Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2023 May 1;120(19):e2218023120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2218023120

Differential processing of a chemosensory cue across life stages sharing the same valence state in Caenorhabditis elegans

Navonil Banerjee a,b, Pei-Yin Shih c,1, Elisa J Rojas Palato a, Paul W Sternberg c,2, Elissa A Hallem a,b,2
PMCID: PMC10175840  PMID: 37126715

Significance

Animals frequently show the same preference toward a chemosensory cue under widely varying external and internal conditions. Whether such chemosensory cues involve similar neural mechanisms across conditions is unclear. Here, we show that carbon dioxide (CO2) is processed through distinct neural mechanisms in C. elegans at two different life stages that show the same preference for CO2. These mechanistic differences are manifested in altered CO2-evoked neuronal activity and motor output. A life stage–specific change in neural connectivity and insulin signaling contribute to these circuit differences by modulating the functional properties of an interneuron. We demonstrate that distinct neural mechanisms may underlie the same preference for a chemosensory cue and highlight the importance of physiological context in understanding chemosensory behaviors.

Keywords: behavior, neural circuits, chemotaxis, carbon dioxide, dauer larva

Abstract

Many chemosensory cues evoke responses of the same valence under widely varying physiological conditions. It remains unclear whether similar or distinct neural mechanisms are involved in the detection and processing of such chemosensory cues across contexts. We show that in Caenorhabditis elegans, a chemosensory cue is processed by distinct neural mechanisms at two different life stages that share the same valence state. Both starved adults and dauer larvae are attracted to carbon dioxide (CO2), but CO2 evokes different patterns of neural activity and different motor outputs at the two life stages. Moreover, the same interneuron within the CO2 microcircuit plays a different role in driving CO2-evoked motor output at the two life stages. The dauer-specific patterns of CO2-evoked activity in this interneuron require a dauer-specific gap junction complex and insulin signaling. Our results demonstrate that functionally distinct microcircuits are engaged in response to a chemosensory cue that triggers the same valence state at different life stages, revealing an unexpected complexity to chemosensory processing.


Chemosensation is crucial for animals to successfully navigate their environments and accomplish essential goal-directed behaviors such as locating food, searching for mates, and escaping predators (1). As a result, many chemosensory behaviors and their underlying mechanisms are highly flexible and can be modulated by an animal’s internal physiological state (27). For example, the same chemosensory cue can evoke distinct brain-wide activity dynamics in thirsty vs. sated mice (8) and distinct neural activity in fed vs. starved Drosophila larvae (9). In some cases, changes in internal physiological state result in a switch in chemosensory valence, i.e., whether the chemosensory cue is perceived as attractive or aversive (1013). In contrast, other chemosensory cues can evoke responses of the same valence under very different physiological conditions (1416). How the same valence state is maintained given the constraints posed by changes in internal physiology on chemosensory processing remains poorly understood.

We explore these mechanisms using the chemosensory responses of Caenorhabditis elegans to carbon dioxide (CO2). C. elegans has a small nervous system with a well-characterized connectome (1719). In addition, C. elegans responds robustly to a diverse array of chemosensory cues, including CO2 (20). CO2 is an ambiguous cue for C. elegans, as elevated CO2 levels in its natural habitat may signal food, predators, pathogens, or conspecifics (20, 21). Accordingly, C. elegans shows flexible responses to CO2 such that CO2 can be either attractive or repulsive depending on immediate context, prior experience, and life stage (2228). For example, while well-fed C. elegans adults are repelled by CO2, starvation results in a shift in CO2 response valence such that starved adults are attracted to CO2 (22, 23, 28).

Under adverse environmental conditions such as absence of food, high temperature, and overcrowding, C. elegans enters the developmentally arrested dauer larval stage (29, 30). Dauer entry is accompanied by a dramatic reprogramming of internal physiology that promotes developmental arrest and prolonged survival under unfavorable conditions (31). Like starved adults, dauer larvae are robustly attracted to CO2 despite their dramatically different physiology (24). Although the neural mechanisms responsible for the detection and processing of CO2 have been partly elucidated in starved adults (28), the dauer CO2 circuit had not been investigated.

Here, we show that distinct neural mechanisms are involved in the detection and processing of CO2 in dauers and starved adults. At a circuit level, we observe differences in the functional properties of the CO2-detecting BAG neurons as well as downstream interneurons. The BAG sensory neurons show reduced CO2-evoked calcium responses in dauers compared to starved adults. In addition, the RIG, AIB, AVE, and AIY interneurons respond differently to CO2 at the two life stages. A dauer-specific gap junction complex and insulin signaling contribute to the dauer-specific response properties of the AIB interneurons. Differences in the functional CO2 microcircuit are reflected in distinct locomotory patterns that are triggered by acute CO2 exposure at the two life stages. In addition, the AIB interneurons have opposing effects on CO2-evoked movement in starved adults vs. dauers: AIB promotes CO2-evoked reversals in starved adults but inhibits CO2-evoked reversals in dauers. Together, our findings illustrate that functionally distinct microcircuits are engaged by a chemosensory cue at two different life stages that share the same valence state, highlighting the importance of physiological context in understanding chemosensory behaviors.

Results

The BAG Neurons of Dauer Larvae Show Reduced CO2-Evoked Activity.

