Skip to main content
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. 2023 Apr 24;17:1172845. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1172845

Plasticity in parental behavior and vasopressin: responses to co-parenting, pup age, and an acute stressor are experience-dependent

Lisa C Hiura 1, Vanessa A Lazaro 1, Alexander G Ophir 1,*
PMCID: PMC10164929  PMID: 37168139

Abstract

Introduction

The impact of variation in parental caregiving has lasting implications for the development of offspring. However, the ways in which parents impact each other in the context of caregiving is comparatively less understood, but can account for much of the variation observed in the postnatal environment. Prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) demonstrate a range of postnatal social groups, including pups raised by biparental pairs and by their mothers alone. In addition to the challenges of providing parental care, prairie vole parents often experience acute natural stressors (e.g., predation, foraging demands, and thermoregulation) that could alter the way co-parents interact.

Methods

We investigated how variation in the experience of raising offspring impacts parental behavior and neurobiology by administering an acute handling stressor on prairie vole families of single mothers and biparental parents over the course of offspring postnatal development.

Results

Mothers and fathers exhibited robust behavioral plasticity in response to the age of their pups, but in sex-dependent ways. Pup-directed care from mothers did not vary as a function of their partner’s presence, but did covary with the number of hypothalamic vasopressin neurons in experience-dependent ways. The relationship between vasopressin neuron numbers and fathers’ behaviors was also contingent upon the stress handling manipulation, suggesting that brain-behavior associations exhibit stress-induced plasticity.

Conclusion

These results demonstrate that the behavioral and neuroendocrine profiles of adults are sensitive to distinct and interacting experiences as a parent, and extend our knowledge of the neural mechanisms that may facilitate parental behavioral plasticity.

Keywords: acute stress, maternal care, Microtus, offspring age-dependent parental care, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN), paternal care, prairie vole, supraoptic nucleus of the hypothalamus (SON)

1. Introduction

Parents adapt their behaviors in response to environmental contexts, and one of the most influential contexts that a parent will experience is the dynamic social environment of familial interactions (Royle et al., 2014). All mammalian mothers interact with their offspring to varying degrees over the course of offspring development, but for the 5−10% of mammalian species that are biparental (Kleiman and Malcolm, 1981), partner interactions make up a substantial portion of the parents’ social environment. Several species (primarily studied in birds) have been found to flexibly respond to the actions or presence of a parenting partner (Harrison et al., 2009). Adapting parental behavior to meet the demands of a variable social environment can increase reproductive fitness, but social environmental variation can have long term consequences on the developing offspring and the parental investment they receive. Moreover, the dynamic responses of parents to the social environment have the potential to exhaust parental effort and impact parental brain function and behavioral phenotype. Yet, the ways in which caregiving and the maternal (and paternal) brain adapts to having a co-parent remain underexplored in mammals.

Prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) are small rodents that form socially monogamous pair bonds and exhibit biparental care. With the exception of nursing, fathers exhibit all of the same caregiving behaviors as mothers (Thomas and Birney, 1979). Nevertheless, paternal care is not necessary for pup survival in prairie voles, and in the wild, biparental pairs are observed at about the same frequency as single mothers (Getz et al., 1993). Moreover, several studies have removed fathers from the natal nest to assess the consequences of variation in the early social environment on offspring development. These studies have determined that the presence of a father during the rearing period has significant impacts on pup development, including rates of physical maturation (Wang and Novak, 1992), alloparental care and pair bonding as adults (Ahern and Young, 2009), parental behavior toward their own offspring (Ahern et al., 2011), and neuropeptide receptor binding densities and mRNA expression (Ahern and Young, 2009; Prounis et al., 2015; Bales and Saltzman, 2016). Interestingly, far fewer studies have investigated the role of father-presence on mammalian mothers. This is likely due to the comparatively fewer numbers of biparental species suitable for these inquiries. One such study in prairie voles found that single mothers show more passive stress-coping and anxiety-like behaviors alongside greater mRNA expression of hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone compared to mothers who remained paired with their mate (Bosch et al., 2018). This finding reveals that removal of the parenting partner can have substantial behavioral and neuroendocrinological consequences for prairie vole mothers during the perinatal period.

Parental behaviors in male and female monogamous rodents are in part regulated by the neuromodulatory hormone vasopressin (VP; Horrell et al., 2018). Vasopressin is largely produced in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and the supraoptic nucleus (SON) of the hypothalamus, and these primary cell groups provide the bulk of VP peptide signals in the central and peripheral nervous systems (Brownstein et al., 1980). Interestingly, VP mRNA levels in the PVN and the SON are greater in prairie vole parents compared to sexually naïve controls (Wang et al., 2000). Moreover, when examining several brain regions implicated in the modulation of social behavior, prairie vole mothers and fathers show higher levels of oxytocin-cFos and vasopressin-cFos colocalization in the presence of pups compared to when they are separated from their litter or exposed to an object control, demonstrating that these cell populations are transcriptionally responsive to pup stimuli in both sexes (Kirkpatrick et al., 1994; Kenkel et al., 2012; Kelly et al., 2017). Critically, VP is not only involved in parental behavior; a variety of stressors induce the secretion of VP from the PVN, which functions as a key regulator of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis responsivity (Herman and Tasker, 2016). For example, an acute swim stressor increased VP release within the PVN and SON of adult male rats (Wotjak et al., 1998) and heightened pup-directed care in adult virgin male prairie voles (Bales et al., 2006). In addition, rats bred for high-anxiety traits had increased expression of VP mRNA in hypothalamic nuclei, and antagonism of PVN VP receptors reduced anxiety-like behaviors in such rats (Wigger et al., 2004). Together these findings suggest a functional role for VP in mediating endocrine and behavioral stress-responses, which may convolute our understanding of VP’s role in the simultaneous modulation of parental behaviors (Saltzman et al., 2017).

To better dissect the potential intersections between parental experience-dependent plasticity, acute stress, and the involvement of VP, we investigated how environmental variation during parenting impacts the behaviors of prairie vole mothers and fathers across the pre-wean stage of offspring development. Specifically, we implemented a social context manipulation (father removal) and an acute stress induction paradigm (experimenter handling) to produce diversity in parental rearing experiences. We chose experimenter handling because brief scruffing in rodents produces stress-induced catalepsy (Amir et al., 1981) and transient stress-related increases in heart rate and body temperature (Cinelli et al., 2007). Routine laboratory handling has also been found to induce elevations in corticosterone and prolactin, both of which are strongly implicated in physiological stress responses (Balcombe et al., 2004). Importantly for the present study, prairie voles that were directly handled by an experimenter showed elevated pup-directed care behaviors when compared to prairie voles that were simply transferred in cups (Tyler et al., 2005). Here, we leverage this handling-stress paradigm in conjunction with our manipulation of the presence or absence of fathers to diversify parental care experiences. We measured parents’ home cage behaviors and parent performance in an open field test, then subsequently immunolabeled VP-ir neurons in the PVN and SON to characterize the relationship(s) between biobehavioral plasticity and experiences as a parent.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Experimental animals

All animals used in these experiments were sexually mature, virgin F1 progeny of wild-caught breeders and were reared in the laboratory at Cornell University. All animals had ad libitum access to water and food (Rodent Chow 5001, LabDiet, St. Louis, MO, USA) and were housed in polycarbonate rodent cages (29 × 18 × 13 cm) lined with Sani-chip bedding under a 14:10 light-dark cycle (light on at 8:00 AM, off at 10:00 PM). Ambient temperature was maintained at 20°C +2. Sex was assessed and assigned at weaning based on differences in external genitalia. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Cornell University (2013-0102) and were consistent with the guidelines set forth by ARRIVE and the National Institutes of Health.

