Skip to main content
PeerJ logoLink to PeerJ
. 2023 May 8;11:e15318. doi: 10.7717/peerj.15318

Cardiac sensitivity to rewards in cognitively inflexible nonclinical participants

José Luis Mata 1,#, Azahara Leonor Miranda Gálvez 1,✉,#, Francisca López Torrecillas 1, Laura Miccoli 2
Editor: Marco Filardi
PMCID: PMC10174053  PMID: 37180586

Abstract

Background

In psychopathologies characterized by compulsive decision-making, core impairments include cognitive inflexibility and excessive sensitivity to rewards. It has been posited that traits shared by nonclinical individuals and psychiatric patients could help explain the pathogenesis of compulsive decision-making.

Methods

To investigate whether cognitive inflexibility predisposes nonclinical individuals to poor choices and hyper-reactivity to reward, we recruited people with high and low scores for cognitive persistence and used the Iowa Gambling Task to assess decision-making and cardiac reactivity to monetary gains/losses.

Results

As is frequently observed in psychophysiological research, the data indicated discrepancies among self-reports, behavior, and physiology. Cognitive inflexibility was not related to worse performance; however, monetary gains, in line with the literature, prompted marked cardiac accelerations. Consistent with our research goal, only inflexible participants showed large cardiac accelerations during the largest monetary wins.

Discussion

Taken together, the data confirm an association between cognitive persistence and physiological reward sensitivity in a nonclinical population. The findings are in line with recent theories on the development of compulsive behaviors that consider cognitive inflexibility as a transdiagnostic impairment and predisposing factor for excessive reactivity to rewards, and might act both as a preexisting individual trait and drug-induced deficit.

Keywords: Emotion, Individual differences, Heart rate, Iowa Gambling Task, Psychopathology, Cognitive inflexibility, Compulsivity, Reinforcement learning, Reward sensitivity

Introduction

Theories on impaired decision-making across a range of psychopathologies characterized by compulsivity, including behavioral and substance use disorders, identify cognitive inflexibility as a core transdiagnostic impairment (Parnaudeau, Bolkan & Kellendonk, 2018; Voon et al., 2017; Wyckmans et al., 2019). Cognitive inflexibility broadly refers to difficulty in readjusting one’s choices after changes in reward contingency (Perandrés-Gómez et al., 2021). It is taken to indicate the inability to modify cognitive representations, and hence behavior, in response to changing conditions: the solutions that were appropriate during training are inappropriately applied to new problems (Taatgen et al., 2008). The consequence of such cognitive rigidity, “the tendency of an individual not to change” (Schultz & Searleman, 2002), is that the individual perseveres with a behavior despite its negative outcomes (Bechara et al., 1997; Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009). Recent human and animal models (Groman et al., 2019; Wyckmans et al., 2019) suggest that adaptive decision-making alternates efficiently between strategies that focus on long-term benefits and those that focus on immediate gain: long-term strategies are assumed to be deficient in compulsive disorders (Bell & Polaschek, 2017; Voon et al., 2017). However, recent studies hint at a more complex and heterogeneous pattern of alternation between decisional strategies (Collins & Cockburn, 2020; Deserno & Hauser, 2020; Gueguen, Schweitzer & Konova, 2021) and suggest that individual differences play a central role in addiction development (Fraser & Janak, 2019; Groman et al., 2019; Sebold et al., 2017; Vandaele & Ahmed, 2021). An individual trait that seems particularly crucial during the initial phases of reinforcement learning is hypersensitivity to reward, i.e., the tendency to overreact to pleasant cues (Fraser & Janak, 2019; Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009; Voon et al., 2017).

Overall, the findings indicate that pathological decision-making tends to be characterized by cognitive inflexibility as well as hyperreactivity to rewards (Banca, Harrison & Voon, 2016; Bell & Polaschek, 2017; Groman et al., 2019; Parnaudeau, Bolkan & Kellendonk, 2018; Perandrés-Gómez et al., 2021; Vandaele & Ahmed, 2021; Voon et al., 2017; Wyckmans et al., 2019). It has been proposed that research on traits that are shared by psychiatric patients and nonclinical individuals might aid understanding the pathogenesis of compulsive disorders (Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009). Accordingly, the present study aimed to answer the following research questions: If participants who are nonclinical exhibit trait cognitive inflexibility, will they show–akin to clinical populations–physiological hyperreactivity to rewards following feedback about wins? Moreover, from a behavioral point of view, will nonclinical participants high in cognitive inflexibility–and therefore potentially prone to maladaptive decisions–display impaired decision-making? Thus, we focused on individual differences in nonclinical participants to identify traits that might contribute to the development of pathological reinforcement learning. The observation of hyperreactivity to rewards and/or impaired decision-making among participants who are nonclinical yet score highly for cognitive inflexibility would further support the role that cognitive inflexibility plays in promoting maladaptive reactivity to rewarding cues.