Well-fed C. elegans adults are repelled by CO2, whereas both starved adults and dauer larvae are attracted to CO2 (Fig. 1A) (24, 27, 28). Does the altered physiology of dauers modify the functional properties of neurons within the CO2 microcircuit? To address this question, we first monitored the CO2-evoked calcium activity of the primary CO2-detecting BAG neurons, which are required for CO2 attraction in both starved adults and dauers (22, 24, 28, 32). Using the genetically encoded ratiometric calcium indicator yellow cameleon YC3.60, we found that the BAG neurons of both starved adults and dauers are activated by CO2 (Fig. 1 BD). However, the BAG neurons of dauers show a reduced response to CO2 relative to that of starved adults (Fig. 1C). Thus, functional differences in the CO2 microcircuit between starved adults and dauers occur at the sensory neuron level.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

The BAG sensory neurons show reduced calcium responses in dauers. (A) Well-fed adults are repelled by CO2, whereas starved adults and dauers are attracted to CO2. n = 16 to 18 trials per life stage and condition. ****P < 0.0001, ns = not significant (> 0.9999), Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn’s posttest. (B) Calcium responses in the BAG neurons of starved adults and dauers. Solid lines indicate mean calcium responses and shadings indicate SEM. Gray bars indicate timing and duration of the CO2 pulse. Calcium responses were measured using the ratiometric calcium indicator yellow cameleon YC3.60. Responses are to 15% CO2. (C) Quantification of the maximum responses of BAG in starved adults and dauers. Each data point represents the response of a single animal. Solid lines in violin plots show medians and dotted lines show interquartile ranges. **P < 0.01, Welch’s t test. n = 14 to 16 animals per life stage. Responses are to 15% CO2. (D) Heatmaps of BAG calcium responses. Each row represents the response of an individual animal. Response magnitudes in the heatmaps are color-coded according to the scale (% ΔR/R0) shown to the right. Responses are ordered by hierarchical cluster analysis. Gray bars indicate the timing and duration of the CO2 pulse.

To test whether the reduced CO2-evoked calcium activity of BAG in dauers results from decreased expression of the putative CO2 receptor GCY-9 (32, 33), we used a strain expressing green fluorescent protein (GFP) under the control of the gcy-9 promoter and quantified fluorescent intensities in BAG neuron cell bodies at the two life stages. We did not observe a noticeable difference in GFP expression in the BAG cell bodies of starved adults vs. dauers (SI Appendix, Fig. S1 A and B), suggesting that the reduced response of dauer BAG neurons does not reflect decreased CO2 receptor expression. It also does not reflect altered expression of the calcium indicator (SI Appendix, Fig. S1 C and D). The reduced response of dauer BAG neurons to CO2 could result from reduced diffusion of CO2 through the thicker cuticle of dauers and/or reduced sensitivity of dauer BAG neurons through a mechanism independent of gcy-9 expression.

CO2 Microcircuit Interneurons Show Distinct Patterns of CO2-Evoked Activity in Dauers.

We next investigated the CO2-evoked neural activity of four interneurons that are postsynaptic to BAG: RIG, AIY, AIB, and AVE (1719). RIG was previously reported to undergo starvation-induced changes in CO2-evoked activity in adults––it displays excitatory CO2-evoked activity in well-fed adults but no activity in starved adults (27, 28). We monitored CO2-evoked calcium responses in dauers and found that the RIG neurons of dauers show excitatory responses that are similar to those of the RIG neurons of well-fed adults, even though well-fed adults and dauers show CO2 responses of opposite valence (Fig. 2 AC). However, in dauers but not well-fed adults, the excitatory response in RIG was followed by an inhibitory response that initiated a few seconds after the termination of the CO2 pulse (Fig. 2 A, B, and D). As in well-fed adults (27), CO2-evoked responses in RIG were eliminated in dauers where the BAG neurons were genetically ablated (SI Appendix, Fig. S2 AC), indicating that the CO2-evoked activity in dauer RIG neurons is dependent on sensory input from BAG. Thus, differences in chemosensory processing between starved adults and dauers are reflected at the level of RIG activity.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

The RIG and AIY interneurons respond similarly to CO2 in well-fed adults and dauers. (A) CO2-evoked calcium responses of RIG in well-fed adults and dauers. Solid lines represent mean calcium responses; shading represents SEM. Gray bars indicate the timing and duration of the CO2 pulse. Calcium responses were measured using the ratiometric calcium indicator yellow cameleon YC3.60. Responses are to 15% CO2. (B) Heatmaps of RIG calcium responses. Each row represents the response of an individual animal. Gray bars indicate the timing and duration of the CO2 pulse. Response magnitudes are color-coded according to the scale (% ΔR/R0) shown to the right. Responses are ordered by hierarchical cluster analysis. (C and D) Quantification of the maximum (C) and minimum (D) responses of RIG in well-fed adults and dauers. ****< 0.0001, Mann–Whitney test. ns = not significant (P = 0.8989), Welch’s t test. Each data point represents the response of a single animal. Solid lines in violin plots show medians and dotted lines show interquartile ranges. For AD, n = 14 to 15 animals per life stage. (E) CO2-evoked calcium responses of AIY in well-fed adults and dauers. Conventions and conditions are as in panel A. (F) Heatmaps of AIY calcium responses. Conventions and conditions are as in panel B. (G) Quantification of the minimum responses of AIY in well-fed adults and dauers. ns = not significant (P = 0.7377), Mann–Whitney test. Conventions are as in panel C. For EG, n = 11 to 14 animals per life stage.

We then examined the CO2-evoked calcium responses of the AIY interneurons. Whereas AIY neurons in well-fed adults are inhibited by CO2, they show stochastic responses to CO2 in starved adults such that roughly equal proportions of animals display excitatory and inhibitory activities in AIY (27, 28). We found that the AIY neurons of dauers show inhibitory calcium responses to CO2 that are indistinguishable from those of well-fed adults (Fig. 2 EG). To confirm that the inhibitory activity displayed by AIY in dauers was evoked by CO2, we measured AIY activity in response to an air control, where the CO2 pulse was replaced with an air pulse of equivalent duration. We found that the responses of AIY in both fed adults and dauers to CO2 were significantly different from the responses to the air control, confirming that the inhibitory responses were evoked by CO2 (SI Appendix, Fig. S2 DF). Thus, like RIG, AIY shows distinct CO2-evoked calcium activity in starved adults vs. dauers.