2.2. Family conditions

We created two experimentally designed factors to manipulate the animals: Fathers Present or Absent, and Experimentally Handled or Non-handled. Twenty days after breeding pairs were formed (age range from P60-P182 at pairing), males in families assigned to the Father-Absent condition were permanently removed from the home cage. Males from families assigned to the Father-Present condition were briefly removed, then returned to the home cage to control for nest disturbances. Nests were monitored daily for the birth of pups. At birth, litters were culled to three pups to control for the effect of litter size. Male offspring were preferentially spared for use in a separate related study. Weekly cage changes were conducted on postnatal days (PND) 2, 9, and 16 for all animals in this study. Cage changes and observations were performed between 10:30 AM-4:30 PM during the light phase. Voles follow ultradian cycles of activity with periods of 1−4 h rather than nocturnal or diurnal patterns commonly observed in other laboratory rodents (Gerkema and van der Leest, 1992; Lewis and Curtis, 2016). In the Handled condition, parents were scruffed and transferred by a gloved hand to a clean cage. In the Non-handled condition, families were gently scooped into a plastic beaker and transferred to a clean cage. When pups were not attached to their mothers, they were transferred in the same manner as their parent(s). We acknowledge that cage changes alone might have served as a mild stressor, and we therefore ensured that all subjects experienced the same number of cage changes on the same schedule. The only difference that remained is the mode of transfer to a new cage (by hand or by cup). As mentioned above, previous work has reported that increases in heart rate, body temperature, “stress” hormone levels, and catalepsy and other anxiety-like behaviors result from such a handling manipulation (Amir et al., 1981; Balcombe et al., 2004; Tyler et al., 2005; Bales et al., 2007; Cinelli et al., 2007; Hurst and West, 2010). Although any handling manipulation could potentially function as a stressor, prior work found that anxiety-like behaviors are immediately elevated in mice that are handled/restrained by hand compared to mice that are moved in a container or cupped hand (Hurst and West, 2010). Taken together, these studies provide sufficient justification to infer that a mild stress response is elicited in subjects from the handling manipulation itself. Thus, we anticipated that conducting cage changes by hand would serve as an acute stressor relative to any mild stress induced by cage changes via cup. Altogether we created four groups: Father Present/Non-handled (N = 11), Father Absent/Non-handled (N = 9), Father Present/Handled (N = 14), Father Absent/Handled (N = 8); see Figure 1. All pups were weaned at PND21.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Schematic representation of the four experimental conditions. Mothers reared offspring in either the presence (top row) or in the absence (bottom row) of a male co-parent. During weekly cage changes, families were transferred to a clean cage either using a cup (left column) or by gently scruffing by gloved hand (right column).

2.3. Home cage analyses

One-hour home cage videos were recorded on PND2, PND9, and PND16 immediately following each cage change. We selected these postnatal days for observation because they map onto behaviorally relevant developmental timepoints (Hiura et al., 2018; Kelly et al., 2018). Briefly, PND2 pups are relatively immobile with closed eyes and are entirely dependent on parental care for food and warmth. At PND9, pups open their eyes and become more physically and socially exploratory. By PND16, pups are capable of consuming solid foods and are able to survive independently. GoPro HERO3 video cameras (GoPro Inc, San Mateo, CA, USA) filmed overhead views of each family. Technical issues with the cameras resulted in several shortened videos, and thus the first 50 min of each video was used for subsequent behavioral analyses. Raters blind to the experimental conditions scored the behaviors of parents using Observer XT v14 (Noldus Information Technology, Leesburg, VA, USA).

2.4. Open-field test

The day after pups were weaned, parents were run through a 10 min open-field test (OFT). Tests were conducted between 10:30 AM-4:30 PM and recorded overhead on video cameras (Sony HDR-CX330 camcorder, Sony, New York City, NY, USA). For pairs in the Father-Present condition, parents were tested simultaneously in separate arenas. Tests were conducted under standard fluorescent overhead lights with a paper filter to evenly diffuse light across the arena. At the beginning of each test, the subject parent was gently placed into the center of the transparent Plexiglas arena (57 cm × 57 cm) by cup. The arena floor consisted of a 4 × 4 grid of squares (14.25 cm × 14.25 cm), and the centermost four squares were designated as the arena “center” region. Video recordings were scored using EthoVision XT v13 (Noldus Information Technology, Leesburg, VA, USA) for number of visits to the center and the total distance moved in the arena.

2.5. Histology and immunocytochemistry

Immediately following the OFT, parents were rapidly anesthetized by isoflurane and perfused with 0.1M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS, pH = 7.4) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. Brains were post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (24 h) then 30% sucrose (48 h) and stored at −80°C. Brains were coronally cryosectioned into three series of 40 μm slices and stored at −80°C in cryoprotectant. A single series of free-floating sections from each subject was fluorescently labeled for VP immunoreactivity (-ir). Sections were rinsed twice in PBS (30 m) and blocked (1 h, PBS + 10% normal donkey serum + 0.03% Triton-X-100) before being incubated in primary antibodies (48 h, Guinea Pig anti-VP 1:1000, Peninsula Laboratories, San Carlos, CA, USA). Sections were rinsed in PBS (2 × 30 m), incubated in biotinylated donkey anti-Guinea pig (1 h, 1:8000, Jackson Immunoresearch, West Grove, PA), rinsed in PBS (2 × 15 m), incubated at room temp in secondary antibodies (2 h, streptavidin conjugated to Alexa Fluor 488 3:1000, ThermoFisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA), and washed in PBS (overnight at 4°C). Sections were mounted onto microscope slides and cover-slipped with Prolong Gold antifade + DAPI nuclear stain (ThermoFisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA).

2.6. Microscopy and quantification

Photomicrographs of the PVN and SON were taken at 10x on a Zeiss AxioImager II scope with an AxioCam MRm attachment, z-drive, and Apotome optical dissector (Carl Zeiss Inc., Gottingen, Germany). Two sections (coronal separation of 240 μm) were monochromatically imaged and manually counted for VP-ir cells using GNU Image Manipulation Program (GIMP, 2.8.22) and ImageJ (National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA). Cell counts of each region were combined across rostral and caudal sections of each subject and total counts per region were statistically analyzed.

2.7. Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using R software v.4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2013). Duration data from home cage recordings were assessed using linear mixed models (LMM) via the ‘lme4’ package (Bates et al., 2015), and p-values were derived from Type-II Wald χ2 tests (two-tailed) using the “Anova” function of the “car” package (Fox and Weisberg, 2019). Count data from home cage recordings and the OFT, and duration data from the OFT were analyzed using generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) assuming a negative binomial distribution or Gamma distribution with the glmmTMB package (Brooks et al., 2017), followed by Type-II Wald χ2 tests for significant factors (two-tailed). Models comparing behaviors of mothers included Father-Present/Absent condition, Experimental Handling/Non-handling condition, and postnatal day as fixed factors, and animal ID as a random factor. Models comparing behaviors between mothers and fathers included Experimental Handling/Non-handling condition, parent, and postnatal day as fixed factors, and animal ID as a random factor. Models comparing behaviors between fathers included Experimental Handling/Non-handling condition and postnatal day as fixed factors, and animal ID as a random factor. Pup grooming and pup retrievals were included as covariates in separate GLMMs with negative binomial distributions to predict cell counts, and fixed effects included handling condition (for maternal and paternal data) and father condition (for maternal data). Regression diagnostic plots and tests for over/underdispersion, heteroscedasticity, and zero-inflation were used to assess model fits, and model residuals were checked using the “DHARMa” package (Hartig, 2019). Tukey-corrected post-hoc contrasts and corresponding p-values of factors for both LMMs and GLMMs were extracted from the ‘emmeans’ package (Lenth et al., 2020). For all statistical models, random effects were excluded in instances where models failed to converge with their inclusion. A 0.05 α-level cutoff was used to determine statistical significance. In cases where model interactions were significant, we report the highest order interaction and related post-hoc contrast(s) in lieu of lower order effects when they involve the same factors. Three-way interactions were omitted from all models as model overcomplexity yielded convergence difficulties and risked overfitting the data.