The idea that physiological reactivity might play a central role in decision-making emerged with the ‘somatic marker’ hypothesis (Bechara et al., 1994; Bechara, Dolan & Hindes, 2002; Bechara & Damasio, 2000), which proposes that, in ambiguous situations, physiological responses associated with certain earlier cues might guide decision-making. The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) (ibidem) provided a framework to investigate complex decision-making in patients and controls (Bull, Tippett & Addis, 2015; Steingroever et al., 2013). In the IGT, participants try to win as much money as possible by choosing from among four card decks (A–D) 100 times; following each deck choice, they receive feedback on monetary wins or losses. However, the difference between “advantageous” (C, D) and “disadvantageous” decks (A, B) is not intuitive and it has been estimated that, after completing the IGT, about one third of healthy controls still do not understand its underlying logic and perseveres with “disadvantageous” decks despite considerable losses, see also the ‘prominent deck B’ phenomenon (Crone et al., 2004; Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009; Visagan, Xiang & Lamar, 2012).

While performance on the IGT (number of correct trials across successive blocks) is typically characterized by a learning curve and progressively stronger preference for advantageous decks (Crone et al., 2004; Werner, Duschek & Schandry, 2009), the physiological activity that precedes deck choice (typically the skin conductance response (SCR) and heart rate (HR)) suggests that, halfway through the task, disadvantageous decks prompt changes in SCR before feedback about losses/wins (Bechara et al., 1996; Goudriaan et al., 2006; Werner, Duschek & Schandry, 2009). Thus, according to the somatic marker hypothesis, anticipatory physiology promotes task understanding (Bechara et al., 1994; Werner, Duschek & Schandry, 2009). In addition, throughout the IGT, after each deck choice, feedback about monetary losses/wins allows to investigate the impact of rewarding/punishing cues on post-choice physiology. Here, skin conductance shows less consistent patterns (see Simonovic et al., 2019), whereas HR differentiates rewarding from aversive cues so that feedback on monetary wins, relative to losses, prompt repeated cardiac accelerations (Goudriaan et al., 2006; Herman, Esposito & Tsakiris, 2021; Studer, Scheibehenne & Clark, 2016). In tasks related and unrelated to gambling, cardiac accelerations are observed during rewarding, pleasant cues to such an extent that increasing heart rate during emotionally relevant cues is regarded as a reliable index of appetitive motivation (Bradley, Cuthbert & Lang, 1993; Bradley et al., 2001). In contrast to anticipatory physiology, which is more obvious in good performers, participants seem to react to wins and losses regardless of their level of task understanding (Crone et al., 2004; Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009), which suggests independence of reward sensitivity from the processing of task-relevant information (Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009).

Against this background, in the present study we used the Persistence Scale of the TCI-R (Temperament and Character Inventory-Revisited; Gutiérrez-Zotes et al., 2004) to identify participants scoring high or low on cognitive inflexibility. Consistent with current hypotheses on the role of inflexibility (Groman et al., 2019; Wyckmans et al., 2019) and reward sensitivity (e.g., Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009; Studer, Scheibehenne & Clark, 2016; Voon et al., 2017) in the development of addictions, we aimed to investigate: (1) whether participants scoring high in cognitive inflexibility show worse decision-making, i.e., more disadvantageous choices, than cognitively flexible participants; (2) whether cognitively inflexible participants show greater cardiac sensitivity after IGT wins; and (3) whether the highest monetary wins amplify individual differences in physiological sensitivity to rewards, which overall would point toward increased physiological reactivity to rewarding, pleasant cues.

The mutual presence of cognitive inflexibility and heightened sensitivity to reward in a sample of nonclinical individuals would further support the hypothesis that they both might contribute to the development of maladaptive reinforcement learning.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The participants were 48 students (14 men) from the University of Granada, ranging in age from 18 to 37 years (M = 20.6; SD = 4.0) who participated for course credits. Students who did not want to (or could not) participate in the study were offered alternative ways to obtain course credits. The experimenter in charge of data collection was not part of the teaching staff and selected participants based solely on their extreme scores on the TCI-R Persistence subscale, Spanish version (Temperament and Character Inventory-Revisited, Gutiérrez-Zotes et al., 2004). Participant selection was based on a blinded procedure: the experimenter used random digits rather than full names to identify the participants with the most extreme Cognitive Inflexibility scores. From among the initial sample of students who filled out the questionnaire, those with the highest scores (within the top 10% for the whole sample) and lowest scores (within the bottom 10%), i.e., participants in the upper (high cognitive inflexibility, M = 125.2, SD = 10.7, n = 23) and lower deciles (low cognitive inflexibility, M = 101.4, SD = 7.3, n = 25), were selected. Figure 1 shows the study procedure. The Cognitive Inflexibility scores for the upper and lower deciles matched those classified as ‘high’ (M = 124, averaged by sex) and ‘low’ (M = 102, averaged by sex), respectively, based on Spanish normative data (ibidem). Participants were included only if they did not suffer from physical or psychiatric disorders and did not use any psychoactive drugs. Because of equipment failures and/or excessive artifacts, the final sample size was 48 for performance data and 46 for heart rate data. Using G* Power for ANOVA designs (Faul et al., 2007) and including one 2-level within variable and one 2-level between variable, we set error probability ( α) at 0.05 and statistical power (1 − β) at 0.8. Applying these parameters, the current sample size (n = 46) was deemed sufficient to detect small effects (partial η2 = 0.02). All participants provided written informed consent. The UGR Ethical Committee approved the experimental protocol (IRB·#2994/CEIH/2022), that complied with the APA ethical standards and the Declaration of Helsinki.

