Abstract
Objective:
Global-local visuospatial processing has been widely investigated in both healthy and clinical populations. Recent studies indicated that individuals with ADHD lack a global processing bias. However, the extant literature regarding global-local processing style focuses solely on the visual modality.
Methods:
ADHD (N = 21) and typically developed (TD) controls (N = 24) underwent an auditory global-local task, in which they had to decide whether the melody is ascending or descending in global or local conditions.
Results:
TD controls exhibited a classic global processing bias in the auditory task. The ADHD group exhibited no global processing bias, indicating similar processing for global and local dimensions, implying that individuals with ADHD are distracted by incongruent information in global and local conditions similarly, in both visual and auditory tasks.
Conclusion:
A lack of global processing bias in ADHD is not limited to the visuospatial modality and likely reflects a broader and more general processing style.
Keywords: ADHD, global-local, processing bias, auditory processing, attention
The global processing bias has been first demonstrated in Navon’s (1977) seminal work entitled “Forest before trees,” suggesting a tendency toward processing the global features of visuospatial stimuli before processing its local features. Since then, the global processing bias has been widely investigated and replicated mainly in visuospatial tasks, demonstrating that when a visual stimulus that is composed of global and local features is presented (e.g., a large “H” composed of small “S”s), the global dimension (e.g., the large “H”) is processed automatically (Fink et al., 1997; Kimchi & Palmer, 1982; Loftus & Harley, 2004; Macrae & Lewis, 2002; Sanocki, 1993), while the local dimension (e.g., small “S”s) is processed more slowly and is subjected to interference from the global dimension (the so-called “Navon effect”). Researchers have shown that in various psychopathologies, the global processing bias effect is weaker, nonexistent, and sometimes even reversed and a local processing bias exists (i.e., priority to the local dimension; de Fockert & Cooper, 2014; Koldewyn et al., 2013; Plaisted et al., 1999; Porter & Coltheart, 2006; Scherf et al., 2008; Shilton et al., 2019; Yovel et al., 2005). Specifically, recent studies suggested that individuals with ADHD process visual scenes without a global processing bias (Cardillo et al., 2020; Cohen & Kalanthroff, 2019), or with a local processing bias (Song & Hakoda, 2015). Researchers have even suggested that a lack of global processing bias might be an underlying mechanism of a few known phenomena in ADHD. For example, some researchers have suggested that the difficulty of individuals with ADHD in social interactions can be (at least partially) attributed to deficits in processing the “big picture” (Kalanthroff et al., 2013; Nijmeijer et al., 2009 (e.g., not using the context to identify the peer’s emotions (Singh et al., 1998; Yuill & Lyon, 2007), or focusing visual attention in irrelevant areas of the face to recognize emotions (Boakes et al., 2008; Serrano et al., 2018). Furthermore, researchers have also suggested that the lack of a global processing bias may explain the impairment in set-shifting and flexibility evident in ADHD (Lawrence et al., 2004; O’Brien et al., 2010; Toplak et al., 2008;). However, current literature on the global-local processing bias, in general, and in ADHD specifically, is limited to the visuospatial domain, raising the question of whether the lack of global processing bias in ADHD affects only visuospatial information or rather it reflects a broader and more general cognitive processing bias. The absence of evidence for the lack of global processing bias from modalities other than the visuospatial modality, weakens the claim that the lack of global processing bias in ADHD affects general processing style, might underlie the disorder’s phenotype and is clinically relevant to patients with ADHD.
The auditory modality stands out as a good candidate to investigate global-local processing outside of the visuospatial field. Auditory processing has been studied in both clinical and healthy populations in the context of music perception (Deruelle et al., 2005; Dowling, 1982; Goswami, 2011; Peretz, 1990). Music can be spatially and temporally hierarchically organized in timescales and subdivisions, allowing for the investigation of various abilities such as cognitive control, attention, inhibition, and levels of processing (Carrer, 2015; Grinspun et al., 2020; Lipe, 1995). Indeed, a few studies have previously used an auditory task to measure global-local processing (Bouvet et al., 2014; Ouimet, Foster, & Hyde, 2012; Peretz, 1990). Using this task, researchers have examined the ability to distinguish between the hierarchical organization of auditory stimuli. As in the visual global-local task, the auditory task is designed so that each stimulus is composed of both a global and a local dimension and that these dimensions can be congruent or incongruent. Thus, when participants are required to attend to one dimension and ignore the other, we can examine how they process the sensory information.
Although only a handful of studies have tested global-local processing in the auditory modality, results from healthy adults show that an auditory global advantage (global processing bias) exists in detecting changes in the melodic contour (Fujioka et al., 2004; Ouimet, Foster, & Hyde, 2012) when allocating attention to changes in melody within long time spans (and not between every pair of notes) (Justus & List, 2005; List et al., 2007; Sanders & Poeppel, 2007). One such task is the “melodic global-local task” (Justus & List, 2005; see Figure 1). In this task, participants are asked to listen to a nine notes melody in each trial. These notes are grouped to form three triplets of notes. Each triplet could appear as ascending (from the lowest note to the highest) or as descending (from the highest note to the lowest). The “melody” itself could also appear as ascending (the three triplets are ordered from the lowest pitch to the highest) or as descending (the three triplets are ordered from the highest pitch to the lowest). Participants are asked to focus on either the local dimension (are the notes within the triplets ascending or descending) or on the global dimension (is the overall “melody” ascending or descending), as the two dimensions can be congruent (e.g., both ascending) or incongruent (e.g., within each triplet the notes are descending but each triplet is higher than the prior triplet). Similar to the visuospatial task, it has been shown that performance is improved in the global task and that the global dimension creates more interference when participants are instructed to attend to the local dimension than vice versa. As mentioned above, this task was mainly used with healthy adults (Bouvet et al., 2011; Fujioka et al., 2004; Ouimet, Foster, & Hyde, 2012). However, evidence from patients with autism suggests that just as in the visuospatial task, global processing bias does not exist for these individuals in the auditory task (Bouvet et al., 2014; Ouimet, Foster, Tryfon, et al., 2012). This provides initial evidence that the absence of global processing bias in clinical populations might not be limited to the visuospatial domain.
Figure 1.
The melodic global-local task: (a) an example of two congruent trials. In the left panel, both the global melody and the local triplets are ascending whilst in the right panel, both are descending. (b) An example of two incongruent trials. In the left panel, the global melody is descending whilst the local triplets are ascending. In the right panel, the global melody is ascending whilst the local triplets are descending.
The current study has two goals. First, we wish to add to the very limited literature on global-local processing outside the visuospatial domain by replicating the global processing bias in typically developed (TD) adults using an auditory task. Second, we wish to investigate global-local processing outside the visuospatial domain in adult participants with ADHD, for the first time. By that, we wish to determine whether the lack of global processing bias in ADHD is limited to the visuospatial domain or whether it indicates a broader more general processing mechanism. Therefore, individuals with ADHD and TD adults completed the auditory global-local task (Ouimet, Foster, & Hyde, 2012), in which they were asked to identify whether a “melody” (global level) or inner triplet groups (local level) was going “up” or “down.” We hypothesized that while the TD group will exhibit a global processing bias (larger interference by the incongruent global dimension when attending to the local dimension) the ADHD group will not.
Method
Participants
Twenty-four participants with ADHD and 26 typically developed (TD) participants took part in this study for partial course credit or a small monetary payment (~15 USD). All participants had normal auditory abilities, normal or corrected-to-normal vision had no history of any psychiatric disorder (other than ADHD for the ADHD group), were naïve as to the purposes of the experiment, and reported having no formal musical education or training (defined as 3 years or more of musical education/training). All participants in both groups were undergraduate university students. Participants in the ADHD group had a formal diagnosis of ADHD conducted by the Institute for the Diagnosis of Learning Disabilities and Attention Deficits (ELAH) at the Israel National Institute for Testing and Evaluations (NITE; established by the associated heads of the universities in Israel) and were recognized as having ADHD by their academic institution. The diagnosis included 2 questionnaires and 20 computerized tests that assessed cognitive functions, language (reading and writing), arithmetic thinking, attention, memory, perception, and general processing speed. The diagnosis was conducted in three meetings: Two meetings of approximately 2 hr each, for the questionnaires and tasks, and 1 meeting for the in-person interview. Importantly, the above-mentioned ADHD diagnostic procedure is completely compatible with DSM–5 criteria. Importantly, to be diagnosed with ADHD, a previous indication of ADHD, prior to the age of 12 years, was obligatory. ADHD participants declared that they did not use any psychiatric drugs other than methylphenidate-based drugs (Ritalin or Concerta) and that they refrained from taking their ADHD-related medication on the day of the experiment. TD participants reported having no history of ADHD and having no suspicions of having undiagnosed ADHD. Five participants (three ADHD and two TD controls) either failed to complete the study procedures or exhibited poor performance on the task 1 and were, therefore, excluded from analyses. Of the 21 valid participants in the ADHD group, 13 were female and 8 were men, with mean age = 24.29 (SD = 3.13). The TD control group consisted of 24 participants, 16 females and 8 men, mean age = 22.79 (SD = 2.02).
A power analysis using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007) indicated that the current sample allowed for examination of the three-way interaction (Congruency × Attention Condition × Group) at a power >95% to test small to medium effects size with a Type I error (α < .05).
Stimuli and Procedure
The melodic global-local task (Ouimet, Foster, & Hyde, 2012) included 160 melodic-like musical trials, divided into two blocks (global vs. local) of 80 trials each (Figure 1).
The stimuli were created using Sibelius software (version 6; Avid technology). Each “melody” consisted of nine tones on an a-tonal scale, to avoid familiarity bias resulting from exposure to western music. The nine tones were grouped to form three triplets of notes. Each triplet could appear as ascending (from the lowest note to the highest) or as descending (from the highest note to the lowest). The “melody” itself could also appear as ascending (the three triplets are ordered from the lowest pitch to the highest) or as descending (the three triplets are ordered from the highest pitch to the lowest). Within each “melody,” all three triplets were always in the same direction (ascending or descending; Figure 1). These patterns were manipulated to create two conditions: a congruent condition, in which the melody and the triplets were both ascending or descending (Figure 1a), and an incongruent condition, in which the melody was ascending, and the triplets were descending, or vice versa (Figure 1b). In the local block, participants were asked to focus on the local level of the auditory stimuli by indicating whether the notes within the triplets are ascending or descending. In the global block, participants were asked to focus on the global level (i.e., changed throughout the melody) by indicating whether the pattern of the triplets was ascending or descending. After each vocal stimulus, participants were asked to make an accurate decision as quickly as possible, using the keyboard, with no time limit. Prior to the beginning of the task, participants completed a practice block with feedback for accuracy, which was identical to the experimental block and was not further analyzed. Study procedures were administered via Gorilla Experiment Builder (www.gorilla.sc; Anwyl-Irvine et al., 2019).
Results
To assess the performance in the melodic global-local task, we calculated the accuracy rate (correct-response ratio) for each individual in each auditory attention condition (global vs. local) and each congruency condition (congruent vs. incongruent). A three-way mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) was carried out on accuracy rates, with auditory attention condition and congruency as within-subject factors, and group (ADHD vs. typically developed [TD] controls) as a between-subject factor (Figure 2). Age and gender were included as covariates in all analyses (to account for the minor yet existing differences between the groups). The results revealed higher accuracy rates for the global compared to the local auditory attention condition (F[1,41] = 4.213, p = .047, = 0.093), but no main effects for congruency (F[1,41] = 1.526, p = .224, = 0.036), nor group (F[1,41] = 1.844, p = .182, = 0.043). Most importantly, the three-way interaction was significant, (F[1,41] = 4.932, p = .032, = 0.107).
Figure 2.
Mean accuracy rates (correct response ratio) for the melodic global-local task as a function of congruency and global/local conditions for the typically developed (TD) control group and the ADHD group. Error bars represent one standard error from the mean based on Cousineau’s (2005) method for within-subject designs.
To further investigate this three-way interaction according to our a-priori hypotheses, 2 two-way ANOVAs were carried out on accuracy rates data for each group separately, with attention condition and congruency as within-subject factors. For the TD control group, a significant simple main effect was observed for the auditory attention condition (F[1, 23] = 8.860, p = .007, = 0.278), indicating higher accuracy rates for the global compared to the local conditions. The simple main effect for the congruency condition was not significant (F[1,23] = 4.136, p = .054, = 0.152). The two-way simple interaction was significant (F[1,23] = 10.651, p = .003, = 0.317), indicating a classic global processing bias (i.e., Navon effect), such that accuracy rates were lower for the incongruent condition compared to the congruent condition, but only for the local condition (t[23] = 2.915, p = .008, d = 0.772) and not for the global condition (t[23] = 1.550, p = .135, d = 0.190; see Figure 2). For the ADHD group, the simple main effect for the congruency condition was significant (F[1,20] = 22.582, p < .001, = 0.530), indicating higher accuracy rates for the congruent compared to the incongruent condition. There was no significant simple main effect for the auditory attention condition (F[1,20] = 1.682, p = .209, = 0.078), and no significant two-way interaction (F[1,20] = 0.515, p = .481, = 0.025), indicating lower accuracy rates for the incongruent compared to congruent condition, in both the global (t[20] = 3.778, p = .001, d = 0.606) and local conditions (t[20] = 3.937, p = .001, d = 0.951; Figure 2), with no significant differences between them.
Discussion
The current study had two goals: (a) to demonstrate the existence of a global processing bias in the auditory modality and (b) to investigate global-local processing in ADHD participants using an auditory task. In typically developed (TD) participants, the results indicated a classic “Navon-like” global processing bias effect: the global dimension of the auditory stimulus was processed more accurately and with less interference from the local dimension, whilst the local dimension was processed less accurately and with more interference from the global dimension. Consistent with our predictions, and with previous global-local studies using visuospatial tasks (Cardillo et al., 2020; Cohen & Kalanthroff, 2019; Gargaro et al., 2018; Kalanthroff et al., 2013; Song & Hakoda, 2015), we did not find an indication for a global processing bias in adults with ADHD. While TD controls displayed a large congruency effect in the local condition and no congruency effect in the global condition, individuals with ADHD exhibited a large congruency effect in both the global and local conditions, with no difference between them. In other words, individuals with ADHD did not exhibit a tendency to prioritize the global compared to the local dimension of the auditory stimulus and thus were distracted by incongruent information, regardless of whether global or local information was presented.
The results of the current study contribute to our understating of auditory processing in ADHD. First, individuals with ADHD are extremely influenced by complex auditory environments (Laffere et al., 2021) and experience significant difficulties in speech-in-noise perception (Michalek et al., 2014), and in processing auditory stimuli with temporal variation (e.g., in a classroom; Cassuto et al., 2013; Schafer et al., 2013). The current study results can shed new light on this well-known phenomenon. In general, temporal patterns can be processed on both local and global levels (Geiser et al., 2014). The auditory stimuli might be processed less accurately in individuals who have no auditory global precedence, mostly in complex auditory environments with multiple tonal streaming. In turn, this might lead individuals with ADHD to “miss” parts (sometimes relevant) of the auditory scene and to focus on the local (sometimes irrelevant) aspect of the auditory scene. Thus, the lack of global bias in auditory processing in ADHD may be one cause for the difficulty to focus and process auditory information when that information is presented in a noisy environment, a common issue for individuals with ADHD.
The current study’s results can explain another implication of ADHD auditory processing, associated with core ADHD daily symptoms, namely that children and adults with ADHD exhibit difficulties in tracking, estimating, and producing the beat of rhythmic sounds (Carrer, 2015; Lense et al., 2021; Puyjarinet et al., 2017). Reduced global auditory bias might account for these difficulties, at least to some extent, as the global auditory bias influences attention allocation throughout grouping and long intervals. Moreover, a few recent investigations have suggested that deficits in temporal information processing (e.g., tapping reproduction task) may be linked to time perception (Slater & Tate, 2018; Tiffin-Richards et al., 2004), an ability that has been shown to be deficient in ADHD (Smith et al., 2002; Toplak & Tannock, 2005). Specifically, in a seminal series of studies, it has been shown that individuals with ADHD tend to overestimate time, such that time is subjectively perceived as longer for these individuals (e.g., Carrer, 2015; Zheng et al., 2022). In fact, exaggerated time estimation has even been suggested as an alternative explanation for the known “delayed aversion” effect in ADHD (Noreika et al., 2013; Toplak et al., 2006). Therefore, it is possible that the lack of auditory global bias found in ADHD may be one cause for the difficulty in estimating rhythmic timing, and thus, in time estimation in general.
The current study results demonstrate, for the first time, that a lack of global processing bias in individuals with ADHD exists beyond the visuospatial modality. The mechanism behind this broader, multi-dimensional bias can be inferred from previous studies. Accumulating evidence have shown that ADHD is characterized by low levels of alertness (e.g., Epstein et al., 1998; Nigg, 2006; L. Tucha et al., 2009). This notion of abnormal alertness levels in ADHD is also supported by both the disorder’s formal definition (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and by the tools used to diagnose it, such as the Continuous Performance Test (CCPT; Conners et al., 2003) and the Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A.; Greenberg et al., 1996), both highly contingent upon alertness levels. In fact, even the common neuropharmacological medications for ADHD (e.g., Methylphenidate) are stimulants, which increase alertness levels (Reid & Borkowski, 1984; O. Tucha et al., 2006). The effect of alertness on global-local processing has been demonstrated. In a series of studies, it has been shown that alertness enhances global processing, in both healthy controls (Tanzer et al., 2016; Weinbach & Henik, 2011) and individuals with ADHD (Kalanthroff et al., 2013). Therefore, it is possible that lower levels of alertness, characterizing ADHD, might result in reduced global processing in a general and multi-dimensional manner. This speculation regarding a potential mechanism which affects the general global-local information process in ADHD is a direction for future studies.
A few limitations of the current study should be considered. First, the experiment took place during the COVID-19 global pandemic. Although, performance (and dropout rates) was similar to previous experiments, it is important to mention that we could not deliver the instruction and ensure performance on the practice block in-person. Second, in order to ensure our results are exclusively driven by the existence of ADHD, we made a significant effort to ensure our participants have no comorbid disorder. However, it is important to mention that in ADHD, comorbid diagnoses are the rule rather than the exception. Thus, future replications of the current study results with a more heterogeneous sample are warranted.
In conclusion, the present investigation provides new insights into the underlying mechanism of information processing in ADHD. Our findings show: (a) that global-local processing can be investigated in the auditory domain and (b) that individuals with ADHD’s lack of global processing bias is not limited to the visuospatial modality and is likely to reflect a broader and more general multi-dimensional processing bias. Lack of global processing bias in the auditory domain might underlie the difficulty of individuals with ADHD to process complex auditory environments and perhaps even to track, estimate, and produce the beat of rhythmic sounds. That lack of global processing bias exists outside of the visuospatial domain indicates that this abnormal processing style may play an important role in ADHD phenotype.
Author Biographies
Aviv Akerman is finishing her PhD in psychology at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem. She received a MA degree in rehabilitation and neuropsychology from the Hebrew University of Jerusalem. Her other research interests are response inhibition, inattentional cognitions, how they emerge into intrusion, and how to stop its emergence.
Amit Etkovitch is finnishing her M.A studies in music therapy at Bar Ilan University. She Received a bachelor’s degree in psychology from the Hebrew University of Jerusalem. She is currently intrested in treating adult hospitalized patients through music.
Eyal Kalanthroff is an associate professor at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Department of Psychology, and the head of the Clinical Neuropsychology Laboratory. He received a PhD in clinical psychology and cognitive neuropsychology from Ben-Gurion University of the Negev in Beer Sheva, Israel. His current research interest is focused on cognitive control, response inhibition and spatial attention in typically developed individuals and in individual diagnosed with various psychopathologies.
Slow reaction times or low accuracy rates—more than two standard deviations from the group mean.
Footnotes
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
ORCID iD: Aviv Akerman https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7900-4869
Reference
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.) American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Anwyl-Irvine A. L., Massonié J., Flitton A., Kirkham N. Z., Evershed J. K. (2019). Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioural experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods, 52, 388–407. 10.3758/s13428-019-01237-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boakes J., Chapman E., Houghton S., West J. (2008). Facial affect interpretation in boys with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 14(1), 82–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouvet L., Rousset S., Valdois S., Donnadieu S. (2011). Global precedence effect in audition and vision: Evidence for similar cognitive styles across modalities. Acta Psychologica, 138(2), 329–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouvet L., Simard-Meilleur A. A., Paignon A., Mottron L., Donnadieu S. (2014). Auditory local bias and reduced global interference in autism. Cognition, 131(3), 367–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardillo R., Vio C., Mammarella I. C. (2020). A comparison of local-global visuospatial processing in autism spectrum disorder, nonverbal learning disability, ADHD and typical development. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 103, 103682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrer L. R. (2015). Music and sound in time processing of children with ADHD. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassuto H., Ben-Simon A., Berger I. (2013). Using environmental distractors in the diagnosis of ADHD. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen E., Kalanthroff E. (2019). Visuospatial processing bias in ADHD: A potential artifact in the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and the Rorschach inkblots test. Psychological Assessment, 31(5), 699–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conners C. K., Epstein J. N., Angold A., Klaric J. (2003). Continuous performance test performance in a normative epidemiological sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(5), 555–562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cousineau D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45. [Google Scholar]
- de Fockert J. W., Cooper A. (2014). Higher levels of depression are associated with reduced global bias in visual processing. Cognition & Emotion, 28(3), 541–549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deruelle C., Schön D., Rondan C., Mancini J. (2005). Global and local music perception in children with Williams syndrome. Neuroreport, 16(6), 631–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dowling W. J. (1982). Melodic information processing and its development. In Deutsch D. (Eds.), Psychology of Music (pp. 413–429). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Epstein J. N., Conners C. K., Sitarenios G., Erhardt D. (1998). Continuous performance test results of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12(2), 155–168. [Google Scholar]
- Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., Buchner A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fink G. R., Halligan P. W., Marshall J. C., Frith C. D., Frackowiak R. S., Dolan R. J. (1997). Neural mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visual stimuli. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 120(10), 1779–1791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujioka T., Trainor L. J., Ross B., Kakigi R., Pantev C. (2004). Musical training enhances automatic encoding of melodic contour and interval structure. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1010–1021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gargaro B. A., May T., Tonge B. J., Sheppard D. M., Bradshaw J. L., Rinehart N. J. (2018). Attentional mechanisms in autism, ADHD, and autism-ADHD using a local-global paradigm. Journal of Attention Disorders, 22(14), 1320–1332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geiser E., Walker K. M., Bendor D. (2014). Global timing: A conceptual framework to investigate the neural basis of rhythm perception in humans and non-human species. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U. (2011). A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 3–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg L. M., Kindschi C. L., Corman C. L. (1996). TOVA test of variables of attention: clinical guide. TOVA Research Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- Grinspun N., Nijs L., Kausel L., Onderdijk K., Sepúlveda N., Rivera-Hutinel A. (2020). Selective attention and inhibitory control of attention are correlated with music audiation. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Justus T., List A. (2005). Auditory attention to frequency and time: An analogy to visual local-global stimuli. Cognition, 98(1), 31–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalanthroff E., Naparstek S., Henik A. (2013). Spatial processing in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 27(5), 546–555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimchi R., Palmer S. E. (1982). Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 8(4), 521–535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koldewyn K., Jiang Y. V., Weigelt S., Kanwisher N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 2329–2340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laffere A., Dick F., Holt L. L., Tierney A. (2021). Attentional modulation of neural entrainment to sound streams in children with and without ADHD. NeuroImage, 224, 117396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence V., Houghton S., Douglas G., Durkin K., Whiting K., Tannock R. (2004). Executive function and ADHD: A comparison of children’s performance during neuropsychological testing and real-world activities. Journal of Attention Disorders, 7(3), 137–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lense M. D., Ladányi E., Rabinowitch T. C., Trainor L., Gordon R. (2021). Rhythm and timing as vulnerabilities in neurodevelopmental disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1835), 20200327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipe A. (1995). The use of music performance tasks in the assessment of cognitive functioning among older adults with dementia. Journal of Music Therapy, 32(3), 137–151. [Google Scholar]
- List A., Justus T., Robertson L. C., Bentin S. (2007). A mismatch negativity study of local–global auditory processing. Brain Research, 1153, 122–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loftus G. R., Harley E. M. (2004). How different spatial-frequency components contribute to visual information acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 30, 104–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macrae C. N., Lewis H. L. (2002). Do I know you? Processing orientation and face recognition. Psychological Science, 13(2), 194–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michalek A. M., Watson S. M., Ash I., Ringleb S., Raymer A. (2014). Effects of noise and audiovisual cues on speech processing in adults with and without ADHD. International Journal of Audiology, 53(3), 145–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navon D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–383. [Google Scholar]
- Nigg J. T. (2006). What causes ADHD? Understanding what goes wrong and why. Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Nijmeijer J. S., Hoekstra P. J., Minderaa R. B., Buitelaar J. K., Altink M. E., Buschgens C. J., Fliers E. A., Rommelse N. N., Sergeant J. A., Hartman C. A. (2009). PDD symptoms in ADHD, an independent familial trait? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(3), 443–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noreika V., Falter C. M., Rubia K. (2013). Timing deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Evidence from neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 235–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ouimet T., Foster N. E., Hyde K. L. (2012). Auditory global-local processing: Effects of attention and musical experience. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(4), 2536–2544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ouimet T., Foster N. E. V., Tryfon A., Hyde K. L. (2012). Auditory-musical processing in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and brain imaging studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 325–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien J. W., Dowell L. R., Mostofsky S. H., Denckla M. B., Mahone E. M. (2010). Neuropsychological profile of executive function in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(7), 656–670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peretz I. (1990). Processing of local and global musical information by unilateral brain-damaged patients. Brain, 113(4), 1185–1205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plaisted K., Swettenham J., Rees L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(5), 733–742. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porter M. A., Coltheart M. (2006). Global and local processing in Williams syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome: Perception, attention, and construction. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30, 771–789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puyjarinet F., Bégel V., Lopez R., Dellacherie D., Dalla Bella S. (2017). Children and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder cannot move to the beat. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid M. K., Borkowski J. G. (1984). Effects of methylphenidate (ritalin) on information processing in hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 169–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders L. D., Poeppel D. (2007). Local and global auditory processing: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Neuropsychologia, 45(6), 1172–1186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanocki T. (1993). Time course of object identification: Evidence for a global-to-local contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 19(4), 878–898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer E. C., Mathews L., Mehta S., Hill M., Munoz A., Bishop R., Moloney M. (2013). Personal FM systems for children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and/or attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): An initial investigation. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(1), 30–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scherf K. S., Luna B., Kimchi R., Minshew N., Behrmann M. (2008). Missing the big picture: Impaired development of global shape processing in autism. Autism Research, 1(2), 114–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serrano V. J., Owens J. S., Hallowell B. (2018). Where children with ADHD direct visual attention during emotion knowledge tasks: Relationships to accuracy, response time, and ADHD symptoms. Journal of Attention Disorders, 22(8), 752–763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shilton A. L., Laycock R., Crewther S. G. (2019). Different effects of trait and state anxiety on global-local visual processing following acute stress. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 23(3), 155–170. [Google Scholar]
- Singh S. D., Ellis C. R., Winton A. S., Singh N. N., Leung J. P., Oswald D. P. (1998). Recognition of facial expressions of emotion by children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Modification, 22(2), 128–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slater J. L., Tate M. C. (2018). Timing deficits in ADHD: Insights from the neuroscience of musical rhythm. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 12, 51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A., Taylor E., Warner Rogers J., Newman S., Rubia K. (2002). Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 529–542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song Y., Hakoda Y. (2015). Lack of global precedence and global-to-local interference without local processing deficit: A robust finding in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder under different visual angles of the Navon task. Neuropsychology, 29(6), 888–894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanzer M., Weinbach N., Mardo E., Henik A., Avidan G. (2016). Phasic alertness enhances processing of face and non-face stimuli in congenital prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 89, 299–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiffin-Richards M. C, Hasselhorn M., Richards M. L., Banaschewski T., Rothenberger A. (2004). Time reproduction in finger tapping tasks by children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or dyslexia. Dyslexia, 10(4), 299–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toplak M. E., Connors L., Shuster J., Knezevic B., Parks S. (2008). Review of cognitive, cognitive-behavioral, and neural-based interventions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Clinical Psychology Review, 28(5), 801–823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toplak M. E., Dockstader C., Tannock R. (2006). Temporal information processing in ADHD: Findings to date and new methods. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 151(1), 15–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toplak M. E., Tannock R. (2005). Time perception: Modality and duration effects in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(5), 639–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucha L., Tucha O., Walitza S., Sontag T. A., Laufkötter R., Linder M., Lange K. W. (2009). Vigilance and sustained attention in children and adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disordersattention Disorders, 12(5), 410–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucha O., Prell S., Mecklinger L., Bormann-Kischkel C., Kübber S., Linder M., Walitza S., Lange K. W. (2006). Effects of methylphenidate on multiple components of attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychopharmacology, 185(3), 315–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinbach N., Henik A. (2011). Phasic alertness can modulate executive control by enhancing global processing of visual stimuli. Cognition, 121(3), 454–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yovel I., Revelle W., Mineka S. (2005). Who sees trees before forest? The obsessive-compulsive style of visual attention. Psychological Science, 16(2), 123–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuill N., Lyon J. (2007). Selective difficulty in recognising facial expressions of emotion in boys with ADHD: General performance impairments or specific problems in social cognition?. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 398–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng Q., Wang X., Chiu K. Y., Shum K. K. (2022). Time perception deficits in children and adolescents with ADHD: A meta-analysis. Journal of Attention Disorders, 26(2), 267–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]