3. Results

3.1. Total parental care pups receive from parents differs by pup age and the presence of a father

We compared the behaviors of parents immediately following the cage changes to capture any acute stressor-induced variation in parental care. We first compared pup grooming and pup retrievals because these behaviors are exhibited by both mothers and fathers. We found that the total pup grooming a litter received from parents was dependent upon an interaction between father presence and pup age (χ2 = 6.48, df = 2, p = 0.04). Pups with fathers present received more grooming than pups without fathers at PND2 [t(97.4) = 4.55, p < 0.0001] and PND9 [t(97.4) = 2.85, p = 0.005], but not PND16 [t(97.4) = 1.54, p = 0.13, Figure 2A].

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Behavioral diversity in prairie vole mothers and fathers as a function of variation in parenting experiences. (A) Total time (in minutes, m) parents groomed pups as a function of father-presence/absence. (B) Total time (m) mothers vs. fathers spent pup grooming at each of the three developmental timepoints (PND2/PND9/PND16). Gray lines correspond to individual parenting pairs. (C) Counts of pup retrievals exhibited by Handled vs. Non-handled parents, averaging over pup age and parent sex. (D) Total maternal and paternal pup retrievals over pup postnatal days. Gray lines correspond to individual mothers across timepoints. (E) Total duration of nursing (m) by mothers over pup postnatal days. Gray lines correspond to individual mothers across timepoints. (F) Duration (m) of off-nest activity (cage exploration + trail-building) by mothers vs. fathers across pup postnatal days. Gray lines correspond to individual parents across timepoints. (G) Correlations between maternal and paternal pup grooming across postnatal days for Non-handled (top) and Handled (bottom) parents in biparental families. Yellow: PND2, Purple: PND9, Green: PND16. (H) Correlations between maternal and paternal pup retrievals across postnatal days for Non-handled (top) and Handled (bottom) parents in biparental families. *p ≤ 0.05 for all figures. Box and whisker plots and individual points visualize data distributions.

For biparental families, pup grooming differed between parents (χ2 = 18.5, df = 1, p = 1.7e-07), where mothers groomed pups significantly more than fathers did [t(117) = 4.2, p < 0.001, Figure 2B]. Further exploratory analysis revealed that this parental difference was significant at PND2 [Mother > Father t(117) = 2.5, p = 0.01] and PND9 [t(117) = 2.98, p = 0.004], but was no longer significantly different by PND16 [t(117) = 1.86, p = 0.07]. Although the number of pup retrievals did not vary by father presence (χ2 = 1.82, df = 1, p = 0.18), it did differ by handling condition (χ2 = 6.2, df = 1, p = 0.01) and pup age (χ2 = 41.3, df = 2, p = 1.1e-9). Specifically, Handled parents retrieved pups more frequently than Non-handled parents [t(111) = −2.46, p = 0.02, Figure 2C]. Total pup retrievals increased from PND2 to PND9 [t(111) = 4.63, p < 0.0001] and from PND2 to PND16 [t(111) = 6.15, p < 0.0001], but not from PND9 to PND16 [t(111) = 1.55, p = 0.37]. When analyzing pup retrievals in biparental families, there was a main effect of postnatal day (χ2 = 32.6, df = 2, p = 8.3e-08) and parent (χ2 = 4.66, df = 1, p = 0.03), and a non-significant trending interaction between postnatal day and parent (χ2 = 5.22, df = 2, p = 0.07). Post-hoc tests revealed that as pups grew older, fathers generally continued to increase their pup retrievals [PND2 < PND9:t(135) = 3.6, p = 0.001; PND2 < PND16: t(135) = 5.4, p < 0.0001; PND9 < PND16: t(135) = 2.2, p = 0.07]. Mothers, however, significantly differed in their pup retrievals between the ages of PND2 and PND16 [t(135) = 3.1, p < 0.008, Figure 2D], but not between PND2 and PND9 [t(135) = 3.3, p = 0.06] or between PND9 and PND16 [t(135) = 0.8, p = 0.72].

3.2. Pup age and experiment handling, but not paternal presence, alters behavior in mothers

When comparing mothers, the total duration of nursing did not differ by paternal presence (χ2 = 0.47, df = 1, p = 0.49) or by handling condition (χ2 = 0.06, df = 1, p = 0.81). There was a significant main effect of postnatal day (χ2 = 83.2, df = 2, p < 2e-16), where PND16 pups received less nursing than they did at either PND2 [t(78) = 8.7, p < 0.001] or PND 9 [t(78) = 6.55, p < 0.001, Figure 2E]. Maternal pup retrievals also differed between handling conditions (χ2 = 7.52, df = 1, p = 0.006), where Handled mothers retrieved their pups more frequently than Non-Handled mothers [t(111) = 2.68, p = 0.009]. Fathers, on the other hand, did not alter their pup retrievals as a function of Handling (χ2 = 0.56, df = 1, p = 0.46). Additionally, the presence of fathers did not alter the amount of pup retrievals exhibited by mothers (χ2 = 1.32, df = 1, p = 0.25).

Autogrooming varied across postnatal ages for both mothers (χ2 = 13.5, df = 2, p = 0.001) and fathers (χ2 = 6.74, df = 2, p = 0.03). Mothers significantly decreased their autogrooming when pups were PND16 compared to both PND2 [t(114) = 3.6, p = 0.002] and PND 9 [t(114) = 3.3, p = 0.004]. Fathers, on the other hand, increased their autogrooming between PND9 and PND16 [t(67) = 2.4, p = 0.05]. Parents also differed in the amount of time they spent exploring and trail building in the home cage (summed as “off nest” activity, χ2 = 6.0, df = 1, p = 0.01). Fathers spent more time engaging in off nest behaviors compared to mothers [t(117) = 2.6, p = 0.01], specifically at PND 9 [t(117) = 2.1, p = 0.04, Figure 2F].

3.3. Handling alters correlations between maternal and paternal pup grooming

When analyzing the relationship between maternal and paternal care combined over all pup ages, parents that were Non-handled showed no correlation in pup grooming (r = 0.08, n = 33, p = 0.65). Conversely, pup grooming was correlated for Handled mothers and fathers (r = 0.36, n = 33, p = 0.04, Figure 2G). Mothers and Fathers showed moderate correlations in pup retrieval when analyzing across all pup ages combined, regardless of if they were Non-Handled (r = 0.36, n = 33, p = 0.04) or Handled (r = 0.35, n = 42, p = 0.02, Figure 2H).

3.4. Handling manipulation and the presence of the father affects mothers’ behavior in the OFT

The open field test (OFT) is commonly used to assess anxiety-like behaviors, and/or exploratory behaviors under laboratory conditions. We investigated the total distance traveled within an OFT arena, and the frequency of visits to the center of the OFT arena to assess how experimental handling and the presence of fathers in the home cage impacted anxiety-like behavior in parents.