Figure 1. Participant flowchart.

Figure 1

Instruments

TCI-R Persistence subscale. To assess cognitive inflexibility, the Persistence subscale from the Temperament and Character Inventory-Revisited was administered (Cloninger, 1999; validated Spanish version, Gutiérrez-Zotes et al., 2004). The authors have permission to use this instrument from the copyright holders. The TCI-R Persistence scale consists of 35 5-point Likert items that evaluate Persistence/Cognitive Inflexibility, which is one of the main components of Cloninger’s psychobiological model of personality (ibidem); in the present sample, the scale showed moderate internal consistency (α = 0.73), similar to Spanish normative data (α = 0.76).

Iowa Gambling Task. All participants performed a computerized version of the IGT (Bechara et al., 1994). During 100 trials, participants tried to win as much money as possible by choosing from among four card decks (A–D) with different win and loss contingencies: decks A and B lead to big wins but even bigger losses (disadvantageous decks), whereas decks C and D lead to smaller wins but, in the long term, result in larger net gains (advantageous decks).

Procedure

Performing the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), each trial began with all decks displayed on the screen. Immediately after the participant’s choice (left mouse button), if the deck was associated with monetary losses, gains were displayed for 2 s followed by larger losses, shown for 2 s and a variable (1–3 s) intertrial interval (ITI). However, if the deck was associated with monetary gains, these were displayed for 2 s and immediately followed by a 3–5 s ITI. To provide constant feedback, cumulative gains were always in view. At the end of the task, participants were asked if they thought one deck was more advantageous than the others and, if they did, they identified it.

For behavioral data analysis, the IGT is traditionally divided into five blocks of 20 trials, and performance is indexed by the number of advantageous/disadvantageous choices across blocks (e.g., Crone et al., 2004); this allows the IGT learning curve to be observed, where participants initially choose decks A and B, but gradually learn that C and D are preferable.

ECG was continuously monitored during a 10-min pre-task rest (baseline), and during the IGT. Nonpolarizable Ag-AgCl electrodes were attached using Eithoven’s lead II (right mid-clavicle, left ankle and right ankle, ground). ECG activity was digitalized at a 1,000 Hz sampling rate using the Biopac MP 100 device (Biopac Systems, Inc., Goleta, CA, USA). Automatic R-wave detection and artifact correction were performed with ECGlab Matlab software (Carvalho et al., 2002) before the extraction of HR values. Mean HR was calculated across baseline and the IGT using KARDIA Matlab software (Perakakis et al., 2010) and custom Matlab scripts (Matlab 2013a; MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Monetary feedback was delivered immediately after the deck choice. To examine how post-choice monetary feedback modulated cardiac responses, HR changes were assessed every half-second for 6 s (12 bins) with respect to a 1 s pre-feedback baseline. Zero gain trials (n = 48; 1.05% of all trials), which provided no gain or loss, were excluded from the analysis.

Data analysis

For behavioral data, in line with the literature (Bechara & Damasio, 2000; Werner, Duschek & Schandry, 2009) we examined the number of trials across successive blocks in which cognitively inflexible or flexible participants chose advantageous or disadvantageous decks. A mixed-design analysis of variance of the average number of trials was performed, with Deck Type (two levels: disadvantageous vs. advantageous) and Block (five levels: 1–5) as within-participant variables and Cognitive Inflexibility (two levels: low vs. high) as a between-participant variable.

For cardiac data, a mixed ANOVA of average HR changes included Monetary Feedback (two levels: losses vs. wins) and Bins across each trial (12 levels: 1–6 s) as within-participant variables, and Cognitive Inflexibility (two levels: low vs. high) as a between-participant variable. Subsequently, to investigate whether the largest losses or gains prompted distinct cardiac patterns in cognitively inflexible and flexible participants, we examined HR changes after monetary feedback below or above the 10th percentile. An additional mixed ANOVA examined HR changes following the Largest Monetary Feedback (two levels: greatest losses vs. greatest wins; repeated-measures variable), while the other variables, i.e., Bins and Cognitive Inflexibility, remained unchanged.

All statistical analyses were performed using Statistica v.13 (Dell Inc., Round Rock, TX, USA, 2015). The level of significance was set at 0.05; Greenhouse–Geisser adjustment was applied as necessary, and partial η2 was used as a measure of effect size. Based on existing literature, the number of trials was expected to increase across blocks for advantageous decks and monetary gains were expected to prompt larger cardiac accelerations than losses. Accordingly, a priori pairwise comparisons were planned and performed to test specific contrasts. All graphs include 95% CIs, which display the variability around the mean more accurately and clearly than standard errors and standard deviations (Motulsky, 2010).

Results

For behavioral data results, as shown in Fig. 2, the statistically significant two-way interaction between Block and Deck Type [F (2.9,133.5) = 9.5, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.17] indicates the typical learning trend seen during the IGT. Pairwise comparisons revealed that during the first 20 trials (Block 1) participants chose disadvantageous decks more often (p < 0.0001). However, starting from Block 3 (trials 40–60), advantageous decks were preferred (all pairwise comparisons < 0.05), although confidence intervals revealed variability in the participants’ choices. The lack of significant main and interaction effects for Cognitive Inflexibility indicated that it had no impact on the participants’ deck type preference or learning over successive blocks [F (1,43) = 0.370, p = 0.546].

Figure 2. Behavioral data.

Figure 2

IGT learning trend (i.e., the number of choices of advantageous/disadvantageous decks) across the five blocks. Participants gradually learnt to favor advantageous over disadvantageous decks.

For cardiac data, there was a main effect of Monetary Feedback (post-choice information on Losses/Wins) [F (1,44) = 26.4, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.37], indicating larger cardiac accelerations after wins compared to losses. A significant interaction between Monetary Feedback and Bins was found across trials [F (2.7,119.3) = 12.1, p < 0.0001, η2p = 0.22]; pairwise comparisons revealed that 1.5 s after the feedback (from Bin 3), HR was larger following monetary gains compared to losses (pairwise comparisons < 0.01 after 1.5 s; all comparisons < 0.0001 2.5–5.5 s after monetary feedback). The significant main effect of Cognitive Inflexibility [F (1,44) = 5.2, p < 0.05, η2p = 0.11] indicated that cognitively flexible participants had a higher heart rate, regardless of monetary feedback; no further effects reached statistical significance.

In heart rate changes after the Largest Losses/Wins, the main effect of Largest Monetary Feedback [F (1,44) = 20.9, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.32] indicated that the upper 10th percentile of monetary gains prompted larger cardiac accelerations than the greatest losses, confirming the pattern observed following overall Losses/Wins feedback. Similarly, pairwise comparisons of the significant interaction between Largest Monetary Feedback and Bins across trials [F (2.3,99.2) = 8.4, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.16] showed that, 2 s after feedback was provided, HR changes were larger, indicating cardiac accelerations, following the greatest monetary gains (the pairwise comparisons were < 0.01 2 s after the largest Losses/Wins; starting from 3 s after the start of the trial until the end of the trial, all p-values were < 0.001). The significant interaction between Cognitive Inflexibility and the Largest Monetary Feedback [F (1,44) = 5.4, p < 0.05, η2p = 0.11], in Fig. 3, indicated that only cognitively inflexible participants showed larger cardiac accelerations after monetary gains, reflecting greater sensitivity to rewards (p < 0.001); this was not seen in low cognitive inflexibility individuals.

Figure 3. Cardiac data after the Largest Monetary Feedback.

Figure 3

Group differences in heart rate (bpm) were found following the largest monetary gains, suggesting greater sensitivity to rewards in cognitively inflexible participants.

Discussion

The present study examined whether nonclinical individuals with high cognitive inflexibility show poor performance and physiological hyper-reactivity to rewards during a complex decision-making task. The data indicated that, over time, the participants learned to avoid disadvantageous options, thus confirming the behavioral patterns expected in the IGT. However, cognitive inflexibility did not affect performance. When we examined sensitivity to rewards (as indexed by cardiac reactions to IGT wins), we observed that, in line with the literature, monetary gains prompted greater cardiac accelerations than monetary losses (Goudriaan et al., 2006; Miu, Heilman & Houser, 2008; Studer, Scheibehenne & Clark, 2016). Moreover, when comparing reactions to the largest monetary wins and losses, we observed that only cognitively inflexible individuals showed larger cardiac accelerations during the greatest wins. Taken together, the data confirmed the hypothesized role of individual traits in modulating reactions to rewarding cues, where individuals with high cognitive inflexibility showed greater reward sensitivity. However, cognitive inflexibility did not modulate performance on the IGT, as evidenced by the number of disadvantageous choices, therefore indicating a mismatch between behavioral performance and post-choice physiology (which is discussed below).

Cognitive inflexibility, which manifests behaviorally as difficulty switching to an alternative option when a previously rewarding cue leads to negative outcomes, has been identified as a core feature of psychiatric disorders characterized by compulsivity, such as behavioral and substance use disorders (Banca, Harrison & Voon, 2016; Perandrés-Gómez et al., 2021). Recent theories on the development of addictions assign a central role to cognitive inflexibility as a transdiagnostic impairment of higher-order cortical functions (e.g., Parnaudeau, Bolkan & Kellendonk, 2018; Voon et al., 2017) that accompanies compulsive behavior, and might act both as a preexisting trait and a drug-induced deficit (Groman et al., 2019). According to these theories, cognitive flexibility is expected to aid decision-making by promoting efficient alternation between reward-driven (“model-free”) and long-term (“model-based”) strategies (Groman et al., 2019; Voon et al., 2017; Wyckmans et al., 2019). Although this dichotomy has been criticized as too simplistic and narrow given the heterogeneity of decisional strategies (Collins & Cockburn, 2020; Deserno & Hauser, 2020; Gueguen, Schweitzer & Konova, 2021; Vandaele & Ahmed, 2021), both clinical and animal data indicate that individual differences in reward sensitivity play a key role in decisional strategies adopted during pathological reinforcement learning (Fraser & Janak, 2019; Groman et al., 2019). Consistent with this, the nonclinical university students in the present study scoring highly for cognitive inflexibility showed greater cardiac accelerations after the largest wins compared to cognitively flexible participants. Here, it should be noted that greater reward sensitivity has repeatedly been observed in psychiatric and substance use disorders characterized by cognitive inflexibility (Groman et al., 2019; Parnaudeau, Bolkan & Kellendonk, 2018; Perandrés-Gómez et al., 2021; Verdejo-García, Lawrence & Clark, 2008; Wyckmans et al., 2019). By contrast, the current study found an association between higher cognitive inflexibility and heightened reward sensitivity in nonclinical individuals, thus confirming an association between a psychological trait (cognitive inflexibility) and physiological correlate (cardiac hyper-reactivity to rewards) in a sample free from psychopathology.

Concerning the role of sensitivity to reward in the context of cognitive inflexibility, an fMRI study (Gusnard et al., 2003) investigated neural correlates of cognitive inflexibility, as assessed by Cloninger’s TCI Persistence Scale (Cloninger et al., 1994). Cognitive inflexibility was consistently associated with greater (high-cognitive inflexibility individuals) or reduced (low-cognitive-inflexibility individuals) activity in areas of a neurocircuit including the occipital and medial prefrontal cortices and ventral striatum; this circuit is functionally associated with “reward-related activities” and hypothesized to “guide behavior based on contextually relevant incentive-based information” (Gusnard et al., 2003). The current data therefore confirm Gusnard and colleagues’ findings, suggesting that cognitive inflexibility might be associated with greater reward sensitivity, as indexed by central and peripheral physiological markers.

In the IGT, it has been repeatedly observed that monetary gains prompt cardiac accelerations (Crone et al., 2004; Goudriaan et al., 2006). In tasks related or unrelated to gambling, increased heart rate reliably accompanies the presentation of rewarding, pleasant cues to such an extent that cardiac accelerations during emotionally relevant cues are regarded as a reliable index of appetitive motivation, i.e., the evolutionarily adaptive urge to pay sustained attention to motivationally positive cues (Anderson & Brown, 1984; Bradley, Cuthbert & Lang, 1993; Bradley et al., 2001; Greenwald, Cook & Lang, 1989). However, in the Iowa Gambling Task, the processes through which sensitivity to rewards may influence performance are less clear. Bechara & Damasio (2000) hypothesized that, by increasing monetary gains, participants would be more motivated and thus perform better. However, the data indicated that performance did not improve with larger wins (ibidem; Lee et al., 2014). Similarly, in the frequently observed ‘prominent deck B’ phenomenon (Crone et al., 2004; Lin et al., 2007; Visagan, Xiang & Lamar, 2012), a high frequency of wins associated with deck B makes it difficult for participants to resist it despite ‘its bad final outcomes’ (see Visagan, Xiang & Lamar, 2012). Based on the current findings, we hypothesize that high monetary wins and the associated cardiac accelerations might create an overall feeling of pleasure wherein some IGT cues assume greater motivational relevance; this rewarding context might distract some vulnerable individuals from long-term, gradual gains and bias them toward immediate ones that are in fact best avoided. The data presented here therefore seem consistent also with the literature on delayed reward discounting, which indicates that there is a decrease in subjective value of any reward that must be postponed: it is difficult to refrain from selecting immediate rewards (Bari & Robbins, 2013; Bickel et al., 2014; Hamilton & Potenza, 2012; Stevens et al., 2014). As an individual trait, delay discounting has been ‘linked to unfavorable addiction treatment outcomes’ (Stevens et al., 2014) and identified as an endophenotype for multiple psychiatric disorders (Bickel et al., 2019). Further data are needed to elucidate the behavioral and physiological correlates of the traits that render some individuals with high reward sensitivity vulnerable to poor decision-making.

In the current study, the association between subjective cognitive inflexibility and physiological reward sensitivity was not accompanied by worse performance. In line with previous studies (e.g., Crone et al., 2004; Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009; Werner, Duschek & Schandry, 2009), the discrepancy between performance and post-choice physiology seen herein suggests that trial-by-trial rewards take precedence over long-term strategies (Crone et al., 2004). Consistent with this, it has been argued that pathological hypersensitivity to reward might impair decision-making because of biases in motivational relevance rather than failure to acquire relevant information (Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn, 2009). The present data confirm that exaggerated reward sensitivity, as indexed by physiological reactivity, can occur despite adequate information processing, as indexed by performance in the IGT. The observation of discrepancies among self-report, behavioral, and physiological data is not uncommon in emotion research: among fear disorder patients, those who report the most severe symptomatology tend to show reduced physiological reactivity to phobic cues (Lang, McTeague & Bradley, 2016; McTeague & Lang, 2012). Similarly, among nonclinical participants presented with emotionally relevant cues, subjective and behavioral indices frequently diverge from physiological responses (Bradley et al., 2001; Bradley & Lang, 2007). Therefore, the current findings are consistent with research and clinical perspectives, and demonstrate the importance of combining physiological, behavioral, and self-report indices of psychopathology to provide clinicians and researchers with more complete and reliable reference data (Insel & Cuthbert, 2015); the ultimate goal of this approach is to identify vulnerable individuals with respect to a wide spectrum of psychopathologies, and to tailor treatment based on specific biobehavioral correlates (Kypriotakis, Cinciripini & Versace, 2020).

The current findings might help account for previous inconsistencies regarding the role of cognitive inflexibility in addictions: while some data indicate that cognitive inflexibility assists individuals in refraining from maladaptive habits, other data show that its presence constitutes an obstacle to recovery (e.g., Etter, 2010; Kalman et al., 2010; López-Torrecillas et al., 2014). We hypothesize that such discrepancies might originate from a lack of conceptual clarity, given that cognitive (in)flexibility has been regarded as a negative trait reflecting cognitive rigidity and resistance to change (e.g., Abbate-Daga et al., 2014; López-Torrecillas et al., 2014; Perandrés-Gómez et al., 2021), as well as a positive trait akin to endurance (i.e., ‘grit’ in the face of adversity; Cloninger et al., 1994; Etter, 2010; Kalman et al., 2010). Indeed, the more recent TCI-R takes a wider view of the construct of cognitive inflexibility, such that it can have both positive and negative outcomes (Josefsson et al., 2013). The data reported here suggest that sensitivity to rewards might predispose cognitively inflexible individuals to suboptimal decision-making. Thus, the findings imply that cognitive inflexibility hinders rather than assists individuals in terms of abstaining from maladaptive habits. The generalizability of the results, however, would be improved by studies refining the concept of cognitive inflexibility, and subsequently validating it.

Conclusions

In summary, cognitive inflexibility and sensitivity to rewards are common in compulsive disorders. We examined whether, in a nonclinical sample, cognitive inflexibility was similarly associated with worse decision-making or increased appetitive reactions to wins. Cognitive inflexibility did not alter performance; however, cognitively inflexible participants showed large cardiac accelerations during the largest wins. Cardiac accelerations, as a reliable biological marker of appetitive motivation, suggest that greater sensitivity to rewards might render cognitively inflexible individuals more vulnerable to the largest gains, thereby predisposing them to inadequate decision-making. The findings, in line with recent research and clinical perspectives, demonstrate the relevance of incorporating physiological, behavioral, and self-report markers/indices of psychopathology, to identify vulnerable individuals and tailor treatment based on specific biobehavioral correlates.

Supplemental Information

Supplemental Information 1. Raw Data for Figure 1.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.15318/supp-1
Supplemental Information 2. Raw Data for Figure 2.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.15318/supp-2

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Michael Irvine for his English editing skills and professor Jose Alfonso Gutiérrez-Zotes for suggestions on earlier versions of the draft.

Funding Statement

The study was supported by grants from the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia to Francisca López-Torrecillas (Grant Code SEJ2006-08390) and from the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades to Azahara Leonor Miranda (Grant Code FPU18/01508). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Additional Information and Declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Author Contributions

José Luis Mata conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, and approved the final draft.

Azahara Leonor Miranda Gálvez performed the experiments, analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, and approved the final draft.

Francisca López Torrecillas conceived and designed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, and approved the final draft.

Laura Miccoli analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, and approved the final draft.

Human Ethics

The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):

The Research Ethics Commitee of the University of Granada granted Ethical approval to carry out the study (2994/CEIH/2022).

Data Availability

The following information was supplied regarding data availability:

The raw data are available in the Supplemental Files.

References

  • Abbate-Daga et al. (2014).Abbate-Daga G, Buzzichelli S, Marzola E, Amianto F, Fassino S. Clinical investigation of set-shifting subtypes in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research. 2014;219(3):592–597. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson & Brown (1984).Anderson G, Brown RI. Real and laboratory gambling, sensation-seeking and arousal. British Journal of Psychology. 1984;75(3):401–410. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1984.tb01910.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Banca, Harrison & Voon (2016).Banca P, Harrison NA, Voon V. Compulsivity across the pathological misuse of drug and non-drug rewards. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2016;10(AUG):20. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bari & Robbins (2013).Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology. 2013;108(Pt 11):44–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bechara & Damasio (2000).Bechara A, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesion. Brain. 2000;123(11):2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bechara et al. (1994).Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50(1–3):7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bechara et al. (1997).Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275(5304):1293–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bechara, Dolan & Hindes (2002).Bechara A, Dolan S, Hindes A. Decision-making and addiction (part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(10):1690–1705. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00016-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bechara et al. (1996).Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 1996;6(2):215–225. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bell & Polaschek (2017).Bell RK, Polaschek DLL. Do high-risk prisoners entering treatment have clinically impaired cognitive impulse control? Psychiatry, Psychology and Law. 2017;24(4):576–593. doi: 10.1080/13218719.2016.1256016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bickel et al. (2019).Bickel WK, Athamneh LN, Basso JC, Mellis AM, DeHart WB, Craft WH, Pope D. Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process: update on the state of the science. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2019;30(Suppl 1):59–64. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bickel et al. (2014).Bickel WK, Johnson MW, Koffarnus MN, MacKillop J, Murphy JG. The behavioral economics of substance use disorders: reinforcement pathologies and their repair. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2014;10(1):641–677. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bradley et al. (2001).Bradley MM, Codispoti M, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ. Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion. 2001;1(3):276–298. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bradley, Cuthbert & Lang (1993).Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ. Pictures as prepulse: attention and emotion in startle modification. Psychophysiology. 1993;30(5):541–545. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02079.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bradley & Lang (2007).Bradley MM, Lang PJ. The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention. In: Coan JA, Allen JJB, editors. Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 29–46. [Google Scholar]
  • Bull, Tippett & Addis (2015).Bull PN, Tippett LJ, Addis DR. Decision making in nonclinical participants on the Iowa Gambling Task: new insights from an operant approach. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:391. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carvalho et al. (2002).Carvalho JL, da Rocha AF, de Oliveira Nascimento FA, Neto JS, Junqueira LF. Development of a Matlab software for analysis of heart rate variability. 6th International Conference on Signal Processing. 2002;2:1488–1491. doi: 10.1109/ICOSP.2002.1180076. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Cloninger (1999).Cloninger CR. The temperament and character inventory-revised. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • Cloninger et al. (1994).Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM, Wetzel RD. The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): a guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • Collins & Cockburn (2020).Collins AGE, Cockburn J. Beyond dichotomies in reinforcement learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2020;21(10):576–586. doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-0355-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Crone et al. (2004).Crone EA, Somsen RJM, van Beek B, van der Molen MW. Heart rate and skin conductance analysis of antecedents and consequences of decision making. Psychophysiology. 2004;41(4):531–540. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00197.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Deserno & Hauser (2020).Deserno L, Hauser TU. Beyond a cognitive dichotomy: can multiple decision systems prove useful to distinguish compulsive and impulsive symptom dimensions? Biological Psychiatry. 2020;88(10):e49–e51. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Etter (2010).Etter JF. Smoking and cloninger’s temperament and character inventory. Nicotine & Tobacco Research: Official Journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco. 2010;12(9):919–926. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntq116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Faul et al. (2007).Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39(2):175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fraser & Janak (2019).Fraser KM, Janak PH. How does drug use shift the balance between model-based and model-free control of decision making? Biological Psychiatry. 2019;85(11):886–888. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Goudriaan et al. (2006).Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, van den Brink W. Psychophysiological determinants and concomitants of deficient decision making in pathological gamblers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2006;84(3):231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Greenwald, Cook & Lang (1989).Greenwald MK, Cook EW, Lang PJ. Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology. 1989;3:51–64. [Google Scholar]
  • Groman et al. (2019).Groman SM, Keistler C, Keip AJ, Hammarlund E, DiLeone RJ, Pittenger C, Lee D, Taylor JR. Orbitofrontal circuits control multiple reinforcement-learning processes. Neuron. 2019;103(4):734–746.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gueguen, Schweitzer & Konova (2021).Gueguen M, Schweitzer EM, Konova AB. Computational theory-driven studies of reinforcement learning and decision-making in addiction: what have we learned? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2021;38:40–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gusnard et al. (2003).Gusnard DA, Ollinger JM, Shulman GL, Cloninger CR, Price JL, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. Persistence and brain circuitry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2003;100(6):3479–3484. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0538050100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gutiérrez-Zotes et al. (2004).Gutiérrez-Zotes JA, Bayón C, Montserrat C, Valero J, Labad A, Cloninger CR, Fernández-Aranda F. Inventario del Temperamento y el Carácter-Revisado (TCI-R). Baremación y datos normativos en una muestra de población general [Temperament and character inventory revised (TCI-R). Standardization and normative data in a general population sample] Actas Españolas de Psiquiatría. 2004;32(1):8–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hamilton & Potenza (2012).Hamilton KR, Potenza MN. Relations among delay discounting, addictions, and money mismanagement: implications and future directions. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2012;38(1):30–42. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2011.643978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Herman, Esposito & Tsakiris (2021).Herman AM, Esposito G, Tsakiris M. Body in the face of uncertainty: the role of autonomic arousal and interoception in decision-making under risk and ambiguity. Psychophysiology. 