In the case of AIB, we found that the interneuron shows small, infrequent responses to CO2 in both well-fed and starved adults (Fig. 3 AC). In contrast, the AIB interneurons of dauers show robust excitatory calcium responses to CO2; these responses were observed in over 60% of the imaged animals (Fig. 3 AC). The maximum peak amplitudes of these excitatory responses were significantly higher than those observed in adults (Fig. 3D). In addition, the AIB responses of dauers to CO2 vs. air were significantly different (SI Appendix, Fig. S3 AC), confirming that the excitatory calcium responses of AIB in dauers are evoked by CO2. Finally, we found that the AVE interneurons are predominantly inhibited by CO2 in dauers; roughly 45% of dauers tested showed inhibitory responses in AVE (Fig. 3 EH). None of the adults tested showed inhibitory responses, suggesting that AVE is inhibited specifically in dauers (Fig. 3G). Moreover, the minimum peak amplitudes of the CO2-evoked calcium responses were significantly lower in dauers relative to adults (Fig. 3H). The AVE responses we observed in dauers were significantly different between CO2 and air controls (SI Appendix, Fig. S4), indicating that they were evoked by CO2. Thus, the AIB and AVE interneurons appear to participate more reliably in the CO2 microcircuit of dauers than adults. Together, our findings demonstrate functional divergence in CO2-processing mechanisms at the interneuron level between starved adults and dauers.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

The AIB and AVE interneurons show distinct CO2-evoked calcium responses in dauers. (A) The AIB interneurons show robust excitatory activity in dauers. CO2-evoked calcium responses of AIB in well-fed adults, starved adults, and dauers. Solid lines represent mean calcium responses; shading represents SEM. Gray bars indicate the timing and duration of the CO2 pulse. Calcium responses were measured using the ratiometric calcium indicator yellow cameleon YC3.60. Responses are to 15% CO2. (B) Heatmaps of AIB calcium responses. Each row represents the response of an individual animal. Response magnitudes in the heatmaps are color-coded according to the scale (% ΔR/R0) shown to the right. Responses are ordered by hierarchical cluster analysis. Gray bars indicate timing and duration of the CO2 pulse. Calcium responses were measured using the ratiometric calcium indicator yellow cameleon YC3.60. Responses are to 15% CO2. (C) Categorical plot displaying the percentage of excitatory, inhibitory, and silent responses in AIB across life stages and conditions. Responses were categorized as either excitatory or inhibitory if the absolute value of the response exceeded 3 SDs of the response to an air control (28). *< 0.05 for excitatory responses of dauers vs. starved adults, Fisher’s exact test. (D) Quantification of the maximum responses of AIB in well-fed adults, starved adults, and dauers. **< 0.01, ns = not significant (> 0.9999), Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn’s posttest. For AD, n = 13 to 17 animals per life stage and condition. (E) The AVE interneurons are primarily inhibited by CO2 in dauers. Calcium responses in the AVE neurons of well-fed adults, starved adults, and dauers. Conventions and conditions are as in panel A. (F) Heatmaps of AVE calcium responses. Conventions and conditions are as in panel B. (G) Categorical plot displaying the percentage of excitatory, inhibitory, and silent responses in AVE across life stages and conditions. Responses were categorized as in panel C. **< 0.01 for inhibitory responses of dauers vs. starved adults, Fisher’s exact test. (H) Quantification of the minimum responses of AVE in well-fed adults, starved adults, and dauers. ***< 0.001, ns = not significant (P = 0.9813), one-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s posttest. For EH, n = 13 to 16 animals per life stage and condition. For D and H, each data point represents the response of a single animal. Solid lines in violin plots show medians and dotted lines show interquartile ranges.

The Dauer-Specific Responses of AIB Require Gap Junctions and Insulin Signaling.

What are the molecular mechanisms that contribute to the dauer-specific response properties of the CO2 microcircuit? To address this question, we focused on the AIB neurons, which respond robustly and consistently to CO2 in dauers but infrequently in starved adults (Fig. 3). A previous study found that AIB and BAG form gap junctions consisting of the subunits CHE-7 and INX-6 specifically in dauers (34). To test whether the excitatory CO2-evoked calcium responses in AIB in dauers arise due to dauer-specific gap junctions, we monitored CO2-evoked calcium activity in AIB in che-7 mutant dauers. We found that the strong excitatory calcium responses were almost entirely absent in che-7 dauers (Fig. 4 AC), suggesting that the CO2-evoked excitatory response in dauers is dependent on the BAG–AIB electrical synapse.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

CO2-evoked activity in AIB is dependent on a BAG–AIB gap junction complex and insulin signaling. (A) Excitatory CO2-evoked calcium responses in AIB are largely eliminated in che-7(ok2373) mutant dauers. Calcium responses of the AIB neurons in wild-type dauers and che-7(ok2373) mutant dauers to CO2. Solid lines indicate mean calcium responses, and shading indicates SEM. Gray bars indicate timing and duration of the CO2 pulse. Calcium responses were measured using the ratiometric calcium indicator yellow cameleon YC3.60. Responses are to 15% CO2. (B) Heatmaps of AIB calcium responses. Each row represents the response of an individual animal. Gray bars indicate the timing and duration of the CO2 pulse. Response magnitudes in the heatmaps are color-coded according to the scale (% ΔR/R0) shown to the right. Responses are ordered by hierarchical cluster analysis. (C) Quantification of the maximum responses of AIB in wild-type and che-7(ok2373) dauers to CO2. Each data point represents the response of a single animal. Solid lines in violin plots show medians and dotted lines show interquartile ranges. **< 0.01, Welch’s t test. n = 15 to 17 animals per genotype. (D) CO2-evoked calcium responses of the AIB neurons of wild-type and daf-2(e1370) mutant dauers. Conditions and conventions are as in panel A. (E) Heatmaps of AIB calcium responses. Conditions and conventions are as in panel B. (F) Quantification of the maximum responses of AIB in wild-type and daf-2(e1370) mutant dauers. **< 0.01, Mann–Whitney test. n = 17 to 20 animals per genotype. Conventions are as in panel C.