The number of times mothers visited the center of the OFT chamber differed by father presence in the natal nest (χ2 = 8.5, df = 1, p = 0.004), where mothers that were co-housed with fathers visited the OFT center significantly more frequently than did single mothers [t(32) = 3.03, p = 0.005, Figure 3A]. There was also a main effect of experimental handling (χ2 = 4.0, df = 1, p = 0.05), where Handled mothers (non-significantly) tended to visit the center of the OFT apparatus more frequently than Non-handled mothers [t(32) = 1.8, p = 0.08]. Conversely, experimental handling did not impact center visits among fathers (χ2 = 0.003, df = 1, p = 0.96). Total distance traveled by mothers in the OFT was also impacted by experimental handling (χ2 = 6.5, df = 1, p = 0.01), where Handled mothers traveled significantly more than Non-handled mothers [t(34) = 2.4, p = 0.02, Figure 3B]. A similar pattern of handling on fathers was observed, but this effect was not significant (χ2 = 3.4, df = 1, p = 0.07). The duration of time mothers spent in the center of the OFT chamber was subject to an interaction between experimental handling and father presence (χ2 = 5.5, df = 1, p = 0.02). However, all post-hoc contrasts were non-significant (all p’s > 0.05). The duration of time fathers spent in the center of the OFT did not differ significantly by experimental handling (χ2 = 3.4, df = 1, p = 0.06).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Parental open field test performance across parental experiences. (A) Total number of visits to the OFT arena center by single vs. paired mothers. (B) Total distance traveled in the OFT arena by parents who were Non-handed vs. Handled. *p ≤ 0.05. Box and whisker plots and individual points visualize.

3.5. Behavioral predictors of vasopressin cell counts vary by experimental condition

Parental care and anxiety-like behaviors have been linked to the neural activity of vasopressin (Hostetler and Ryabinin, 2013; Bales and Saltzman, 2016). We quantified the number of VP-ir cells in hypothalamic subpopulations to ask how their neural expression related to prior parental experiences and behaviors. In mothers, the amount of pup grooming was a significant negative predictor of the number of VP-ir cells in the PVN (χ2 = 4.0, df = 1, p = 0.05, Figure 4A). This relationship was independent of the presence of a partner (χ2 = 1.5, df = 1, p = 0.22) or of handling condition (χ2 = 0.8, df = 1, p = 0.37). There was no main effect of pup retrieval on maternal VP-ir counts in the PVN (χ2 = 1.1, df = 1, p = 0.29).

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

Regression visualizations for VP-ir cell counts using parental care predictors. (A) Estimated marginal means for VP-ir counts in mothers by time spent pup grooming, averaged over levels of father presence and handling condition. (B) Conditional effects on VP-ir counts by time spent pup grooming for single mothers vs. paired mothers, averaged over handling conditions. (C) Conditional effects on VP-ir counts by time spent pup grooming for Handled vs. Non-handled fathers. (D) Conditional effects on VP-ir counts by the number of pup retrievals by Handled vs. Non-handled fathers. Shaded regions reflect 95% confidence intervals.

The direction of the relationship between pup grooming and the number of SON VP-ir cells in mothers depended upon father presence (χ2 = 4.8, df = 1, p = 0.03, Figure 4B). Single mothers exhibited a negative relationship between pup grooming and SON VP-ir counts, whereas paired mothers showed a positive relationship. Conversely, VP-ir counts in the SON were not related to pup retrievals in mothers (χ2 = 0.01, df = 1, p = 0.73).

In the fathers, the relationship between pup grooming and the number of VP-ir cells in the PVN was dependent upon the Handling condition (χ2 = 5.7, df = 1, p = 0.02, Figure 4C). Paternal pup grooming negatively corresponded with PVN VP-ir neurons when fathers were Non-handled, but this relationship diminished when fathers were handled. The number of SON VP-ir cells in fathers was also subject to an interaction between handling condition and pup retrievals (χ2 = 17.1, df = 1, p = 3.5e−05, Figure 4D). Pup retrievals were positively associated with VP-ir cell counts when fathers were Non-handled, but this association was negative when fathers were Handled. Neither pup grooming (χ2 = 0.0002, df = 1, p = 0.99) nor handling condition (χ2 = 1.7, df = 1, p = 0.2) were significant predictors of SON VP-ir counts in fathers.

4. Discussion

A rich understanding of the complex dynamics between parents offers insight into how interactions among caregivers alter the quality of social relationships between mating partners. Furthermore, focus on these dynamics provides additional context for how early life experiences impact offspring social behavior and wellbeing. The present study examined (1) how variation in (co-)parenting experiences for mothers and fathers impact their behaviors inside and outside of the natal nest, and (2) how parental experiences shape a neural signaling system known to be important for parental behavior and anxiety-like traits.

4.1. Parents exhibit offspring age-dependent behavioral plasticity in sex-specific ways

We used a 2 × 2 design integrating the presence and absence of fathers with a salient but minor handling stress manipulation designed to temporarily alter parental behavior (Bales et al., 2007) to model ecologically relevant and distinct rearing conditions (Getz and Carter, 1996). Despite these experimental conditions, offspring age most frequently accounted for significant variation in parental and non-parental behaviors. A possible alternative explanation to this age-dependent variation in parental behavior is that parents may have habituated to the experimental handling treatment over time. However, our results are consistent with prior work showing that prairie vole parents display different rates of care when pups are neonates compared to when they are older, despite using a different testing paradigm (Lonstein and De Vries, 1999). This phenomenon is not unique to biparental voles; many other species have demonstrated robust parental behavioral plasticity in response to offspring age. For example, zebra finch parents whose chicks were swapped with broods that were either older or younger than their own offspring adjusted their caregiving to match the needs of their foster brood (Rehling et al., 2012). These results suggest that responding to the changing needs of the offspring may serve an adaptive function for species that exhibit parental care (Saltzman et al., 2017). There are several ways in which parents can balance the energetic demands of rearing pups with their need to maintain their own body condition and maximize future reproductive potential (Trivers, 1974). For example, this can be accomplished by investing effort to meet the current needs of offspring while decreasing behaviors that no longer appreciably support them. In line with this balancing act, we found that parents decreased caregiving behaviors, including nursing and grooming, as the pups approached an age associated with the ability to self-thermoregulate and consume solid food. Conversely, pup retrieval by both parents increased as pups grew older, a result likely attributable to the refinement of pup motor skills and the onset of their exploratory behaviors. The capacity to decrease the time allocated to intense offspring-directed care might also explain the observed increases in off-nest activities – a trade-off that has been theoretically modeled and empirically tested in other species (Williams, 1966; Kacelnik and Cuthill, 1990; Whittingham, 1993).

Thomas and Birney (1979) argued that male and female prairie voles contribute many of the same behaviors to offspring care. Other experiments have since shown that, compared to mothers, fathers tend to be more variable in their offspring care (e.g., Solomon, 1993; Finton and Ophir, 2020; Kelly et al., 2020). Consistent with all of this, we report sex differences in the patterns of adjustments in pup age-related behaviors, indicating that mothers and fathers exhibit differential sensitivities and responses to offspring developmental stage. Specifically, mothers groomed pups more than fathers did when pups were 2 and 9 days old, recapitulating earlier work demonstrating that prairie vole mothers spend more time in contact with pups than fathers do during the first week of postnatal life, but not beyond (Solomon, 1993). We also found that fathers increase, whereas mothers decrease, their autogrooming as pups age. Notably, a contrast appears to exist between the mothers that prioritize offspring care (i.e., offspring grooming) over self-care (i.e., autogrooming), and the fathers that more readily shifted from pup care to self-care behavior. This difference between parents suggests that despite engaging in the same general behaviors, prairie vole fathers invest relatively less in their offspring and relatively more in themselves compared to mothers. Many of the mechanisms underlying the expression and diversity of parental behavior appear to be sex-specific (Bendesky et al., 2017). Presumably distinct selective pressures on neural mechanisms such as these may have shaped how parents respond to pups of different ages (i.e., age-dependent behavioral plasticity) (Trivers, 1974). Selectively preserving male pups may have also contributed to our results. Theoretical and empirical evidence from other species suggests that parents might use offspring sex to preferentially allocate care (Trivers and Willard, 1973, e.g., Clancey and Byers, 2016; Lynch et al., 2018, but see Salmon and Hehman, 2021). Whatever the reason, our data indicate that parental sex differences exist and can be tracked to when sex-specific trade-offs occur between pup-directed vs. self-directed care behaviors.

4.2. Absence of fathers altered mothers’ anxiety-like, but not pup-directed, behaviors

A co-parenting partner can offset rearing costs and offspring mortality by provisioning resources, defending territories against intruders and predators, providing thermoregulatory and social stimulation, and tending to the young, thereby freeing mothers to engage in non-parental behaviors (Woodroffe and Vincent, 1994). In some mammals, paternal care enhances offspring maturation rates, litter sizes, body condition, and survival (Wang and Novak, 1992; Gubernick et al., 1993; Stockley and Hobson, 2016). Several models of the evolution of biparental care predict that single parents should increase their caregiving behaviors to compensate for the absence of a mate (McNamara et al., 1999), which has been observed in degus (Octodon degus), Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus), rock cavies (Kerodon rupestris), and the Mexican volcano mouse (Neotomodon alstoni) (Elwood and Broom, 1978; Wilson, 1982; Tasse, 1986; Luis et al., 2004). Notably, our current study found no evidence that mothers behaviorally upregulate their care to compensate for the absence of a father, echoing the findings of other studies of prairie vole parental care (McGuire et al., 2007; Ahern and Young, 2009; Ahern et al., 2011; Tabbaa et al., 2017; Bosch et al., 2018; Rogers and Bales, 2019). One potential explanation for this result is that the laboratory conditions under which mothers were tested may have imposed unnaturally low costs required for sufficient offspring care. Implementing sufficiently harsh or challenging environmental conditions may therefore force single mothers to tradeoff pup care and self-care (Wright and Brown, 2002; Kelly et al., 2020). Whether our laboratory conditions were insufficient to induce changes in prairie vole maternal care or not, our data contrasted with other studies that revealed that many biparental rodents show maternal compensation for the absence of fathers (Elwood and Broom, 1978; Wilson, 1982; Tasse, 1986; Luis et al., 2004). This phenomenon raises another unresolved question: when mothers do compensate for the lack of a co-parent, are they responding to altered pup behaviors as a function of paternal presence, or to the direct absence of the fathers themselves (Elwood and Broom, 1978)? Additional work is necessary to identify the specific contextual cues that may elicit parental behavioral plasticity.

Although paternal absence did not appear to modify maternal care, paired mothers visited the center of the OFT chamber more frequently than single mothers. This behavior is often interpreted as representing a less “anxious-like” phenotype [although see Walsh and Cummins (1976) for discussion of the reliability and validity of the OFT]. Because single and partnered mothers did not display other differences in behavior within their home cages, it is possible that anxiety-related behaviors outside of a parental context may be more susceptible to perturbation by experience during the parenting period. These results concur with earlier work in which single mothers show greater levels of anxiety-like and depressive-like phenotypes compared to paired prairie vole mothers in an elevated plus maze and forced swim test, respectively, (Bosch et al., 2018). In both experimental designs, it is challenging to discern if the behavioral results are due to bond dissolution or to the experience of single parenthood. It would be useful to address the potential differential influences of partner loss versus the demands of single-parenthood to best understand how these factors independently or synergistically shape parental anxiety-like phenotypes. Furthermore, using alternate measures of anxiety-like phenotypes may better reveal the suite of sociocognitive processes that are impacted by various stressors.

4.3. Acute handling stress induced coordination of pup grooming between mothers and fathers

Experimental handling significantly increased the activity of parents in terms of caregiving (exemplified by more pup retrievals in the home cage) and general activity (i.e., distance traveled in the OFT). Notably, handling produced a correlation between maternal and paternal pup grooming that was not observed when parents were not handled. This might suggest that behavioral upregulation of grooming induced by the acute handling paradigm promoted coordination of some aspects of caregiving between parents. However, parental correlations in pup retrievals emerged independently of handling experience, which suggests that if the acute stressor synchronized parental care, it did so in a behavior-specific way. Behavioral coordination or synchrony in biparental species is dependent on social experience and context (Prior, 2020). For example, experimentally manipulating brood size led to changes in the synchronization of nest visits in wild zebra finch parents (Mariette and Griffith, 2015). Furthermore, nest visit synchrony in blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) has been linked to weather and altitude, suggesting that biparental coordination is sensitive to ecological conditions (Lejeune et al., 2019). Our data (in conjunction with the aforementioned studies) supports the hypothesis that dyadic care exhibits experience-specific and context-specific plasticity. Little is known regarding the neuroendocrine mechanisms that may facilitate these adaptations, but below we explore the evidence that suggests a potential link between VP cells and experience-dependent neurobehavioral plasticity in parents.

4.4. Associations between vasopressin cell counts and parental behaviors exhibited experience-dependent plasticity in mothers and fathers

Our data demonstrated that both mothers and fathers exhibited predictive relationships between their home cage caregiving behaviors and hypothalamic vasopressinergic cell counts. The duration that mothers spent grooming their pups was inversely associated with the number of VP-ir neurons in the PVN, suggesting that there may be a negative relationship between vasopressin signaling and maternal pup grooming. Considering that VP is involved in parental status and behavior in prairie voles (Bamshad et al., 1994; Wang et al., 1994, 2000) and other rodents (Parker and Lee, 2001; Bosch and Neumann, 2008; Bayerl et al., 2016), it initially seems paradoxical that mothers that expressed higher levels of grooming had fewer VP-ir cells. Interpretations of cell count data are quite challenging because differences in immunoreactivity can represent either peptide production, accumulation due to blocked secretion, or changes in release (Panzica et al., 2001; Goodson and Kabelik, 2009). Under the latter interpretation, the decrease in PVN VP-ir neurons that we found could indicate greater release of bioavailable VP to extrahypothalamic sites, which in turn might have facilitated increases in maternal care. Furthermore, it is possible that sub-populations of PVN VP-positive neurons impacted parental care differently, and it could be useful for future studies to disaggregate the cell counts within the PVN. To determine the functional significance of variation in VP-ir density, future studies could also use acute sampling methods or imaging of fluorescent reporters to determine how real-time fluctuations in VP release relate to the expression of distinct parental behaviors.

Like with the PVN, VP-ir counts in the SON of mothers were also significantly associated with pup grooming. In contrast, however, the direction of the association between pup grooming and VP-ir cell counts was contingent on whether mothers raised offspring with a co-parent or raised offspring alone. Similar experience-dependent interactions of cell counts and parental care were observed in fathers, in which the handling conditions determined if the slopes between VP-ir counts and pup grooming/pup retrievals were positive or negative. Critically, our neural measures took place after the behavioral observation periods and are therefore only correlational, making it challenging to ascertain how the associations between neural and behavioral variables relate to VP-mediated functional control of parental care. Nevertheless, our results reveal a general pattern in which social and non-social experiences shaped the associations between parental behavior and hypothalamic VP cell groups. Similar patterns of experience-dependent mediation of brain-behavior relationships have been described for several neuromodulators in other species. For example, a study of wire-tailed manakins (Pipra filicauda) found that the relationship between circulating testosterone and male social behaviors was inverted between territory-holders and non-territory-holders, indicating that social status dynamically modulates hormone-behavior relationships in this species (Ryder et al., 2020). Furthermore, the association between baseline corticosterone levels and parental success (fledgling numbers) is positive prior to egg laying and negative during the subsequent parental provisioning phase in great tits (Parus major), and is therefore dependent on reproductive stage (Ouyang et al., 2013). Moreover, group-housed mice show a significant positive correlation between serotonergic activity and social investigation, but such a correlation was not found in isolate-housed mice, suggesting that social housing contexts coordinate relationships between serotonin and social behavior (Keesom et al., 2017). Our study supplements these examples supporting the hypothesis that social and stress-related experiences are involved in contingently shaping the relationships between neuropeptides and behavioral phenotypes, even across neurotransmitter classes and taxa.

The ways in which our manipulations of parental experience mediated the direction of brain-behavior phenotypes was different between fathers and mothers. How experience-dependent plasticity manifests in the brains of parents may be tied to sexual dimorphisms in neural and behavioral phenotypes. The majority of what is known about the neuroendocrinological basis of mammalian parental behavior has been characterized in females (Saltzman and Ziegler, 2014). From these studies, it is clear that a symphony of hormones and neuroactive signaling molecules accompany pregnancy and parturition, and are critical for priming primiparous mothers to behave maternally toward their newborn offspring (e.g.,Ophir et al., 2013). Mammalian fathers, on the other hand, do not gestate their young. Thus, the endocrine mechanisms that subserve care in males can greatly differ from those of females, and might vary between species (Horrell et al., 2018). Moreover, VP sources can be found outside the hypothalamus and, although they appear to be relatively sparse (Bamshad et al., 1993; Wang, 1995; Kelly and Ophir, 2015), extrahypothalamic VP positive neurons (particularly in the medial amygdala; Lim et al., 2004) have been implicated in parental behavior (Keverne and Curley, 2004; Bosch and Neumann, 2012). Thus, extrahypothalamic VP might have also contributed to shaping parental responses to pups, particularly between mothers and fathers. Nevertheless, hormones known to play a functional role in the display of paternal behaviors (both centrally and peripherally) do fluctuate and are susceptible to factors such as mating and pair bonding experiences, exposure to pregnant females, and exposure to offspring (Saltzman and Ziegler, 2014). The fact that the VP-behavior relationships we observed were contingent upon other environmental experiences (i.e., presence of a co-parent in mothers, and handling stress in fathers) highlights the remarkable plasticity of the vasopressin signaling system, even within the brains of mature animals. It is noteworthy that the highly variable distributions of forebrain vasopressin receptors across species stands in stark contrast to the deeply conserved patterning of other neuromodulatory systems such as steroid hormone and dopamine receptors (King and Young, 2016). This evidence in conjunction with our data lends credence to the notion that the VP system exhibits high levels of evolvability that may enable this signaling molecule to take on new functions in the modulation of social behavior (Freeman et al., 2020; Young and Zhang, 2021).

Taken together, we investigated how social and non-social variation in parental experiences impact prairie vole parents’ behaviors across pup development, and how these experiences shape a neuroendocrine system that drives and responds to social behaviors. Although manipulating co-parent presence and acute stress did not modulate parental behavior as strongly as pup age did, these variables robustly shaped the associations between VP cell density and pup-directed care in mothers and fathers. Future work should address the functional implications of alterations in hypothalamic neuroendocrine signaling for parental behavior, and search for the underlying mechanisms by which experiences biologically organize the relationships between hormones and parental behavioral phenotypes. Comparisons of interclass species may also reveal to what degree such evolutionary mechanisms may be shared across biparental species.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

All procedures using animals were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Cornell University (2013-0102), and were consistent with the guidelines set forth by ARRIVE and the National Institutes of Health of the United States.

Author contributions

LH and AO conceived and designed the experiments, discussed the results, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. LH conducted behavior testing, brain sectioning, histology, and data analysis. VL assisted with behavioral scoring and brain sectioning. All authors critically revised the article and approved the final version.

Acknowledgments

We thank Mandy Chan for her assistance with behavioral scoring. Thank you to the animal care staff of Cornell University for making this research possible, and to the voles for their critical role in advancing scientific knowledge.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the funding from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to AO (HD079573) and from an NSF Graduate Research Fellowship to LH (1650441).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

  1. Ahern T. H., Young L. J. (2009). The impact of early life family structure on adult social attachment, alloparental behavior, and the neuropeptide systems regulating affiliative behaviors in the monogamous prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster). Front. Behav. Neurosci. 3:17. 10.3389/neuro.08.017.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ahern T. H., Hammock E. A. D., Young L. J. (2011). Parental division of labor, coordination, and the effects of family structure on parenting in monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Dev. Psychobiol. 53 118–131. 10.1002/dev.20498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amir S., Brown Z. W., Amit Z., Ornstein K. (1981). Body pinch induces long lasting cataleptic like immobility in mice: behavioral characterization and the effect of naloxone. Life Sci. 28 1189–1194. 10.1016/0024-3205(81)90697-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Balcombe J. P., Barnard N. D., Sandusky C. (2004). Laboratory routines cause animal stress. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 43 42–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bales K. L., Saltzman W. (2016). Fathering in rodents: neurobiological substrates and consequences for offspring. Horm. Behav. 77 249–259. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.05.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bales K. L., Kramer K. M., Lewis-Reese A. D., Carter S. C. (2006). Effects of stress on parental care are sexually dimorphic in prairie voles. Physiol.Behav. 87 424–429. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bales K. L., Lewis-Reese A. D., Pfeifer L. A., Kramer K. M., Carter S. C. (2007). Early experience affects the traits of monogamy in a sexually dimorphic manner. Dev. Psychobiol. 49 335–342. 10.1002/dev.20216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bamshad M., Novak M. A., De Vries G. J. (1993). Sex and species differences in the vasopressin innervation of sexually naive and parental prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster and meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus. J. Neuroendocrinol. 5 247–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bamshad M., Novak M. A., de Vries G. J. (1994). Cohabitation alters vasopressin innervation and paternal behavior in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Physiol. Behav. 56 751–758. 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90238-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bates D., Maechler M., Bolker B., Walker S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67 1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bayerl D. S., Hönig J. N., Bosch O. J. (2016). Vasopressin V1a, but not V1b, receptors within the PVN of lactating rats mediate maternal care and anxiety-related behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 305 18–22. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.02.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bendesky A., Kwon Y. M., Lassance J. M., Lewarch C. L., Yao S., Peterson B. K., et al. (2017). The genetic basis of parental care evolution in monogamous mice. Nature 544 434–439. 10.1038/nature22074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bosch O. J., Neumann I. D. (2008). Brain vasopressin is an important regulator of maternal behavior independent of dams’ trait anxiety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105 17139–17144. 10.1073/pnas.0807412105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bosch O. J., Neumann I. D. (2012). Both oxytocin and vasopressin are mediators of maternal care and aggression in rodents: from central release to sites of action. Horm. Behav. 61 293–303. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bosch O. J., Pohl T. T., Neumann I. D., Young L. J. (2018). Abandoned prairie vole mothers show normal maternal care but altered emotionality: potential influence of the brain corticotropin-releasing factor system. Behav. Brain Res. 341 114–121. 10.1016/J.BBR.2017.12.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brooks M., Kristensen K., van Benthem K., Magnusson A., Berg C., Nielsen A., et al. (2017). glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 9 378–400. [Google Scholar]
  17. Brownstein M. J., Russell J. T., Gainer H. (1980). Synthesis, transport, and release of posterior pituitary hormones. Science 207 373–378. 10.1126/science.6153132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cinelli P., Rettich A., Seifert B., Bur̈ki K., Arras M. (2007). Comparative analysis and physiological impact of different tissue biopsy methodologies used for the genotyping of laboratory mice. Lab. Anim. 41 143–300. 10.1258/002367707780378113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Clancey E., Byers J. A. (2016). A comprehensive test of the Trivers–Willard hypothesis in pronghorn (Antilocapra americana). J. Mammal. 97 179–186. 10.1093/jmammal/gyv168 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Elwood R. W., Broom D. M. (1978). The influence of litter size and parental behaviour on the development of Mongolian gerbil pups. Anim. Behav. 26 438–454. 10.1016/0003-3472(78)90061-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Finton C. J., Ophir A. G. (2020). Prairie vole offspring only prefer mothers over fathers when mothers are a unique resource, yet fathers are the primary source of variation in parental care. Behav. Process. 171:104022. 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.104022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fox J., Weisberg S. (2019). An {R} companion to applied regression, 3rd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  23. Freeman A. R., Aulino E. A., Caldwell H. K., Ophir A.G. (2020). Comparison of the distribution of oxytocin and vasopressin 1a receptors in rodents reveals conserved and derived patterns of nonapeptide evolution. J. Neuroendocrinol. 32:e12828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gerkema M. P., van der Leest F. (1992). Ongoing ultradian activity rhythms in the common vole, Microtus arvalis, during deprivations of food, water and rest. J. Comp. Physiol. A 168 591–597. 10.1007/BF00215081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Getz L. L., McGuire B., Pizzuto T., Hofmann J. E., Frase B. (1993). Social organization of the prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster). J. Mammal. 74, 44–58. 10.2307/1381904 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Getz L. L., Carter S. C. (1996). Prairie-vole partnerships. Am. Scientist 84 56–62. [Google Scholar]
  27. Goodson J. L., Kabelik D. (2009). Dynamic limbic networks and social diversity in vertebrates: from neural context to neuromodulatory patterning. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30:429–441. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gubernick D. J., Wright S. L., Brown R. E. (1993). The significance of father’s presence for offspring survival in the monogamous California mouse, Peromyscus californicus. Anim. Behav. 46 539–546. 10.1006/anbe.1993.1221 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Harrison F., Barta Z., Cuthill I., Székely T. (2009). How is sexual conflict over parental care resolved? A meta-analysis. J. Evol. Biol. 22 1800–1812. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01792.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hartig F. (2019). DHARMa: residual diagnostics for hierarchical (multi-level/mixed) regression models. R package v. 0.2.6. [Google Scholar]
  31. Herman J. P., Tasker J. G. (2016). Paraventricular hypothalamic mechanisms of chronic stress adaptation. Front. Endocrinol.:137. 10.3389/fendo.2016.00137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hiura L. C., Kelly A. M., Ophir A. G. (2018). Age-specific and context-specific responses of the medial extended amygdala in the developing prairie vole. Dev. Neurobiol. 78 1231–1245. 10.1002/dneu.22648 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Horrell N. D., Hickmott P. W., Saltzman W. (2018). Neural regulation of paternal behavior in mammals: sensory, neuroendocrine, and experiential influences on the paternal brain. Neuroendocr. Regul. Behav. 43 111–160. 10.1086/393848 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hostetler C. M., Ryabinin A. E. (2013). The CRF system and social behavior: a review. Front. Neurosci. 7:92. 10.3389/fnins.2013.00092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hurst J. L., West R. S. (2010). Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nat. Methods 7 825–826. 10.1038/nmeth.1500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kacelnik A., Cuthill I. (1990). Central place foraging in starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). II. Food allocation to chicks. J. Anim. Ecol. 59:655. 10.2307/4887 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Keesom S. M., Sloss B. G., Erbowor-Becksen Z., Hurley L. M. (2017). Social experience alters socially induced serotonergic fluctuations in the inferior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 118 3230–3241. 10.1152/jn.00431.2017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kelly A. M., Ophir A. G. (2015). Compared to what: what can we say about nonapeptide function and social behavior without a frame of reference? Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 6 97–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kelly A. M., Hiura L. C., Saunders A. G., Ophir A. G. (2017). Oxytocin neurons exhibit extensive functional plasticity due to offspring age in mothers and fathers. Integr. Comp. Biol. 57, 603–618. 10.1093/icb/icx036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kelly A. M., Hiura L. C., Ophir A. G. (2018). Rapid nonapeptide synthesis during a critical period of development in the prairie vole: plasticity of the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. Brain Struct. Funct. 223 2547–2560. 10.1007/s00429-018-1640-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kelly A. M., Ong J. Y., Witmer R. A., Ophir A. G. (2020). Paternal deprivation impairs social behavior putatively via epigenetic modification to lateral septum vasopressin receptor. Sci. Adv. 6:eabb9116. 10.1126/sciadv.abb9116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kenkel W. M., Paredes J., Yee J. R., Pournajafi-Nazarloo H., Bales K. L., Carter S. C. (2012). Neuroendocrine and behavioural responses to exposure to an infant in male prairie voles. J. Neuroendocrinol. 24 874–886. 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2012.02301.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Keverne E. B., Curley J. P. (2004). Vasopressin, oxytocin and social behaviour. Curr. Opini. Neurobiol. 14 777–783. 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. King L. B., Young L. J. (2016). “Oxytocin, vasopressin, and diversity in social behavior,” in Molecular neuroendocrinology, eds Murphy D., Gainer H. (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell; ). 10.1002/9781118760369.ch19 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kirkpatrick B., Kim J. W., Insel T. R. (1994). Limbic system fos expression associated with paternal behavior. Brain Res. 658 112–118. 10.1016/S0006-8993(09)90016-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kleiman D. G., Malcolm J. R. (1981). “The evolution of male parental investment in mammals,” in Parental care in mammals, eds Gubernick D. J., Klopfer P. H. (Boston, MA: Springer; ), 347–387. 