2021;58(8):468. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Insel & Cuthbert (2015).Insel TR, Cuthbert BN. Brain disorders? Precisely. Science. 2015;348(6234):499–500. doi: 10.1126/science.aab2358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Josefsson et al. (2013).Josefsson K, Jokela M, Cloninger CR, Hintsanen M, Salo J, Hintsa T, Pulkki-Råback L, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Maturity and change in personality: developmental trends of temperament and character in adulthood. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(3):713–727. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kalman et al. (2010).Kalman D, Hoskinson R, Sambamoorthi U, Garvey AJ. A prospective study of persistence in the prediction of smoking cessation outcome: results from a randomized clinical trial. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35(2):179–182. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kypriotakis, Cinciripini & Versace (2020).Kypriotakis G, Cinciripini PM, Versace F. Modeling neuroaffective biomarkers of drug addiction: a Bayesian nonparametric approach using dirichlet process mixtures. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2020;341(1):108753. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2020.108753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lang, McTeague & Bradley (2016).Lang PJ, McTeague LM, Bradley MM. RDoC, DSM, and the reflex physiology of fear: a biodimensional analysis of the anxiety disorders spectrum. Psychophysiology. 2016;53(3):336–347. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lee et al. (2014).Lee WK, Su YA, Song TJ, Chiu YC, Lin CH. Are normal decision-makers sensitive to changes in value contrast under uncertainty? Evidence from the Iowa Gambling TAsk. PLOS ONE. 2014;9(7):e101878. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lin et al. (2007).Lin C-H, Chiu Y-C, Lee P-L, Hsieh J-C. Is deck B a disadvantageous deck in the Iowa Gambling Task? Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2007;3(1):16. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • López-Torrecillas et al. (2014).López-Torrecillas F, Perales JC, Nieto-Ruiz A, Verdejo-García A. Temperament and impulsivity predictors of smoking cessation outcomes. PLOS ONE. 2014;9(12):e112440. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McTeague & Lang (2012).McTeague LM, Lang PJ. The anxiety spectrum and the reflex physiology of defense: from circumscribed fear to broad distress. Depression and Anxiety. 2012;29(4):264–281. doi: 10.1002/da.21891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Miu, Heilman & Houser (2008).Miu AC, Heilman RM, Houser D. Anxiety impairs decision-making: psychophysiological evidence from an Iowa Gambling Task. Biological Psychology. 2008;77(3):353–358. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Motulsky (2010).Motulsky H. Intuitive biostatistics: a nonmathematical guide to statistical thinking. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • Parnaudeau, Bolkan & Kellendonk (2018).Parnaudeau S, Bolkan SS, Kellendonk C. The mediodorsal thalamus: an essential partner of the prefrontal cortex for cognition. Biological Psychiatry. 2018;83(8):648–656. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Perakakis et al. (2010).Perakakis P, Joffily M, Taylor M, Guerra P, Vila J. KARDIA: a Matlab software for the analysis of cardiac interbeat intervals. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine. 2010;98(1):83–89. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2009.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Perandrés-Gómez et al. (2021).Perandrés-Gómez A, Navas JF, van Timmeren T, Perales JC. Decision-making (in)flexibility in gambling disorder. Addictive Behaviors. 2021;112(1):106534. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rivalan, Ahmed & Dellu-Hagedorn (2009).Rivalan M, Ahmed SH, Dellu-Hagedorn F. Risk-prone individuals prefer the wrong options on a rat version of the Iowa Gambling Task. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66(8):743–749. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schultz & Searleman (2002).Schultz PW, Searleman A. Rigidity of thought and behavior: 100 years of research. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs. 2002;128(2):165–207. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sebold et al. (2017).Sebold M, Nebe S, Garbusow M, Guggenmos M, Schad DJ, Beck A, Kuitunen-Paul S, Sommer C, Frank R, Neu P, Zimmermann US, Rapp MA, Smolka MN, Huys QJM, Schlagenhauf F, Heinz A. When habits are dangerous: alcohol expectancies and habitual decision making predict relapse in alcohol dependence. Biological Psychiatry. 2017;82(11):847–856. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Simonovic et al. (2019).Simonovic B, Stupple E, Gale M, Sheffield D. Sweating the small stuff: a meta-analysis of skin conductance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;19(5):1097–1112. doi: 10.3758/s13415-019-00744-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Steingroever et al. (2013).Steingroever H, Wetzels R, Horstmann A, Neumann J, Wagenmakers EJ. Performance of nonclinical participants on the Iowa Gambling Task. Psychological Assessment. 2013;25(1):180–193. doi: 10.1037/a0029929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Stevens et al. (2014).Stevens L, Verdejo-García A, Goudriaan AE, Roeyers H, Dom G, Vanderplasschen W. Impulsivity as a vulnerability factor for poor addiction treatment outcomes: a review of neurocognitive findings among individuals with substance use disorders. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2014;47(1):58–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2014.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Studer, Scheibehenne & Clark (2016).Studer B, Scheibehenne B, Clark L. Psychophysiological arousal and inter- and intraindividual differences in risk-sensitive decision making. Psychophysiology. 2016;53(6):940–950. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Taatgen et al. (2008).Taatgen NA, Huss D, Dickison D, Anderson JR. The acquisition of robust and flexible cognitive skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2008;137(3):548–565. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.3.548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vandaele & Ahmed (2021).Vandaele Y, Ahmed SH. Habit, choice, and addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2021;46(4):689–698. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00899-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Verdejo-García, Lawrence & Clark (2008).Verdejo-García A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32(4):777–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Visagan, Xiang & Lamar (2012).Visagan R, Xiang A, Lamar M. Comparison of deck-and trial-based approaches to advantageous decision making on the Iowa Gambling Task. Psychological Assessment. 2012;24(2):455–463. doi: 10.1037/a0025932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Voon et al. (2017).Voon V, Reiter A, Sebold M, Groman S. Model-based control in dimensional psychiatry. Biological Psychiatry. 2017;82(6):391–400. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Werner, Duschek & Schandry (2009).Werner NS, Duschek S, Schandry R. Relationships between affective states and decision-making. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2009;74(3):259–265. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wyckmans et al. (2019).Wyckmans F, Otto AR, Sebold M, Daw N, Bechara A, Saeremans M, Kornreich C, Chatard A, Jaafari N, Noël X. Reduced model-based decision-making in gambling disorder. Scientific Reports. 2019;9(1):1458. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56161-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Information 1. Raw Data for Figure 1.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.15318/supp-1
Supplemental Information 2. Raw Data for Figure 2.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.15318/supp-2

Data Availability Statement

The following information was supplied regarding data availability:

The raw data are available in the Supplemental Files.


Articles from PeerJ are provided here courtesy of PeerJ, Inc

RESOURCES