We next sought to identify additional mechanisms that contribute to the dauer-specific response properties of the CO2 microcircuit. The insulin pathway plays an important role in regulating the developmental decision to enter the dauer state, and the altered physiology of dauers has been associated with changes in insulin signaling (30). We therefore tested whether insulin signaling also regulates the CO2 microcircuit of dauers. We found that the excitatory CO2-evoked calcium responses of AIB in wild-type dauers were largely eliminated in dauers lacking daf-2, which encodes the sole C. elegans homolog of the mammalian insulin/IGF receptor (30) (Fig. 4 DF). Thus, AIB activity in dauers is dependent on insulin signaling. To determine whether the absence of CO2-evoked AIB activity in daf-2 mutant dauers is due to a general physiological effect of the loss of insulin signaling, we monitored CO2-evoked activity in dauer RIG neurons. We found that the RIG neurons of daf-2 mutant dauers showed normal CO2-evoked excitatory activity (SI Appendix, Fig. S5 AC), indicating that daf-2 specifically regulates the CO2-evoked activity of AIB. Since we did not observe detectable daf-2 expression in AIB in dauers (SI Appendix, Fig. S6), daf-2 likely functions cell-non-autonomously to modulate AIB activity. Moreover, since BAG–AIB gap junctions are present in daf-2 dauers (34), daf-2 does not modulate AIB activity through regulation of the BAG–AIB gap junction. Together, our results indicate that both changes in the electrical connectome and insulin signaling shape CO2 processing in dauers.

Distinct Motor Programs Are Evoked by CO2 in Starved Adults and Dauers.

Do the distinct functional properties of interneurons in dauers vs. starved adults affect CO2-evoked motor output at the two life stages? To address this question, we exposed animals to an acute CO2 pulse and video-recorded their movement (SI Appendix, Fig. S7 A and B) (35). We then tracked movement trajectories and quantified movement parameters (SI Appendix, Fig. S7 C and D). We found that starved adults exposed to CO2 reduced their speed for the first ~20 to 25 s of the CO2 pulse, after which they resumed movement at their prestimulus speed (Fig. 5A). This decline in speed was specific to CO2 since it did not occur when animals were exposed to an air pulse of equivalent duration (Fig. 5A). When dauers were exposed to the same CO2 pulse, they drastically reduced their speed for almost the entire duration of the CO2 pulse (Fig. 5B). This sharp decline in speed was also evoked by CO2 since it did not occur in response to an air pulse (Fig. 5B). The effect of CO2 on speed reduction was reversible since dauers resumed movement upon termination of the CO2 pulse (SI Appendix, Fig. S7C). We then compared the mean speeds of starved adults and dauers during the first 20 s following the start of the CO2 pulse, and during a later 20-s time window starting 30 s after the onset of the CO2 pulse. We found that the mean speed of starved adults was significantly reduced during the first 20 s of the CO2 pulse but not the later 20-s time window (Fig. 5C). In contrast, the mean speed of dauers was reduced during both time windows (Fig. 5D). In addition, whereas starved adults traveled a similar straight-line distance in response to CO2 vs. air, dauers traveled significantly less distance when exposed to CO2 (SI Appendix, Fig. S7D). Thus, CO2 exposure stimulates a more prolonged decrease in speed in dauers than starved adults.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Distinct motor outputs are evoked by CO2 in starved adults vs. dauers (A and B) Changes in instantaneous speed of movement in starved adults (A) and dauers (B) in response to CO2. Black lines represent mean instantaneous speeds of animals exposed to a 20-s pulse of air (21% O2, balance N2) followed by a 60-s pulse of air, whereas blue lines represent mean instantaneous speeds of animals exposed to a 20-s pulse of air followed by a 60-s pulse of CO2 (2.5% CO2, 21% O2, balance N2). Shadings represent SEM. Yellow shaded boxes represent the timing and duration of the CO2 or air pulse. (C and D) Starved adults show a decrease in their mean speed during the first 20 s of CO2 exposure (C), whereas dauers show a decrease in their mean speed for the duration of CO2 exposure (D). (E) Both starved adults and dauers show reduced forward movement in response to CO2, but this effect is more pronounced for dauers. (F) Both starved adults and dauers show an increase in backward movement in response to CO2, but this effect is more pronounced for starved adults. (G) Both starved adults and dauers show an increased pause duration in response to CO2, but this effect is more pronounced in dauers. For AG, n = 15 to 19 animals per life stage and condition. For CG, each data point represents the response of a single animal. Solid lines in violin plots show medians and dotted lines show interquartile ranges. ****P < 0.0001, **< 0.01, *< 0.05, ns = not significant (= 0.8028), two-way ANOVA with Sidak’s posttest.

We also quantified CO2-evoked changes in the directionality of movement for the two life stages. Both starved adults and dauers showed a significant reduction in the duration of forward movement in response to CO2 (Fig. 5E). However, forward movement duration was more strongly reduced in dauers than starved adults (Fig. 5E). Reverse movement duration increased in response to CO2 for both life stages, although the increase was less pronounced in dauers than starved adults (Fig. 5F). In addition, whereas CO2 stimulated an increase in pause time for both life stages, dauers paused for significantly longer than adults (Fig. 5G). Together, these results indicate that CO2 evokes distinct motor outputs in dauers vs. starved adults and is consistent with distinct CO2-evoked neural activity patterns across the two life stages.

AIB Differentially Regulates CO2-Evoked Motor Output in Starved Adults vs. Dauers.

We next investigated the role of AIB in driving CO2-evoked motor output in dauers vs. starved adults. We first confirmed that the promoter (npr-9) used to genetically target AIB showed the same expression pattern in starved adults and dauers (SI Appendix, Fig. S8). We then compared the CO2-evoked motor outputs of wild-type animals vs. animals where the AIB neurons are genetically ablated (35). We found that like wild-type starved adults, AIB-ablated starved adults terminated forward movement immediately after CO2 exposure (Fig. 6A). However, AIB-ablated starved adults reinitiated forward movement more rapidly than wild-type starved adults (Fig. 6A). In contrast, AIB-ablated dauers reversed during the first 5 s of CO2 exposure like wild-type dauers, but then exhibited more prolonged reversals than wild-type dauers (Fig. 6B). Thus, AIB ablation has distinct effects on the CO2-evoked locomotory patterns of starved adults vs. dauers.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

The AIB interneurons play distinct roles in regulating CO2-evoked motor output in starved adults vs. dauers. (A and B) Changes in instantaneous speed of (A) wild-type vs. AIB-ablated (AIB-) starved adults, and (B) wild-type vs AIB-ablated dauers in response to CO2. Yellow shaded boxes represent the timing and duration of the CO2 pulse. Blue and red lines represent mean instantaneous speeds of wild-type and AIB-ablated animals, respectively. Shadings represent SEM. Animals were exposed to 20 s pulses of air (21% O2, balance N2) followed by 60 s pulses of CO2 (2.5% CO2, 21% O2, balance N2). (C) Both wild-type and AIB-ablated starved adults show a reduction in forward movement duration in response to CO2, but this effect is less pronounced in AIB-ablated starved adults. (D) Wild-type starved adults but not AIB-ablated starved adults increase their pause duration in response to CO2. (E) Both wild-type and AIB-ablated starved adults show an increase in reverse movement duration in response to CO2, but this effect is less pronounced in AIB-ablated starved adults. (F) Both wild-type and AIB-ablated dauers show a similar reduction in forward movement duration in response to CO2. (G) Both wild-type and AIB-ablated dauers show an increase in pause duration in response to CO2, but this effect is less pronounced in AIB-ablated dauers. (H) Both wild-type and AIB-ablated dauers show an increase in reverse movement duration in response to CO2, but this effect is more pronounced in AIB-ablated dauers. For AH, n = 21 to 34 animals per genotype, life stage, and condition. For CH, each data point indicates the behavioral response of a single animal. Solid lines in violin plots show medians and dotted lines show interquartile ranges. ****P < 0.0001, **< 0.01, *< 0.05, ns = not significant (> 0.07), two-way ANOVA with Sidak’s posttest.

To further investigate the role of AIB in driving CO2-evoked motor output in starved adults and dauers, we quantified the directionality of movement in wild-type vs. AIB-ablated starved adults and dauers. We found that for starved adults and dauers, both wild-type and AIB-ablated animals showed a decrease in forward time ratio and an increase in reverse time ratio in response to acute CO2 exposure relative to the air control. However, pause time ratio increased in AIB-ablated dauers but not AIB-ablated starved adults (Fig. 6 CH). Interestingly, AIB-ablated starved adults reversed less than wild-type starved adults in response to CO2, whereas AIB-ablated dauers reversed more than wild-type dauers (Fig. 6 E and H). We further characterized this effect by quantifying the distance traveled in reverse in response to CO2 vs. an air control. For both starved adults and dauers, wild-type and AIB-ablated animals traveled more distance in reverse in response to CO2 than air (SI Appendix, Fig. S9). However, AIB-ablated starved adults traveled a shorter distance in reverse in response to CO2 than wild-type starved adults, whereas AIB-ablated dauers traveled a greater distance in reverse in response to CO2 than wild-type dauers (SI Appendix, Fig. S9). For both life stages, wild-type and AIB-abated animals traveled similar distances in reverse in response to the air controls, indicating that the effect of AIB on reversals is specific to CO2 (SI Appendix, Fig. S9). Thus, AIB exerts opposite effects on CO2-evoked reversals in starved adults vs. dauers: It promotes reversals in starved adults and suppresses reversals in dauers. In addition, we found that the CO2-evoked movement of daf-2(e1370) mutant dauers (where AIB excitatory activity is largely eliminated; Fig. 4 DF) closely resembled that of AIB-ablated dauers (Fig. 6 FH and SI Appendix, Fig. S10), suggesting that the CO2-evoked movement patterns of dauers arise at least in part due to the effects of insulin signaling on AIB activity. Together, our findings demonstrate a life stage–dependent change in the function of an interneuron in regulating chemosensory behavior.

Finally, we asked whether the dauer-specific excitatory response of AIB to CO2 is sufficient to account for the difference in CO2-evoked motor output between starved adults and dauers. We examined starved adults that expressed the bacterially derived voltage-gated sodium channel NaChBac specifically in AIB (AIB::NaChBac), leading to increased AIB excitability (36). We found that AIB::NaChBac starved adults showed a decrease in CO2-evoked forward movement and an increase in CO2-evoked reverse movement relative to wild-type starved adults (SI Appendix, Fig. S11); these responses were opposite to those of AIB-ablated starved adults (Fig. 6 C and E). However, unlike wild-type dauers, AIB::NaChBac starved adults did not show a dramatic increase in pause time in response to CO2 (Fig. 6G and SI Appendix, Fig. S11B). These results suggest that changes in AIB excitability alone in starved adults are not sufficient to generate dauer-like CO2-evoked motor output; rather, differences in motor output between the two life stages likely arise from the combined effects of multiple circuit components.

Discussion

We have shown that the same chemosensory cue (CO2) is processed differently at two life stages that show the same valence state (CO2 attraction) (Fig. 7). Although CO2 is detected by the BAG sensory neurons in both starved adults and dauers, the functional architecture of the CO2 microcircuit differs at the sensory and interneuron levels. The BAG neurons in dauers show reduced excitatory calcium responses to CO2 relative to starved adults, and the interneurons downstream of BAG show distinct CO2-evoked calcium dynamics at the two life stages. Our findings demonstrate that functionally distinct microcircuits may underlie the detection and processing of a chemosensory cue across life stages sharing the same valence state, highlighting the need to consider context when dissecting chemosensory circuit function. Although it is unclear why dauers utilize a distinct CO2 microcircuit compared to starved adults, one possibility is that the dauer circuit reflects the need for dauers to display other dauer-specific behaviors, such as nictation and dauer recovery (30). It remains to be determined whether CO2 acts as a sensory cue to drive these or other dauer-specific behaviors.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7.

Functionally distinct microcircuits are involved in processing CO2 in starved adults vs. dauers. Differences in chemosensory processing are observed at the level of i) sensory neuron activity (the BAG neurons show reduced CO2-evoked activity in dauers), ii) interneuron activity (the same interneurons show distinct patterns of CO2-evoked activity in starved adults vs. dauers), and iii) motor output (distinct movement patterns are evoked by CO2 in starved adults vs. dauers). Excitatory and inhibitory neuronal activities are indicated by blue and orange shadings, respectively; nonresponsive neurons are indicated by gray shading. For BAG, the lighter shade of blue in dauers indicates reduced excitatory activity. For AIB and AVE, color codes indicate the predominant calcium response at the indicated life stages. The blue and orange shadings for AIY in starved adults indicate that the AIY neurons of starved adults respond stochastically to CO2 such that excitatory and inhibitory responses are generated at roughly equal frequencies (28). Solid arrows indicate chemical synapses in adults. Dashed arrows indicate putative chemical synapses in dauers; synapses are putative due to a lack of knowledge about the dauer connectome. The BAG–AIB gap junction in dauers is also shown. Red lines indicate the CO2-evoked movement trajectory of a single representative animal.

The BAG neurons show reduced CO2-evoked activity in dauers compared to starved adults that is independent of gcy-9 expression (Fig. 1 BD and SI Appendix, Fig. S1 AB). Previous studies have shown that CO2-evoked BAG activity in adults may be suppressed by molecular mechanisms that operate downstream of GCY-9 or inhibitory signaling from downstream interneurons (37, 38). Whether similar mechanisms contribute to the reduced BAG response of dauers or whether other sensory cues are required to reach maximal activity levels remains to be elucidated. It is also possible that the reduced BAG activity of dauers is the result of reduced entry of CO2 through the thicker cuticle of dauers.

At the interneuron level, we have shown that the dauer-specific response properties of the AIB interneurons require a dauer-specific BAG–AIB electrical synapse (Fig. 4 AC). The BAG neurons form a chemical synapse with AIB in adults (17), but the presence of this synapse does not appear to be sufficient for robust CO2-evoked activity in AIB (Fig. 3 AD). It is possible that the electrical synapses between BAG and AIB in dauers lead to alterations in the composition and/or function of the chemical synapses between BAG and AIB, as has been shown for a different synapse in C. elegans (39). The insulin receptor DAF-2 appears to act non-cell-autonomously to modulate AIB activity in dauers (Fig. 4 DF and SI Appendix, Fig. S6). In future studies, it will be interesting to determine whether DAF-2 has distinct effects on CO2 attraction in starved adults vs. dauers. Moreover, identifying the site of action of DAF-2, as well as the signaling pathways that act downstream of DAF-2 to functionally modulate the CO2 microcircuit in dauers, would provide additional insight into how insulin signaling sculpts chemosensory behaviors.

While our results illustrate that the interneurons downstream of BAG show distinct CO2-evoked activity patterns in dauers vs. adults, the precise roles of each of these interneurons in driving CO2-evoked behavior remain to be determined. In adults, the AIY interneurons promote forward movement (40, 41). Thus, the stochastic excitatory activity of AIY in starved adults (28) may cause starved adults to resume forward movement after reversing or pausing upon initial CO2 exposure. The inhibitory activity of AIY in dauers (Fig. 2 EG) may suppress forward movement and thereby promote CO2-evoked pausing (Fig. 5B). In the case of RIG, its role in regulating movement at any life stage is poorly understood. The finding that well-fed adults and dauers show similar CO2-evoked excitatory activity in RIG despite showing CO2 responses of opposite valence (Fig. 2 AC) raises the possibility that RIG may play a different role in regulating movement at the two life stages. However, in dauers but not adults, the excitatory response in RIG was followed by an inhibitory response, raising the possibility that this inhibitory response promotes dauer-specific CO2-evoked behavior.

The AIB interneurons promote reversals in starved adults but suppress reversals in dauers upon acute CO2 exposure (Fig. 6 and SI Appendix, Fig. S11). Prior studies have demonstrated a role for AIB in promoting basal reversals in adults (4245) as well as dauers (34). Thus, AIB appears to have a context-dependent role in regulating reversal behavior in dauers. Moreover, given the opposite roles of AIB in regulating CO2-evoked reversals in starved adults vs. dauers, it is possible that the CO2-evoked excitatory activity of AIB in dauers and the lack of CO2-evoked activity in AIB in starved adults both serve to suppress reversals upon acute CO2 exposure, thereby promoting CO2 attraction. In the case of AVE, excitatory activity in AVE is associated with reversals in adults (43, 46). Thus, it is possible that the silencing of AVE in starved adults and the inhibition of AVE in dauers both serve to suppress reversals in response to CO2. However, the functional consequence of CO2-evoked inhibition of AVE, as opposed to silencing, in dauers remains unclear.

Together, our results demonstrate that divergent CO2-evoked neural mechanisms operate at the sensory and interneuron levels in dauers vs. starved adults despite the two life stages sharing the same valence state. In future studies, it will be interesting to determine whether different mechanisms also underlie the same chemosensory valence state in other organisms, including humans. In addition, dauer larvae are developmentally similar to the infective larvae of parasitic nematodes (47), which infect over one billion people worldwide and cause some of the most devastating neglected tropical diseases (48, 49). The infective larvae of multiple parasitic nematode species use CO2 as a host-seeking cue (20, 24, 5052), but the neural mechanisms that drive these responses remain unknown. Thus, a better understanding of how C. elegans responds to CO2 may lead to new strategies for controlling parasitic nematode infections.

Materials and Methods

Behavioral assays were performed essentially as previously described, with some modifications (35). Calcium imaging was performed as previously described (27, 28). Statistical tests were performed using GraphPad Prism v9.3.1. All data from this study are available on GitHub (https://github.com/HallemLab/Banerjee_et_al_2023). For detailed information on all materials and methods, see SI AppendixMaterials and Methods.

Supplementary Material

Appendix 01 (PDF)

Dataset S01 (XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Cynthia Chai for the AIB::NaChBac strain. We thank Astra Bryant, Michelle Castelletto, and Ricardo Frausto for insightful comments on the manuscript. This work was funded by NIH F32 AI147617 (N.B.), NIH MARC T34 GM008563 (E.J.R.P.), NIH UF1 NS111697 (P.W.S.), and NIH R01 DC017959 and an HHMI Faculty Scholar Award (E.A.H.).

Author contributions

N.B., P.-Y.S., P.W.S., and E.A.H. designed research; N.B., P.-Y.S., and E.J.R.P. performed research; N.B. and E.A.H. analyzed data; and N.B., P.-Y.S., P.W.S., and E.A.H. wrote the paper.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interest.

Footnotes

Reviewers: L.R.G., Texas A&M University; and I.C.G.K., Rheinische Friedrich-Wilhelms-Universitat Bonn.

Contributor Information

Paul W. Sternberg, Email: pws@caltech.edu.

Elissa A. Hallem, Email: ehallem@ucla.edu.

Data, Materials, and Software Availability

All study data are included in the article and/or supporting information or are available on GitHub (https://github.com/HallemLab/Banerjee_et_al_2023) (53).

Supporting Information

References

  • 1.Hildebrand J. G., Shepherd G. M., Mechanisms of olfactory discrimination: Converging evidence for common principles across phyla. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 595–631 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sengupta P., Generation and modulation of chemosensory behaviors in C. elegans. Pflugers Arch. 454, 721–734 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sengupta P., The belly rules the nose: Feeding state-dependent modulation of peripheral chemosensory responses. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 68–75 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dweck H. K. M., et al. , The olfactory logic behind fruit odor preferences in larval and adult Drosophila. Cell Rep. 23, 2524–2531 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sayin S., Boehm A. C., Kobler J. M., De Backer J. F., Grunwald Kadow I. C., Internal state dependent odor processing and perception - the role of neuromodulation in the fly olfactory system. Front. Cell Neurosci. 12, 11 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Calhoon G. G., Acute food deprivation rapidly modifies valence-coding microcircuits in the amygdala. bioRxiv [Preprint] (2018). 10.1101/285189 (Deposited 1 October 2022). [DOI]
  • 7.Flavell S. W., Gogolla N., Lovett-Barron M., Zelikowsky M., The emergence and influence of internal states. Neuron 110, 2545–2570 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Allen W. E., et al. , Thirst regulates motivated behavior through modulation of brainwide neural population dynamics. Science 364, 253 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Vogt K., et al. , Internal state configures olfactory behavior and early sensory processing in Drosophila larvae. Sci. Adv. 7, eabd6900 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ribeiro C., Dickson B. J., Sex peptide receptor and neuronal TOR/S6K signaling modulate nutrient balancing in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 20, 1000–1005 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wasserman S., Salomon A., Frye M. A., Drosophila tracks carbon dioxide in flight. Curr. Biol. 23, 301–306 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.van Breugel F., Huda A., Dickinson M. H., Distinct activity-gated pathways mediate attraction and aversion to CO2 in Drosophila. Nature 564, 420–424 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Devineni A. V., Scaplen K. M., Neural circuits underlying behavioral flexibility: Insights from Drosophila. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 821680 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Schmidt H. J., Beauchamp G. K., Adult-like odor preferences and aversions in three-year-old children. Child Dev. 59, 1136–1143 (1988). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Oleszkiewicz A., et al. , Hedonic perception of odors in children aged 5–8 years is similar across 18 countries: Preliminary data. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 157, 111129 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hansson B. S., Knaden M., Sachse S., Stensmyr M. C., Wicher D., Towards plant-odor-related olfactory neuroethology in Drosophila. Chemoecology 20, 51–61 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.White J. G., Southgate E., Thomson J. N., Brenner S., The structure of the nervous system of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 314, 1–340 (1986). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cook S. J., et al. , Whole-animal connectomes of both Caenorhabditis elegans sexes. Nature 571, 63–71 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Varshney L. R., Chen B. L., Paniagua E., Hall D. H., Chklovskii D. B., Structural properties of the Caenorhabditis elegans neuronal network. PLoS Comput. Biol. 7, e1001066 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Banerjee N., Hallem E. A., The role of carbon dioxide in nematode behavior and physiology. Parasitology 147, 841–854 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Carrillo M. A., Hallem E. A., Gas sensing in nematodes. Mol. Neurobiol. 51, 919–931 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hallem E. A., Sternberg P. W., Acute carbon dioxide avoidance in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 8038–8043 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bretscher A. J., Busch K. E., de Bono M., A carbon dioxide avoidance behavior is integrated with responses to ambient oxygen and food in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 8044–8049 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hallem E. A., et al. , A sensory code for host seeking in parasitic nematodes. Curr. Biol. 21, 377–383 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kodama-Namba E., et al. , Cross-modulation of homeostatic responses to temperature, oxygen and carbon dioxide in C. elegans. PLoS Genet. 9, e1004011 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Carrillo M. A., Guillermin M. L., Rengarajan S., Okubo R., Hallem E. A., O2-sensing neurons control CO2 response in C. elegans. J. Neurosci. 33, 9675–9683 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Guillermin M. L., Carrillo M. A., Hallem E. A., A single set of interneurons drives opposite behaviors in C. elegans. Curr. Biol. 27, 2630–2639 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rengarajan S., Yankura K. A., Guillermin M. L., Fung W., Hallem E. A., Feeding state sculpts a circuit for sensory valence in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 1776–1781 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Golden J. W., Riddle D. L., The Caenorhabditis elegans dauer larva: Developmental effects of pheromone, food, and temperature. Dev. Biol. 102, 368–378 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hu P. J., Dauer. In WormBook (2007), pp. 1–19. www.wormbook.org. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Fielenbach N., Antebi A., C. elegans dauer formation and the molecular basis of plasticity. Genes Dev. 22, 2149–2165 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Smith E. S., Martinez-Velazquez L., Ringstad N., A chemoreceptor that detects molecular carbon dioxide. J. Biol. Chem. 288, 37071–37081 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hallem E. A., et al. , Receptor-type guanylate cyclase is required for carbon dioxide sensation by Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 254–259 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bhattacharya A., Aghayeva U., Berghoff E. G., Hobert O., Plasticity of the electrical connectome of C. elegans. Cell 176, 1174–1189 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rojo Romanos T., Ng L., Pocock R., Behavioral assays to study oxygen and carbon dioxide sensing in Caenorhabditis elegans. Bio. Protoc. 8, e2679 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ren D., et al. , A prokaryotic voltage-gated sodium channel. Science 294, 2372–2375 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Beets I., et al. , Natural variation in a dendritic scaffold protein remodels experience-dependent plasticity by altering neuropeptide expression. Neuron 105, 106–121 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Riedl J., Fieseler C., Zimmer M., Tyraminergic corollary discharge filters reafferent perception in a chemosensory neuron. Curr. Biol. 32, 1–11 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Liu P., Chen B., Mailler R., Wang Z. W., Antidromic-rectifying gap junctions amplify chemical transmission at functionally mixed electrical-chemical synapses. Nat. Commun. 8, 14818 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kocabas A., Shen C. H., Guo Z. V., Ramanathan S., Controlling interneuron activity in Caenorhabditis elegans to evoke chemotactic behaviour. Nature 490, 273–277 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Li Z., Liu J., Zheng M., Xu X. Z., Encoding of both analog- and digital-like behavioral outputs by one C. elegans interneuron. Cell 159, 751–765 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Gordus A., Pokala N., Levy S., Flavell S. W., Bargmann C. I., Feedback from network states generates variability in a probabilistic olfactory circuit. Cell 161, 215–227 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kato S., et al. , Global brain dynamics embed the motor command sequence of Caenorhabditis elegans. Cell 163, 656–669 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Nguyen J. P., et al. , Whole-brain calcium imaging with cellular resolution in freely behaving Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, E1074–1081 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Venkatachalam V., et al. , Pan-neuronal imaging in roaming Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, E1082–1088 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Katz M., Corson F., Iwanir S., Biron D., Shaham S., Glia modulate a neuronal circuit for locomotion suppression during sleep in C. elegans. Cell Rep. 22, 2575–2583 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Crook M., The dauer hypothesis and the evolution of parasitism: 20 years on and still going strong. Int. J. Parasitol. 44, 1–8 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lustigman S., et al. , A research agenda for helminth diseases of humans: The problem of helminthiases. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 6, e1582 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Schafer T. W., Skopic A., Parasites of the small intestine. Curr. Gastroenterol. Rep. 8, 312–320 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Castelletto M. L., et al. , Diverse host-seeking behaviors of skin-penetrating nematodes. PLoS Pathog. 10, e1004305 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ruiz F., Castelletto M. L., Gang S. S., Hallem E. A., Experience-dependent olfactory behaviors of the parasitic nematode Heligmosomoides polygyrus. PLoS Pathog. 13, e1006709 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lee J., Dillman A. R., Hallem E. A., Temperature-dependent changes in the host-seeking behaviors of parasitic nematodes. BMC Biol. 14, 36 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Banerjee N., Shih P. Y., Rojas Palato E. J., Sternberg P. W., Hallem E. A., Supplemental data files for Banerjee et al., 2023. Github. https://github.com/HallemLab/Banerjee_et_al_2023. Deposited 4 April 2023.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix 01 (PDF)

Dataset S01 (XLSX)

Data Availability Statement

All study data are included in the article and/or supporting information or are available on GitHub (https://github.com/HallemLab/Banerjee_et_al_2023) (53).


Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES