Abstract
Knowledge of drug concentrations in the brains of behaving subjects remains constrained on a number of dimensions, including poor temporal resolution and lack of real-time data. Here, however, we demonstrate the ability of electrochemical aptamer-based sensors to support seconds-resolved, real-time measurements of drug concentrations in the brains of freely moving rats. Specifically, using such sensors, we achieve <4 μM limits of detection and 10-s resolution in the measurement of procaine in the brains of freely moving rats, permitting the determination of the pharmacokinetics and concentration-behavior relations of the drug with high precision for individual subjects. In parallel, we have used closed-loop feedback-controlled drug delivery to hold intracranial procaine levels constant (±10%) for >1.5 hours. These results demonstrate the utility of such sensors in (i) the determination of the site-specific, seconds-resolved neuropharmacokinetics, (ii) enabling the study of individual subject neuropharmacokinetics and concentration-response relations, and (iii) performing high-precision control over intracranial drug levels.
A sensing platform permits real-time, high precision monitoring and control of in-brain drug concentration of behaving subjects.
INTRODUCTION
Given that the impact of a drug on brain function depends critically on its in-brain pharmacokinetics, accurate, and highly time-resolved measurement of in-brain drug concentrations will be necessary to achieve a detailed understanding of drug-brain interactions. For example, the greater abuse potential of injecting rather than oral ingestion or “snorting” of psychotropic drugs is thought to occur because in-brain drug levels peak a few tens of seconds faster in the former (1–3). The induction of anesthesia likewise typically requires just seconds (e.g., the time required to count backward from 100 to 90). Both observations highlight the important role that seconds-resolved in-brain drug measurements will play in advancing our understanding of psychopharmacodynamics.
In contrast to the few tens of seconds time course of many psychoactive substances, even the best-resolved prior studies of their pharmacokinetics are typically (1) only minutes resolved and (2) not real time (4, 5), with the latter precluding their application to closed-loop feedback control over drug levels. These issues arise because current methods of measuring drugs in the brain are largely limited to postmortem sample collection, positron emission tomography (PET), or microdialysis (6, 7), all of which fail to achieve the performance necessary to address many key questions in behavioral neuropharmacology. Postmortem measurements, for example, provide only a single time point and typically lack spatial resolution (specimens are often homogenized). PET, in contrast, supports the real-time measurement of drug time courses but is limited to those molecules for which high-speed isotopic labeling protocols are available, requires constrained subjects, and suffers from poor (typically minutes) resolution (8–10). Microdialysis, which also supports the collection of multiple, time-resolved measurements, not only provides exceptional versatility in molecular detection and supports studies of freely moving subjects (11, 12) but also suffers from generally poor temporal resolution. Specifically, while a single group (13) has used microdialysis to achieve 2-s resolution measurements of in-brain amino acid concentrations (a feat that does not appear to have since been repeated) and a small number of others have reached tens of seconds resolution [e.g., (14, 15), see (16) for recent review], such studies remain far from commonplace, much less “turn key.” For example, a recent survey of a representative set of 64 in-brain microdialysis studies found the best resolved of these used 5-min sampling and most (82%) used sampling times of 20 min or greater (17). In addition, although numerous authors have described “on-line” setups in which dialysate is delivered directly to analytical instrumentation for measurement (18), minute-long sample transport and analysis times mean that even these microdialysis studies are not meaningfully real-time, precluding their application to emerging engineering approaches, such as feedback control of in vivo drug concentrations (19, 20) and closed-loop optogenetics (21, 22).
In contrast to in-brain measurements of drugs, current methods of measuring some neurotransmitters in the brain achieve far superior temporal resolution. Specifically, the direct electrochemical detection of monoamines and the enzymatic electrochemical detection of glutamate and acetylcholine commonly achieve resolution of ~1 s (23). [Note that, while the interrogation frequency of such sensors is often >>1 Hz, this is not synonymous with their time resolution, which is limited to a few seconds by the slow diffusion of target to the sensor surface; see, e.g., figure 1 of (24), table 1 of (25), and table 2 of (16)]. These approaches, however, depend on the electrochemical or enzymatic reactivity of their targets and thus cannot be generally adapted to the measurement of most psychoactive drugs (the one exception being alcohol). Here, however, we describe the adaptation of electrochemical aptamer-based (EAB) sensors (Fig. 1), a versatile, receptor-based, electrochemical detection approach that is independent of the reactivity of its targets [e.g., (26, 27)], to the problem of performing seconds-resolved, real-time, multihour measurements of and feedback control over the intracranial concentration of a psychoactive drug in freely behaving rats.
RESULTS
EAB sensors are composed of a redox reporter–modified aptamer (nucleic acid–based receptors selected in vitro to bind specific molecular targets) that is covalently attached to the surface of a gold electrode via an alkane thiol chain (Fig. 1A). A binding-induced conformational change in this aptamer alters the rate of electron transfer from the redox reporter, changing in turn the peak current observed when the sensors are interrogated using square-wave voltammetry, with the relevant interrogations requiring 10 to 11 s to perform. As the sensor responds to its target more rapidly than this (fig. S3), this interrogation time scale defines the sensor’s time resolution.
Here, we have developed an EAB sensor for the detection of the anesthetic procaine and have used it to monitor the drug intracranially in live rats. This compound, which has local, general, and epidural anesthetic properties (28–31), had previously only been measured in the brain with ~5-min temporal resolution (5), a time frame that is both 30-fold poorer than the results presented here and too slow to accurately resolve the drug’s pharmacokinetics or support detailed comparison of drug concentration to drug-induced behavioral effects. To develop an EAB sensor against procaine, we first modified a previously developed aptamer (32) to improve its affinity and signal gain (fig. S1), with the resulting sequence achieving a dissociation constant (Kd) of 960 ± 30 μM (this and all other confidence intervals reflect estimated 95% confidence levels). After modifying this aptamer with a methylene blue redox reporter and a six-carbon alkane thiol, we deposit it on a 75-μm-diameter gold wire and “backfill” with 6-mercaptohexanol to form a continuous monolayer. The resulting EAB sensor easily measures the clinically relevant, 40 to 180 μM procaine concentrations seen in human plasma during the administration of this drug (33). For example, when challenged in vitro with 200 μM procaine in undiluted cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), the sensor responds with a ~30% change in signaling current (fig. S1). In contrast, the sensor does not measurably respond to either of the primary metabolites of procaine (fig. S1) or to any of the neurotransmitters thought to be modulated by this drug (fig. S4).
In-brain EAB sensors can be emplaced using standard surgical techniques. For insertion, animals are briefly sedated using isofluorane, and the sensor is inserted through a 19-gauge cannula that was stereotaxically implanted and cemented to the skull at least 1 week before the experiment. Given the dimensions of the probe (Fig. 1B) and the placement of the cannula (see Materials and Methods), the working electrode portion of the sensor sits entirely in the region of interest [to ensure correct placement, we fix, slice, and image all brains after the experiment (e.g., fig. S5)]. Notably, while this procedure ensures that the probes are tightly secured to the cannula and therefore to the animal’s skull, we use free-moving animals in our studies, and thus, mechanical noise is sometimes seen when the animal strikes the probe against the side of its chamber. The resulting, distorted voltammograms create substantial outliers in our recorded time-concentration plots (see, e.g., fig. S6), which we systematically remove applying a Hampel filter (with k = 25); doing so removes fewer than 1 in 20 data points.
When placed in freely moving, normally behaving rats, intracranial EAB sensors support seconds-resolved drug concentration measurements. For example, when placed in the hippocampus of a free-moving rat undergoing an infusion (5 mg/kg/min) to a total dose of 80 mg/kg, we see a steady rise in in-brain procaine levels to 78.6 μM (Fig. 1D). This value is similar to the only prior measurements of intracranial procaine levels that we are aware of (5). These experiments, which used the removal of CSF from anesthetized dogs with a sampling interval of 3 min (or greater), observed peak concentrations of 150 μM some 12 min after an infusion (50 mg/kg), values reasonably close to those we report here for the rat. As expected, given the rapid metabolism of procaine (28, 33, 34), intracranial drug levels then rapidly decrease with a half-life (t1/2) of 13.8 min. The precision of the resulting measurements is exceptional: Using noise measurements collected during the preinfusion baseline of this specific experiment, we estimate that this sensor’s limit of detection (at the level of 3 SDs above baseline) is 2.72 μM. Notably, this limit of detection is not a fundamental limitation of the EAB approach; rather, it is defined by the affinity of the aptamer for its target, the gain of the specific sensor, the noise seen in the specific in vivo setting with this specific EAB architecture (35), and the conservative statistical threshold we have used here.
Intracranial EAB measurements are highly reproducible. To show this, we placed a cannula to provide access to the left lateral ventricle of a single rat in a repeatable fashion. Then, on separate occasions 6 days apart, we inserted two different EAB sensors into the lateral ventricle of this animal and recorded the concentration of procaine in the CSF before, during, and after constant intravenous infusions (5 mg/kg/min) to a total dose of 80 mg/kg (Fig. 1E). In the two measurement sets, the drug rose to effectively indistinguishable maximum concentrations (Cmax = 63.4 ± 0.3 and 65.7 ± 0.3 μM, respectively) shortly after the end of the infusion before then falling with t1/2s (12.1 ± 0.1 and 10.9 ± 0.1 min, respectively) that are likewise nearly identical.
Given that our goal is to measure drug transport across the blood-brain barrier, we believe that it is important to confirm that the barrier remains intact after sensor insertion. To confirm this, we performed a control experiment using the chemotherapeutic doxorubicin, a drug that is known to not cross the barrier (36, 37) and for which there exists a particularly low limit of detection EAB sensor (26). We placed this sensor in the lateral ventricle of a rat and dosed the animal intravenously with doxorubicin levels that, were the drug to cross into the brain, would produce ~20 μM (assuming nonselective equilibration across the total fluid volume of the rat; see fig. S2), a value 100-fold above this sensor’s limit of detection. Despite this, we did not see any measurable change in sensor output (Fig. 1F), confirming that our experiments do not significantly disrupt the barrier, an observation that is perhaps not unexpected given that EAB sensors should be no more disruptive than the well-established microdialysis approaches that have preceded them.
The exceptional resolution of EAB sensors renders it possible to resolve the pharmacokinetics of individual animals with good statistical significance. To illustrate this, we placed sensors into the lateral ventricles of nine animals [we used the ventricles because the exact site of action of anesthetics, such as procaine, remains unknown (38)]. Using these, we then monitored the drug in the CSF before, during, and after a constant intravenous infusion (5 mg/kg/min) to total dosing of 80 or 160 mg/kg. Across these nine animals, procaine first reaches 3.7 μM (which is the 3-σ = 99.7% confidence limit of detection seen for the noisiest of the dozen sensors used in the work described here) in the ventricles some 71 to 220 s after the initiation of infusion. For six of these animals, we dosed for 16 min to a total of 80 mg/kg. In these animals, we saw the drug rise to peak concentrations of 56 to 80 μM shortly after the end of the infusion (Fig. 2). In contrast, for the three animals, we dosed to a total of 160 mg/kg, the corresponding peak concentrations range from 126 to 197 μM (Fig. 3). In all nine animals, the rapid metabolism of procaine again causes its concentration to fall shortly after the cessation of the infusion.
The unprecedented time resolution of EAB-derived drug measurements provides hundreds of concentration values during the ~100 to 150 min between the initiation of drug exposure and the elimination of the drug from the brain to undetectable levels. These unprecedentedly highly time-dense data render it possible to estimate key pharmacokinetic variables, such as the Cmax and the elimination t1/2, with precision typically better than 1% (at 95% confidence intervals). With this level of precision, we can easily identify pharmacokinetic differences between individual animals with good statistical confidence. To illustrate these points, we explored the single-exponential drug kinetics seen following the cessation of infusion (Figs. 2 and 3). For the infusions (80 mg/kg), the t1/2s of these decays range from 8.6 ± 0.1 to 15.7 ± 0.1 min (Table 1), indicating small but nevertheless highly statistically significant animal-to-animal variation in procaine metabolism. Following the infusion (160 mg/kg), the observed t1/2s range from 15.9 ± 0.1 to 20.3 ± 0.1 min. Notably, the 12.6 ± 2.1 min average t1/2 seen across the six animals that received doses of 80 mg/kg is slightly, but statistically significantly, shorter than the 18.1 ± 2.5 min average seen for the three animals that received the 180 mg/kg dose, suggesting that the esterase that degrades procaine may approach saturation at higher the drug concentrations achieved in the latter experiments, slowing the drug’s elimination.
Table 1. Neuropharmacokinetics parameters for individual subjects.
Animal number | Time to first detection* (min) | Cmax (μM) | t1/2 (min) |
---|---|---|---|
Lateral ventricle, 80 mg/kg dosing | |||
1 | 1.18 | 66.4 ± 0.3 | 13.8 ± 0.1 |
2 | 1.98 | 55.9 ± 0.3 | 12.0 ± 0.1 |
3 | 2.72 | 66.3 ± 0.3 | 14.3 ± 0.1 |
4 | 2.92 | 60.6 ± 0.3 | 15.7 ± 0.1 |
5 | 1.23 | 65.7 ± 0.3 | 10.9 ± 0.1 |
6 | 1.6 | 79.5 ± 0.3 | 8.6 ± 0.1 |
Lateral ventricle, 160 mg/kg dosing | |||
7 | 2.9 | 197.2 ± 0.3 | 15.9 ± 0.1 |
8 | 3.67 | 126.0 ± 0.2 | 20.3 ± 0.1 |
9 | 2.17 | 154.5 ± 0.2 | 18.2 ± 0.1 |
*Defined as the time taken to surpass the limit of detection, here defined as 3.7 μM (see the main text).
The excellent time resolution and high precision of EAB sensors also render it possible to identify relations between drug pharmacokinetics and drug-induced changes in behavior (i.e., pharmacodynamics) at the level of individual animals with generally good statistical confidence (Fig. 4). As systemic procaine acts as a general anesthetic, the behavioral response we used was locomotion, which we measured using a locomotor chamber equipped with arrays of infrared beams. As expected, locomotor behavior falls for all animals as intracranial procaine levels rise and are then recovered as the drug is metabolized and its concentration falls (Figs. 2 and 3). To put these relationships on a more quantitative footing, we first estimated the Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient (rS), which provides a measure of the monotonicity of a relationship, between intracranial procaine and locomotion in the X-Y plane. As expected, this estimated correlation is negative (higher procaine equates to reduced locomotion) for all nine animals (Table 2) and is statistically significant for eight of them. Notably, the single animal for which this correlation is not significant is among the animals that exhibited the least locomotion before infusion, suggesting, perhaps not unreasonably, that the correlation between locomotion and intracranial procaine levels fails if the animal is inactive even before the application of the anesthetic.
Table 2. Modeling of locomotion during procaine exposure in individual subjects.
Animal no. | Mean locomotion before infusion (breaks per minute) | r s | Linear model | Hyperbolic model | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept (breaks per minute) | Slope | r p | BIC* | Intercept (breaks per minute) | Kd (μM) | BIC* | |||
Lateral ventricle, 80 mg/kg dosing | |||||||||
1 | 20.1 | −0.83† | 7 ± 3 | −0.11 ± 0.06 | −0.71† | 90.6 | 15 ± 2 | 3.6 ± 1.4 | 58.5 |
2 | 8.5 | −0.32 | 4 ± 3 | −0.03 ± 0.08 | −0.21 | 85.8 | 4 ± 4 | 70 ± 290 | 85.9 |
3 | 21.3 | −0.85† | 10 ± 2 | −0.17 ± 0.05 | −0.88† | 65.1 | 12 ± 5 | 16 ± 14 | 72.4 |
4 | 5.8 | −0.76† | 10 ± 4 | −0.18 ± 0.09 | −0.74† | 80.6 | 16 ± 5 | 6 ± 4 | 70.2 |
5 | 10.5 | −0.73† | 5 ± 2 | −0.09 ± 0.07 | −0.64† | 63.7 | 5 ± 4 | 13 ± 27 | 65.3 |
6 | 8.5 | −0.64† | 3 ± 2 | −0.04 ± 0.03 | −0.53† | 80.4 | 9 ± 2 | 1.2 ± 0.9 | 49.3 |
Lateral ventricle, 160 mg/kg dosing | |||||||||
7 | 15.3 | −0.75† | 12 ± 3 | −0.08 ± 0.03 | −0.67† | 261.2 | 18 ± 8 | 22 ± 20 | 259.7 |
8 | 21.1 | −0.91† | 26 ± 4 | −0.26 ± 0.06 | −0.88† | 152.6 | 30 ± 10 | 26 ± 20 | 167.4 |
9 | 0 | −0.73† | 6 ± 2 | −0.05 ± 0.02 | −0.62† | 186.2 | 10 ± 5 | 21 ± 22 | 186.2 |
*Bolded text denote models that the BIC indicates are preferred.
†Denotes estimated correlations are significantly different from 0 at a significance level of α = 0.05
Our use of the Spearman’s correlation to analyze our data does not assume any specific mathematical relationship (other than monotonicity) between the intracranial concentration of the drug and the resulting behavior. In contrast, the more widely used Pearson’s correlation coefficient assumes that the relationship between the two is linear. Using this approach, we once again find that the estimated correlation coefficient, rP, associated with the relationship between intracranial drug levels and X-Y plane locomotion is negative for all nine animals (Fig. 4, blue curves) and statistically significant for eight of them (Table 2; see also figs. S7 and S8 and tables S1 and S2 for the equivalent studies using other measures of locomotion). The linear relationship assumed by the Pearson correlation, however, is likely naive. For example, if anesthesia arises due to the binding of procaine to a specific, saturable biological target (such as for, e.g., a neurotransmitter receptor), then the effect might scale hyperbolically (i.e., follow a “Langmuir isotherm”) rather than linearly with drug concentration. To test this, we fitted all nine datasets to hyperbolic relationships (Fig. 4, green curves) and then used the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) (39) to judge whether this model is statistically preferred over the linear model. We find that, using an established “rule of thumb” (40) that two models whose BICs fall within 2 units of one another describe the data equally as well, the hyperbolic model is preferred (has the smallest BIC value) or matches the linear model for seven of the nine animals we characterized and produces estimates of the receptor’s Kd in the low micromolar range (Table 2). This said, we recognize that even a hyperbolic model may be a naively simple description of the complex physiological processes by which rising procaine concentrations ultimately produce anesthesia. The model, for example, assumes that the degree to which locomotion is reduced is simply proportional to fractional occupancy of the relevant receptor that is bound at a given drug concentration.
The concentration information provided by EAB sensors is not only continuous but also real time, providing opportunities to perform closed-loop feedback control over in vivo drug levels. Previously, we have used a proportional-integral-derivative (PID) feedback controller to achieve and maintain specific, desired plasma drug concentrations (19, 20). Here, we have extended this capability to the control of CSF drug levels using our new intracranial EAB sensor. To do so required that we overcome a new challenge. Specifically, because the measurements are collected intracranially while the drug is injected intravenously, the controller needs to account for nontrivial drug transport delays, requiring careful tuning of the controller. We do not, however, know the exact pharmacokinetics of any specific animal before beginning the experiment. To reduce this problem, we tuned our controller using pharmacokinetic data collected during our continuous infusion experiments (Figs. 2 and 3). With the resulting control, we achieved a targeted, 100 μM procaine concentration about 1 hour after the initiation of control, which we then held for more than 1.5 hours. During the latter period, the root mean square deviation from the set point concentration was just 15 μM, illustrating the high degree of control this approach can achieve. As expected, we see a significant decrease in locomotion during the period of control (Fig. 5). After the control was released, we observe a steady loss of procaine (with a t1/2 of approximately 20 min) and increasing levels of locomotion.
DISCUSSION
Here, we have adapted EAB sensors to the problem of monitoring and controlling the concentration of a psychoactive drug in situ in brain ventricles and tissues of live rats in experiments that were performed in parallel with simultaneous observations of the resulting behavioral (locomotor) responses. The resulting measurements achieved time resolution of 10 to 11 s in experiments that spanned hours and enabled the determination of high-accuracy, single-animal, pharmacokinetic profiles. The resulting measurements are also real time and thus provide a means of not only measuring intracranial drug levels but also using closed-loop control systems to actively maintain drug concentrations at an arbitrarily defined level for a prolonged period of time.
The EAB approach described here represents a significant advance in our ability to monitor and control brain chemistry. We are aware of only three prior reports of drug measurements being performed in situ (as opposed to ex vivo) in the brains of behaving animals, none of which were real time and all three of which required averaging over multiple animals to achieve statistical significance. In two of these, Rocchitta et al. (41, 42) used an enzymatic alcohol sensor to measure ethanol in the brains of awake, behaving rats. In the third, Taylor et al. (43) used a sensor similar to that described here to monitor cocaine in the brains of anesthetized rats after intravenous dosing (2 mg/kg), but they had to average over four sensors placed in the brains of two animals. That study reports peak in-brain cocaine concentrations of 15 μM, a value exceeding by several-fold the peak levels measured in prior microdialysis studies using similar drug doses (4, 41, 42, 44–48).
In vivo EAB sensor measurements represent a vast improvement in the time resolution with which drugs can be measured at specific sites in the brain. For example, the measurements presented here represent an order of magnitude improvement in time resolution over the most highly time-resolved microdialysis studies we have found of intracranial drug concentrations [i.e., 1-min sampling intervals with ~1-min equilibration (4)] and a two orders of magnitude improvement over typical microdialysis studies of drugs in the brain [i.e., 10- to 20-min sampling intervals as reviewed in (17)]. We believe that this vastly improved resolution, which concomitantly comes with a vastly improved ability to estimate key pharmacokinetic profiles for each subject, will improve our understanding of pharmacology and its effects on behavior. For example, because of the difficulty of measuring intracranial drug concentrations with good precision (much less in real time), current experimental approaches in neuropsychopharmacology almost invariably focus on dose-controlled experiments in drug-induced behavioral perturbations, which are then correlated with total drug dose rather than the concentration (much less the rate of change of concentration) of the drug in the brain. While dose-controlled approaches have long highlighted individual differences in animals’ behavioral responses, they simply cannot reveal the effects of individual differences in animal neuropharmacokinetics. The high-resolution, high-precision, in situ concentration measurements provided by EAB sensors, in contrast, offer opportunities to instead correlate behaviors with the in-brain drug levels (and time-resolved changes in these levels) that are driving them. Given this, it is perhaps not unexpected that, here, EAB sensors have enabled the identification of statistically significant correlations (rP and rS as great as −0.88 and −0.91, respectively) between intracranial drug levels and behavior (locomotor function) within individual animals.
Moving beyond mere measurements of intracranial drug levels, the EAB sensor–enabled ability to control drug levels in the CSF provides further, likely even more powerful routes to overcoming the shortcomings of prior experimental approaches in neuropsychopharmacology to account for concentration-based factors. Applying EAB-driven feedback control, for example, researchers will be able to precisely manipulate the neuropharmacokinetics of drugs across individual animals, as has been shown with plasma pharmacokinetics (19), so as to quantitatively elucidate the relationships between neuropharmacokinetics and behavior. For example, by providing a means of investigating, in an entirely controlled manner, the role that individual pharmacokinetics play in the manifestation and maintenance of addiction, this approach could prove a powerful tool for addiction research. Similarly, the ability to hold drug concentrations static at any desired level could be used to examine neuroadaptive and behavioral responses.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Chemical reagents and materials
All reagents were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO) and used as received. The synthesis of the high-performance liquid chromatography–purified, reporter-and-thiol–modified, procaine-binding aptamer was obtained from Biosearch Technologies (Novato, CA), dissolved in deionized water to 100 μM, aliquoted into separate tubes, and stored at −20°C until used.
Sensor fabrication and in vitro measurements
The in-brain “probes” we use consist of three parts: the sensor itself (Fig. 1A), an indwelling cannula (Fig. 1B), and a flexible leash, all of which were custom-constructed on the basis of our design by Plastics One (Roanoke, VA). The indwelling cannula was constructed from a 19-gauge stainless steel cannula (2 mm length below pedestal) with a plastic screw-top connector (total length of 13 mm). The sensor was constructed from a 22-gauge threaded stainless steel cannula (15 mm length below pedestal), which served as the counter/pseudo-reference electrode, filled with Teflon-coated gold wire (76 μm in diameter, 3 mm exposed, active sensor length, 19 mm in total length), and surrounded by a plastic screw-top connector with two ports for electrode pins. The electrode leash (50 cm) was built to contain male and female ends for electrode pins on opposite sides and is protected with a stainless-steel mesh exterior.
We first validated the EAB sensor in vitro using devices fabricated on 3-mm-diameter gold wire electrodes, which we tested in undiluted bovine CSF (BioIVT, Westbury, NY) to ensure that they achieved physiologically relevant detection limits and selectivity (i.e., that they reject any endogenous molecules in the CSF). To fabricate sensors on these electrodes, we first cleaned them by electrochemically with the following protocol: (i) 900 cycles between −1 and −1.6 V in a solution of 0.5 M NaOH at 1 V s−1 to remove any residual thiol/organic contaminants on the electrode surface and (ii) pulsed between 0 and 2 V for at least 16,000 cycles with a pulse length of 20 ms in 0.5 M H2SO4 to increase the electrode roughness. To prepare the aptamer for attachment to the gold surface, we first reduced 4 μl of a 100 μM solution of the appropriate thiol and methylene blue–modified oligonucleotide (5′–thiol–C6–PO4–AGAC AAG GAA AAT CCT TCA ACG AAG TGG GTCG–PO4–methylene blue–3′) using 4 μl of 10 mM of tris(2-carboxyethyl) phosphine hydrochloride for 1 hour at 25°C. We then diluted the solution to 500 nM with 1× phosphate-buffered saline. We then submerged freshly cleaned electrodes in this solution for 1 hour before moving them to a 10 mM 6-mercaptohexanol solution overnight to form a self-assembled monolayer.
Our custom in-brain probes consist of two major parts—an electrode leash and an implantable gold/stainless-steel electrode pair (P1, Roanoke, VA). The former (50 cm length) is composed of two copper wires encased in a braided stainless-steel mesh leash. At each end of the leash are two gold-coated copper pins (1 mm apart) that emerge from a plastic casing and a screw-on plastic collar (8 mm length). One end of this is attached to the implanted electrodes and the other to a two-channel swivel commutator. Before using the electrode pair, we cleaned the gold electrode and applied the DNA aptamer and self-assembling monolayer to it as described above.
To optimize the electrochemical parameters used in sensor interrogation, we conducted a 50-point titration of our sensors at eight different square-wave frequencies in vitro in undiluted bovine CSF at 37°C and determined the “signal-on” (150 Hz) and “signal-off” (30 Hz) frequencies with the largest relative changes in current. Using serial voltammetric measurements performed at these frequencies, we corrected the baseline drift seen in vivo using “kinetic differential measurements” [KDM; detailed in (49)]. To convert KDM signals into drug concentrations, we fitted the in vitro titration data to the equation: , where y is the KDM signal, BMAX is the signal change seen at saturating target concentrations, [target] is the drug concentration, and Kd is the Kd of the aptamer.
Animal housing and care
Adult female (225 to 250 g) and male (275 to 300 g) Sprague-Dawley rats obtained with a jugular catheter already emplaced (Charles River Laboratories, Hollister, CA); the chronic indwelling jugular catheters enabled rapid drug delivery. The rats were pair-housed in a standard light cycle room (08:00 on, 20:00 off) and allowed ad libitum access to food and water. Daily monitoring was performed by trained animal technicians to ensure animal health and behavioral responsiveness. This monitoring uncovered no evidence that cannula and sensor implantation cause harm. All animals continued to gain weight after implantation of the cannula through to the completion of our experiments. The housing and care of all rats were conducted in accordance with the guidelines set forth by the “Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Rats,” 8th edition, (50), and all experiments were approved by the University of California, Santa Barbara Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Surgery
We induced anesthesia before surgery using 4% isoflurane gas (Medline, Northfield, IL) and maintained it using 2 to 3% for the remainder of surgical procedures. After inducing anesthesia, we shaved the head of the rats and disinfected the surgical site with 10% betadine solution and 70% isopropanol before injecting the local anesthetic lidocaine (2 mg/kg subcutaneously) into each incision site. Temperature, heart rate, SPO2 (specific peripheral oxygen concentration), and respiration rate were recorded and maintained within veterinary recommended levels during surgical procedures. Next, we implanted a permanent 19-gauge stainless-steel guide cannula (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) using stereotaxic coordinates aimed at the lateral ventricle [anteroposterior (AP) = −0.48, mediolateral (ML) = +1.6, dorsoventral (DV) = −1] or the hippocampus (AP = −4.8, ML = +5.1, DV = −1), which was affixed to the skull with metal screws and covered in dental cement. Once the cannula was implanted, a removable 1.2-cm-long 22-gauge stainless-steel obturator (the length of which places its tip at the same depth as the probe tip) was placed into the cannula. After surgery, rats are handled daily, and catheters are flushed to ensure patency as well as general health of the rat with a minimum of 7 days before experimentation. Following the experiment, we removed the brains from the rats and performed histology to confirm sensor placement (see below).
In vivo measurements
Before commencing measurements, we weighed each rat (mean ± SEM weights for females, 290 ± 13 g, and for males, 418 ± 6 g) and induced anesthesia using 3% isoflurane gas and maintained anesthesia as we first removed the obdurator, inserted the sensor through the cannula (~10 to 15 s), and then affixed the sensor in place by the screw fittings on the surgically implanted 19-gauge guide cannula. We then placed the rat into a Kinder Scientific Motor Monitor system (Fig. 1E), allowed them to recover from anesthesia, and began recording locomotion via the supplied Motor Monitor software. We commenced EAB measurements using square-wave voltammetry on a CH1208C mini-potentiostat, alternating between square-wave frequencies of 30 Hz (swept at 0.02 V/s) and 150 Hz (swept at 0.15 V/s), both at an amplitude of 25 mV and over a voltage window of approximately 0.1 to −0.27 V (relative to the stainless-steel pseudo-reference). To visualize our measurements in real time, we used Software for the Analysis and Continuous Monitoring of Electrochemical Systems (SACMES), an open-source Python script previously reported by our laboratories (51).
Feedback control
We use a PID feedback controller to achieve the targeted level of procaine. PID controllers have three tunable parameters corresponding to their namesake processes. We use the data from our infusion experiments (Figs. 2 and 3) to inform on the tuning parameter values that would result in acceptable performance had we conducted feedback control on all of these animals. We use these tuned values for the PID controller that we used in the experiment shown (Fig. 5). Because our tuning is based on the pharmacokinetics of animals other than the animal we are actually experimenting on, we also implemented a fail-safe mechanism to avoid possible overshooting (and overdosing). This mechanism stops the pump from injecting the drug if the intracranial concentration exceeds 110% of the targeted value, and it does not reactivate the PID controller unless the concentration level falls below 105% of the targeted value.
Histology
To ensure proper sensor placement, we euthanized the rats at the end of the experiment using an intravenous injection of 0.1 ml of Somnasol (Covetrus, Chicago, IL) before performing histology. To do so, we decapitated the animals with a guillotine (Braintree Scientific, Braintree, MA), collected their brains in 10% formalin, incubated them in this for 1 week, and then transferred them to 30% sucrose solution for 3 days. Following this, we blocked the brains at the cerebellum, flash-frozen them over dry ice for 1 min, mounted them on a stand, and sliced them into 40-μm sections using a cryostat. These were then Nissl-stained and examined on a Nikon E800 microscope equipped with a Hamamatsu Orca camera to verify cannula placements (fig. S5 for example lateral ventricle and hippocampus placement).
Statistical analyses
The high-frequency measurements provided by EAB sensors vastly increase the number of concentration measurements within any fixed time interval (e.g., by two orders of magnitude over typical microdialysis studies) of in-brain drug concentrations in individual animals. The resulting high number of measurements for each animal enables us to perform within-subject statistical analyses on individual animals, which we believe is effectively unprecedented. Thus, motivated, we explored the potential association between drugs and locomotion behavior, after binning the data collected after the start of infusion into different concentration levels of drug present in the tissue, adjusted for amount of time within that concentration bin. We binned concentrations into 5 μM increments (−2.5 to 2.5 μM, 2.5 to 7.5 μM, etc.; with inclusivity on the upper bound), labeled by their midpoints. Concentration measurements falling below 0 μM (which arise due to sensor noise) were grouped into the 0 μM bin. We considered data only after the start of infusion. Within each concentration bin, we calculated the sum of all locomotion counts divided by the total amount of time (in minutes) spent in that bin; effectively returning a new measurement (breaks per minute) average for that concentration bin.
We investigated the relationship between procaine concentration bins and beam breaks per minute using two different correlation measures: those being the Pearson’s correlation coefficient and Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient. Pearson’s correlation coefficient measures the strength and direction of a linear association (here being the concentration bin midpoints and the breaks per minute)(52). Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient measures the strength and direction of a monotonic association between two variables (53). Note that the Spearman’s rank correlation is the Pearson’s correlation between the ranked data. Table 1 presents estimated Spearman’s rank and Pearson’s correlation coefficients for each animal, along with whether we rejected the null hypothesis of 0 correlation (for Pearson’s) or lack of monotonicity (for Spearman’s) at a significance level of α = 0.05 (i.e., whether a P value less than significance level α resulted in a rejection of the null hypothesis). Results were obtained using the cor.test function in R (54). We present results for both correlation coefficients in this paper, although Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient is more appropriate due to the monotonic yet curved pattern data exhibit in a majority of the panels in Fig. 5. Simple linear regression lines are included in Fig. 5 as a visual reference for the Pearson’s correlation estimate. A “*” in Table 1 indicates rejection of the null hypothesis that either (i) no linear relationship (Pearson’s) or (ii) no monotonic relationship (Spearman’s rank) between the binned concentration midpoints and the breaks per minute (i.e., H0: ρp = 0 or H0: ρs = 0 as in Table 1 notation). Because of ties when ranking the data for estimating Spearman’s rank correlation, approximate P values were calculated and used to determine significance (53).
The hyperbolic function, h, used to model the binned concentration midpoints and break per minute data (results of which are seen in Table 2 and Fig. 4) is
where c is the functional input of concentration bin midpoint, Kd is the receptor’s dissociation parameter, and M0 is the intercept parameter of the model—both of which are the unknown parameters of the model to be estimated.
Acknowledgments
Funding: This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health grants (R01DA51100 to K.W.P. and T.E.K. and R01AI145206 to K.W.P.), by the Otis Williams Postdoctoral Fellowship Fund (to K.K.L.), and by the Department of Defense through the National Defense Science and Engineering Graduate Fellowship (to N.A.E.).
Author contributions: J.G., M.K.E., K.M.H., K.K.L., N.A.E., and J.M.G. designed and conducted the experiments. K.L.P., P.D.-D., and N.A.-C. developed the in-brain electrode prototype and designed and conducted preliminary experiments. J.G., M.K.E., J.P.H., M.H.M., and W.M. analyzed and modeled the data. M.K.E. and J.P.H. wrote the code for feedback control. J.G., K.L.P., K.W.P., and T.E.K. wrote the manuscript. K.W.P., J.P.H., and T.E.K. provided financial support. K.W.P. and T.E.K. conceived and designed the initial project and supervised its overall execution and share overall responsibility for the presented results and final approval of the article.
Competing interests: K.W.P. owns equity in and consults for a company that is commercializing in vivo EAB sensors. Following the completion of this work, J.G. and K.K.L. became employees at a company that is commercializing in vivo EAB sensors. All other authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Data and materials availability: All data needed to evaluate the conclusions in the paper are present in the paper and/or the Supplementary Materials.
Supplementary Materials
This PDF file includes:
REFERENCES AND NOTES
- 1.J. I. Javaid, M. W. Fischman, C. R. Schuster, H. Dekirmenjian, J. M. Davis, Cocaine plasma concentration: Relation to physiological and subjective effects in humans. Science 202, 227–228 (1978). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.A. R. Jeffcoat, M. Perez-Reyes, J. M. Hill, B. M. Sadler, C. E. Cook, Cocaine disposition in humans after intravenous injection, nasal insufflation (snorting), or smoking. Drug Metab. Dispos. 17, 153–159 (1989). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.J. K. Melichar, M. R. Daglish, D. J. Nutt, Addiction and withdrawal–current views. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 1, 84–90 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.E.-A. Minogianis, W. M. Shams, O. S. Mabrouk, J. M. T. Wong, W. G. Brake, R. T. Kennedy, P. Souich, A. N. Samaha, Varying the rate of intravenous cocaine infusion influences the temporal dynamics of both drug and dopamine concentrations in the striatum. Eur. J. Neurosci. 50, 2054–2064 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.J. E. Usubiaga, F. Moya, J. A. Wikinski, R. Wikinski, L. E. Usubiaga, Relationship between the passage of local anaesthetics across the blood-brain barrier and their effects on the central nervous system. Br. J. Anaesth. 39, 943–947 (1967). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.A. Kitamura, T. Okura, K. Higuchi, Y. Deguchi, Cocktail-dosing microdialysis study to simultaneously assess delivery of multiple organic-cationic drugs to the brain. J. Pharm. Sci. 105, 935–940 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.B. Reiter, T. Stimpfl, Quantification of drugs in brain samples. J. Anal. Toxicol. 39, 702–706 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.P. K. Garg, S. J. Lokitz, R. Nazih, S. Garg, Biodistribution and radiation dosimetry of 11C-nicotine from whole-body pet imaging in humans. J. Nucl. Med. 58, 473–478 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.W. W. Moses, Fundamental limits of spatial resolution in PET. Nucl. Instrum. Methods Phys. Res. A 648 (Suppl. 1), S236–S240 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.J. E. Rose, A. G. Mukhin, S. J. Lokitz, T. G. Turkington, J. Herskovic, F. M. Behm, S. Garg, P. K. Garg, Kinetics of brain nicotine accumulation in dependent and nondependent smokers assessed with PET and cigarettes containing 11C-nicotine. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 5190–5195 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.U. Ungerstedt, Introduction to Intracerebral Microdialysis (Elsevier, 1991). [Google Scholar]
- 12.V. I. Chefer, A. C. Thompson, A. Zapata, T. S. Shippenberg, Overview of brain microdialysis. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. CHAPTER 7, Unit7.1 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.M. Wang, N. D. Hershey, O. S. Mabrouk, R. T. Kennedy, Collection, storage, and electrophoretic analysis of nanoliter microdialysis samples collected from awake animals in vivo. Anal. Bioanal. Chem. 400, 2013–2023 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.C. K. Su, S. C. Hsia, Y. C. Sun, A high-throughput microdialysis-parallel solid phase extraction-inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry hyphenated system for continuous monitoring of extracellular metal ions in living rat brain. J. Chromatogr. A 1326, 73–79 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.T. Ngernsutivorakul, D. J. Steyer, A. C. Valenta, R. T. Kennedy, In vivo chemical monitoring at high spatiotemporal resolution using microfabricated sampling probes and droplet-based microfluidics coupled to mass spectrometry. Anal. Chem. 90, 10943–10950 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.T. Ngernsutivorakul, T. S. White, R. T. Kennedy, Microfabricated probes for studying brain chemistry: A review. ChemPhysChem 19, 1128–1142 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.H. Yang, A. B. Thompson, B. J. McIntosh, S. C. Altieri, A. M. Andrews, Physiologically relevant changes in serotonin resolved by fast microdialysis. ACS Chem. Nerosci. 4, 790–798 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.R. A. Saylor, S. M. Lunte, A review of microdialysis coupled to microchip electrophoresis for monitoring biological events. J. Chromatogr. A 1382, 48–64 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.N. Arroyo-Currás, G. Ortega, D. A. Copp, K. L. Ploense, Z. A. Plaxco, T. E. Kippin, J. P. Hespanha, K. W. Plaxco, High-precision control of plasma drug levels using feedback-controlled dosing. ACS Pharmacol. Transl. Sci. 1, 110–118 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.P. Dauphin-Ducharme, K. Yang, N. Arroyo-Currás, K. L. Ploense, Y. Zhang, J. Gerson, M. Kurnik, T. E. Kippin, M. N. Stojanovic, K. W. Plaxco, Electrochemical aptamer-based sensors for improved therapeutic drug monitoring and high-precision, feedback-controlled drug delivery. ACS Sens. 4, 2832–2837 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.J. P. Newman, M.-f. Fong, D. C. Millard, C. J. Whitmire, G. B. Stanley, S. M. Potter, Optogenetic feedback control of neural activity. eLife 4, e07192 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.M. Prsa, G. L. Galiñanes, D. Huber, Rapid integration of artificial sensory feedback during operant conditioning of motor cortex neurons. Neuron 93, 929–939.e6 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.M. Shin, Y. Wang, J. R. Borgus, B. J. Venton, Electrochemistry at the synapse. Annu. Rev. Anal. Chem. 12, 297–321 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.R. B. Keithley, P. Takmakov, E. S. Bucher, A. M. Belle, C. A. Owesson-White, J. Park, R. M. Wightman, Higher sensitivity dopamine measurements with faster-scan cyclic voltammetry. Anal. Chem. 83, 3563–3571 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.S. Qin, M. Van Der Zeyden, W. H. Oldenziel, T. I. F. H. Cremers, B. H. C. Westerink, Microsensors for in vivo measurement of glutamate in brain tissue. Sensors 8, 6860–6884 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.N. Arroyo-Currás, J. Somerson, P. A. Vieira, K. L. Ploense, T. E. Kippin, K. W. Plaxco, Real-time measurement of small molecules directly in awake, ambulatory animals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 645–650 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.A. Idili, N. Arroyo-Currás, K. L. Ploense, A. T. Csordas, M. Kuwahara, T. E. Kippin, K. W. Plaxco, Seconds-resolved pharmacokinetic measurements of the chemotherapeutic irinotecan in situ in the living body. Chem. Sci. 10, 8164–8170 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.P. H. Azimi, Concentrations of procaine and aqueous penicillin in the cerebrospinal fluid of infants treated for congenital syphilis. J. Pediatr. 124, 649–653 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.W. F. Tu, G. F. Lin, J. F. Shen, J. G. Xu, Changes in erythrocyte membrane ATPases and plasma lipid peroxides in upper abdominal surgery under intravenous procaine-balanced anesthesia. World J. Gastroenterol. 4, 430–433 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.G. Veneziano, J. D. Tobias, Chloroprocaine for epidural anesthesia in infants and children. Pediatr. Anesth. 27, 581–590 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.L. Winter, Intravenous procaine infusions in the postoperative period. Ann. Surg. 132, 143–146 (1950). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.A. A. Shoara, O. Reinstein, O. A. Borhani, T. R. Martin, S. Slavkovic, Z. R. Churcher, P. E. Johnson, Development of a thermal-stable structure-switching cocaine-binding aptamer. Biochimie 145, 137–144 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.A. B. Seifen, A. A. Ferrari, E. E. Seifen, D. S. Thompson, J. Chapman, Pharmacokinetics of intravenous procaine infusion in humans. Anesth. Analg. 58, 382–386 (1979). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.W. W. Qin, Simultaneous determination of procaine, lidocaine, ropivacaine, tetracaine and bupivacaine in human plasma by high-performance liquid chromatography. J. Chromatogr. B Analyt. Technol. Biomed. Life Sci. 878, 1185–1189 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.A. M. Downs, J. Gerson, M. N. Hossain, K. Ploense, M. Pham, H. B. Kraatz, T. Kippin, K. W. Plaxco, Nanoporous gold for the miniaturization of in vivo electrochemical aptamer-based sensors. ACS Sens. 6, 2299–2306 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.V. Hanušová, P. Tomšík, L. Kriesfalusyová, A. Pakostová, I. Boušová, L. Skálová, In vivo effect of oracin on doxorubicin reduction, biodistribution and efficacy in Ehrlich tumor bearing mice. Pharmacol. Rep. 65, 445–452 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.D. Zou, W. Wang, D. Lei, Y. Yin, P. Ren, J. Chen, T. Yin, B. Wang, G. Wang, Y. Wang, Penetration of blood–brain barrier and antitumor activity and nerve repair in glioma by doxorubicin-loaded monosialoganglioside micelles system. Int. J. Nanomed. 12, 4879–4889 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.G. W. Edmonds, Intravenous use of procaine in general anesthesia. JAMA 141, 761 (1949). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.G. Schwarz, Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–464 (1978). [Google Scholar]
- 40.F. J. Fabozzi, The Basics of Financial Econometrics (John Wiley & Sons Inc., 2014).
- 41.G. Rocchitta, O. Secchi, M. D. Alvau, R. Migheli, G. Calia, G. Bazzu, D. Farina, M. S. Desole, R. D. O’Neill, P. A. Serra, Development and characterization of an implantable biosensor for telemetric monitoring of ethanol in the brain of freely moving rats. Anal. Chem. 84, 7072–7079 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.G. Rocchitta, A. T. Peana, G. Bazzu, A. Cossu, S. Carta, P. Arrigo, A. Bacciu, R. Migheli, D. Farina, M. Zinellu, E. Acquas, P. A. Serra, Simultaneous wireless and high-resolution detection of nucleus accumbens shell ethanol concentrations and free motion of rats upon voluntary ethanol intake. Alcohol 78, 69–78 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.I. M. Taylor, Z. du, E. T. Bigelow, J. R. Eles, A. R. Horner, K. A. Catt, S. G. Weber, B. G. Jamieson, X. T. Cui, Aptamer-functionalized neural recording electrodes for the direct measurement of cocaine in vivo. J. Mater. Chem. B 5, 2445–2458 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.S. H. Ahmed, D. Lin, G. F. Koob, L. H. Parsons, Escalation of cocaine self-administration does not depend on altered cocaine-induced nucleus accumbens dopamine levels. J. Neurochem. 86, 102–113 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.C. W. Bradberry, J. B. Nobiletti, J. D. Elsworth, B. Murphy, P. Jatlow, R. H. Roth, Cocaine and cocaethylene: Microdialysis comparison of brain drug levels and effects on dopamine and serotonin. J. Neurochem. 60, 1429–1435 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Y. L. Hurd, J. Kehr, U. Ungerstedt, In vivo microdialysis as a technique to monitor drug transport: Correlation of extracellular cocaine levels and dopamine overflow in the rat brain. J. Neurochem. 51, 1314–1316 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.H.-T. Pan, S. Menacherry, J. B. Justice Jr., Differences in the pharmacokinetics of cocaine in naive and cocaine-experienced rats. J. Neurochem. 56, 1299–1306 (1991). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.H. O. Pettit, A. J. Pettit, Disposition of cocaine in blood and brain after a single pretreatment. Brain Res. 651, 261–268 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.B. S. Ferguson, D. A. Hoggarth, D. Maliniak, K. Ploense, R. J. White, N. Woodward, K. Hsieh, A. J. Bonham, M. Eisenstein, T. Kippin, K. W. Plaxco, H. T. Soh, Real-time, aptamer-based tracking of circulating therapeutic agents in living animals. Sci. Transl. Med. 5, 213ra165 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.IACUC, Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals: Eighth Edition (The National Academies Press, 2011). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.S. D. Curtis, K. L. Ploense, M. Kurnik, G. Ortega, C. Parolo, T. E. Kippin, K. W. Plaxco, N. Arroyo-Currás, Open source software for the real-time control, processing, and visualization of high-volume electrochemical data. Anal. Chem. 91, 12321–12328 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.D. D. Wackerly, W. Mendenhall, R. L. Scheaffer, Mathematical statistics with applications (Thomson Brooks/Cole, 2008).
- 53.M. Hollander, D. A. Wolfe, E. Chicken, Nonparametric Statistical Methods, 3rd Edition|Wiley (John Wiley & Sons Inc., 2014).
- 54.R Core Team, R: A language and environemnt for statistical computing (2020); www.R-project.org/.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.