10.1007/978-1-4613-3150-6_9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lejeune L., Savage J. L., Bründl A. C., Thiney A., Russell A. F., Chaine A. S. (2019). Environmental effects on parental care visitation patterns in blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus. Front. Ecol. Evol. 7:356. 10.3389/fevo.2019.00356 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lenth R., Singmann H., Love J., Buerkner P., Herve M. (2020). emmeans: estimated marginal means. R package version 1.4. 4. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lewis R., Curtis J. T. (2016). Male prairie voles display cardiovascular dipping associated with an ultradian activity cycle. Physiol. Behav. 156 106–116. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.01.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lim M. M., Wang Z., Olazabal D. E., Ren X., Terwilliger E. F., Young L. J. (2004). Enhanced partner preference in a promiscuous species by manipulating the expression of a single gene. Nature 429 754–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lonstein J. S., De Vries G. J. (1999). Comparison of the parental behavior of pair-bonded female and male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Physiol. Behav. 66 33–40. 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00270-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Luis J., Cervantes F. A., Martínez M., Cardenas R., Delgado J., Carmona A. (2004). Male influence on maternal behavior and offspring of captive volcano mice (Neotomodon alstoni) from Mexico. J. Mammal. 85 268–272. 10.1644/bwg-104 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lynch R., Wasielewski H., Cronk L. (2018). Sexual conflict and the Trivers-Willard hypothesis: females prefer daughters and males prefer sons. Sci. Rep. 8:15463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mariette M. M., Griffith S. C. (2015). The adaptive significance of provisioning and foraging coordination between breeding partners. Am. Nat. 185 270–280. 10.1086/679441 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. McGuire B., Parker E., Bemis W. E. (2007). Sex differences, effects of male presence and coordination of nest visits in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) during the immediate postnatal period. Am. Midland Nat. 157 187–201. 10.1674/0003-00312007157[187:sdeomp]2.0.co;2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. McNamara J. M., Gasson C. E., Houston A. I. (1999). Incorporating rules for responding into evolutionary games. Nature 401 368–371. 10.1038/43869 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ophir A. G., Sorochman G., Evans B. L., Prounis G. S. (2013). Stability and dynamics of forebrain V1aR and OTR during pregnancy in prairie voles. J. Neuroendocrinol. 25, 719–728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ouyang J. Q., Sharp P., Quetting M., Hau M. (2013). Endocrine phenotype, reproductive success and survival in the great tit, Parus major. J. Evolut. Biol. 26 1988–1998. 10.1111/jeb.12202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Panzica G. C., Aste N., Castagna C., Viglietti-Panzica C., Balthazart J. (2001). Steroid-induced plasticity in the sexually dimorphic vasotocinergic innervation of the avian brain: behavioral implications. Brain Res. Rev. 37 178–200. 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00118-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Parker K. J., Lee T. M. (2001). Central vasopressin administration regulates the onset of facultative paternal behavior in Microtus pennsylvanicus (Meadow voles). Horm. Behav. 39 285–294. 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1655 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Prior N. (2020). What’s in a moment: what can be learned about pair bonding from studying moment-to-moment behavioral synchrony between partners? Front. Psychol. 11:1370. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01370 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Prounis G. S., Foley L., Rehman A., Ophir A. G. (2015). Perinatal and juvenile social environments interact to shape cognitive behaviour and neural phenotype in prairie voles. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 282:20152236. 10.1098/rspb.2015.2236 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
  64. Rehling A., Spiller I., Krause E. T., Nager R. G., Monaghan P., Trillmich F. (2012). Flexibility in the duration of parental care: zebra finch parents respond to offspring needs. Anim. Behav. 83 35–39. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Rogers F. D., Bales K. L. (2019). Revisiting paternal absence: Female alloparental replacement of fathers recovers partner preference formation in female, but not male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Dev. Psychobiol. 62:21943. 10.1002/dev.21943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Royle N. J., Russell A. F., Wilson A. J. (2014). The evolution of flexible parenting. Science 345 776–781. 10.1126/science.1253294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ryder T. B., Dakin R., Vernasco B. J., Evans B. S., Horton B. M., Moore I. T. (2020). Testosterone modulates status-specific patterns of cooperation in a social network. Am. Nat. 195 82–94. 10.1086/706236 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Salmon C., Hehman J. (2021). “The trivers–willard hypothesis,” in The SAGE handbook of evolutionary psychology: foundations of evolutionary psychology, ed. Shackleford T. K. (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; ), 188–204. 10.4135/9781529739442.n11 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Saltzman W., Ziegler T. E. (2014). Functional significance of hormonal changes in mammalian fathers. J. Neuroendocrinol. 26 685–696. 10.1111/jne.12176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Saltzman W., Harris B. N., De Jong T. R., Perea-Rodriguez J. P., Horrell N. D., Zhao M., et al. (2017). Paternal care in biparental rodents: intra- and inter-individual variation. Integr. Comp. Biol. 57 589–602. 10.1093/icb/icx047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Solomon N. G. (1993). Comparison of parental behavior in male and female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Can. J. Zool. 71 434–437. 10.1139/z93-061 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Stockley P., Hobson L. (2016). Paternal care and litter size coevolution in mammals. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 283:20160140. 10.1098/rspb.2016.0140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tabbaa M., Lei K., Liu Y., Wang Z. X. (2017). Paternal deprivation affects social behaviors and neurochemical systems in the offspring of socially monogamous prairie voles. Neuroscience 343 284–297. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tasse J. (1986). Maternal and paternal care in the rock cavy, Kerodon rupestris, a South American hystricomorph rodent. Zoo Biol. 5 27–43. 10.1002/zoo.1430050105 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Thomas J. A., Birney E. C. (1979). Parental care and mating system of the prairie vole, Microtus ochrogaster. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 5 171–186. 10.1007/BF00293304 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Trivers R. L. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict. Am. Zool. 14 249–264. [Google Scholar]
  77. Trivers R. L., Willard D. E. (1973). Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Am. Assoc. Adv. Sci. 179 90–92. 10.1126/science.179.4068.90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Tyler A. N., Michel G. F., Bales K. L., Carter C. S. (2005). Do brief early disturbances of parents affect parental care in the bi-parental prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster)? Dev. Psychobiol. 47:451. [Google Scholar]
  79. Walsh R. N., Cummins R. A. (1976). The open-field test: a critical review. Psychol. Bull. 83 482–504. 10.1037/0033-2909.83.3.482 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wang Z. (1995). Species differences in the vasopressin-immunoreactive pathways in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and medial amygdaloid nucleus in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Behav. Neurosci. 109 305–311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wang Z. X., Novak M. A. (1992). Influence of the social environment on parental behavior and pup development of meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) and prairie voles (M. ochrogaster). J. Comp. Psychol. 106 163–171. 10.1037/0735-7036.106.2.163 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wang Z. X., Ferris C. F., De Vries G. J. (1994). Role of septal vasopressin innervation in paternal behavior in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91 400–404. 10.1073/pnas.91.1.400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wang Z. X., Liu Y., Young L. J., Insel T. R. (2000). Hypothalamic vasopressin gene expression increases in both males and females postpartum in a biparental rodent. J. Neuroendocrinol. 12 111–120. 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2000.00435.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Whittingham L. A. (1993). Effects of nestling provisioning on the time-activity budgets of male red-winged blackbirds. Condor 95 730–734. 10.2307/1369620 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Wigger A., Sánchez M. M., Mathys K. C., Ebner K., Frank E., Liu D., et al. (2004). Alterations in central neuropeptide expression, release, and receptor binding in rats bred for high anxiety: critical role of vasopressin. Neuropsychopharmacology 29 1–14. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Williams G. C. (1966). Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack’s principle. Am. Na. 100 687–690. 10.1086/282461 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wilson S. C. (1982). Contact-promoting behavior, social development, and relationship with parents in sibling juvenile degus (Octodon degus). Dev. Psychobiol. 15 257–268. 10.1002/dev.420150309 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Woodroffe R., Vincent A. (1994). Mother’s little helpers: patterns of male care in mammals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 9 294–297. 10.1016/0169-5347(94)90033-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wotjak C. T., Ganster J., Kohl G., Holsboer F., Landgraf R., Engelmann M. (1998). Dissociated central and peripheral release of vasopressin, but not oxytocin, in response to repeated swim stress: new insights into the secretory capacities of peptidergic neurons. Neuroscience 85 1209–1222. 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00683-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wright S. L., Brown R. E. (2002). The importance of paternal care on pup survival and pup growth in Peromyscus californicus when required to work for food. Behav. Process. 60 41–52. 10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00101-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Young L. J., Zhang Q. (2021). On the origins of diversity in social behavior. Japanese J. Anim. Psychol. 71 45–61. 10.2502/janip.71.1.4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.


Articles from Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES