Skip to main content
Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2023 May 18:1–15. Online ahead of print. doi: 10.1038/s44159-023-00190-z

A systemic approach to the psychology of racial bias within individuals and society

Allison L Skinner-Dorkenoo 1,, Meghan George 2, James E Wages III 3, Sirenia Sánchez 2, Sylvia P Perry 2,4,5,
PMCID: PMC10196321  PMID: 37361392

Abstract

Historically, the field of psychology has focused on racial biases at an individual level, considering the effects of various stimuli on individual racial attitudes and biases. This approach has provided valuable information, but not enough focus has been placed on the systemic nature of racial biases. In this Review, we examine the bidirectional relation between individual-level racial biases and broader societal systems through a systemic lens. We argue that systemic factors operating across levels — from the interpersonal to the cultural — contribute to the production and reinforcement of racial biases in children and adults. We consider the effects of five systemic factors on racial biases in the USA: power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives and values, segregated communities, shared stereotypes and nonverbal messages. We discuss evidence that these factors shape individual-level racial biases, and that individual-level biases shape systems and institutions to reproduce systemic racial biases and inequalities. We conclude with suggestions for interventions that could limit the effects of these influences and discuss future directions for the field.

Subject terms: Social behaviour, Human behaviour, Psychology, Culture, Society


Psychology research typically focuses on biases at the individual level rather than across broader societal systems. In this Review, Skinner-Dorkenoo and colleagues consider how systemic factors contribute to individual-level racial biases in the USA and vice versa.

Introduction

The field of psychology so far has primarily focused on racial bias at an individual level, centring the effects of various stimuli on the racial biases of individuals111. Racial bias refers to favouring or providing preferential treatment to members of one racial group over another. There is no doubt that the approach of focusing on personally held racial prejudices and discrimination has provided valuable information about the psychology of racial biases. However, this approach largely ignores the systemic nature of racial biases and the ways in which racial biases are shaped by the broader cultural systems in which people live1214. For instance, the focus on individual-level biases has contributed to the burgeoning industry of diversity and implicit bias trainings15 that aim to address problems such as police brutality against Black people16. Although potentially useful for changing individual attitudes and/or biases, these interventions seem unlikely to adequately address the underlying causes of bias, which stem from the systems and structures that create and reinforce racial inequality.

The overemphasis on the individual level in research on racial biases seems to have come at the expense of psychological research and theorizing about the impact of broader contextual factors, and how individual-level racial biases reinforce broader systemic patterns of oppression. Models of nested levels of influence in the study of race relations17, human development18 and culture19,20 can be used to consider the relation between racial bias and broader systemic factors (Fig. 1). Each level of influence influences the innermost level of individual attitudes, and conversely, individual-level attitudes also influence systems21.

Fig. 1. Diagram of nested levels of influence.

Fig. 1

a, The nested-levels framework focuses on how each systemic level of influence influences individual-level attitudes, and how individual-level attitudes influence systems. The most proximal influences on racial bias are personal and interpersonal experiences, which are nested within communities and institutions that set the local context for interpersonal experiences. Communities are situated within broader cultural contexts that shape the norms, values and beliefs that structure society. At the outermost level are temporal influences, which capture how past manifestations of these systems continue to influence members of society. b, There is a bidirectional influence from each of the systemic levels to the individual level and from the individual level back out to each of the systemic levels, within the five factors discussed in the Review.

Five key systemic factors — power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives and values, segregated communities, shared stereotypes, and nonverbal messages — influence racial bias across the nested levels of this framework. Although they are certainly not the only systemic factors that influence racial biases, we focus on these five because we believe them to be particularly relevant to the development of racial biases in the contemporary USA. At the innermost level are the most proximal influences on individual-level racial bias — personal and interpersonal experiences, such as socialization from caregivers and interracial friendships. These experiences are nested within communities and institutions that set the local context for interpersonal experiences, such as the racial diversity in a school or neighbourhood community. Communities are situated within a broader cultural context that shapes the norms, values, and beliefs that structure society. At the outermost level are temporal influences, which capture how past interpersonal, institutional or community, and societal influences continue to influence members of society throughout their lives. Although the primary focus is on how each level of influence affects individual-level attitudes, the levels also influence one another. For instance, culture can shape organizations and interpersonal experiences within that culture, as well as individual-level biases. Likewise, individual-level racial biases can mould interpersonal experiences, which can shape factors at the organizational and community level.

In this Review, we recognize the bidirectional relation between individual-level racial biases and broader societal systems across levels of influence. In some cases, there is clear evidence of the causal chain from system to individual and from individual to system, whereas in others, there might be evidence of an association, but the direction of influence is unclear. In many cases, individual-level biases and broader societal systems might be mutually reinforcing, but for clarity we take the approach of assessing each direction of influence separately. We examine the role of power and privilege, cultural narratives and values, racial segregation, shared cultural stereotypes and nonverbal signals in racial bias. In each section, we first discuss what is known about the contextual factors that produce and reinforce individual-level biases before highlighting how individual-level biases shape institutions and systems. Although our primary focus is on how these systemic factors influence racial biases, we also use the nested levels of influence framework to examine how individual-level racial biases held by the public can contribute to the reinforcement and perpetuation of systemic oppression at the interpersonal, institutional and/or community, societal and temporal levels. We conclude with implications for interventions aimed at reducing racial bias and discuss future directions for research.

To appropriately analyse the factors that perpetuate racial bias at a systemic level, it is critical to contextualize within culture. We focus on the USA because it is the context that we have the cultural knowledge to discuss and where most research on racial bias development has been conducted. The racial context of the USA is distinct in several ways. Notably, white European colonizers violently stole the land that comprises the USA from indigenous inhabitants across multiple centuries22. Furthermore, the enslavement of Black people was legal and common practice for over 200 years in North America23,24. Citizenship was largely restricted to white people for most of the history of the USA, with full citizenship not open to people of all ethnicities until 1952 (ref. 25). Throughout this history, white people have accrued power, wealth, status and numerical majority status through systems that intentionally oppress and marginalize people of colour, including Native Americans, African Americans and members of other ethnic groups. Although modern laws bar racial discrimination, substantial racial inequalities between white people and people of colour persist in the USA, in areas including wealth, education and health2628. Given this history of racism in the USA, the systemic factors that perpetuate racial biases into the present might be somewhat distinct from other contexts. Despite this focus on the USA, this Review could be valuable for understanding similar patterns of bias and oppression outside the USA. Some of these similarities are highlighted in the book Caste: The Origins Of Our Discontents29 — which draws parallels between the systems of oppression of Black people in the USA, Jewish people in Nazi Germany and Dalit people in India. Although thoroughly analysing such parallels is beyond the scope of this Review, we briefly discuss systemic factors that perpetuate biases based on socially constructed categories — such as race — in other cultural contexts in the concluding section.

Power and privilege disparities

Power and privilege disparities set the initial conditions within which other factors operate. Systemic inequalities in the distribution of power and privilege serve as the societal backdrop in the USA, directly contributing to individual-level racial biases. Many residents of the USA grow up in an environment in which their doctors, lawyers, teachers, government officials, entrepreneurs, and people occupying other respected roles are white3033. Thus, to these residents, the USA might look like it ‘belongs to’ white people.

Children who are socialized in an unequal society, without systemic explanations for why power and privilege have been concentrated among certain people, often internalize that system34. Both children and adults tend to conclude that the way things are structured in society is the way they should be3542. For example, experimental evidence suggests that children in the USA tend to prefer people who are relatively more fortunate, even if that good fortune is simply due to luck43, and when given the opportunity to rectify existing resource inequalities, they often exacerbate inequalities by giving more to the person who already has more resources44. These laboratory findings suggest that when children are socialized in an environment in which people in a particular social group have more resources, they will tend to favour people in that social group and distribute resources in ways that perpetuate resource disparities. Thus, in the USA — where racial wealth disparities are large and growing28 — systemic inequalities predispose children to infer that white people are better than and deserve to have more than people of colour45. Among adults, markers of structural racial inequalities can also predict racial bias. Attending a university with few faculty members of colour, living in an area that has high poverty rates among Black residents and living in a community with low economic mobility all predict heightened implicit bias against Black people among non-Black American residents7,46. In other words, children and adults are motivated to justify the systems in which they are socialized41,42.

When progressive changes in society challenge ingrained expectations of inequality, racial biases can be heightened. White residents of the USA who were exposed to information about the increasing racial diversity of the USA subsequently exhibited increased bias favouring white people relative to those who were not exposed to this information4750. Emphasizing the racially historic milestone of Barack Obama being elected as president of the USA also increased implicit pro-white bias among white American residents, relative to those who were not exposed to this information50. This research suggests that racial inequalities at various levels of influence — from local communities to the broader cultural context — can lead people to believe that inequality is natural and justified, and that racial progress that challenges those inequalities might further intensify individual-level racial bias.

Next, we turn to the role of individual-level racial biases in perpetuating systemic racial inequalities. Individual-level racial biases have been argued to impact systemic disparities in power and privilege in a variety of domains, including government representation, population health, education, employment, and immigration51. Perhaps the most direct impact of individual-level racial biases on systemic outcomes can be seen in voting behaviour. Greater individual-level anti-Black bias was associated with a lower likelihood of voting for Barack Obama in the 2008 American presidential election and reduced support for his healthcare reform proposal52,53. With regard to health, white residents of the USA (especially those with higher racial bias) were less supportive of COVID-19 pandemic precautions when they were more aware — based on prior knowledge or experimental exposure to information — that COVID-19 was disproportionately affecting people of colour in the USA5456. Other work has tied racial disparities in health and healthcare access to the average individual-level bias against Black people held by white residents in their county of residence53,57. In locations where white residents had higher racial biases, Medicaid disability expenditures (which particularly benefit people of colour) were lower, and Black residents had reduced access to healthcare and increased rates of circulatory-disease-related death53,57. Educational disparities have also been linked to individual-level racial biases. In American counties where the individual-level bias against Black people is stronger, there are larger racial disparities in school disciplinary actions, with Black students being subjected to more suspensions, expulsions, and referrals to law enforcement than white students58. These findings suggest that individual-level biases of residents of the USA can influence presidential elections, community health inequalities and school disciplinary actions, reflecting long-term temporal impacts on the privilege and power afforded to people of colour at cultural, community, and interpersonal levels.

In sum, racial disparities in power and privilege have been built into the societal system of the USA, resulting in wide-ranging effects on the life outcomes of residents of the USA. This system also affects the attitudes of those living within the system, leading to expectations of inequality and beliefs that inequality is justified. These beliefs and expectations then motivate the individuals that make up the system to behave in ways that maintain the system of inequality, such that individuals’ attitudes reinforce the system of inequality that produced them.

Cultural narratives and values

The concentration of power and privilege among white people in the USA means that white people largely write the histories, set the norms and define the values of American society. This centring of white people can be seen in historical narratives, cultural products and cultural beliefs, which can all contribute to the development of individual-level racial biases59. Below, we discuss the role of each of these aspects of culture in shaping racial biases.

Historical narratives

Historical narratives can play an important part in how people view themselves and others in society10. In educational curricula in the USA, national history tends to be taught through a white-affirming lens, such that the attitudes, values and perspectives of white people are implicitly or explicitly justified by the historical narrative60,61. The perspectives and experiences of people of colour are often omitted from curricula entirely. For instance, despite their many historical contributions to the USA62, Asian Americans are vastly underrepresented in American textbooks6366.

For much of the history of the USA, when people of colour were discussed in textbooks, they were described with derogatory and dehumanizing stereotypes that justified their marginalization60,67. Moreover, egregious acts perpetrated by white people have often been presented in a sanitized way that minimizes, glosses over, and justifies them6870. As an example, ‘manifest destiny’ (the worldview that white people were destined to expand their territory to the west coast of North America) is often presented uncritically as a justification for atrocities that white people committed against the original Native American and Mexican inhabitants of the continent60,71. Similarly, Confederate symbols — which celebrate the southern American states that went to war with northern American states to maintain the institution of slavery — are argued to be a race-neutral representation of Southern pride in some textbooks and by some modern pundits and continue to be displayed at some courthouses7274. The way history is usually portrayed in American society therefore obscures the relation between contemporary systems and racial injustices of the past61. Some states have even created laws explicitly barring the teaching of critical history related to race75.

The way history is presented in society shapes individual-level attitudes about race and racism. School curricula can be vital contributors to ethnocentric biases in childhood76. Furthermore, adults who have less knowledge of the racial injustices of the past tend to be less aware of present-day racism77. Exposure to historical narratives that centre white people and glorify the nation reduce awareness of racial injustices among school children, college students, and adult museum visitors70,78. Furthermore, experimental evidence suggests that exposure to the Confederate flag (versus no exposure to the flag) can increase racial biases and promote racial injustice79. Thus, how history is portrayed at a cultural level, in textbooks, and in community schools and institutions (such as museums and memorials) has the potential to influence individual-level racial biases.

Individuals receive and simultaneously reproduce and uphold these historical narratives. For instance, it was argued in 1963 that the individual-level racial biases of historians were to blame for the history of Native Americans being oversimplified, mischaracterized and/or overlooked entirely80. Although there have been changes to the framing of the history of the USA over the intervening 60 years, many of the issues identified persist to this day81. For example, Christopher Columbus is still often credited with and praised for ‘discovering’ North America, even though it was already inhabited by thriving interconnected societies of millions of people22.

The effects of individual-level biases can also be seen at the community and interpersonal levels. The same history and culture can be represented differently depending on who is curating and constructing the representation78,82. As an example, students who reported that being white was central to their identity reported more negative attitudes towards Black History Month representations that were curated and displayed in schools in which the majority of students were Black (versus those that were curated and displayed in schools in which the majority of students were white)70. This finding is particularly meaningful because Black History Month representations in schools with a majority of Black students were generally more supportive of anti-racism than those in schools with a majority of white students. Another study provided evidence that patrons of the Ellis Island Immigration Museum tended to identify exhibits that painted the USA in a positive light as more important than exhibits highlighting historical injustices78. Patrons who reported more assimilationist attitudes — such as believing that to ‘be truly American’ means speaking English — particularly disliked the exhibits highlighting historical injustices. All of these findings converge to suggest that individual-level biases shape the way people in the USA think about and portray history at personal, community, cultural and temporal levels.

Cultural products

At a societal level, the cultural products — such as art forms, varieties of dress and appearance, and styles of speech — of white people tend to be the most highly regarded. The art, music, and dance that are most culturally valued in the USA are rooted in white European traditions83,84. For instance, professional dance schools largely focus on ballet and modern dance85 and music departments primarily emphasize classical music education — marginalizing the study of dance and music traditions developed by artists of colour86,87. Expectations for appropriate dress and communication are also centred around norms established by white Americans. In some cases, schools88 and workplaces89 have created policies and municipalities have passed laws90 prohibiting styles of dress that are culturally linked to people of colour, such as durags, hijabs, and sagging pants. Even when these marginalized styles are not explicitly banned, there might be added scrutiny of individuals who wear them91. As an example, Black women are often expected to conform to white femininity norms by straightening their hair to be perceived as professional90,92,93. Furthermore, grammatical rules and standard linguistic styles in the USA are based on the language practices of white Americans94 and deviations from these norms — such as use of African American Vernacular English (AAVE) — are often cited as evidence of inferiority9598. Although there can be conditional acceptance and appropriation of elements of the cultures of people of colour such as styles of dress99, the typical situation is the prioritization of cultural products of white people.

In summary, the cultural products of people of colour are devalued and stigmatized at a societal level, which results in individual-level biases against those who use and produce these cultural products. This contribution to individual-level racial bias is particularly insidious because it creates conditions under which people can obliviously perpetuate racial biases, believing their bias to be against the cultural products they perceive as inferior rather than the people associated with the products. In one hypothetical scenario at an interpersonal level, parents might pass along individual-level racial biases to their children when parents conclude that the classmate with dreadlocked hair (a hairstyle with African roots that is common among Black Americans) looks like a troublemaker. In this way, Americans can look down on Black people who engage with Black cultural products — like listening to hip hop music, having dreadlocks or using AAVE — while still believing themselves to be ‘not racist’. Devaluing Black cultural products perpetuates racism, because doing so suggests that Black culture is inferior to white culture.

Individual-level biases can also feed back to contribute to the reproduction of biased cultural products. White people are more likely than people of other races to rise to positions of economic power and influence100, and therefore to enact their individual biases and serve as gatekeepers for the promotion of certain cultural products over others. At the most extreme, individual-level biases of white people in power have resulted in cultural genocide, such as through Native American boarding schools, which systematically destroyed Native American families and communities101. The white people who created and promoted Native American boarding schools argued that Native American communities were dangerous and that Native children needed to be rescued by ‘good Christians’102. These arguments were consistent with widespread beliefs about the cultural inferiority of Native Americans, which allowed racist policies, such as those forcing all Native American children to live in boarding schools that barred them from practising their culture, seem acceptable.

Evidence of the effects of widely held individual-level white-centric biases can also be observed on a cultural level in academic fields, influencing their methods, standards, and knowledge bases (for example, in mathematics103,104, psychology105, and written composition87,94). As an example, individual-level biases towards white cultural products arguably led to the development of writing standards that privilege white styles of discourse106. From a historical perspective, although Native Americans had a diverse array of numeric systems that were still in use at the turn of the twentieth century107, individual-level biases towards white people and culture led ‘Western mathematics’ to be established as the standard for mathematics education in the USA, marginalizing indigenous knowledge103. The effect of individual-level biases among key decision-makers can also be seen in policy decisions, such as whose cultural holidays are officially recognized as public holidays108 and whose cultural knowledge frames the questions used in standardized testing109111.

Cultural beliefs

Widely held cultural beliefs and philosophies can shape the way in which individuals make sense of society, contributing to individual-level racial biases. In the USA, the ‘American dream’ — which asserts that anyone can achieve success if they are willing to put in the work — is a dominant philosophy in government, education and the media. The belief that people who merit success will ultimately achieve it can serve as a beacon of hope for those who are striving to raise their social status, while also justifying to people with the highest status that they have rightfully earned their positions. Thus, promoting the American dream reinforces tendencies for people to justify and reinforce the systems of inequality in which they were socialized41,42,112,113. Indeed, priming American residents with messages promoting meritocracy (versus other types of messages) can reduce recognition of unearned privilege114 and increase blame placed on people disadvantaged by societal systems115. Given the many racial inequalities in American society, teaching children that people who deserve success achieve it conveys the implicit message that most people of colour do not merit the status and success that white people enjoy in the USA45 — imparting individual-level racial bias.

Racially colourblind ideology (the idea of disregarding the issue of race) is another cultural philosophy that can contribute to the perpetuation of individual-level racial biases. Polling data from 2020 indicated that approximately 40% of American residents believed that paying less attention to race would improve racial inequalities in society116. Yet, evidence suggests that racially colourblind messages provide a way for racially biased messages to discreetly influence public attitudes. For example, white Americans are much more likely to be persuaded to adopt policy positions through subtle racial appeals referring to Black people — such as references to the ‘inner city’ or ‘culture of poverty’ — than they are to be persuaded by explicit racial appeals that refer to Black people as lazy and uneducated welfare recipients117,118. Politicians have deliberately used these ‘dog whistles’ to capitalize on racial stereotypes and gain public support for racist political agendas119,120. Even ostensibly well intentioned colourblind attitudes and policies can perpetuate racial biases121. For instance, exposing children to the racially colourblind mindset, compared to a diversity mindset, reduced their ability to identify racially biased incidents and appropriately report them to teachers122. Promoting racial colourblindness at a societal, institutional or even an interpersonal level can reduce individual-level awareness of systemic racism and increase susceptibility to racially biased messages.

In the reverse direction, individual-level racial biases can also influence cultural beliefs. Americans with higher levels of racial bias tend to report a stronger belief in meritocracy123,124. Moreover, young, white adults with relatively high socio-economic status are more likely than people of other races and lower economic status to believe that the USA is a meritocracy125. Individual-level racial biases are also associated with more support for racially colourblind sentiments, such as ‘society would be better off if we all stopped talking about race’126,127. Some psychologists have theorized that racial biases can motivate racially colourblind perspectives that help to maintain ignorance of racial injustices128. Racially colourblind ideology has also served to perpetuate systemic racism through government policies (Box 1). Taken together, individual-level racial biases have been associated with cultural philosophies that obscure and reinforce racial inequalities across societal levels.

Altogether, historical narratives that exclude people of colour and downplay the history of racism in the USA, cultural norms that devalue the cultural products of people of colour, and cultural beliefs that obscure systems of inequality contribute to the development and maintenance of individual-level racial biases. Once individual-level racial biases have been established, these attitudes cumulatively shape how history is told, what cultural knowledge and products make up the mainstream, and what cultural beliefs are promoted.

Box 1 Colourblind ideology.

In the USA, there are numerous examples of societal-level colourblind racial ideology being used to uphold systemic racial oppression. For instance, following the Brown v. Board of Education Supreme Court ruling (1954), separate schools for Black and white students were ruled unconstitutional. Consequently, school boards in multiple American states continued to segregate students by race but claimed that school assignments were now based on ‘fit’ and ‘ability’, rather than race264. In this example, the colourblind ideology of assignment by ability was a means of perpetuating systemic oppression while also obscuring systemic racism and reinforcing racist stereotypes about intellectual abilities. A similar series of events took place following the Batson v. Kentucky (1986) Supreme Court decision, which ruled that potential jurors could not be removed on the basis of their race. Following the decision, attorneys continued to remove Black potential jurors, but now provided a colourblind rationale265,266. Jurors can be struck for such arbitrary reasons as wearing a hat, being unemployed, or the prosecutor simply having a bad feeling about them266.

Perhaps the greatest effect of the use of colourblind ideology emerged as part of the so-called War on Drugs, a federal campaign in the USA that began in 1971 to crack down on the possession and sale of illegal drugs. One of its most notorious policies set a penalty 100 times higher for possession of substances that were disproportionately used by Black American residents than for substances disproportionately used by white American residents210. The War on Drugs has been credited with the massive increase in the prison population and dramatic growth of racial disparities in incarceration rates in the USA during the late twentieth century267. As of 2021, Black Americans were incarcerated at roughly five times the rate of white Americans268,269. Overall, racially colourblind ideology has allowed systemic policies that are ostensibly race-neutral to remain racially biased in effect.

Segregated communities

Segregation in neighbourhoods, workplaces, and classrooms is another systemic influence on individual-level racial bias. Segregation in the USA is often a result of the power and privilege disparities and cultural narratives discussed above. For instance, ‘redlining’ was a mid-twentieth-century policy in which neighbourhoods were graded (from ‘desirable’ to ‘hazardous’) according to their supposed risk of decreasing in value. Neighbourhoods populated by people of colour were assigned the lowest scores (demarcated by a red border on a map), which both reduced the value of these homes and prevented homebuyers from accessing federally backed and insured loans129,130. The enduring impact of this policy is that families of colour have not been able to build the generational wealth through homeownership that white people have28,131. To this day, homeowners in neighbourhoods that were deemed hazardous have dramatically less home equity than homeowners in areas that were deemed desirable132. Thus, residents of the USA see that nice homes and other symbols of wealth are associated with white people, promoting and reinforcing individual-level pro-white biases130.

The echoes of this policy also reinforce residential segregation because homebuyers with adequate means (who are more likely to be white) are motivated to avoid redlined neighbourhoods, where homes tend to be devalued and appreciate far less over time132. Thus, racial segregation remains commonplace in the USA. For instance, the average white American lives in a neighbourhood whose residents are 75% white133. Similarly, workplace racial segregation was higher in the 2010s than it was in the 1980s and 1990s134. Although public school segregation was ruled unconstitutional in 1954 (ref. 135), the last school district only fully integrated in 2017 (ref. 136). Even within racially integrated schools, a biased system of sorting students into different and unequal course tracks results in overrepresentation of white students in the more advanced and better resourced tracks137.

Ongoing segregation also limits intergroup contact138 — contact between people belonging to different racial or ethnic groups — one of the most reliable and best studied predictors of reduced individual-level bias139. Close contact between groups has been argued to reduce prejudice when those groups share common goals, hold equal status, cooperate with each other and are supported by the broader societal system138. Systemic racism in the USA reduces the likelihood of these conditions being met, but the bulk of the evidence indicates that even when optimal conditions are not met, positive intergroup contact reduces prejudice140142. Thus, the fact that so many social environments — including communities, institutions and interpersonal experiences — in the USA remain racially segregated probably contributes to the persistence of individual-level racial biases.

Limited opportunities for close contact might be particularly detrimental when groups share the same geographic space but lack close contact with one another. A 2015 study found that racial biases among white residents of the USA were highest in states where the Black population was largest143. A follow-up study examined how this pattern relates to intergroup contact, finding that living in a state with more Black residents was only associated with increased racial bias among white residents who had limited close contact with Black people144. Thus, when white people live alongside people of colour in their communities without forming close relationships, racial bias might increase. As such, racial segregation in racially diverse regions and states seems particularly likely to engender individual-level racial biases because intergroup exposure can elicit group threat145, without the psychological benefits provided by meaningful emotional connections developed through close intergroup contact.

Individual-level racial biases can also reinforce segregated communities and systems. Black communities in the USA are stereotyped as being impoverished, crime-ridden, rundown, dangerous, dirty, and ‘ghetto’146,147. If key decision-makers or a critical mass of members of the public hold these biases, devaluation of physical spaces associated with people of colour (such as schools and neighbourhoods) can result148151. There is experimental evidence that homes in predominantly Black neighbourhoods and homes owned by Black (versus white) people tend to be devalued. When white residents of the USA were asked to assign value to a home, they thought the home was worth less if an image of a Black (relative to white) family appeared in front of the home, even though all other factors were the same146. The same patterns emerge in actual housing data152. Schools in predominantly Black neighbourhoods are undervalued, receiving less funding per student than schools in predominantly white neighbourhoods153.

This disregard for neighbourhoods populated by people of colour can also be seen in decisions about infrastructure, such as where to place hazardous waste dumps and what communities to displace when new amenities such as freeways and railroads are introduced into communities. Eminent domain — a legal power that forces private citizens to sell land to the government for public projects —is more likely to be used in communities of colour154,155 and has myriad negative consequences, including loss of wealth and disruption of community ties and stability156,157. Experimental evidence has indicated that white Americans are less likely to oppose placing a hypothetical chemical plant next to a Black neighbourhood than next to a white neighbourhood146. Real-world data are consistent with this hypothetical scenario: the highest polluting industries and sites for toxic waste disposal in the USA tend to be in areas with large populations of Black people158,159. By contrast, experimental evidence indicates that landmarks that are of interest to white people (such as their workplaces, schools, pools, golf clubs and tennis clubs) tend to be placed relatively far from communities of colour160. Furthermore, analyses of existing institutional policies show that recreation facilities that are closer to communities of colour have more exclusionary barriers — such as fees or dress codes160,161. The history of practices of this kind explain the relatively low swimming rates among Black Americans and dramatically higher drowning rates among Black (relative to white) American residents162164.

Overall, individual-level racial biases can contribute to the devaluation and negative stereotyping of physical spaces occupied by people of colour, which make those spaces less desirable to white people—reinforcing racial segregation. These racial biases might ultimately contribute to a host of systemic and structural racial inequalities within domains ranging from health and wealth to education, which adversely impact people of colour for generations.

Shared stereotypes

Shared stereotypes can spread cultural narratives and justify oppressive systems. Propaganda campaigns have been used to spread stereotypes about people of colour throughout the history of the USA. For instance, nineteenth-century media stereotypes of men of Chinese descent as weak and effeminate165,166 were used to marginalize these men, painting them as poorly suited for traditionally masculine jobs and undesirable as husbands167. Propaganda about intellect, criminality, and sexuality has been used by the media, politicians, industries, and scientists to exercise social control over Black Americans and justify their enslavement and subordination for centuries23,168,169 (and see a preprint170).

Stereotypical representations of people of colour continue in the contemporary media34. On television, people of colour tend to be depicted in negative and stereotypical ways171173, as outside mainstream contemporary society174 or not represented at all175. News coverage also tends to depict people of colour in a negative light176178 and Black criminal suspects are often overrepresented in news media — perpetuating stereotypes that Black people are ‘criminal’ and ‘reckless’179,180. These stereotypes represent only a few of the many ways people of colour are negatively stereotyped in the USA. Meanwhile, white people are overrepresented in media coverage of crime victims174,181,182.

Societal stereotypes are also shared in more subtle ways. Shared cultural stereotypes can develop through the selection and transmission of specific pieces of information about social groups183,184 and consequently stereotypes that have no connection to actual traits can form and spread widely. A variety of linguistic biases have been identified as ways in which stereotypes are subtly spread through social groups185187. For instance, more specific and trivial wording — such as ‘stole a pack of gum’ — might be used to describe the theft behaviours of white people, whereas more abstract and dramatic terms — such as ‘shoplifted’ — might be used for Black people188. There is also a noted tendency for media to use the passive voice when reporting on structural and systemic harms perpetrated against people of colour — reporting, for instance, that ‘Black Lives Matter protesters were teargassed’ as opposed to the more active phrasing ‘police teargassed Black Lives Matter protesters’189. Knowledge of these shared stereotypes can lead to individual-level biases190,191. White residents of the USA with more exposure to stereotypical portrayals of people of colour tend to report more racial-stereotype-consistent perceptions177,192,193 and greater bias against people of colour175. More time spent watching local news — often containing stereotypical representations of people of colour — has been associated with increased bias against members of marginalized groups194196. Thus, shared stereotypes at several of the nested levels of influence, from interpersonal interactions to the societal level through media, can shape individual-level racial biases.

Individual-level racial biases also have the potential to shape shared stereotypes at various levels. For example, a white individual’s racial biases might lead them to perceive a Black man walking in their neighbourhood as suspicious. The white individual might share their concerns with their neighbours (in person or through online message boards), spreading associations between Black people and criminality. After being primed by this message, another neighbour might call the police on a Black man in the neighbourhood. Neighbours who see the police treating the man as a suspect might infer that he is a criminal, further reinforcing inaccurate stereotypes of Black people as criminals. Given widespread anti-Black biases in the USA197 and known tendencies to associate Black people with crime198,199, scenarios like this are not unlikely. Indeed, Black men in the USA are more likely than white men to be stopped, searched, handcuffed, and arrested by police200203. Racial biases can also result in much graver outcomes. White American residents with stronger anti-Black biases also tend to be stronger supporters of gun rights204,205 (though less so if they are primed to think about Black gun owners206,207), and in another study white residents of the USA were less concerned about gun deaths when the victims are Black than when they are white208. Moreover, where residents had greater implicit biases against Black people, there was more disproportionate police lethal force directed towards Black residents209.

The fact that most people in the USA have individual-level racial biases144 also limits support for policy change and allows systemic racial inequalities in the criminal justice system to be overlooked8,210,211. Perceiving Black men as threatening is associated with increased fear of crime and reduced support for system-level reform, such as police body cameras and matching the demographics of a police force to their community212. Individual-level racial bias might also lead the public to reify racial inequalities when faced with evidence of racial disparities in the American criminal justice system213,214. For instance, Americans who were told about more extreme racial disparities in the prison population of the USA were less willing to support system-level reforms215. Overall, individual-level racial biases can lead to behaviours that transmit racial stereotypes at interpersonal and community levels, which ultimately have the potential to reinforce systemic racial inequalities in communities and in broader society with long-term ramifications.

Nonverbal messages

People are constantly exposed to nonverbal messages, such as facial expressions and body language, in contexts from workplaces and schools216 to depictions in the media217. These nonverbal signals can often convey societal messages, such as racial biases and cultural messages about race. For example, for most of the history of the USA, especially in the southern states, Black people were expected to demean themselves to white people through nonverbal behaviours, for instance by stepping off the sidewalk and removing their hats when a white person passed by218. These nonverbal signals were choreographed to communicate white superiority, and failure to adhere to this racial choreography could have violent — even deadly — consequences for Black people24,218. These racialized expectations for respect persist. For instance, analyses of recorded police interactions with the public from within the past five years indicated that officers spoke less respectfully and had less friendliness and respect in their tone of voice when interacting with Black men than with white men219,220.

Nonverbal racial biases have also been documented in American media. For example, white characters on primetime television often receive more warm, positive nonverbal signals, such as smiles, than do Black characters217. Exposure to nonverbal biases of this kind can influence the attitudes of children and adults216,221223, increasing negative attitudes towards Black people217,224226. Even exposure to nonverbal bias towards a single member of a group has the potential to produce group biases. For instance, adults and children who observed a series of brief interactions in which one person systematically received more warm, friendly nonverbal signals than another person subsequently favoured the person who received the more positive nonverbal signals, and also favoured others of their fictitious nationality222,227. Thus, if a sufficient number of people in a community or society — especially people with social influence, such as teachers or community leaders — display nonverbal biases favouring white people, other individuals in that context will also develop the same biases. For instance, if children observe a systematic pattern of white role models seeming more anxious when passing Black people on the street, that will shape their attitudes and emotions about who they feel safe around.

Individual-level biases can also shape people, communities, and culture. Individuals with more racially biased attitudes tend to show more racially biased nonverbal behaviour in interracial interactions228231. Thus, individual-level attitudes seem to shape nonverbal behaviour in interpersonal interactions. Exposure to this nonverbal manifestation of individual-level bias can also influence the behaviours of others. Children who are exposed to biased nonverbal signals in social interactions often adopt the biased behaviours they observe221,222. For instance, preschool children who observed a member of one group receive more nonverbal warmth than a member of another group were subsequently more likely to choose their playmates from the former group222. Furthermore, 40% of preschool children who were exposed to biased nonverbal signals in a study mimicked the biased nonverbal signals they observed232.

If biased nonverbal signals and the attitudes they convey are easily transmitted and adopted by an individual, it follows that they will propagate beyond the individual to their social network. Thus, nonverbal biases can be thought of as an expanding system of transmission wherein exposure to nonverbal biases contributes to the development of racially biased attitudes; those attitudes then manifest as biased nonverbal signals, which others within the social network will then see, leading them to develop biases, and so on233.

The tendency to transmit racial biases nonverbally might be exacerbated by the fact that people tend to justify the nonverbal biases they observe. Even when the targets of (positively or negatively) biased nonverbal signals displayed identical behaviours, observers were more likely to report that the targets’ behaviour (57%), rather than how the target was treated (30%), influenced their attitudes towards the target223,227. In other words, when the only thing that varied was how targets were treated by others, observers still justified their own attitudes by attributing them to the targets’ actions.

Taken together, if the majority of individuals making up a social system hold the same individual racial biases, repeated exposure and reinforcement of those racial biases through nonverbal behaviours can potentially shape attitudes and reinforce systemic oppression at interpersonal, institutional (classrooms) and societal (media) levels. Moreover, when nonverbal messages propagate racial biases to children, they spread racial biases temporally, to future generations.

Implications for psychological interventions

Given our thesis that systemic factors strongly contribute to the development and perpetuation of racist attitudes and beliefs, the importance of changing these systemic factors (including systems, policies, and practices) cannot be overstated. However, we focus our discussion here on psychological interventions (Table 1). We frame our discussion of interventions in terms of what can be done at a psychological level to interrupt or limit the effects of the systemic influences and feedback cycles identified above.

Table 1.

Systemic influences and associated intervention opportunities

Systemic influence System → individual Individual → system Intervention opportunities
Power and privilege

Children and adults tend to conclude that the way things are (such as racial wealth disparities) is the way they should be3542

Increased exposure to structural racial inequalities is associated with increased racial bias7,46

Racial progress that challenges inequalities can intensify individual-level racial bias4750

Racial biases seem to promote systemic disparities in power and privilege in a variety of domains51, including government representation52,53, healthcare5357 and education58

Parental racial socialization can help children to develop a critical understanding of the systemic biases in societal systems, undermining the tendency to perceive the way things are as the way they should be239,242,243

Teaching children about the systemic and interpersonal racism faced by historical figures can reduce racial biases and increase the value placed on racial fairness259

Cultural narratives and values

The attitudes, values and perspectives of white people tend to be implicitly justified by dominant historical narratives60,61, which probably contributes to individual biases favouring white people70,74,7679

Cultural products of white people — such as art forms8387, dress and appearance8890,92, and styles of speech94 — tend to be the most highly valued, resulting in individual-level biases against people who deviate from these styles9598

Colourblind ideologies and the message that people who deserve success achieve it implicitly convey that people of colour do not merit the same status and success as white people45

Individual-level biases influence how historical events are represented in society70,78,82

The impact of white-centric biases can be observed in the methods, standards, and knowledge bases used in the fields of psychology105, mathematics103,104 and written composition94,106

Americans with stronger racial biases tend to report a stronger belief in meritocracy123,124 and greater support for racially colourblind ideology126,127

Parental racial socialization can facilitate children’s awareness and understanding of racial biases and systemic racism238,240243

Racial-bias awareness might help people to identify the subtle racial biases embedded in historical narratives, cultural products, and cultural beliefs256

Increasing awareness of the systemic injustices of the past can increase empathy towards marginalized groups260

Segregation

Persistent racial segregation133,134 limits opportunities for positive intergroup contact, which is known to reduce racial biases138142

Limited positive cross-race contact in racially diverse areas might be particularly likely to engender individual-level racial biases144

Individual-level racial biases can lead to stereotypes about physical spaces146151

Biases against predominantly Black spaces can result in devaluation of these spaces through property appraisals, use of eminent domain, or placement of toxic waste disposal sites152159

Racial socialization can provide white children with extended contact with people of colour, even if their local community is largely homogeneous139,244250

Education about the structural factors that led to current patterns of residential segregation can help to increase awareness of systemic racism and the policies that perpetuate it258

Shared stereotypes Shared racial stereotypes are spread through word of mouth183,184, linguistic biases185187, media biases171173 and propaganda23,165,166,168,169,176178, resulting in individual-level racial biases175,177,190195 Individual-level racial biases can lead to racially biased responses and behaviour (such as reporting a Black man as ‘suspicious’ to police) that further reinforces racial stereotypes, disparities and systemic racial inequalities209,212,213,215

Educating children and adults about historical racial injustices can reduce racial stereotyping258,259

Teaching people about the historical construction of racial stereotypes might help them to detect and reject racially biased messages258,259

Nonverbal biases Nonverbal biases that represent shared cultural biases and/or are transmitted through media can systematically impact racial attitudes217,233

Individual-level implicit racial biases predict racially biased nonverbal signals228231

Biased attitudes can lead to biased nonverbal signals and behaviour221,222,232

Historical education can frame nonverbal biases against people of colour as unjust227

Promoting racial bias awareness might increase awareness of subtle biases (such as nonverbal signals)

One potential lever with which to reduce the spread of individual racial biases is for white parents to teach their children directly about the importance of race and racism (as parents of colour do), a process called racial socialization. With the proliferation of colourblind racial ideology, many white American residents believe that disregarding race would help to minimize racism234, and white parents have feared that acknowledging race could facilitate the development of racial biases in their children234,235. Although some scholars have cautioned that there might be pitfalls to white parents talking to their children about race and racism, such as ill-equipped parents being ineffectual or producing counterproductive effects236, many scholars agree that racial socialization in white American families is a step in the right direction139,237239.

Parental racial socialization has the potential to provide children with a new lens through which they can interpret the societal system in which they are immersed and help them to recognize systemic racism240,241. Consequently, it can provide a counternarrative to the systemic factors reviewed here that perpetuate systemic racism. Racial socialization of children can prevent them from accepting, internalizing, and justifying the existing system of power and privilege, and instead enable them to see it as unjust and in need of reform. Initial evidence suggests that racial socialization in white American families can reduce racial biases among children239,242,243. In a preprint that has not yet undergone peer review, 8–12-year-old white American children who engaged in a semi-structured conversation about race and interpersonal racism with a parent showed a statistically significant decrease in implicit anti-Black racial bias from after the conversation compared to before239. As noted in the section on segregation, direct interracial contact — which is known to reduce racial biases138142 — is often limited by social and residential segregation. Thus, when direct contact with children of colour is not an option, parents can use books, films and other media to help children to understand the varied lived experiences of children in other racial and ethnic groups and ultimately to reduce their prejudices244250.

Alongside the socialization of children, another valuable intervention effort is to develop bias awareness among adults. Greater awareness of one’s own biases is associated with less racially biased attitudes and intentions to engage in less racially biased behaviour251253. White Americans who are more aware of their own racial biases are more willing to accept feedback on their racial biases and are more likely to detect evidence of subtle racial biases in themselves and others253. Thus, people who are more bias-aware might also be more likely to recognize systemic racial biases. When white Americans are presented with blatantly racist messages and are aware of the bias in the message, they tend to resist the influence of the message117,254,255. As such, helping people to recognize the subtle biases communicated through cultural narratives, nonverbal signals, and shared stereotypes across various levels of influence — such as racial ‘dog whistles’ — could reduce the effects of these biases. Consistent with the notion that racial bias awareness increases awareness of systemic racism, white American parents with greater bias awareness indicated that they were more likely to discuss racial current events reflective of systemic racism with their 8–12-year-old children256.

Our final intervention recommendation is education about the history and systems that led to current racial injustices and inequalities. People who are more knowledgeable about the racial injustices of the past tend to be more aware of the systemic racism that persists in the present77,257. There is also growing evidence that educating children and adults about historical racial injustices can improve racial attitudes, increase empathy, and raise awareness of systemic racism258260. For instance, white American children who learned about the accomplishments of Black historical figures — and the systemic and interpersonal racism they faced — showed less racial stereotyping and more positive attitudes towards Black people, and placed greater value on racial fairness than children who learned only about the historical figures’ accomplishments259. A preprint that has not yet undergone peer review also suggests that simply informing people of a group’s history of being treated unjustly can help to buffer the spread of nonverbal biases about that group227. Historical education can challenge people’s tendencies to infer that power and privilege disparities are justified and further call into question the cultural narratives that elevate white people and white culture.

These recommendations for intervention are overlapping and complementary. It is important to identify multiple ways to intervene in the nested levels of influence that shape attitudes and the ways that attitudes contribute to the perpetuation of systemic issues. By encouraging racial socialization, bias awareness, and accurate historical education, Americans can make new meaning of societal systems, understand better how the country got here, and begin to chip away at the racism so deeply ingrained in their society. These interventions would be particularly effective if implemented at a systemic level, for example, by revising the teaching of history and incorporating critical racial education into public school curricula.

Summary and future directions

In this Review, we approached the question of how racial biases develop, observing through a systemic lens how influences at the interpersonal, community, societal and temporal levels interact with racial biases. We considered the effects of five cross-cutting systemic factors on racial biases: unequal distributions of power and privilege, historical narratives and cultural products, racial segregation, shared cultural stereotypes, and nonverbal signals. We provided evidence that each of these five systemic factors shapes individual-level racial biases, and that individual-level racial biases shape systems and inequalities at each of the nested levels. Similar arguments about individuals and systems mutually reinforcing one another have been made in the context of organizations and the workplace11. We also reviewed interventions that could buffer the effects of the systemic influences we identified. Specifically, we reviewed the literature on racial socialization in white families, racial bias awareness and historical education — highlighting the promise of each.

The psychological study of racial attitudes has largely focused on individual-level biases, dedicating relatively little research to the systemic forces that shape racial biases. However, psychological scholars should seek opportunities to understand better how systems influence racial biases. Given the public availability of large-scale surveys and polling data indexing racial attitudes, there is great potential for scholars to use changes in state-level policies or practices as opportunities to examine the influence of systemic factors on racial attitudes.

Although many of the processes we describe here might generalize to other cultural and national contexts, the most relevant factors and how they manifest might vary considerably across contexts. In Box 2, we briefly discuss literature from non-USA contexts and the ways in which it converges and diverges from patterns observed in the USA. However, we by no means provide a comprehensive review. Ultimately, much more research on prejudice and racism needs to be conducted outside the USA and other nations with a majority of white residents261263. Research across a variety of cultural contexts is essential to the development of a comprehensive understanding of the societal systems that produce and reinforce racial (and other) biases, and the psychological processes that contribute to the reproduction of societal inequalities.

Although there is a growing body of literature tying systemic influences to individual-level biases, the literature in this area is still relatively sparse. For instance, it has yet to be experimentally tested how exposure to cultural products influences racial attitudes. There is also much work to be done regarding the development of interventions. For instance, it is important to identify optimal strategies for approaching racial socialization in white families and determining its impact on children’s racial attitudes. As an example, it is unknown whether the effects of parental racial socialization vary as a function of whether it involves teaching about interpersonal versus systemic racism. There is also still much to be learned about how often these conversations should take place and how long their effects persist. Racial bias awareness seems to be helpful for recognizing interpersonal biases, but it remains to be seen whether racial bias awareness facilitates recognition of systemic racism. Perhaps most importantly, it is unclear whether interventions aimed at developing racial bias awareness can be effective. We posit that interventions that integrate efforts to increase bias awareness, conversations about systemic and interpersonal racism, and education about historical injustices would pack the most powerful punch — but work that empirically tests the efficacy of such interventions is still needed. Moreover, applied interventions that examine how these factors operate in systemic contexts — such as schools — will be an important future step.

To fully interrupt the processes we have described, interventions will need to target organizations, policies, institutions and systems, as well as how individuals interact with each of these areas of influence. For interventions to truly be successful, they will need to cut across the nested levels of influence, lest any progress at one level be washed out by the continuing biases present at other levels. Large systemic changes of this kind require buy-in from key stakeholders and the public, and because change can take time, developing strategies to target how the public understands and makes sense of systems — and whether they are seen as just — might be a key step on the path towards an equitable society.

Box 2 Systemic bias outside the USA.

Many of the factors reviewed here also apply in other cultural contexts. For instance, power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives, segregation and shared stereotypes have been identified as contributors to individual-level racial biases in South Africa270,271. Power and privilege disparities in South Africa are stark; in 2017 white people held 67% of the top management positions but accounted for less than 8% of the country’s population272. Such dramatic status disparities have been identified as a key contributor to racial biases among South African children, across races273. Furthermore, media sources in South Africa have been criticized for promoting societal narratives that justify racial oppression and reinforce negative stereotypes about Black South Africans271.

Turning to a different cultural context, the extreme power and privilege disparities inherent in the statuses assigned to groups within the Hindu caste system have been identified as important contributors to the perpetuation of caste-based biases among Indian children274. Likewise, power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives, segregation, shared stereotypes and nonverbal biases have been identified as contributors to Israeli Jewish children’s biases against Arab people275279.

Some aspects of social bias development are not shared across cultures. For instance, in the contemporary USA, norms of egalitarianism and laws prohibiting discrimination limit the extent to which members of society are explicitly socialized to favour white people and disparage people of colour. Thus, racial biases tend to be transmitted in implicit, coded and plausibly deniable ways. In cultural contexts in which openly expressing biased and discriminatory views is socially acceptable, as was the case in earlier periods of American history, more explicit socialization of bias often takes place. For instance, within the Jewish population in Israel, people tend to be relatively accepting of anti-Arab biases, so children might be deliberately socialized to avoid and distrust Arab people276,277,279.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank K. G. Rogbeer, J. Tran and B. MacDonald for assistance in proofreading and formatting earlier drafts of this manuscript.

Author contributions

A.L.S.-D. researched data for the article. All authors wrote the article. All authors reviewed and/or edited the manuscript before submission.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Boaz Hameiri, Keith Payne and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Allison L. Skinner-Dorkenoo, Email: allison.skinner@uga.edu

Sylvia P. Perry, Email: sylvia.perry@northwestern.edu

References

  • 1.Adams G, Edkins V, Lacka D, Pickett KM, Cheryan S. Teaching about racism: pernicious implications of the standard portrayal. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2008;30:349–361. doi: 10.1080/01973530802502309. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Buchanan NT, Perez M, Prinstein MJ, Thurston IB. Upending racism in psychological science: strategies to change how science is conducted, reported, reviewed & disseminated. Am. Psychol. 2021;76:1097–1112. doi: 10.1037/amp0000905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Malherbe N, Ratele K, Adams G, Reddy G, Suffla S. A decolonial Africa(n)-centered psychology of antiracism. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2021;25:437–450. doi: 10.1177/10892680211022992. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Payne BK, Hannay JW. Implicit bias reflects systemic racism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2021;25:927–936. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Payne BK, Vuletich HA, Lundberg KB. The bias of crowds: how implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychol. Inq. 2017;28:233–248. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2017.1335568. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Payne BK, Vuletich HA. Policy insights from advances in implicit bias research. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2018;5:49–56. doi: 10.1177/2372732217746190. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Payne BK, Vuletich HA, Brown-Iannuzzi JL. Historical roots of implicit bias in slavery. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2019;116:11693. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1818816116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rucker JM, Richeson JA. Toward an understanding of structural racism: implications for criminal justice. Science. 2021;374:286–290. doi: 10.1126/science.abj7779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Salter P, Adams G. Toward a critical race psychology. Soc. Person. Psychol. Compass. 2013;7:781–793. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12068. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Salter PS, Adams G, Perez MJ. Racism in the structure of everyday worlds: a cultural-psychological perspective. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018;27:150–155. doi: 10.1177/0963721417724239. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stephens NM, Rivera LA, Townsend SSM. The cycle of workplace bias and how to interrupt it. Res. Organ. Behav. 2020;40:100137. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dobbin F, Kalev A. Why doesn’t diversity training work? The challenge for industry and academia. Anthropol. Now. 2018;10:48–55. doi: 10.1080/19428200.2018.1493182. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bowleg L, Boone CA, Holt SL, del Río-González AM, Mbaba M. Beyond “heartfelt condolences”: a critical take on mainstream psychology’s responses to anti-Black police brutality. Am. Psychol. 2022;77:362–380. doi: 10.1037/amp0000899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chater, N. & Loewenstein, G. F. The i-frame and the s-frame: how focusing on the individual-level solutions has led behavioral public policy astray. Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 1–60 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 15.Onyeador IN, Hudson STJ, Lewis NA. Moving beyond implicit bias training: policy insights for increasing organizational diversity. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2021;8:19–26. doi: 10.1177/2372732220983840. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lai CK, Lisnek JA. The impact of implicit-bias-oriented diversity training on police officers’ beliefs, motivations, and actions. Psychol. Sci. 2023;34:424–434. doi: 10.1177/09567976221150617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ray V. A theory of racialized organizations. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2019;84:26–53. doi: 10.1177/0003122418822335. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bronfenbrenner U. Ecology of the family as a context for human development: research perspectives. Dev. Psychol. 1986;22:723–742. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.723. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brady LM, Fryberg SA, Shoda Y. Expanding the interpretive power of psychological science by attending to culture. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2018;115:11406–11413. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1803526115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Markus HR, Kitayama S. Cultures and selves: a cycle of mutual constitution. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2010;5:420–430. doi: 10.1177/1745691610375557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Skinner-Dorkenoo AL, Sarmal A, André CJ, Rogbeer K. How microaggressions reinforce and perpetuate systemic racism in the U.S. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2021;16:903–925. doi: 10.1177/17456916211002543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dunbar-Ortiz, R. An Indigenous Peoples’ History Of The United States (Beacon, 2014).
  • 23.Kendi, I. X. Stamped From The Beginning: The Definitive History Of Racist Ideas In America (Avalon, 2016). This book reviews the historical construction of race in the USA and how it has been used to manipulate individuals and systems.
  • 24.Hannah-Jones, N. The 1619 Project: A New Origin Story (Penguin/Random House, 2021).
  • 25.Kunnan AJ. Politics and legislation in citizenship testing in the United States. Annu. Rev. Appl. Ling. 2009;29:37–48. doi: 10.1017/S0267190509090047. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gamoran, A. The future of educational inequality: what went wrong and how can we fix it? Grant Foundationhttps://wtgrantfoundation.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/11/The-Future-of-Educational-Inequality-Adam-Gamoran.pdf (2015).
  • 27.Hunt B, Whitman S. Black:white health disparities in the United States and Chicago: 1990–2010. J. Racial Ethn. Health Dispar. 2015;2:93–100. doi: 10.1007/s40615-014-0052-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Shapiro, T., Meschede, T. & Osoro, S. The Roots Of The Widening Racial Wealth Gap: Explaining The Black–White Economic Divide (Policy Brief) https://heller.brandeis.edu/iere/pdfs/racial-wealth-equity/racial-wealth-gap/roots-widening-racial-wealth-gap.pdf (Institute on Assets and Social Policy, 2013).
  • 29.Wilkerson, I. Caste: The Origins Of Our Discontents (Random House, 2020).
  • 30.Spiegelman, M. Race and ethnicity of public school teachers and their students. National Centre for Educational Statisticshttps://nces.ed.gov/pubs2020/2020103/index.asp (2020).
  • 31.Melillo, G. 118th Congress is most racially diverse to date: research. The Hillhttps://thehill.com/changing-america/respect/diversity-inclusion/3805580-118th-congress-is-most-racially-diverse-to-date-research/ (2023).
  • 32.ABA profile of the legal profession 2022. ABAhttps://www.americanbar.org/content/dam/aba/administrative/news/2022/07/profile-report-2022.pdf (2022).
  • 33.Strickland, B. Diversity among CEOs, CFOs continues to rise. J. Accountancyhttps://www.journalofaccountancy.com/news/2022/aug/diversity-among-ceos-cfos-continues-rise.html (2022).
  • 34.Roberts SO, Rizzo MT. The psychology of American racism. Am. Psychol. 2021;76:475–487. doi: 10.1037/amp0000642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.García-Sánchez E, Van der Toorn J, Rodríguez-Bailón R, Willis GB. The vicious cycle of economic inequality: the role of ideology in shaping the relationship between “what is” and “what ought to be” in 41 countries. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2019;10:991–1001. doi: 10.1177/1948550618811500. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kay AC, et al. Inequality, discrimination, and the power of the status quo: direct evidence for a motivation to see the way things are as the way they should be. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2009;97:421–434. doi: 10.1037/a0015997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Roberts SO. Descriptive-to-prescriptive (D2P) reasoning: an early emerging bias to maintain the status quo. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2022;33:289–322. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2021.1963591. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Roberts SO, Guo C, Ho AK, Gelman SA. Children’s descriptive-to-prescriptive tendency replicates (and varies) cross-culturally: evidence from China. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2018;165:148–160. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Roberts SO, Gelman SA, Ho AK. So it is, so it shall be: group regularities license children’s prescriptive judgments. Cogn. Sci. 2017;41:576–600. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Roberts SO, Ho AK, Gelman SA. Group presence, category labels, and generic statements influence children to treat descriptive group regularities as prescriptive. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2017;158:19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Jost JT, Banaji MR, Nosek BA. A decade of system justification theory: accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychol. 2004;25:881–919. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Jost, J. T., Fitzsimons, G. & Kay, A. C. in Handbook Of Experimental Existential Psychology (eds Greenberg, J., Koole, S. L. & Pyszczynski, T.) 263–283 (Guilford, 2004).
  • 43.Olson KR, Banaji MR, Dweck CS, Spelke ES. Children’s biased evaluations of lucky versus unlucky people and their social groups. Psychol. Sci. 2006;17:845–846. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01792.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Olson KR, Dweck CS, Spelke ES, Banaji MR. Children’s responses to group-based inequalities: perpetuation and rectification. Soc. Cogn. 2011;29:270–287. doi: 10.1521/soco.2011.29.3.270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Perry SP, Skinner-Dorkenoo AL, Wages JE, Abaied JL. Systemic considerations in child development and the pursuit of racial equality in the United States. Psychol. Inq. 2021;32:180–186. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2021.1971453. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Vuletich HA, Payne BK. Stability and change in implicit bias. Psychol. Sci. 2019;30:854–886. doi: 10.1177/0956797619844270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Craig MA, Rucker JM, Richeson JA. Racial and political dynamics of an approaching “majority–minority” United States. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 2018;677:204–214. doi: 10.1177/0002716218766269. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Craig MA, Rucker JM, Richeson JA. The pitfalls and promise of increasing racial diversity: threat, contact, and race relations in the 21st century. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018;27:188–193. doi: 10.1177/0963721417727860. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Craig MA, Richeson JA. More diverse yet less tolerant? How the increasingly diverse racial landscape affects white Americans’ racial attitudes. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2014;40:750–761. doi: 10.1177/0146167214524993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Skinner AL, Cheadle JE. The “Obama effect”? Priming contemporary racial milestones increases implicit racial bias among whites. Soc. Cogn. 2016;34:544–558. doi: 10.1521/soco.2016.34.6.544. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Glaser J, Spencer K, Charbonneau A. Racial bias and public policy. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2014;1:88–94. doi: 10.1177/2372732214550403. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Piston S. How explicit racial prejudice hurt Obama in the 2008 election. Polit. Behav. 2010;32:431–451. doi: 10.1007/s11109-010-9108-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Leitner JB, Hehman E, Snowden LR. States higher in racial bias spend less on disabled Medicaid enrollees. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018;208:150–157. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Harell A, Lieberman E. How information about race-based health disparities affects policy preferences: evidence from a survey experiment about the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. Soc. Sci. Med. 2021;277:113884. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Skinner-Dorkenoo AL, et al. Highlighting COVID-19 racial disparities can reduce support for safety precautions among white U.S. residents. Soc. Sci. Med. 2022;301:114951. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Stephens-Dougan, L. White Americans’ reactions to racial disparities in COVID-19. Am. Pol. Sci. Rev. 117, 773–780 (2022).
  • 57.Leitner JB, Hehman E, Ayduk O, Mendoza-Denton R. Blacks’ death rate due to circulatory diseases is positively related to whites’ explicit racial bias: a nationwide investigation using Project Implicit. Psychol. Sci. 2016;27:1299–1311. doi: 10.1177/0956797616658450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Riddle T, Sinclair S. Racial disparities in school-based disciplinary actions are associated with county-level rates of racial bias. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2019;116:8255–8260. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1808307116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Jones JM. Psychological knowledge and the new American dilemma of race. J. Soc. Issues. 1998;54:641–662. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1998.tb01241.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Foster SJ. The struggle for American identity: treatment of ethnic groups in United States history textbooks. Hist. Educ. 1999;28:251–278. doi: 10.1080/004676099284618. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Shuster K. Teaching hard history. Southern Poverty Law Centerhttps://www.splcenter.org/20180131/teaching-hard-history (2018).
  • 62.Tajima-Penña, R. Asian Americans [documentary film] (United States Public Broadcasting Services, 2020).
  • 63.An S. Asian Americans in American history: an AsianCrit perspective on Asian American inclusion in state U.S. history curriculum standards. Theory Res. Soc. Educ. 2016;44:244–276. doi: 10.1080/00933104.2016.1170646. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Constante, A. How advocates are writing Asian American stories back into history books. NBC Newshttps://www.nbcnews.com (2019).
  • 65.Harada, V. H. The treatment of Asian Americans in U.S. history textbooks published 1994–1996. ERIChttps://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED448072.pdf (2000).
  • 66.Lee, E. Why are Asian Americans missing from our textbooks? Pacific Standardhttps://psmag.com/news/why-are-asian-americans-missing-from-our-textbooks (2017).
  • 67.Dai JD, Lopez JJ, Brady LM, Eason AE, Fryberg SA. Erasing and dehumanizing Natives to protect positive national identity: the Native mascot example. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass. 2021;15:1–16. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12632. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Eason AE, Pope T, Becenti KM, Fryberg SA. Sanitizing history: national identification, negative stereotypes, and support for eliminating Columbus Day and adopting Indigenous Peoples Day. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 2020;27:1–17. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kurtiş T, Adams G, Yellow Bird M. Generosity or genocide? Identity implications of silence in American Thanksgiving commemorations. Memory. 2010;18:208–224. doi: 10.1080/09658210903176478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Salter PS, Adams G. On the intentionality of cultural products: representations of Black history as psychological affordances. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1–21. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Sue DW, et al. Disarming racial microaggressions: microintervention strategies for targets, white allies, and bystanders. Am. Psychol. 2019;74:128–142. doi: 10.1037/amp0000296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Smith C. Unstated flag in Mississippi. Southeast. Geogr. 2021;61:105–106. doi: 10.1353/sgo.2021.0007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Trenticosta C, Collins WC. Death and Dixie: how the courthouse Confederate flag influences capital cases in Louisiana. Harv. J. Racial Ethn. Just. 2011;27:125–164. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.O’Connell HA. Monuments outlive history: confederate monuments, the legacy of slavery, and black–white inequality. Ethn. Racial Stud. 2020;43:460–478. doi: 10.1080/01419870.2019.1635259. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Morgan H. Resisting the movement to ban critical race theory from schools. Clearing House J. Educ. Strateg. Issues Ideas. 2022;95:35–41. doi: 10.1080/00098655.2021.2025023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Barrett M, Oppenheimer L. Findings, theories and methods in the study of children’s national identifications and national attitudes. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2011;8:5–24. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2010.533955. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Nelson JC, Adams G, Salter PS. The Marley hypothesis: denial of racism reflects ignorance of history. Psychol. Sci. 2013;24:213–218. doi: 10.1177/0956797612451466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Mukherjee S, Salter PS, Molina LE. Museum spaces as psychological affordances: representations of immigration history and national identity. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:692. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Ehrlinger J, et al. How exposure to the Confederate flag affects willingness to vote for Barack Obama. Political Psychol. 2011;32:131–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00797.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Forbes JD. The historian and the Indian: racial bias in American history. Americas. 1963;19:349–362. doi: 10.2307/979504. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Smithers GD. Putting ethnohistory to work: Jack Forbes and the remaking of American historical consciousness. Ethnohistory. 2021;68:29–51. doi: 10.1215/00141801-8702324. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Adams G, Salter PS, Kurtiş T, Naemi P, Estrada-Villalta S. Subordinated knowledge as a tool for creative maladjustment and resistance to racial oppression. J. Soc. Issues. 2018;74:337–354. doi: 10.1111/josi.12272. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Acuff, J. B. in The Palgrave Handbook Of Race And The Arts In Education (eds Kraehe, A., Gaztambide-Fernández, R. & Carpenter, B. II) 515–533 (Palgrave Macmillan, 2018).
  • 84.Roy WG, Dowd TJ. What is sociological about music? Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010;36:183–203. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102618. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.McCarthy-Brown N. Decolonizing dance curriculum in higher education: one credit at a time. J. Dance Educ. 2014;14:125–129. doi: 10.1080/15290824.2014.887204. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Bradley D. The sounds of silence: talking race in music education. Action Crit. Theory Music Educ. 2007;6:132–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Kajikawa, L. in Seeing Race Again: Countering Colorblindness Across The Disciplines (eds Crenshaw, K. W., Harris, L. C, HoSang, D. M. & Lipsitz, G.) 155–174 (Univ. California Press, 2019).
  • 88.Edwards TK, Marshall C. Undressing policy: a critical analysis of North Carolina (USA) public school dress codes. Gend. Educ. 2020;32:732–750. doi: 10.1080/09540253.2018.1503234. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Greene DW. A multidimensional analysis of what not to wear in the workplace: hijabs and natural hair. FIU Law Rev. 2013;8:333–362. doi: 10.25148/lawrev.8.2.8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Armstrong, J. & Kuck, S. Saggy pants ordinance analysis: disparate impact versus respectability. Int. J. Bus. Soc. Sci. 11, 10.30845/ijbss.v11n9p1 (2020).
  • 91.Douglas B, Lewis CW, Douglas A, Scott ME, Garrison-Wade D. The impact of white teachers on the academic achievement of black students: an exploratory qualitative analysis. Educ. Found. 2008;22:47–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Opie TR, Phillips KW. Hair penalties: the negative influence of Afrocentric hair on ratings of Black women’s dominance and professionalism. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:1311. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Koval CZ, Rosette AS. The natural hair bias in job recruitment. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2020;12:741–750. doi: 10.1177/1948550620937937. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Inoue AB. Classroom writing assessment as an antiracist practice. Pedagogy. 2019;19:373–404. doi: 10.1215/15314200-7615366. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Baugh, J. Beyond Ebonics: Linguistic Pride And Racial Prejudice (Oxford Univ. Press, 2000).
  • 96.Craft JT, Wright KE, Weissler RE, Queen R. Language and discrimination: generating meaning, perceiving identities, and discriminating outcomes. Annu. Rev. Ling. 2020;6:389–407. doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011718-011659. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Rickford JR, King S. Language and linguistics on trial: hearing Rachel Jeantel (and other vernacular speakers) in the courtroom and beyond. Language. 2016;92:948–988. doi: 10.1353/lan.2016.0078. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Williams RL. The Ebonics controversy. J. Black Psychol. 1997;23:208–214. doi: 10.1177/00957984970233002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Arya R. Cultural appropriation: what it is and why it matters? Sociol. Compass. 2021;15:12923. [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Chetty R, Hendren N, Jones MR, Porter SR. Race and economic opportunity in the United States: an intergenerational perspective. Quar. J. Econ. 2020;135:711–783. doi: 10.1093/qje/qjz042. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Piccard A. Death by boarding school: the last acceptable racism and the United States’ genocide of Native Americans. Gonz. L. Rev. 2013;49:137–185. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Nagle, R. This Land [podcast] https://crooked.com/podcast-series/this-land/ (Crooked Media, 2021).
  • 103.Bishop AJ. Western mathematics: the secret weapon of cultural imperialism. Race Class. 1990;32:51–65. doi: 10.1177/030639689003200204. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Iseke-Barnes JM. Ethnomathematics and language in decolonizing mathematics. Gend. Class. 2000;7:133–149. [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Roberts SO, Bareket-Shavit C, Dollins FA, Goldie PD, Mortenson E. Racial inequality in psychological research: trends of the past and recommendations for the future. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2020;15:1295–1309. doi: 10.1177/1745691620927709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Inoue, A. B. Antiracist Writing Assessment Ecologies: Teaching And Assessing Writing For A Socially Just Future (Parlor, 2015).
  • 107.Eells WC. Number systems of the North American Indians. Am. Math. Mon. 1913;20:263–272. doi: 10.1080/00029890.1913.11997976. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Mukherjee S, Perez MJ. All Americans are not perceived as “true” Americans: implications for policy. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2021;8:34–41. doi: 10.1177/2372732220984806. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Boykin AW. Experimental psychology from a black perspective: issues and examples. J. Black Psychol. 1977;3:29–49. doi: 10.1177/009579847700300209. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Sue DW. Microaggressions and “evidence”: empirical or experiential reality? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017;12:170–172. doi: 10.1177/1745691616664437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Johnston PMG. Towards culturally appropriate assessment? A contribution to the debates. High. Educ. Q. 2010;64:231–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2273.2010.00463.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Ledgerwood A, Mandisodza AN, Jost JT, Pohl MJ. Working for the system: motivated defense of meritocratic beliefs. Soc. Cogn. 2011;29:322–340. doi: 10.1521/soco.2011.29.3.322. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.McCoy SK, Major B. Priming meritocracy and the psychological justification of inequality. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2007;43:341–351. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.04.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Knowles ED, Lowery BS. Meritocracy, self-concerns, and whites’ denial of racial inequity. Self Ident. 2012;11:202–222. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2010.542015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Levy SR, West TL, Ramirez L, Karafantis DM. The protestant work ethic: a lay theory with dual intergroup implications. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 2006;9:95–115. doi: 10.1177/1368430206059874. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Horowitz, J. M., Parker, K., Brown, A. & Cox, K. Amid national reckoning, Americans divided on whether increased focus on race will lead to major policy change. Pew Research Centrehttps://www.pewresearch.org/ (2020).
  • 117.Wetts R, Willer R. Who is called by the dog whistle? Experimental evidence that racial resentment and political ideology condition responses to racially encoded messages. Socius. 2019;5:2378023119866268. doi: 10.1177/2378023119866268. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Valentino NA, Hutchings VL, White IK. Cues that matter: how political ads prime racial attitudes during campaigns. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 2002;96:75–90. doi: 10.1017/S0003055402004240. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Mendelberg, T. The Race Card (Princeton Univ. Press, 2017).
  • 120.Saul, J. in New Work On Speech Acts (eds Fogal, D., Harris, D. W. & Moss, M.) 360–383 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2018).
  • 121.Plaut VC, Thomas KM, Hurd K, Romano CA. Do color blindness and multiculturalism remedy or foster discrimination and racism? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018;27:200–206. doi: 10.1177/0963721418766068. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Apfelbaum EP, Pauker K, Sommers SR, Ambady N. In blind pursuit of racial equality? Psychol. Sci. 2010;21:1587–1592. doi: 10.1177/0956797610384741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Federico CM, Sidanius J. Racism, ideology, and affirmative action, revisited: The antecedents and consequences of “principled objections” to affirmative action. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002;82:488–502. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Sidanius J, Devereux E, Pratto F. A comparison of symbolic racism theory and social dominance theory as explanations for racial policy attitudes. J. Soc. Psychol. 1992;132:377–395. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1992.9924713. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Reynolds J, Xian H. Perceptions of meritocracy in the land of opportunity. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 2014;36:121–137. [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Mazzocco PJ, Cooper LW, Flint M. Different shades of racial colorblindness: the role of prejudice. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 2012;15:167–178. doi: 10.1177/1368430211424763. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Whitley BE, Jr, Webster GD. The relationships of intergroup ideologies to ethnic prejudice: a meta-analysis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2019;23:207–237. doi: 10.1177/1088868318761423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Neville HA, Lilly RL, Lee RM, Duran G, Browne LV. Construction and initial validation of the Color-Blind Racial Attitudes Scale (CoBRAS) J. Couns. Psychol. 2000;47:59–70. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.47.1.59. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Rothstein, R. The Color Of Law: A Forgotten History Of How Our Government Segregated America (Liveright, 2017).
  • 130.Mitchell, B. & Franco, J. HOLC “redlining” maps: the persistent structure of segregation and economic inequality. National Community Reinvestment Coalitionhttps://ncrc.org/holc/ (2018).
  • 131.Wilson, E. K. The legal foundations of white supremacy. DePaul J. Soc. Just.11, https://via.library.depaul.edu/jsj/vol11/iss2/6 (2018).
  • 132.Anderson, D. Redlining’s legacy of inequality: $212,000 less home equity, low homeownership rates for Black families. Redfin Newshttps://www.redfin.com/news/redlining-real-estate-racial-wealth-gap/ (2020).
  • 133.Logan, J. R. & Stults, B. The Persistence of Segregation in the Metropolis: New Findings from the 2010 Censushttps://s4.ad.brown.edu/Projects/Diversity/data/report/report2.pdf (2011).
  • 134.Ferguson JP, Koning R. Firm turnover and the return of racial establishment segregation. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2018;83:445–474. doi: 10.1177/0003122418767438. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Brown v. Board of Education of Topeka, 347 U.S. 483 Justiahttps://supreme.justia.com/cases/federal/us/347/483/ (1954).
  • 136.Office of Public Affairs. Court approves desegregation plan for Cleveland, Mississippi, schools: Cleveland School District to open consolidated middle and high schools by August 2017. US Department of Justicehttps://www.justice.gov/opa/pr/court-approves-desegregation-plan-cleveland-mississippi-schools (2017).
  • 137.Francis DV, Darity WA. Separate and unequal under one roof: how the legacy of racialized tracking perpetuates within-school segregation. RSF Russell Sage Found. J. Soc. Sci. 2021;7:187–202. [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Allport, G. W. The Nature of Prejudice (Addison Wesley, 1954).
  • 139.Skinner AL, Meltzoff AN. Childhood experiences and intergroup biases among children. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2019;13:211–240. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12054. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Paluck EL, Green SA, Green DP. The contact hypothesis re-evaluated. Behav. Public. Policy. 2019;3:129–158. doi: 10.1017/bpp.2018.25. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Pettigrew TF, Tropp LR. A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2006;90:751–783. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Pettigrew TF, Tropp LR, Wagner U, Christ O. Recent advances in intergroup contact theory. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2011;35:271–280. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.03.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Rae JR, Newheiser A-K, Olson KR. Exposure to racial out-groups and implicit race bias in the United States. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2015;6:535–543. doi: 10.1177/1948550614567357. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Rae JR, Skinner-Dorkenoo AL, Reiman A, Schmid K, Hewstone M. Mixed evidence for interactive effects of outgroup proportions and intergroup contact on racial bias in the United States. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2022;13:476–489. doi: 10.1177/19485506211027756. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Quillian L. Group threat and regional change in attitudes toward African-Americans. Am. J. Sociol. 1996;102:816–860. doi: 10.1086/230998. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Bonam CM, Bergsieker HB, Eberhardt JL. Polluting black space. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2016;145:1561–1582. doi: 10.1037/xge0000226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Quillian L, Pager D. Black neighbors, higher crime? The role of racial stereotypes in evaluations of neighborhood crime. Am. J. Sociol. 2001;10:717–767. doi: 10.1086/338938. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Bonam CM, Taylor VJ, Yantis C. Racialized physical space as cultural product. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass. 2017;11:1–12. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12340. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Bonam C, Yantis C, Taylor VJ. Invisible middle-class Black space: asymmetrical person and space stereotyping at the race–class nexus. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 2020;23:24–47. doi: 10.1177/1368430218784189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Boykin, C. M. S. Negatively Stereotyping Historically Black Colleges and Universities as an Intergroup Process (Univ. California, 2018).
  • 151.Yantis C, Bonam CM. Inconceivable middle-class black space: the architecture and consequences of space-focused stereotype content at the race–class nexus. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2021;47:1101–1118. doi: 10.1177/0146167220960270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Howell J, Korver-Glenn E. Neighborhoods, race, and the twenty-first-century housing appraisal industry. Sociol. Race Ethn. 2018;4:473–490. doi: 10.1177/2332649218755178. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Sosina, V. E. & Weathers, E. S. Pathways to inequality: between-district segregation and racial disparities in school district expenditures. AERA Open5, 10.1177/2332858419872445 (2019).
  • 154.Adams, C. Georgia destroyed a Black neighborhood. Now former residents want justice. NBC Newshttps://www.nbcnews.com/news (2021).
  • 155.Carpenter DM, Ross JK. Testing O’Connor and Thomas: does the use of eminent domain target poor and minority communities? Urban Stud. 2009;46:2447–2461. doi: 10.1177/0042098009342597. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Phillips S, Sillah MR. A house is not a home: effect of eminent domain abuse on the poor, African Americans, and the elderly. Hous. Soc. 2009;36:115–136. doi: 10.1080/08882746.2009.11430573. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Fraley JM. Eminent domain and unfettered discretion: lessons from a history of US territorial takings. Penn State Law Rev. 2021;126:609. [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Banzhaf S, Ma L, Timmins C. Environmental justice: the economics of race, place, and pollution. J. Econ. Perspect. 2019;33:185–208. doi: 10.1257/jep.33.1.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Blodgett AD. An analysis of pollution and community advocacy in ‘Cancer Alley’: setting an example for the environmental justice movement in St James Parish, Louisiana. Local Env. 2006;11:647–661. doi: 10.1080/13549830600853700. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Anicich EM, Jachimowicz JM, Osborne MR, Phillips LT. Structuring local environments to avoid racial diversity: anxiety drives whites’ geographical and institutional self-segregation preferences. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2021;95:104117. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2021.104117. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.DeLuca JR. Submersed in social segregation: the (re)production of social capital through swim club membership. J. Sport. Soc. Issues. 2013;37:340–363. doi: 10.1177/0193723513498605. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Gilchrist J, Parker EM. Racial and ethnic disparities in fatal unintentional drowning among persons less than 30 years of age — United States, 1999–2010. J. Saf. Res. 2014;50:139–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2014.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Saluja G, et al. Swimming pool drownings among US residents aged 5–24 years: understanding racial/ethnic disparities. Am. J. Public Health. 2006;96:728–733. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.057067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Wiltse J. The Black–white swimming disparity in America: a deadly legacy of swimming pool discrimination. J. Sport. Soc. Issues. 2014;38:366–389. doi: 10.1177/0193723513520553. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Han C. Geisha of a different kind: gay Asian men and the gendering of sexual identity. Sex. Cult. 2006;10:3–28. doi: 10.1007/s12119-006-1018-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Sun C, Liberman R, Butler A, Lee SY, Webb R. Shifting receptions: Asian American stereotypes and the exploration of comprehensive media literacy. Commun. Rev. 2015;18:294–314. doi: 10.1080/10714421.2015.1085778. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Chen CH. Feminization of Asian (American) men in the U.S. mass media: an analysis of The Ballad of Little Jo. J. Commun. Inq. 1996;20:57–71. doi: 10.1177/019685999602000204. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Franklin JH. “Birth of a nation”: propaganda as history. Mass. Rev. 1979;20:417–434. [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Taylor CM. W. E. B. DuBois’s challenge to scientific racism. J. Black Stud. 1981;11:449–460. doi: 10.1177/002193478101100405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Melson-Silimon, A., Spivey, B. N. & Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L. The construction of racial stereotypes and how they serve as racial propaganda. Preprint at PsyArxivhttps://psyarxiv.com/a2m84 (2023).
  • 171.Dixon TL, Linz DG. Overrepresentation and underrepresentation of African Americans and Latinos as lawbreakers on television news. J. Commun. 2000;50:131–154. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2000.tb02845.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Mastro D, Behm-Morawitz E. Latino representation on primetime television. J. Mass. Commun. Q. 2005;82:110–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Monk-Turner E, Heiserman M, Johnson C, Cotton V, Jackson M. The portrayal of racial minorities on prime time television: a replication of the Mastro and Greenberg study a decade later. Stud. Pop. Cul. 2010;32:101–114. [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Leavitt PA, Covarrubias R, Perez YA, Fryberg SA. “Frozen in time”: the impact of Native American media representations on identity and self‐understanding. J. Soc. Issues. 2015;71:39–53. doi: 10.1111/josi.12095. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Tukachinsky R, Mastro D, Yarchi M. Documenting portrayals of race/ethnicity on primetime television over a 20-year span and their association with national-level racial/ethnic attitudes. J. Soc. Issues. 2015;71:17–38. doi: 10.1111/josi.12094. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Bjornstrom EES, Kaufman RL, Peterson RD, Slater MD. Race and ethnic representations of lawbreakers and victims in crime news: a national study of television coverage. Soc. Probl. 2011;57:269–293. doi: 10.1525/sp.2010.57.2.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Dukes KN, Gaither SE. Black racial stereotypes and victim blaming: implications for media coverage and criminal proceedings in cases of police violence against racial and ethnic minorities. J. Soc. Issues. 2017;73:789–807. doi: 10.1111/josi.12248. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Sommers SR, Apfelbaum EP, Dukes KN, Toosi N, Wang EJ. Race and media coverage of hurricane Katrina: analysis, implications, and future research questions. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy. 2006;6:39–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2006.00103.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Skinner AL, Perry SP, Gaither S. Not quite monoracial: biracial stereotypes explored. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2019;46:377–392. doi: 10.1177/0146167219858344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Wages, J. E. III, Perry, S. P., Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L. & Bodenhausen, G. V. Reckless gambles and responsible ventures: racialized prototypes of risk-taking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 122, 202–221 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 181.2020 NCIC Missing person and unidentified person statistics. FBIhttps://www.fbi.gov/file-repository/2020-ncic-missing-person-and-unidentified-person-statistics.pdf/view (2021).
  • 182.Sommers Z. Missing white woman syndrome: an empirical analysis of race and gender disparities in online news coverage of missing persons. J. Crim. Law Criminol. 2016;106:275–314. [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Martin D, et al. The spontaneous formation of stereotypes via cumulative cultural evolution. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25:1777–1786. doi: 10.1177/0956797614541129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Martin D, Cunningham SJ, Hutchison J, Slessor G, Smith K. How societal stereotypes might form and evolve via cumulative cultural evolution. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass. 2017;11:e12338. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12338. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Beukeboom CJ, Burgers C. How stereotypes are shared through language: a review and introduction of the Social Categories and Stereotypes Communication (SCSC) framework. Rev. Commun. Res. 2019;7:1–37. doi: 10.12840/issn.2255-4165.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Beukeboom CJ, et al. The negation bias in stereotype maintenance: a replication in five languages. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 2019;39:219–236. doi: 10.1177/0261927X19869759. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Burgers C, Beukeboom CJ. Stereotype transmission and maintenance through interpersonal communication: the irony bias. Commun. Res. 2014;43:414–441. doi: 10.1177/0093650214534975. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Gorham BW. News media’s relationship with stereotyping: the linguistic intergroup bias in response to crime news. J. Commun. 2006;56:289–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00020.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Kilgo D. Media bias delegitimizes Black-rights protesters. Nature. 2021;593:315. doi: 10.1038/d41586-021-01314-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self‐esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 1995;102:4–27. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Macrae CN, Bodenhausen GV. Social cognition: thinking categorically about others. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2000;51:93–120. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Busselle R, Crandall H. Television viewing and perceptions about race differences in socioeconomic success. J. Broadcast. Electron. Media. 2002;46:265–282. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4602_6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Oliver, M. B. & Armstrong, G. B. in Entertaining Crime: Television Reality Programs (eds Fishman, M. & Cavender, G.) 19–35 (Aldine de Gruyter, 1998).
  • 194.Arendt F. Cultivation effects of a newspaper on reality estimates and explicit and implicit attitudes. J. Media Psychol. 2010;22:147–159. doi: 10.1027/1864-1105/a000020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Arendt F, Northup T. Effects of long-term exposure to news stereotypes on implicit and explicit attitudes. Int. J. Commun. 2015;9:732–751. [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Lynott D, et al. Are you what you read? Predicting implicit attitudes to immigration based on linguistic distributional cues from newspaper readership; a pre-registered study. Front. Psychol. 2019;10:842. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Lai, C. & Banaji, M. in Difference Without Domination (eds Allen, D. & Somanathan, R.) 115–146 (Univ. Chicago Press, 2020).
  • 198.Eberhardt JL, Goff PA, Purdie VJ, Davies PG. Seeing Black: race, crime, and visual processing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2004;87:876–893. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 199.Mekawi Y, Bresin K. Is the evidence from racial bias shooting task studies a smoking gun? Results from a meta-analysis. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2015;61:120–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Cooley E, et al. Racial biases in officers’ decisions to frisk are amplified for Black people stopped among groups leading to similar biases in searches, arrests, and use of force. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2020;11:761–769. doi: 10.1177/1948550619876638. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Goel S, Rao JM, Shroff R. Precinct or prejudice? Understanding racial disparities in New York City’s stop-and-frisk policy. Ann. Appl. Stat. 2016;10:365–394. doi: 10.1214/15-AOAS897. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.Hetey, R. C., Monin, B., Maitreyi, A. & Eberhardt, J. L. Data for change: a statistical analysis of police stops, searches, handcuffings, and arrests in Oakland, Calif., 2013–2014. Stanford SPARQhttps://searchworks.stanford.edu/view/by412gh2838 (2016).
  • 203.Levchak PJ. Stop-and-frisk in New York City: estimating racial disparities in post-stop outcomes. J. Crim. Justice. 2021;73:101784. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2021.101784. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 204.Filindra A, Kaplan NJ. Racial resentment and whites’ gun policy preferences in contemporary America. Pol. Behav. 2016;38:255–275. doi: 10.1007/s11109-015-9326-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 205.O’Brien K, Forrest W, Lynott D, Daly M. Racism, gun ownership and gun control: biased attitudes in US whites may influence policy decisions. PLoS One. 2013;8:e77552. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 206.Higgenbotham GD, Sears DO, Goldstein L. When an irresistible prejudice meets immovable politics: Black legal gun ownership undermines racially resentful white Americans’ gun rights advocacy. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2023;152:410–424. doi: 10.1037/xge0001275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.Hayes M, Fortunato D, Hibbing MV. Race–gender bias in white Americans’ preferences for gun availability. J. Public Policy. 2021;41:818–834. doi: 10.1017/S0143814X20000288. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 208.Walker H, Collingwood L, Bunyasi TL. White response to black death: a racialized theory of white attitudes towards gun control. Du Bois Rev. 2020;17:165–188. doi: 10.1017/S1742058X20000156. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 209.Hehman E, Flake JK, Calanchini J. Disproportionate use of lethal force in policing is associated with regional racial biases of residents. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2017;9:393–401. doi: 10.1177/1948550617711229. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 210.Alexander, M. The New Jim Crow: Mass Incarceration In The Age Of Colorblindness (New Press, 2010). This book examines how policing policies and widespread racial biases work together to marginalize and control people of colour in the USA.
  • 211.Carson, E. A. Prisoners in 2019. Bureau of Justice Statisticshttps://bjs.ojp.gov/content/pub/pdf/p19.pdf (2020).
  • 212.Skinner AL, Haas IJ. Perceived threat associated with police officers and Black men predicts support for policing policy reform. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1057. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 213.Hetey RC, Eberhardt JL. The numbers don’t speak for themselves: racial disparities and the persistence of inequality in the criminal justice system. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018;27:183–187. doi: 10.1177/0963721418763931. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 214.Peffley M, Hurwitz J. Persuasion and resistance: race and the death penalty in America. Am. J. Pol. Sci. 2007;51:996–1012. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2007.00293.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 215.Hetey RC, Eberhardt JL. Racial disparities in incarceration increase acceptance of punitive policies. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25:1949–1954. doi: 10.1177/0956797614540307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 216.Brey E, Shutts K. Children use nonverbal cues from an adult to evaluate peers. J. Cogn. Dev. 2018;19:121–136. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2018.1449749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 217.Weisbuch M, Pauker K, Ambady N. The subtle transmission of race bias via televised nonverbal behavior. Science. 2009;326:1711–1714. doi: 10.1126/science.1178358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 218.Ritterhouse, J. L. Growing Up Jim Crow: How Black And White Southern Children Learned Race (Univ. North Carolina Press, 2006).
  • 219.Camp NP, Voigt R, Jurafsky D, Eberhardt JL. The thin blue waveform: racial disparities in officer prosody undermine institutional trust in the police. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2021;121:1157–1171. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 220.Voigt R, et al. Language from police body camera footage shows racial disparities in officer respect. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2017;114:6521–6526. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1702413114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 221.Skinner AL, Meltzoff AN, Olson KR. “Catching” social bias: exposure to biased nonverbal signals creates social biases in preschool children. Psychol. Sci. 2017;28:216–224. doi: 10.1177/0956797616678930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 222.Skinner AL, Olson KR, Meltzoff AN. Acquiring group bias: observing other people’s nonverbal signals can create social group biases. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2020;119:824–838. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 223.Skinner AL, Perry S. Are attitudes contagious? Exposure to biased nonverbal signals can create novel social attitudes. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2020;46:514–524. doi: 10.1177/0146167219862616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 224.Castelli L, Carraro L, Pavan G, Murelli E, Carraro A. The power of the unsaid: the influence of nonverbal cues on implicit attitudes. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2012;42:1376–1393. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00903.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 225.Castelli L, De Dea C, Nesdale D. Learning social attitudes: children’s sensitivity to the nonverbal behaviors of adult models during interracial interactions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008;34:1504–1513. doi: 10.1177/0146167208322769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 226.Willard G, Isaac K-J, Carney DR. Some evidence for the nonverbal contagion of racial bias. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2015;128:96–107. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.04.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 227.Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L. & Sánchez, S. The nonverbal spread of group prejudice and preliminary evidence of strategies to mitigate prejudice transmission. Preprint at OSFpreprintshttps://osf.io/8cyg2 (2022).
  • 228.Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Johnson C, Johnson B, Howard A. On the nature of prejudice: automatic and controlled processes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1997;33:510–540. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1997.1331. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 229.Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002;82:62–68. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 230.Fazio RH, Jackson JR, Dunton BC, Williams CJ. Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1995;69:1013–1027. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 231.McConnell AR, Liebold JM. Relations among the implicit association test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2001;37:435–442. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2000.1470. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 232.Skinner AL, Osnaya A, Patel B, Perry SP. Mimicking others’ nonverbal signals is associated with increased attitude contagion. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2020;44:117–131. doi: 10.1007/s10919-019-00322-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 233.Weisbuch M, Pauker K. The nonverbal transmission of intergroup bias: a model of bias contagion with implications for social policy. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2011;5:257–291. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01032.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 234.Vittrup B. Color blind or color conscious? White American mothers’ approaches to racial socialization. J. Fam. Issues. 2018;39:668–692. doi: 10.1177/0192513X16676858. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 235.Zucker JK, Patterson MM. Racial socialization practices among white American parents: relations to racial attitudes, racial identity, and school diversity. J. Fam. Issues. 2018;39:3903–3930. doi: 10.1177/0192513X18800766. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 236.Scott KE, Shutts K, Devine PG. Parents’ role in addressing children’s racial bias: the case of speculation without evidence. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2020;15:1178–1186. doi: 10.1177/1745691620927702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 237.Loyd AB, Gaither SE. Racial/ethnic socialization for white youth: what we know and future directions. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2018;59:54–64. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2018.05.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 238.Perry SP, Skinner-Dorkenoo AL, Abaied JL, Waters SF. Applying the evidence we have: support for having race conversations in white U.S. families. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2022;17:895–900. doi: 10.1177/17456916211029950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 239.Perry, S. P., Skinner, A. L., Abaied, J., Osnaya, A. & Waters, S. F. Exploring how parent–child conversations about race influence children’s implicit biases. Preprint at PsyArXiv10.31234/osf.io/3xdg8 (2021).
  • 240.Hagerman, M. White Kids: Growing Up With Privilege In A Racially Divided America (New York Univ. Press, 2018).
  • 241.Hagerman MA. White racial socialization: progressive fathers on raising “antiracist” children. J. Marriage Fam. 2017;79:60–74. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12325. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 242.Katz PA. Racists or tolerant multiculturalists? How do they begin? Am. Psychol. 2003;58:897–909. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.11.897b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 243.Vittrup B, Holden GW. Exploring the impact of educational television and parent–child discussions on children’s racial attitudes. Anal. Soc. Issues Public. Policy. 2011;11:82–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2010.01223.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 244.Cameron L, Rutland A. Extended contact through story reading in school: reducing children’s prejudice toward the disabled. J. Soc. Issues. 2006;62:469–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2006.00469.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 245.Cameron L, Rutland A, Brown R, Douch R. Changing children’s intergroup attitudes toward refugees: testing different models of extended contact. Child. Dev. 2006;77:1208–1219. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00929.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 246.Cameron L, Rutland A, Brown R. Promoting children’s positive intergroup attitudes towards stigmatized groups: extended contact and multiple classification skills training. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2007;31:454–466. doi: 10.1177/0165025407081474. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 247.Cameron L, Rutland A, Hossain R, Petley R. When and why does extended contact work? The role of high quality direct contact and group norms in the development of positive ethnic intergroup attitudes amongst children. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 2011;14:193–206. doi: 10.1177/1368430210390535. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 248.Cameron L, Rutland A, Turner R, Holman-Nicolas R, Powell C. “Changing attitudes with a little imagination”: imagined contact effects on young children’s intergroup bias. An. Psicol. 2011;27:708–717. [Google Scholar]
  • 249.Turner RN, Hewstone M, Voci A. Reducing explicit and implicit outgroup prejudice via direct and extended contact: the mediating role of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007;93:369–388. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 250.Vezzali L, Stathi S, Giovannini D, Capozza D, Visintin EP. “And the best essay is…”: extended contact and cross-group friendships at school. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2015;54:601–615. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 251.Devine PG, Forscher PS, Austin AJ, Cox WTL. Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: a prejudice habit-breaking intervention. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2012;48:1267–1278. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 252.Parker LR, Monteith MJ, Moss-Racusin CA, Van Camp AR. Promoting concern about gender bias with evidence-based confrontation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2018;74:8–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.07.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 253.Perry SP, Dovidio JF, Murphy MC, van Ryn M. The joint effect of bias awareness and self-reported prejudice on intergroup anxiety and intentions for intergroup contact. Cultur. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 2015;21:89–96. doi: 10.1037/a0037147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 254.Haney-López, I. Dog Whistle Politics: How Coded Racial Appeals Have Reinvented Racism And Wrecked The Middle Class (Oxford Univ. Press, 2014).
  • 255.Mendelberg, T. The Race Card: Campaign Strategy, Implicit Messages, And The Norm of Equality (Princeton Univ. Press, 2001).
  • 256.Perry SP, Skinner AL, Abaied JL. Bias awareness predicts color conscious racial socialization methods among white parents. J. Soc. Issues. 2019;75:1035–1056. doi: 10.1111/josi.12348. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 257.Zell E, Lesick TL. Ignorance of history and political differences in perception of racism in the United States. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2022;13:1022–1031. doi: 10.1177/19485506211056493. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 258.Bonam CM, Nair Das V, Coleman BR, Salter P. Ignoring history, denying racism: mounting evidence for the Marley hypothesis and epistemologies of ignorance. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2019;10:257–265. doi: 10.1177/1948550617751583. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 259.Hughes JM, Bigler RS, Levy SR. Consequences of learning about historical racism among European American and African American children. Child. Dev. 2007;78:1689–1705. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01096.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 260.Neufeld KHS, Starzyk KB, Boese GD, Efimoff IH, Wright S. “The more you know”: critical historical knowledge about Indian residential schools increases non-Indigenous Canadians’ empathy for Indigenous Peoples. Polit. Psychol. 2022;43:617–633. doi: 10.1111/pops.12783. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 261.Adams G, Dobles I, Gómez LH, Kurtiş T, Molina LE. Decolonizing psychological science: introduction to the special thematic section. J. Soc. Polit. Psychol. 2015;3:213–238. doi: 10.5964/jspp.v3i1.564. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 262.Cheon BK, Melani I, Hong YY. How USA-centric is psychology? An archival study of implicit assumptions of generalizability of findings to human nature based on origins of study samples. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2020;11:928–937. doi: 10.1177/1948550620927269. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 263.Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 2010;33:61–83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 264.Anderson, C. White Rage: The Unspoken Truth Of Our Racial Divide (Bloomsbury, 2016). This book examines the history of racial progress in the USA, highlighting how the racial biases of white residents and biased systems have worked in concert to maintain racial oppression over the course of USA history.
  • 265.Sommers SR, Norton MI. Race-based judgments, race-neutral justifications: experimental examination of peremptory use and the Batson challenge procedure. Law Hum. Behav. 2007;31:261–273. doi: 10.1007/s10979-006-9048-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 266.Raphael M, Ungvarsky E. Excuses, excuses: neutral explanations under Batson v. Kentucky. Univ. Mich. J. Law Reform. 1993;27:229–275. [Google Scholar]
  • 267.Rosino ML, Hughey MW. The war on drugs, racial meanings, and structural racism: a holistic and reproductive approach. Am. J. Econ. Sociol. 2018;77:849–892. doi: 10.1111/ajes.12228. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 268.Wessler, M. Updated charts provide insights on racial disparities, correctional control, jail suicides, and more. Prison Policy Initiativehttps://www.prisonpolicy.org/blog/2022/05/19/updated_charts/ (2022).
  • 269.Nellis, A. The color of justice: racial and ethnic disparities in state prisons. The Sentencing Projecthttps://www.sentencingproject.org/reports/the-color-of-justice-racial-and-ethnic-disparity-in-state-prisons-the-sentencing-project/ (2021).
  • 270.Dixon J, Tredoux C, Durrheim K, Finchilescu G, Clack B. ‘The inner citadels of the color line’: mapping the micro‐ecology of racial segregation in everyday life spaces. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass. 2008;2:1547–1569. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00123.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 271.Durrheim K, Quayle M, Whitehead K, Kriel A. Denying racism: discursive strategies used by the South African media. Crit. Arts. 2005;19:167–186. doi: 10.1080/02560040585310111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 272.Race in South Africa: ‘We haven’t learnt we are human beings first.’ BBC Newshttps://www.bbc.com/news/world-africa-55333625 (2021).
  • 273.Shutts K, Kinzler KD, Katz RC, Tredoux C, Spelke ES. Race preferences in children: insights from South Africa. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:1283–1291. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01072.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 274.Dunham Y, Srinivasan M, Dotsch R, Barner D. Religion insulates ingroup evaluations: the development of intergroup attitudes in India. Dev. Sci. 2014;17:311–319. doi: 10.1111/desc.12105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 275.Bar-Tal, D. Intractable Conflicts: Socio-Psychological Foundations And Dynamics (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2011).
  • 276.Bar-Tal D, Diamond AH, Nasie M. Political socialization of young children in intractable conflicts: conception and evidence. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2017;41:415–425. doi: 10.1177/0165025416652508. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 277.Diesendruck G, Birnbaum D, Deeb I, Segall G, Ben-Eliyahu A. The development of social essentialism: the case of Israeli children’s inferences about Jews and Arabs. Child. Dev. 2010;81:757–777. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01432.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 278.Nasie M, Diamond AH, Bar-Tal D. Young children in intractable conflicts: the Israeli case. Person. Soc. Psych. Rev. 2016;20:365–392. doi: 10.1177/1088868315607800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 279.Teichman Y. The development of Israeli children’s images of Jews and Arabs and their expression in human figure drawings. Dev. Psych. 2001;37:749–761. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Nature Reviews Psychology are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES