Keywords: hand, proprioception, reaching, recalibration, sensorimotor map
Abstract
Spatial perception of our hand is closely linked to our ability to move the hand accurately. We might therefore expect that reach planning would take into account any changes in perceived hand position; in other words, that perception and action relating to the hand should depend on a common sensorimotor map. However, there is evidence to suggest that changes in perceived hand position affect a body representation that functions separately from the body representation used to control movement. Here, we examined target-directed reaching before and after participants either did (Mismatch group) or did not (Veridical group) experience a cue conflict known to elicit recalibration in perceived hand position. For the reaching task, participants grasped a robotic manipulandum that positioned their unseen hand for each trial. Participants then briskly moved the handle straight ahead to a visual target, receiving no performance feedback. For the perceptual calibration task, participants estimated the locations of visual, proprioceptive, or combined cues about their unseen hand. The Mismatch group experienced a gradual 70-mm forward mismatch between visual and proprioceptive cues, resulting in forward proprioceptive recalibration. Participants made significantly shorter reaches after this manipulation, consistent with feeling their hand to be further forward than it was, but reaching performance returned to baseline levels after only 10 reaches. The Veridical group, after exposure to veridically aligned visual and proprioceptive cues about the hand, showed no change in reach distance. These results suggest that perceptual recalibration affects the same sensorimotor map that is used to plan target-directed reaches.
NEW & NOTEWORTHY If perceived hand position changes, we might assume this affects the sensorimotor map and, in turn, reaches made with that hand. However, there is evidence for separate body representations involved in perception versus action. After a cross-sensory conflict that results in proprioceptive recalibration in the forward direction, participants made shorter reaches as predicted, but only briefly. This suggests perceptual recalibration does affect the sensorimotor map used to plan reaches, but the interaction may be short-lived.
INTRODUCTION
To plan and execute hand movements to interact efficiently with objects in the environment, the brain must have an accurate representation of the hand’s position. This representation is thought to be multisensory, including both visual information from the eyes and proprioceptive information from the muscles and joints of the upper limb (1). When both a visual estimate (hV) and a proprioceptive estimate (hP) of true hand position (H) are available, these are weighted and combined to form a single integrated estimate (hVP):
(1) |
where wV is the weight of vision relative to proprioception (i.e., wV = 0.7 implies 70% reliance on vision and 30% reliance on proprioception). Weighting may be determined by relative variance in the sensory signals (1–3) as well as top-down influences such as attention or task demands (4, 5). hV and hP do not agree perfectly even in normal circumstances (6), but in the presence of an externally imposed conflict between these cues, visuo-proprioceptive recalibration occurs (7–9). This may involve both a shift of the proprioceptive estimate closer to the visual estimate (ΔhP) and vice versa (ΔhV).
Proprioceptive recalibration can be conceptualized as perceiving the endpoint effector (hand) to be closer to a visual cue than it is in reality; in other words, a high-level change in bodily perception. This does not necessarily imply a change in the lower-level proprioceptive signals about arm configuration. Indeed, we recently found evidence that proprioceptive recalibration is somatotopically focal (10). When visual cues of fingertip position are shifted forward from proprioceptive cues, the fingertip feels as though it has shifted further away from the body. The knuckle was also felt to be forward-shifted to a lesser extent, but wrist and elbow perception did not show any evidence of a shift (10). In other words, when induced this way, “proprioceptive recalibration” is really a localized distortion in the high-level proprioceptively derived representation of fingertip position that is evident when only somatosensory cues are available.
We might expect that reach planning would take into account any such changes in perceived hand position; in other words, that perception and action relating to the hand should depend on a common sensorimotor map. We recently observed somatotopically focal changes in the excitability of primary motor cortex (M1) that were specifically related to visuo-proprioceptive recalibration, after controlling for motor behavior (10, 11); findings like these are suggestive of a close relationship between perception of hand position and motor execution involving that hand. However, substantial work has suggested that changes in perceived hand position may affect a body representation that functions separately from the body representation used to control movement; these have been referred to as the body image and body schema, respectively (12–14).
Literature addressing some form of visuo-proprioceptive recalibration is mixed as to whether movement is affected. One challenge is the lack of research specifically focused on visuo-proprioceptive recalibration, unconfounded by other processes that are often the primary focus. For example, there is relevant literature using the rubber hand illusion (RHI), a paradigm that creates illusory body ownership over a fake arm through synchronous stroking of the seen fake arm and the hidden real arm (15). The RHI in a sense involves a visuo-proprioceptive cue conflict, since the fake arm is meant to be a visual cue related to the real arm, and proprioceptive recalibration (described as drift in this literature) is thought to occur (16, 17). Kammers et al. (12) concluded that the RHI affected the body image but not the body schema, after observing that perceptual judgments, but not ballistic motor responses, were sensitive to the RHI. On the other hand, in the original RHI study by Botvinick and Cohen (15), the illusion was assessed by having participants point at the perceived location of their other hand, a motor response that clearly was affected by the RHI (12). A complication is that in both studies, the pointing movements (15) and ballistic movements (12) were directed toward the other hand, potentially causing participants to access both body schema and body image.
A second relevant body of literature is the subset of visuomotor adaptation research that measures proprioceptive recalibration. Visuomotor adaptation is a cerebellum-dependent process in which participants experience a systematic perturbation of their movements and gradually compensate by updating their sensorimotor map to reduce systematic errors (18, 19). A common method of perturbation is cursor rotation, where participants move their unseen hand to guide a cursor to a target on a screen. The cursor can be rotated, e.g., a movement straight ahead results in the cursor moving 30° to the right. In addition to causing movement errors that lead to updating of motor commands, the mismatch between hand and cursor creates a visuo-proprioceptive conflict. Indeed, proprioceptive recalibration has now been documented extensively in this paradigm (8, 20, 21).
Several studies have used a modified cursor rotation paradigm to eliminate the motor adaptation aspect. Participants move their hand along a set channel that gradually deviates from the cursor, which always moves to the target so that no movement error is apparent but the visuo-proprioceptive mismatch is still created (22–24). Participants indeed recalibrate proprioception in these circumstances, and additionally, target-directed reaches with no cursor show a shift in direction consistent with the change in felt hand movement direction (22–24).
It makes sense that a visuo-proprioceptive mismatch created in the context of cursor rotation, with the hand actively or passively moved in a direction rotated from the cursor and target, would alter the same sensorimotor map used to plan target-directed reaches. However, it is difficult to generalize this finding to visuo-proprioceptive recalibration in general, which does not require active or passive movement, or even a movement target. In the cursor rotation paradigm, the cue conflict exists only in the context of target-directed movement; the conflict changes from zero at the home position to some maximum angular deviation at the target position.
Here, we ask a slightly different question: does visuo-proprioceptive recalibration, triggered with a cue conflict at a static position, affect the same sensorimotor map used to make active target-directed reaching movements? Or does it affect a separate body representation, as in the body image versus body schema concept? To answer this question, two groups of participants made straight-ahead target-directed reaches, with no cursor, before and after experiencing either a visuo-proprioceptive conflict (Mismatch group) or veridical visuo-proprioceptive cues (Veridical group) while the hand was stationary. The cue conflict was introduced by gradually shifting the visual cue forward from the hand to a maximum of 70 mm (Fig. 1). Proprioceptive recalibration was thus expected in the forward direction (Fig. 1Bii). We therefore predicted that the Mismatch group, feeling the hand to be further forward than it really was after proprioceptive recalibration, would make shorter reaches after experiencing the conflict (Fig. 1C, i and ii), while Veridical group reach distance would remain unchanged.
Figure 1.
Top: participant holds handle of robotic manipulandum in right hand. Images viewed in mirror of two-dimensional (2-D) VR display appear to be in line with top of manipulandum handle. Not pictured: fabric preventing vision of the upper arms. No direct vision of hands is possible. A: precalibration reaches. Participant performs straight-ahead reaching with no visual feedback about hand position, stopping at perceived position of visual target. The hand is brought passively back to the invisible starting position after each trial. B: visuo-proprioceptive calibration. Bi: participants use their left index finger on a touchscreen to indicate the perceived position of visual (V), proprioceptive (P), and combined (VP) targets. No performance feedback or knowledge of results was available at any time. For the Veridical group, VP targets remained veridical throughout. Bii: for the Mismatch group, the visual component (white disc) gradually shifted forward to a maximum of 70 mm. This generally results in both proprioceptive recalibration toward the visual target (ΔhP) and the visual recalibration toward the proprioceptive target (ΔhV). Transparent hands and writing were not visible to participants. C: postcalibration reaches. Same procedure as precalibration reaches. C, i and ii: for the Mismatch group, if ΔhP affects reaching, we predict participants will stop short of the visual target because they feel their hand is further from them (closer to the target) than it is. With only proprioceptive information about hand position, planned movements (red dashed lines) should be shorter relative to preperturbation (black arrow) for the Mismatch group but not the Veridical group.
METHODS
Participants
Thirty-two healthy right-handed adults participated in the study, which consisted of one laboratory visit. Inclusion criteria were aged 18–45 and right-handed with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Exclusion criteria were any muscular, orthopedic, or neurological disorders. All enrolled participants reported that they met these inclusion and exclusion criteria. The study was approved by Indiana University Institutional Review Board, and all participants gave written informed consent. Participants were randomly assigned to the Mismatch group (n = 16, mean age 19.9 ± 1.2 yr, 4 males) or the Veridical group (n = 16, mean age 20.5 ± 1.8 yr, 6 males) using a random sequence of ones and twos (16 of each) generated in MATLAB 2021a (MathWorks). The study was single-blind, with the experimenter knowing the participant’s group assignment but the participant not knowing.
Apparatus
Participants were seated at a reflected rear projection apparatus to perform a task with three parts (Fig. 1), grasping the handle of a KINARM Endpoint 2 D robotic manipulandum (BKIN) with their right hand throughout. Positional accuracy of the manipulandum, with high-resolution secondary encoders, is 3 microns; inertial load of the passive manipulandum is 0.8/1.0 kg (minor/major axes). Participants had no direct vision of their hand but viewed a task display that appeared to be in the plane of the manipulandum (Fig. 1A).
Procedures
The experimental session consisted of three parts. First, precalibration straight-ahead right-hand reaches to a visual target with no visual feedback about the right hand (Fig. 1A). Second, a visuo-proprioceptive calibration task with either mismatched or veridical visual and proprioceptive cues about the right hand, depending on group assignment (Fig. 1B). Third, postcalibration straight-ahead right-handed reaches to a visual target with no visual feedback about the right hand (Fig. 1C). The experiment was preceded by instructions about the two tasks (reaching and calibration) and practice of each task. The whole session took about 1 h.
Straight-ahead reaching.
Before and after the visuo-proprioceptive calibration task (Fig. 1, A and C), participants were asked to grasp the manipulandum handle in their right hand and briskly move from the starting position to the target. The hand was brought passively to the starting position for each trial, as the starting position was not visible. No online or end-point visual feedback about hand position was given at any point in this task. Reach endpoint was defined as the position at which movement velocity dropped below 5% of peak velocity.
In addition to practice trials, participants performed 20 reaches precalibration and 20 reaches postcalibration. The starting position was located at the participant’s body midline, ∼20 cm in front of their chest. The visual target was located 10 cm forward of the starting position.
Reaches were binned into sets of 5. To determine if reach endpoints were closer or further from the participant after the calibration task, the y-coordinates of each set of 5 reach endpoints were averaged within participants.
Visuo-proprioceptive calibration task.
Visuo-proprioceptive estimates of hand position are most commonly measured and/or perturbed with a bimanual task, using an “indicator” (left) hand to indicate the participant’s perception of the “target” (right) hand’s position when visual, proprioceptive, or both types of information about the target are available (2, 6, 25–27). Participants were therefore asked to use their unseen left index finger to indicate on a 32 in. touchscreen (PQLabs) where they perceived a series of targets (Table 1) related to the right (target) hand (Fig. 1B), which grasped a stationary manipulandum handle at the target position beneath the touchscreen. It was not physically possible to place the touchscreen in the horizontal plane of the visual task display at the top of the manipulandum handle due to the design of the manipulandum. Therefore, for this task, we moved the actual manipulandum out of the workspace and placed a replica manipulandum handle under the touchscreen for participants to grasp on P and VP targets. The replica was the same height and diameter as the real handle and covered with an identical rubber grip.
Table 1.
Targets in visuo-proprioceptive calibration task
Visuo-proprio (VP) target | Participant grasped stationary manipulandum handle in the right hand beneath the touchscreen. A white disc appeared to be at the top of the handle. Participant was signaled to move the unseen left indicator finger, on the upper side of the touchscreen, from one of five start positions to where he perceived the VP target to be. |
Proprioceptive (P) target | Identical to the VP target except no white disc. Participant was asked to indicate where he perceived the center of the handle as accurately as possible by placing the unseen indicator finger at that position. |
Visual (V) target | The white disc was displayed at the target position, but the right hand was down at the participant’s side. Participant was asked to move his unseen indicator finger to where he perceived the white disc. |
Both groups performed 84 trials: 42 VP, 21 V, and 21 P, in repeating order (VP-V-VP-P). For the Veridical group, the white disk was always displayed veridically at the top of the replica manipulandum handle. For the Mismatch group, the white disk moved 1.67 mm forward on each VP trial. Participants do not generally notice this perturbation, which results in a 70-mm visuo-proprioceptive mismatch by the end of the 84 trials (Fig. 1Bii) (7, 28–30). Indicator finger start position was jittered to prevent participants from being influenced by indicator finger movements in the previous trial. Importantly, there was no speed requirement and no performance feedback or knowledge of results, to preclude motor adaptation.
If the proprioceptive estimate of the right hand, as shown by left indicator finger endpoints, moves forward to close the visuo-proprioceptive gap (ΔhP), then we observe overshoot on P targets. Similarly, if perceived position of the white disk moves closer to the right hand (ΔhV), then we observe undershoot on V targets. VP trials are used to create the mismatch, whereas V and P trials are used to assess visual and proprioceptive recalibration. Thus, outcome measures are based on V and P trials. We quantified visual and proprioceptive recalibration (ΔhV and ΔhP) as previously shown (28–30). After calculating mean indicator finger end point positions in the y-dimension on the first and last four V and P trials, respectively, we computed the difference relative to actual target position, which was constant for P targets but shifts 70 mm for V targets (Mismatch group only):
(2) |
(3) |
Statistical Analysis
Data were processed using MATLAB 2021a (MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA). A mixed-model ANOVA was performed on the straight-ahead reaching endpoints, with within-participant factor “reach set” (the last set of 5 precalibration reach endpoints and the four sets of 5 postcalibration reach endpoints) and between-participant factor “group” (Mismatch and Veridical). For a significant interaction, paired sample t tests were performed within each group, comparing the last set of precalibration reaches with each set of postcalibration reaches.
For the Mismatch group only, the magnitude of change in reach endpoint (first set of postcalibration reaches minus last set of precalibration reaches) was compared with the magnitude of proprioceptive recalibration (ΔhP) and the magnitude of total recalibration (ΔhP + ΔhV) in a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA. For a significant effect, change in reach endpoint was compared with the other two magnitudes with a paired sample t test.
For the post hoc t tests, false discovery rate was controlled by the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure (31) with α set to 0.05. In the text, adjusted P values are indicated as Padj. Data and analysis code are publicly available at https://osf.io/zy49x/.
RESULTS
On average, participants in the Mismatch group recalibrated proprioception 17.4 ± 4.0 mm and vision 38.1 ± 6.1 mm (means ± SE) in response to a gradually imposed 70-mm mismatch between visual and proprioceptive cues. The example participant in Fig. 2 recalibrated to a degree consistent with the group behavior. After exposure to the cue conflict, this participant reached shorter distances with the recalibrated hand, consistent with predictions (Fig. 1C); however, this was only evident in the first set of five trials postmismatch (Fig. 2C).
Figure 2.
Example participant in the Mismatch group. A: pre-mismatch reaches. Movement paths of the 5 right-handed reaches immediately preceding mismatch task. With no performance feedback or knowledge of results, this participant consistently overshot the reach target (gray bar). B: the mismatch task gradually imposed 70 mm of visuo-proprioceptive mismatch by shifting the white disc (V target) forward from the stationary right hand (P target). This participant recalibrated both proprioception (ΔhP = 29.6 mm) and vision (ΔhV = 28.9 mm). C: four sets of 5 right-handed reaches following mismatch task. The first set of 5 reaches undershot the pre-mismatch mean (dashed line) by 16.4 mm.
Participants’ average reach distances on the four sets of five right-handed reaches (3, A, i and iii and B, i and iii) were analyzed with a mixed-model ANOVA with factors Group (Mismatch and Veridical) and Reach Set (5 sets) (Fig. 4). There was a significant Reach Set × Group interaction (F4,120 = 2.81, P = 0.028, ηp2 = 0.008), suggesting that the two groups differed in reach distance across the five reach sets. There was also an effect of Reach Set on reach distance (F4,120 = 3.97, P = 0.0046, ηp2 = 0.01), but no main effect of Group (F1,120 = 0.19, P = 0.66, ηp2 = 0.006).
Figure 3.
Group reaching distances pre- and postcalibration task. A: mismatch group (n = 16 participants). Ai: mean distance of the 5 right-handed reaches immediately preceding calibration task. Dots represent individual participants. Reaches started at 0 mm and the target was at 100 mm. Aii: mismatch task. Participants pointed with their left indicator finger to P targets (right hand, dashed gray line), V targets (white disc, solid gray line), and VP targets (combined), with a 70 mm mismatch gradually imposed. On average, participants recalibrated both vision and proprioception (ΔhV = 38.1 mm, ΔhP = 17.4 mm). Aiii: mean distance of the five sets of 5 right-handed reaches immediately following the calibration task. *First and second sets of 5 reaches were significantly different from the precalibration task reaches (mixed model ANOVA post hoc t test Padj < 0.05). B: veridical group (n = 16 participants). Postcalibration reaches did not differ significantly from precalibration task reaches. All error bars and shaded regions represent standard error.
Figure 4.
Interaction plot of reach distance across reach sets in the Mismatch vs. Veridical groups (n = 16 participants per group). Reach set 1 corresponds to the 5 precalibration task reaches. Reach sets 2–5 correspond to the four sets of 5 postcalibration reaches.
Within each group, paired sample t tests were used to compare each set of postcalibration reaches (Fig. 3iii) with the precalibration reach set (Fig. 3i). For the Mismatch group, the first and second sets of postcalibration task reaches were significantly different from the precalibration set (t15 = 3.47, Padj = 0.014; t15 = 2.86, Padj = 0.024). The third and fourth sets were not significantly different from the precalibration reaches (t15 = 1.52, Padj = 0.20; t15 = 0.76, Padj = 0.46), suggesting any effect of mismatch on reach distance did not last beyond the first two sets of five reaches. For the Veridical group, none of the postcalibration task sets of reaches differed significantly from the precalibration set (all P > 0.5). Finally, comparing the precalibration reaches across groups did not yield any evidence that the two groups reached different distances before the calibration task (t30 = 0.26, P = 0.80).
For the mismatch group, we compared participants’ change in right-hand reach undershoot from the last five precalibration reaches to the first five postcalibration reaches with total recalibration magnitude (visual plus proprioceptive) and with proprioceptive recalibration (Fig. 5). Change in reach undershoot was significantly smaller than total recalibration (t15 = −4.32, Padj = 0.0012), but not significantly different from proprioceptive recalibration (t15 = 0.46, P = 0.65). This could indicate that change in reach performance does not reflect the sum of visual and proprioceptive recalibration (Fig. 1C), although other interpretations are possible.
Figure 5.
In the Mismatch group (n = 16 participants), reach undershoot changed by an average magnitude of 20.9 mm in the predicted direction. This magnitude was significantly smaller than the total magnitude of recalibration (visual plus proprioceptive), which averaged 55.5 mm (*post hoc paired-sample t test Padj < 0.05). Change in reach undershoot did not differ significantly from the magnitude of proprioceptive recalibration, which averaged 17.4 mm.
Participants were questioned about their experience at the conclusion of the session. Most participants did not perceive any forward mismatch between the visual and proprioceptive targets during the calibration task. Two participants in each group reported perceiving such a mismatch, which is consistent with our previous work (7). Participants were also asked to rate their quality of sleep the previous night, their level of attention during the task, and how fatigued they felt after the task, on a scale of 1 to 10. We found no indication that these might differ across groups. Sleep was rated 6.9 ± 1.0 (mean ± 95% CI) by the Mismatch group and 6.6 ± 0.8 by the Veridical group. Attention was rated 7.2 ± 0.5 by the Mismatch group and 7.8 ± 0.6 by the Veridical group. Fatigue was rated 4.1 ± 1.4 by the Mismatch group and 3.8 ± 0.8 by the Veridical group.
DISCUSSION
Here, we asked whether reaching movements show evidence of change after visuo-proprioceptive recalibration in hand position estimates. The Mismatch group, after exposure to a gradual 70-mm visuo-proprioceptive mismatch, made significantly shorter reaching movements. The magnitude of change in reach distance was similar to the magnitude of proprioceptive recalibration. However, reaching performance returned to baseline levels after only 10 reaches. The Veridical group, after exposure to veridically aligned visual and proprioceptive cues about the hand, showed no evidence of a change in reach distance. Taken together, these results suggest that visuo-proprioceptive recalibration of hand estimates does affect reaching movements, but only briefly.
Reach Distance Affected by Visuo-Proprioceptive Recalibration
We predicted that reach distance would shorten after exposure to a 70-mm forward displacement of visual cues from proprioceptive cues about the hand. We reasoned that with this direction of cue conflict, proprioceptive recalibration would be expected in the forward direction. In other words, participants would come to feel that their target hand was further forward than it actually was. They would then execute reaches of smaller magnitudes, feeling there was less distance to travel from their proprioceptively perceived hand position to the visual target. Results of the present study support this prediction. The Mismatch group reached shorter distances after exposure to the visuo-proprioceptive mismatch, compared with baseline. Importantly, the Veridical group showed no change in reaching after exposure to veridical visuo-proprioceptive cues, indicating that the reach distance change was specific to the experience of a visuo-proprioceptive cue conflict. Subjects tended to overshoot the reaching target in both groups. This may be related to the lack of visual feedback about hand position at reach initiation, which is known to bias reaching (32, 33).
There may be more than one mechanism by which recalibration could alter reaching movements. Sober and Sabes (34, 35) demonstrated that visual cues predominate for planning a movement vector in extrinsic space, but proprioceptive cues dominate in converting the movement vector to a motor command in joint space. However, this assumes that visual cues of the hand’s starting position are available for planning the movement vector. In the present study, there was no visual cue about the hand’s starting position in the reaching trials, so coordinate transformations could not be avoided and proprioceptive cues had to be used in computing the movement vector. With the unseen hand positioned at the unseen starting position, subjects had to use their (forward-shifted) proprioceptive estimate of hand position to plan the movement vector to the visual target. In other words, the present results are consistent with perceptual recalibration causing a change in movement vector planning. We have no reason to think anything changed about how the muscle-/joint-level commands were determined, although we did not test this explicitly.
An important question to consider is whether it is possible that the reaching hand experienced motor adaptation during the calibration task or any other motor learning process that could affect reach distance; this would confound our interpretation of the role of proprioceptive recalibration in the change in reach distance. Importantly, the calibration task was designed to preclude any such confound. Participants never received any information about where their indicator finger landed in relation to the target, so there was no error signal that could drive motor adaptation. In addition, participants were explicitly instructed to place their indicator finger at the perceived target location, with no time constraints. The one form of motor learning that should be possible in these conditions is for the indicator finger’s pointing movements to become less variable across the calibration task. In other words, if the participant executes a movement of their indicator finger and proprioceptive feedback from the indicator hand suggests the finger did not land in the planned position, the brain could fine-tune the motor command to make more accurate predictions. However, this form of learning should not be considered a confound, as it would occur similarly in both the Misaligned and Veridical groups.
We hypothesized that in the absence of visual feedback about the hand during reaching, only the proprioceptive recalibration would contribute to target undershoot. However, we also considered the possibility that recalibration of visual estimates of the hand could include everything in the visual scene. In other words, it is possible that when visual recalibration occurs, people interpret that as the visual scene being closer than it looks, rather than specifically visual information about the hand. If that were the case, we would expect change in reach undershoot to be larger than proprioceptive recalibration, and more similar to total recalibration, which it was not. On the other hand, given the present study design of shifting cue forward for the Misaligned group, one may wonder if some part of visual target undershoot can be attributed to participant “laziness” or disinclination to point further away with their indicator hand. If so, it would inflate our estimate of total recalibration. If this were the case, in a similar study with two target positions 40 mm apart, we would expect greater undershoot at the further-away target position; however, we found that undershoot was nearly identical at the two target positions (36). Regardless, our primary conclusion that perceptual recalibration affects the (same or different) sensorimotor map used for reaching does not depend on the exact quantitative values of perceptual shift or reach shortening. Rather, it is based on the shortened distance of no-feedback reaches that developed in the Mismatch but not the Veridical group; reach shortening is the predicted direction of change whether caused by proprioceptive recalibration, visual recalibration, or both.
Reach Distance Reduction Was Transient
It is interesting to note that the reduction in reach distance was no longer detectable after only 10 reaches. Motor adaptation, in contrast, can be retained even after a year (37). Proprioceptive recalibration that results from motor adaptation can, itself, still be evident after 24 h (38, 39). There are several possible interpretations of this difference. First, more exposure to a visuo-proprioceptive mismatch might result in a longer-lasting reduction in reach distance; there were only 42 visuo-proprioceptive exposure trials in the present study, while motor adaptation studies frequently involve hundreds of trials. Second, perhaps the active movement stimulates proprioceptors in a way that overrides proprioceptive recalibration generated at a static position. This possibility could be considered consistent with RHI studies that found the illusion is reduced by active movement of the stimulated hand (12).
It is also possible that proprioceptive recalibration, induced with only a cue conflict and not motor adaptation, is itself transient, so effects on reach distance are also transient. However, evidence suggests that proprioceptive recalibration induced with a cue conflict is robustly retained 24 h later (36). In an experiment where visuo-proprioceptive recalibration was elicited similarly to the present study, short-term retention of proprioceptive recalibration was partially disrupted after 1 min of circle tracing (one circle per second) with the recalibrated finger and 4 min of rest; on average, proprioceptive recalibration was reduced 38% by this intervention (36). In the present study, if the transience of reach undershoot is due to proprioceptive recalibration being equally transient, it would mean that proprioceptive recalibration was 100% lost within 10 no-feedback reaches. Given the findings of Wali et al. (36), we consider it more likely that substantial proprioceptive recalibration remains even after reach undershoot has worn off. If indeed proprioceptive recalibration lasts longer than 10 reach trials, it is possible the effect on reach distance does not. This would be consistent with the involvement of multiple body representations. Proprioceptive recalibration might occur in the body image, which only briefly interacts with the body schema used to plan reaches. Indeed, different body representations have been associated with different dynamics and timescales (14). Of course, these possible interpretations are not mutually exclusive. Further studies are needed to better understand what factors influence the time course of visuo-proprioceptive recalibration effects on movement.
Implications for the Sensorimotor Map and Other Body Representations
Our prediction of shortened reach distance after visuo-proprioceptive recalibration was based on the idea that perception of body parts is closely linked to motor control and that reach planning would therefore take into account any changes in perceived hand position to maintain movement accuracy. Consistent with this idea, we have previously observed changes in the excitability of primary motor cortex (M1) that were specifically related to visuo-proprioceptive recalibration, after controlling for motor behavior (11). Furthermore, M1 changes were somatotopically focal, limited to the M1 representation of the finger that experienced visuo-proprioceptive cue conflict (10). Findings like these are suggestive of a close relationship between perception of hand position and motor execution involving that hand.
On the other hand, there is evidence to suggest that changes in perceived hand position may affect a body representation that functions separately from the body representation used to control movement. Paillard (13) refers to these representations as the body image and the body schema, respectively, based on a double dissociation observed in neuropsychological patients. In other words, some patients can correctly use information about their body positioning to move (intact body schema), but do not correctly perceive their body positioning (disrupted body image), while others have the opposite problem (13). This allows us to infer that there are at least two dissociable body representations, although some have suggested that body image should be further divided (12, 14).
The literature includes examples where a perceptual manipulation clearly does not affect motor performance (12), examples where it clearly does (8, 22), and also gray areas (15). Some of this literature uses the rubber hand illusion (RHI) in which synchronous stroking of a seen fake arm and the felt real arm creates the illusion of body ownership over the fake arm (15). Kammers et al. found that perceptual bodily judgments were sensitive to the RHI, but ballistic motor responses were not. The authors thus concluded that the illusion affected the body image, but not the body schema (12). However, while the RHI entails a visuo-proprioceptive discrepancy like the present study, there are also important differences to be considered. Our paradigm lacks synchronous tactile stimulation, and the visual stimulus is reduced to a disembodied white disk. The RHI is associated with “visual capture,” where the visual signal is so much stronger than the proprioceptive signal that most recalibration is likely proprioceptive rather than visual. In contrast, our paradigm is associated with a slightly stronger weight of proprioception compared with vision and greater visual recalibration than proprioceptive (40, 41). Indeed, Kammers et al. suggested that weighting of vision versus proprioception in multisensory integration could explain their results, with perceptual judgments relying heavily on vision and ballistic movements relying on proprioception.
Even within the RHI literature, there are gray areas in terms of which body representation appears affected. Kammers et al. noted that the classic RHI study by Botvinick and Cohen assessed illusion strength by asking participants to make pointing movements to where they perceived their other hand. The pointing movements were clearly sensitive to the illusion (15), unlike the ballistic movements used by Kammers et al. However, the pointing movements may have accessed a perceptual judgment in a way that ballistic movements do not (12).
On the other end of the spectrum, there is evidence that exposure to a visuo-proprioceptive mismatch in a cursor rotation task robustly affects reaching movement (8, 22). The cursor rotation paradigm is often used to elicit visuomotor adaptation: participants move to a visual target while the corresponding cursor deviates by some angular magnitude. This results in systematic movement errors, which are reduced by trial-and-error adaptation of the motor command. Proprioceptive recalibration also occurs, due to both sensory prediction errors and the cross-sensory mismatch created by the deviation of the cursor from true hand position (21). Salomonczyk et al. (8) modified the cursor rotation paradigm to remove the movement errors that could drive visuomotor adaptation; instead, participants moved their hand actively or passively along a set channel that was gradually deviated from the cursor, which always went straight to the target. After this exposure, participants made self-guided reaches with no cursor. These no-cursor reaches showed a directional change after exposure to the visuo-proprioceptive mismatch, and the change in reach direction was correlated with participants’ magnitude of proprioceptive recalibration (8). In fact, Tsay et al. (42) recently suggested that these types of results support the idea of implicit motor adaptation being driven by proprioceptive recalibration, not the other way around. We checked our Mismatch group for a correlation between proprioceptive recalibration and change in reach distance, as an exploratory analysis, but there was no association (r < 0.1). A sample of 16 is too small to definitively answer this question (43), but it would be valuable to examine such individual differences in a larger study.
Although a visuo-proprioceptive mismatch created in the context of cursor rotation seems to clearly alter the body schema, it is difficult to generalize such findings to visuo-proprioceptive recalibration in general, which does not require either active or passive movement or even a reaching target. The nature of a cursor rotation is that it exists in the context of target-directed movement; there is no visuo-proprioceptive mismatch at the home position, and the mismatch linearly increases as the person approaches the target. In addition, the mismatch is applied in body-centered coordinates; moving the hand to the right would result in a mismatch of the opposite direction in extrinsic space compared with moving the hand to the left. In contrast, the present study imposed the mismatch with the hand positioned in a static location and that hand made no movements toward a target while the mismatch was imposed.
In sum, the present paradigm has features not only in common with both the RHI and the cursor rotation task but also features that differ. Based on the above studies, we would suggest that our visuo-proprioceptive mismatch task affected primarily the body image, as participants were explicitly asked to indicate their perceived positions of static hand targets as accurately as possible without regard to speed (12, 15). The straight-ahead reaches presumably accessed the body schema, which makes it surprising that we observed a shortening of reaches. However, it should be noted that the body schema and body image likely interact with each other (14), making it challenging to draw more specific conclusions.
Conclusions
We predicted that straight-ahead reach distance would shorten after exposure to a forward displacement of visual cues from proprioceptive cues about the hand, which leads to proprioceptive recalibration in the forward direction. Results support this prediction, but the reduction of reach distance was transient. This is consistent with some degree of separation between body representations for perception and action.
DATA AVAILABILITY
Data and analysis code are publicly available at https://osf.io/zy49x/.
GRANTS
This study was supported by National Science Foundation grant 1753915 to H.J.B.
DISCLOSURES
No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
H.J.B. conceived and designed research; Y.L. performed experiments; H.J.B. and Y.L. analyzed data; H.J.B. and Y.L. interpreted results of experiments; H.J.B. and Y.L. prepared figures; Y.L. drafted manuscript; H.J.B. edited and revised manuscript; H.J.B. and Y.L. approved final version of manuscript.
REFERENCES
- 1. Ghahramani Z, Wolpert DM, Jordan MI. Computational models for sensorimotor integration. In: Self-Organization, Computational Maps and Motor Control, edited by Morasso PG, Sanguineti V.. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1997, p. 117–147. [Google Scholar]
- 2. van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, Denier van der Gon JJ. Localization of a seen finger is based exclusively on proprioception and on vision of the finger. Exp Brain Res 125: 43–49, 1999. doi: 10.1007/s002210050656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Ernst MO, Banks MS. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415: 429–433, 2002. doi: 10.1038/415429a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Limanowski J. Precision control for a flexible body representation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 134: 104401, 2022. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Limanowski J, Friston K. Attentional modulation of vision versus proprioception during action. Cereb Cortex 30: 1637–1648, 2020. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Smeets JBJ, van den Dobbelsteen JJ, de Grave DDJ, van Beers RJ, Brenner E. Sensory integration does not lead to sensory calibration. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103: 18781–18786, 2006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607687103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Hsiao A, Lee-Miller T, Block HJ. Conscious awareness of a visuo-proprioceptive mismatch: effect on cross-sensory recalibration. Front Neurosci 16: 958513, 2022. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.958513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Salomonczyk D, Cressman EK, Henriques DYP. The role of the cross-sensory error signal in visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 228: 313–325, 2013. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3564-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Welch RB. Perceptual Modification. New York: Academic Press, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Mirdamadi JL, Seigel CR, Husch SD, Block HJ. Somatotopic specificity of perceptual and neurophysiological changes associated with visuo-proprioceptive realignment. Cereb Cortex 32: 1184–1199, 2022. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Munoz-Rubke F, Mirdamadi JL, Lynch AK, Block HJ. Modality-specific changes in motor cortex excitability after visuo-proprioceptive realignment. J Cogn Neurosci 29: 2054–2067, 2017. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Kammers MPM, de Vignemont F, Verhagen L, Dijkerman HC. The rubber hand illusion in action. Neuropsychologia 47: 204–211, 2009. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Paillard J. Body schema and body image—a double dissociation in deafferented patients. In: Motor Control, Today and Tomorrow, edited by Gantchev GN, Mori S, Massion J.. Sofia, Bulgaria: Prof. M. Drinov Academic Publishing House, 1999. p. 197–214. [Google Scholar]
- 14. de Vignemont F. Body schema and body image–pros and cons. Neuropsychologia 48: 669–680, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Botvinick M, Cohen J. Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature 391: 756, 1998. doi: 10.1038/35784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Abdulkarim Z, Hayatou Z, Ehrsson HH. Sustained rubber hand illusion after the end of visuotactile stimulation with a similar time course for the reduction of subjective ownership and proprioceptive drift. Exp Brain Res 239: 3471–3486, 2021. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06211-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Abdulkarim Z, Ehrsson HH. Recalibration of hand position sense during unconscious active and passive movement. Exp Brain Res 236: 551–561, 2018. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5137-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Robertson EM, Miall RC. Visuomotor adaptation during inactivation of the dentate nucleus. Neuroreport 10: 1029–1034, 1999. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904060-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Seidler RD, Noll DC. Neuroanatomical correlates of motor acquisition and motor transfer. J Neurophysiol 99: 1836–1845, 2008. doi: 10.1152/jn.01187.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Ostry DJ, Gribble PL. Sensory plasticity in human motor learning. Trends Neurosci 39: 114–123, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Rossi C, Bastian AJ, Therrien AS. Mechanisms of proprioceptive realignment in human motor learning. Curr Opin Physiol 20: 186–197, 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.cophys.2021.01.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Cressman EK, Henriques DYP. Reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration following exposure to misaligned sensory input. J Neurophysiol 103: 1888–1895, 2010. doi: 10.1152/jn.01002.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Mostafa AA, 't Hart BM, Henriques DYP. Motor learning without moving: proprioceptive and predictive hand localization after passive visuoproprioceptive discrepancy training. PLoS One 14: e0221861, 2019. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Ruttle JE, 't Hart BM, Henriques DYP. The fast contribution of visual-proprioceptive discrepancy to reach aftereffects and proprioceptive recalibration. PLoS One 13: e0200621, 2018. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, Denier van der Gon JJ. The precision of proprioceptive position sense. Exp Brain Res 122: 367–377, 1998. doi: 10.1007/s002210050525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, Denier van der Gon JJ. How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information. Exp Brain Res 111: 253–261, 1996. doi: 10.1007/BF00227302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. van Beers RJ, Wolpert DM, Haggard P. When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation. Curr Biol 12: 834–837, 2002. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(02)00836-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Block H, Bastian A, Celnik P. Virtual lesion of angular gyrus disrupts the relationship between visuoproprioceptive weighting and realignment. J Cogn Neurosci 25: 636–648, 2013. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Block HJ, Bastian AJ. Sensory weighting and realignment: independent compensatory processes. J Neurophysiol 106: 59–70, 2011. doi: 10.1152/jn.00641.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Block HJ, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar involvement in motor but not sensory adaptation. Neuropsychologia 50: 1766–1775, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol 57: 289–300, 1995. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Vindras P, Desmurget M, Prablanc C, Viviani P. Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. J Neurophysiol 79: 3290–3294, 1998. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.6.3290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Vindras P, Desmurget M, Viviani P. Error parsing in visuomotor pointing reveals independent processing of amplitude and direction. J Neurophysiol 94: 1212–1224, 2005. doi: 10.1152/jn.01295.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Sober SJ, Sabes PN. Multisensory integration during motor planning. J Neurosci 23: 6982–6992, 2003. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-18-06982.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Sober SJ, Sabes PN. Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning. Nat Neurosci 8: 490–497, 2005. doi: 10.1038/nn1427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Wali M, Lee-Miller T, Babu R, Block HJ. Retention of visuo-proprioceptive recalibration in estimating hand position. Sci Rep 13: 6097, 2023. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-33290-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Yamamoto K, Hoffman DS, Strick PL. Rapid and long-lasting plasticity of input-output mapping. J Neurophysiol 96: 2797–2801, 2006. doi: 10.1152/jn.00209.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Maksimovic S, Cressman EK. Long-term retention of proprioceptive recalibration. Neuropsychologia 114: 65–76, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Nourouzpour N, Salomonczyk D, Cressman EK, Henriques DYP. Retention of proprioceptive recalibration following visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 233: 1019–1029, 2015. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4176-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Block HJ, Sexton BM. Visuo-proprioceptive control of the hand in older adults. Multisens Res 34: 93–111, 2020. doi: 10.1163/22134808-bja10032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Liu Y, Sexton BM, Block HJ. Spatial bias in estimating the position of visual and proprioceptive targets. J Neurophysiol 119: 1879–1888, 2018. doi: 10.1152/jn.00633.2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Tsay JS, Kim H, Haith AM, Ivry RB. Understanding implicit sensorimotor adaptation as a process of proprioceptive re-alignment. eLife 11: e76639, 2022. doi: 10.7554/eLife.76639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Makin TR, Orban de Xivry JJ. Ten common statistical mistakes to watch out for when writing or reviewing a manuscript. eLife 8: e48175, 2019. doi: 10.7554/eLife.48175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data and analysis code are publicly available at https://osf.io/zy49x/.