeconomic inequality in health, health care and health financing by means of rank-dependent indices: a recipe for good practice. J Health Econ. 2011. Jul;30(4):685–94. 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2011.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wooldridge JM. Introductory econometrics: A modern approach. Boston: Cengage learning; 2015. Available from: https://www.cengage.com/c/isbn/9781337558860/ [cited 2021 Nov 1].
  • 32.White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med. 2011. Feb 20;30(4):377–99. 10.1002/sim.4067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. Volume 81. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons; 2004. 10.1002/9780470316696 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Havari E, Mazzonna F. Can we trust older people’s statements on their childhood circumstances? Evidence from SHARELIFE. Eur J Popul. 2015;31(3):233–57. 10.1007/s10680-014-9332-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Chen Y, Guo M, Xu H, Liu Z, Barry LC, Wu C. Association between early life circumstances and depressive symptoms among Chinese older adults: results from China health and retirement longitudinal study. J Affect Disord. 2021. Sep 1;292:345–51. 10.1016/j.jad.2021.05.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cao X, Ma C, Zheng Z, He L, Hao M, Chen X, et al. Contribution of life course circumstances to the acceleration of phenotypic and functional ageing: a retrospective study. EClinicalMedicine. 2022. Jul 10;51:101548. 10.1016/j.eclinm.2022.101548 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Grasshoff J, Beller J, Kuhlmann BG, Geyer S. Increasingly capable at the ripe old age? Cognitive abilities from 2004 to 2013 in Germany, Spain, and Sweden. PLoS One. 2021. Jul 1;16(7):e0254038. 10.1371/journal.pone.0254038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Melrose RJ, Brewster P, Marquine MJ, MacKay-Brandt A, Reed B, Farias ST, et al. Early life development in a multiethnic sample and the relation to late life cognition. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015. Jul;70(4):519–31. 10.1093/geronb/gbt126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Guxens M, Lubczyńska MJ, Muetzel RL, Dalmau-Bueno A, Jaddoe VWV, Hoek G, et al. Air pollution exposure during fetal life, brain morphology, and cognitive function in school-age children. Biol Psychiatry. 2018. Aug 15;84(4):295–303. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Jackowski A, Perera TD, Abdallah CG, Garrido G, Tang CY, Martinez J, et al. Early-life stress, corpus callosum development, hippocampal volumetrics, and anxious behavior in male nonhuman primates. Psychiatry Res. 2011. Apr 30;192(1):37–44. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.11.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Geronimus AT. Deep integration: letting the epigenome out of the bottle without losing sight of the structural origins of population health. Am J Public Health. 2013. Oct;103(Suppl 1) S1:S56–63. 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301380 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Carleton RN, Thibodeau MA, Teale MJ, Welch PG, Abrams MP, Robinson T, et al. The center for epidemiologic studies depression scale: a review with a theoretical and empirical examination of item content and factor structure. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e58067. 10.1371/journal.pone.0058067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Musliner KL, Seifuddin F, Judy JA, Pirooznia M, Goes FS, Zandi PP. Polygenic risk, stressful life events and depressive symptoms in older adults: a polygenic score analysis. Psychol Med. 2015. Jun;45(8):1709–20. 10.1017/S0033291714002839 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Birnie K, Cooper R, Martin RM, Kuh D, Sayer AA, Alvarado BE, et al. ; HALCyon study team. Childhood socioeconomic position and objectively measured physical capability levels in adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2011. Jan 26;6(1):e15564. 10.1371/journal.pone.0015564 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kemoun P, Ader I, Planat-Benard V, Dray C, Fazilleau N, Monsarrat P, et al. A gerophysiology perspective on healthy ageing. Ageing Res Rev. 2022. Jan;73:101537. 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wagg E, Blyth FM, Cumming RG, Khalatbari-Soltani S. Socioeconomic position and healthy ageing: a systematic review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2021. Aug;69:101365. 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wang Y, Yang W, Avendano M. Income-Related inequalities in informal care: evidence from the Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey in China. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2021. Sep 13;76(8):1691–6. 10.1093/geronb/gbab043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Xiong P, Liang X, Chen H, Chen L, Zuo L, Jing C, et al. Association between childhood neighborhood quality and the risk of cognitive dysfunction in Chinese middle-aged and elderly population: the moderation effect of body mass index. Front Aging Neurosci. 2021. May 13;13:645189. 10.3389/fnagi.2021.645189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hufe P, Kanbur R, Peichl A. Measuring unfair inequality: reconciling equality of opportunity and freedom from poverty. Rev Econ Stud. 2022;89(6):3345–80. 10.1093/restud/rdab101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Bulletin of the World Health Organization are provided here courtesy of World Health Organization

RESOURCES