Summary.
A foundational pressure in the evolution of all animals is the ability to travel through the world, inherently coupling the sensory and motor systems. While this relationship has been explored in several species1–4, it has been largely overlooked in primates, which have typically relied on paradigms in which head-restrained subjects view stimuli on screens5. Natural visual behaviors, by contrast, are typified by locomotion through the environment guided by active sensing as animals explore and interact with the world4,6, a relationship well-illustrated by prey-capture7–12. Here, we characterized prey-capture in wild marmoset monkeys as they negotiated their dynamic, arboreal habitat to illustrate the inherent role of vision as an active process in natural nonhuman primate behavior. Not only do marmosets share the core properties of vision that typify the primate Order13–18, but they are prolific hunters that prey on a diverse set of prey animals19–22. Marmosets pursued prey using vision in several different contexts, but executed precise visually-guided motor control that predominantly involved grasping with hands for successful capture of prey. Applying markerless-tracking for the first time in wild primates yielded novel findings that precisely quantified how marmosets track insects prior to initiating an attack and the rapid visually-guided corrections of the hands during capture. These findings offer the first detailed insight into the active nature of vision to guide multiple facets of a natural goal-directed behavior in wild primates, and can inform future laboratory studies of natural primate visual behaviors and the supporting neural processes.
eToc
Ngo, Gorman and colleagues characterized the natural prey-capture behavior of wild marmoset monkeys to illustrate the integral role of active vision for effective hunting. Markerless-tracking analyses revealed how marmosets visually track insects and how visual feedback guides and adjusts rapid hand movements to capture flying insects.
Results and Discussion.
[1]. Prey-capture Strategies.
Here we analyzed 288 4K UHD videos of marmosets engaged in active prey-pursuit and capture of insects in northeastern Brazil to characterize the relationship between natural visual and positional behavior in a wild primate. Hunting behaviors were grouped into three of the more typical tactics distinguished by the sequence of visually-guided motor actions employed to capture different prey types in the dynamic forest environment19. These three strategies were largely determined by the prey itself, such as whether it was moving on a substrate or flying, or was stationary and relying on cryptic coloration, with variation within each strategy reflecting the nuanced interactions between prey-behavior and the immediate substrate idiosyncrasies. Although our analyses highlight hunting of insects, marmosets also actively hunt other prey (e.g., lizards, N = 27 videos).
Figure 1A illustrates ‘mouth capture’, the strategy employed by marmosets to track small, mobile prey with limited capture avoidance behaviors (e.g., ants, termites, etc., N = 80 Videos; Video S1) characterized by the overwhelming use of their mouth to capture insects than their hands (n = 73 observations; Mouth: 90.4%, Hands: 9.6%; p < 0.01, Fisher Exact Test). The prevalence of mouth use in this context contrasted with all other prey-capture strategies.
Figure 1. Marmoset Prey-Capture Strategies.
(A) ‘Mouth Capture’. The marmoset stands over the prey and captures multiple insects with their mouth. See also Videos S1. (B) ‘Stalk/Pause and Lunge’. The animal identifies the prey, gets into position and pauses before making a ballistic grasp. See also Videos S2. (C) ‘Capture in Flight’. The monkey identifies and tracks the prey before capturing it with a visually guided grasp. See also Videos S3.
Figure 1B depicts ‘stalk/pause and lunge’, the strategy employed by marmosets to capture stationary prey that often relied on camouflage to avoid capture (e.g., stick bugs, moths, grasshoppers, N = 93 videos; Video S2). Notably, insects that evolved mimicry increase the difficulty of their detection and are akin to a natural visual pop-out task to marmosets23,24. Some of these species, while stationary on a substrate, enact fast evasive behaviors when pursued by predators. Grasshoppers and dragonflies, for example, will rapidly jump or fly away if predators are detected, while stick bugs evade predation by holding their legs against their body and drop to the ground to hide among the leaves25. As a result, when hunting these prey, marmosets position themselves for the attack, sometimes slowly stalking the prey (Video S2). Once in position, marmosets typically pause for 1–5s before initiating a high-speed, ballistic grasp of the prey (Video S2).
Hunting flying insects is particularly challenging for marmosets with respect to both the demands on active vision and the role of sensory feedback to guide precise, high-speed hand movements necessary for successful capture (Figure 1C). In the recordings of marmosets pursuing flying beetles, their behavior typically abided by one of two strategies. It either involved a stationary monkey visually tracking the prey for a period of several seconds before initiating the ballistic grasp (N = 47 videos; Video S3), or the animal simultaneously visually tracking and physically following the prey through the arboreal substrate as the insect’s flight pattern changed (N = 68 videos; Video S3). Two-handed captures were more common for flying insects than one-handed or mouth captures (64.7%, N = 224 observations), likely because this tactic yielded a notably higher success rate (82.4%) than one-handed captures (x2 (1) = 9.5, p = 0.002).
[2]. Positional Behavior during Arboreal Prey Capture.
The stability and orientation of the substrate in which prey-pursuit occurred were significant factors that affected the variability of prey capture. Marmosets countered these challenges with creative changes in positional behavior. The use of adaptive positioning with multiple limbs on horizontal branches, including reaching both above (Figure 2A) and below (Figure 2B) the branch, and vertical branches that, likewise, involved pursuing prey above (Figure 2C) and below (Figure 2D) the substrate. Notably, one shared similarity across all positional behaviors during prey capture was optimizing visual access of the target while balancing the need for stability, sometimes extending their body using only their hind limbs as support at a range of angles from the substrate, often on small unstable branches. We quantified these positional behaviors by measuring the monkey’s angle of attack and the extent to which individuals extended their body in the attack (Figure S1). The average percent body length extension during prey capture for marmosets on top of the substrate was 54.3% ± 32.0% (mean ± SD), while body extension when clinging under the substrate was 65.8% ± 36.3% (mean ± SD). Marmosets hunting on a vertical substrate with the normal vector of the head pointing upwards extended their body 57% ± 29.1% (mean ± SD), while marmosets hunting on a vertical substrate with the normal vector of the head pointing downwards extended their body 51.1% ± 39.7% (mean ± SD) (Figure 2E). When marmosets are under a horizontal substrate, they will typically extend downwards 95% of the time (n = 20/21, N = 156 observations) as they are already hanging from the branch with their lower limbs, whereas when they are on top of the horizontal substrate, they will extend downwards only about 12% of the time (n = 9/73, N = 156 observations). These results illustrate how dynamic biomechanical movements are integral for active vision and successful prey capture in marmosets as these changes in positional behavior effectively function to optimize the view of the prey in a complex environment.
Figure 2. Positional Behavior Preferences Based on Substrate Orientation.
(A-D) Illustrations depicting the range of positional behaviors during hunting (reaching above or below) while the marmoset is situated (A) on horizontal substrate, (B) under horizontal substrate, (C) on vertical substrate with the normal vector of the marmoset’s head pointing up, and (D) on vertical substrate with the normal vector of the marmoset’s head is pointing down. (E) The length of each vector on the polar plots (shown in black, red, magenta, and blue) corresponds to the percent change in body length of extension calculated in pixel units. The polar plots also portray the marmoset’s angle of extension when reaching for prey depending on the substrate being grasped. Vectors pointing below the horizontal axis refer to marmosets reaching downwards while hunting. Horizontal substrates are shown to the left and vertical substrates to the right. See also Figure S1.
[3]. Gaze-Tracking of Flying Insects.
To precisely quantify different facets of visual behaviors during prey-capture in wild marmosets, we next applied the markerless tracking technology SLEAP26,27 to a subset of videos that met a set of criteria related to video recording quality and modeling accuracy (See Methods and Figure S2). These analyses focused on hunts of flying insects because this context highlights the unique challenge of successfully capturing a moving insect in three-dimensional space and the role of vision as an active process for a goal-directed, high-precision motor action. We distinguished between two phases of the marmoset hunt based on the visual challenges. In the first phase, marmosets visually identify and track the prey prior to attacking. During the second phase, marmosets initiate the ballistic grasp based on the anticipated location of the prey.
We first analyzed head movements in the final seconds prior to the initiation of the ballistic grasp as a proxy for gaze-tracking, including how they covaried with the flight path of the insect prey in a subset of 5 videos that met our criteria. Frame grabs from an exemplar video show three time points over 500ms immediately before the ballistic grasp is initiated and highlight the close relationship between head and insect movements that occur during this time period (Figure 3A). Correlation between insect and head movements was largely limited to the final period - 1.5s - before initiating capture. Figure 3B shows the change in XY coordinates for the head and insect movements in the same exemplar video over a longer period of time. This observation was also true for all videos analyzed. The Pearson correlation coefficient between head and insect movements remained between 0.5 – 1 (N = 5 observations) for the entirety of this time period (Figure 3C). One other notable result from this analysis was the prodigious increase in head velocity over the 500ms prior to initiating the capture action (Figure 3D). This likely suggests that marmosets were closely tracking the flight path, with vision providing important information about the speed and motion direction of the prey, and potentially reflecting a change in the attentional demands necessary to accurately capture the flying beetle. This analysis may also suggest that marmosets have a preference for a particular type of insect flight behavior when initiating an attack, when the insects are moving along a continuous path rather than at times when the path was less predictable.
Figure 3. Gaze tracking flying insects during prey-pursuit.
Gaze-tracking was characterized by the relationship between marmoset head and insect movements prior to initiating capture using SLEAP for accurate annotations. See also Figure S2. (A) Three single frames from the final 500ms before prey capture illustrate coordinated movements between the marmoset head and insect flight trajectory. (B) Raw XY positional data of both the marmoset’s head movement (bottom right) and insect’s flight pattern (top left) from the example screen shown in A over the 2s prior to initiating the ballistic grasp for prey capture. (C) Pearson’s correlation coefficient between the marmoset head and insect flight in a sliding window over the 2.5s before prey capture was initiated. Zero (0) indicates the onset of the ballistic grasp.(D) Average velocity (cm/s) of the marmoset head over the same time period as in C. 95% Confidence Intervals are shown in shading for C and D.
[4]. Visually-Guided Prey-Capture.
Because the position of flying insects is constantly changing, successful capture relies on a complement of two overlapping visually-guided processes. First, marmosets must anticipate the likely position of the insect in three-dimensional space. Second, as insects frequently change their flight pattern, marmosets need to make quick adjustments in response to changes in insect trajectory after the ballistic grasp is initiated. Notably, these elements of the behavior may be unique to primates, as they involve visual control of coordinated hand movements and adjustment of hand shape to effectively grasp prey (or other objects) in three-dimensional space28. To quantify the second phase of the hunt, visually-guided prey-capture, we again applied SLEAP to a subset of 15 videos that met our criteria (See Methods) Figure 4A depicts a parallel series of frame grabs from an exemplar video and the respective XY coordinates of the hand movements when a marmoset reaches for and grasps a flying insect that illustrate how the respective position of the hands and insect change over this time. Importantly, the hands did not follow the optimal trajectory in any of the videos (Figure 4B), and diverged in several quantifiable ways. First, the hand movements had several inflections reflecting the changes in trajectory during the motor action (Avg = 2.4, Range = 0 – 7; Figure 4C). Second, the average tortuosity for the hand movement trajectory was 3.08, which significantly diverged from the optimal path (t(24) = 5.31, p < 0.0001; Figure 4D). These corrections in hand trajectory are occurring at high speeds, as the latency to peak velocity occurred at an average of 224.44ms (Figure 4E), while the mean duration of the entire ballistic grasp during capture was 375.61ms (Figure 4F, N = 123 observations). Our analyses indicate that marmosets made real-time visually-guided corrections to the path of the hand trajectory during the short interval of time from the initiation of the motor action until the insect was captured, likely due to changes in the prey’s flight path or potentially other insects in the visual field. The trajectories of the left and right hands were not statistically different (t = −0.56, p = 0.589). This finding shows that, like other primates29–31, wild marmoset hand movements are under continuous visual control when targeting the capture of flying beetles, and can be modified through feedback in response to the exact types of natural challenges the visual system evolved mechanisms to overcome.
Figure 4. Visually-guided prey-capture.
(A) Left plots four continuous time periods during prey capture from an exemplar video and the XY coordinates of the marmoset’s right (red line) and left (yellow line) hands and insect (green line), right plots only the XY coordinates. The dark lines show the change in movement during the time interval plotted over that specific period of time, while lightly shaded lines indicate the preceding time periods. (B) Summary of A shows the path of both hands and the insect, as well as the optimal trajectory in blue for each hand. (C) Number of inflection points during reaches from all videos analyzed. (D) Plots the tortuosity index for all prey-capture reaches. (E) Latency to peak velocity (ms) during ballistic grasps for flying prey. (F) Duration of ballistic movement (ms) from initiation of the motor action to successful grasp of the prey. See also Figure S2.
Here we demonstrate that active vision is integral to successful prey-capture in wild marmosets. Far from being a passive process, vision is closely coupled to all elements of prey capture in marmosets, including creative biomechanical positioning on often precarious substrates, similarly to other species7–11. A compelling advantage of this natural behavior is that it comprises of visual processes studied independently in primate vision for decades - discrimination, recognition, motor-planning, decision-making, and visually-guided selection, amongst others - within a single, cohesive visual behavior 35. These visual processes are in fact not separable, but operations within an integrated behavioral sequence that is representative of the distinct challenges that together have driven the evolution of the complementary mechanisms in the primate visual system, and in particular precise visually-guided reaching and control of hand movements and articulation that may be unique to our Order. While marmoset hunting strategies were largely determined by the behavior of the prey type, in contrast to many classic neuroethological behaviors, prey capture itself was far from stereotyped in these monkeys. Rather, the general hunting strategies were highly variable, likely reflecting the need to be adaptable to the immediate environment to find creative solutions for successful predation. The behavioral variability that emerges due to an interaction between a presumably ideal strategy for obtaining prey and the specific ecological challenges underscores the advantages of primate prey-capture to elucidate the likely supporting perceptual and cognitive processing, including decision-making and cognitive control, under natural variable conditions and can be used as principles that guide future experimental work on these key issues. As not all theoretical models of vision developed in more traditional laboratory settings are likely reflective of how this system functions under real-world scenarios32, natural primate behaviors such as prey-capture will likely be necessary to elucidate remaining key questions about primate brain function.
STAR Methods
RESOURCE AVAILABILITY
Further information and requests for resources should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Cory Miller (corymiller@ucsd.edu)
Materials Availability.
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Data and Code Availability.
All data and original code have been deposited in a Dryad repository (https://doi.org/10.6076/D1P88) and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOI is listed in the key resources table.
Key resources table
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Antibodies | ||
Bacterial and virus strains | ||
Biological samples | ||
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
Critical commercial assays | ||
Deposited data | ||
Data and Software | Dryad | https://doi.org/10.6076/D1P88 |
Experimental models: Cell lines | ||
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
Oligonucleotides | ||
Recombinant DNA | ||
Software and algorithms | ||
Other | ||
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
Study Site and Subjects.
High-resolution videos were recorded of wild marmosets inhabiting the semiarid scrub-forests in the Baracuhy Biological Field Station in Northeast Brazil (7°31’42”S, 36°17’50”W)33,34. Data collection was conducted between March 2020 and June 2021 following two social groups- House Group (mean group size of 9 animals) and Coqueiro Group (mean group size of 8 animals).
METHOD DETAILS
Data Analysis.
Analyses were performed on the 288 Ultra HD video recordings (4K; width: 3840 pixels; height: 2160 pixels) of marmoset prey-capture on the Sony FDR AX-53 camcorder at a frame rate of 29.970 fps. Videos may contain one or more observations of a hunting strategy by one or more subjects being filmed. 287 videos that were not reflected in the final analyses exhibit marmosets foraging for food by leaf manipulation or opportunistic prey capture19. Videos recorded in the field were uploaded in Brazil onto a shared server and cataloged at UCSD.
Positional behavior during arboreal prey capture.
Quantitative analysis shown in Figure 2 was performed using Adobe Premiere Pro version 22.0 and Adobe Photoshop version 22.5.1. Premiere was used to quantify the duration of a subject’s hunting action frame by frame up to hundredths of a second, including the pause before a ballistic movement, and the start to completion of a ballistic movement based on the type of prey the marmoset went after. Using Premiere, the start frame of the ballistic movement marks when the marmoset’s body is at its original position. The end frame of the ballistic movement reflects when the marmoset is most fully extended, reaching out towards its prey. Frames depicting a side profile of the moment before and after a ballistic movement was achieved were exported into Adobe Photoshop for further examination.
Since body lengths of animals in the wild were not possible, the ruler tool in Adobe Photoshop was used to obtain measurements in pixels including the marmoset’s body length pre- and post-ballistic movement in exported frames, allowing us to attain the percent length of extension. For the pre-ballistic measurement of the marmoset’s body, it was measured from the base of the tail or foot to the top of their forehead or mouth, depending on the visibility of each body part in the frame. The post-ballistic measurement was measured from either the same tail base or foot—if visible and in instances where they are using their legs to extend—to the extended body part being used to grab (either the extended hand(s) or mouth). Additionally, the ruler tool was used to estimate the angle of extension from the substrate a subject opts for depending on how they are oriented on that substrate. The angle was measured from the parallel substrate, to the base of where the marmoset is clinging onto, and to the extended arms. Owing to the limitations of a 2D video, videos demonstrating body extension along the Z plane were not taken into consideration for measuring to minimize the chance of error.
The polar plot in Figure 2E was created using a custom MATLAB script to demonstrate the marmoset’s angle of extension depending on the substrate they are hunting on (horizontal or vertical) and the length of the arrow depicts a visual representation of the percent body length of extension in pixels. The “-” sign refers to the downward direction the marmoset is reaching in (Figure 2E). As a way to classify differences in the marmoset’s orientation relative to the vertical substrate, we used the direction of the normal vector extending from the top of their head, as this was the most visible point of reference in the frames, to define if the marmosets were extending upright (normal vector points up) or “supine” (normal vector points down). To obtain the normalized percent change in length of extension, the following formula was used:
Since only one camera angle was often available for analysis, the ability to measure velocity during a capture was not feasible. Instead, using the aforementioned formula, we examined the normalized percent change in the marmoset’s body length over the duration of the ballistic movement during a hunting sequence to capture their change in motion.
Markerless tracking analyses.
To more precisely quantify the marmoset’s visuomotor integration that occurs when hunting, we employed computer vision technology for markerless tracking on one dynamic hunting strategy involving a one- or two-handed capture mid-air following rapid head-gaze tracking behavior of flying beetles (Coleoptera). Down-sampled videos (1280 × 720) were uploaded onto SLEAP (Social LEAP Estimates Animal Poses) 26,27 where deep learning was employed for markerless tracking of the marmoset body, whereas the flying insect was hand annotated either on a separate project or following the inference process. The right hand, left hand, middle top of head, left ear tuft, and right ear tuft of the marmoset in the video were manually annotated in a small percentage (~15%) of frames. Then, active learning takes place as we train a neural network through the single animal pipeline to estimate positions of the marmoset’s body parts by running inference until satisfied with the accuracy of the network’s predictions. A custom MATLAB script was used to verify the accuracy of the computer’s predictions by comparing the average distance between the hand labels to the predicted labels (Figure S2). While pose estimation was computed for the entire video clip (typically ~15s) to improve the robustness and accuracy of the model, analyses presented here focused on the few seconds before and during insect capture. These select frames were further refined for analysis by additional hand annotations. H5 files were then exported for use in additional analyses. The frame numbers for start and end behaviors, such as gaze tracking and ballistic movement, were hand marked and verified by two other people in the Miller Lab.
Gaze-tracking flying beetles.
Analysis was performed on videos that met the following criteria: (1) Video maintained stable, continuous focus of both the insect and the animals head for at least 2 seconds prior to initiating capture, (2) the animal was not chasing the insect during this period or otherwise locomoting, but remained sitting on the substrate and (3) there were no obstructions of either the insect or marmoset that would affect annotation of the video. Five videos met these strict criteria and were used in the analysis typically due to the flying insect not remaining in focus for sufficient periods of time during the video. The frame number of both the start of the marmoset gaze and the end of the gaze/start of the ballistic movement was marked in each video. The start of the gaze was defined as when the marmoset’s head movement stops scanning and the head moves in closer before the body starts to move in the same direction. The end of the gaze period is defined as when the marmoset’s head movement shifts away from the targeted prey, usually to redirect its attention to the route they are trying to take as they move closer or to check if there are other competitors around. The gaze can then resume when the head movement turns back to the direction of where the target is/where the target is moving. Videos were processed in Python 3.6 and the aforementioned time periods mentioned during the gaze period were analyzed (Figure 3A). Gaze estimates in 3B were made between the SLEAP label that demarcated the middle of the head and the SLEAP bug label. A line was drawn from the two to show an approximate gaze line. In 3C, velocity of both the head SLEAP labels and the insect’s SLEAP label were calculated. A rolling correlation was taken to quantify the relationship between the head and the bug in the final second. The Python pandas package outputs the Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient in a sliding bin window with a bin size of 10. In order to characterize the period of high correlation before the bug catch, a rolling average of bin size 10 was taken and any correlation coefficient above 0.5 was considered significant. A 95% confidence interval was calculated using Python in order to show the variability of the data in both 3C and 3D.
Visually-guided prey capture.
These analyses were performed on videos that maintained stable, continuous focus and/or accurate model prediction of both the insect and at least one of the animal’s hands for the duration of time from the initiation of the ballistic grasp till prey capture. The frame number of both the start and end of the marmoset’s ballistic movement was marked in each video. Start of ballistic grasp was defined as when the marmoset lifts its hand(s) from the branch or hand(s) move away from the body towards the target without stopping until the target is caught. End of ballistic grasp was defined as when the target prey is caught. Analyses were performed on 15 videos that met these criteria and had the least occlusion by vegetation. Lines were drawn from the start of the ballistic movement to the point of prey capture to demonstrate the most direct path - the optimal trajectory - of a reach if the insect’s flight path was perfectly predicted. We next applied two different measurements to quantify how much marmoset hand trajectories deviated from the optimal trajectory: ‘Number of Inflection points’ and ‘Tortuosity’. The left and right hands were analyzed separately in each video because both hands were not always clearly visible. This yielded 15 left hand events and 9 right hand events from the videos used in analyses here.
Inflection points. This analysis identified the number of instances that marmosets modified the direction of their hand movement during the ballistic grasp leading to prey capture. This measure was taken by first calculating the distances from the ideal line and then calculating the number of local maxima and minima. Values were validated by marking the point at which a local max and min were found.
Tortuosity. - To better quantify the changes and curvature of the arm reaches during prey capture of flying insects, we measured the tortuosity of the reach trajectory. Tortuosity was calculated to show the ratio between the distance of the optimal trajectory or total length (L) and the actual movement of the hand or the path length (C). This ratio was then . In order to test for significance, a paired t-test was performed against the null hypothesis of 1, since 1 would be the value of if .
Latency to peak velocity. We calculated the velocity of the hand over the course of the ballistic action to determine the time at which the velocity was at its maximum.
To calculate the duration of ballistic reaches to capture flying insects for Figure 4E, we lowered our selection criteria to all videos in which the body was visible (N = 123 observations). The frame numbers were denoted for the duration of the ballistic movement and the average was calculated.
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Statistical Analysis.
Statistical analyses of SLEAP output were performed using Python. Excel (Microsoft Corporation) was used to conduct all other statistical analyses, including mean values and standard deviation. We used the Fisher Exact Test with probability mass function to compare methods of capture on small, mobile prey. To examine whether there was a preference for capturing small, flying beetles using one or two hands, a X2 test was applied. Statistical significance was set to P < 0.05 (two tailed) for all analyses. Statistical analysis of figure 4E was a two sided t-test performed in Python.
DATA AND SOFTWARE AVAILABILITY
All data and original code have been deposited in a Dryad repository (https://doi.org/10.6076/D1P88) and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOI is listed in the key resources table.
Supplementary Material
Video S1. Exemplar videos of ‘Mouth Capture’ strategy. Related to Figure 1A. Clip 1 shows an adult female marmoset from the House Group follows a scattered trail of ants along the tree trunk/branches and uses her mouth as the primary approach of hunting. Clip 2 shows an adult female marmoset belonging to the House Group tracks ants moving along the tree trunk and uses ‘mouth capture’. Clip 3 shows an adult female marmoset from the House Group searches for ants traveling along multiple branches and continues to move closer to grab with mainly her mouth.
Video S2. Exemplar videos of ‘Stalk/Pause and Lunge’ strategy. Related to Figure 1B. Clip 1 shows an adult male from the Coqueiro Group slowly drags his body while climbing up a tree 6 meters high. He pauses for 1s as his hands slowly synchronize upwards before a high-speed ballistic lunge is achieved to grab the stick bug that is camouflaging with the surrounding environment. Clip 2 shows an adult female belonging to the Coquiero Group quietly stalking a stick bug resting above and moves closer until she is within arm’s reach of quickly seizing the prey using both hands. Clip 3 shows an adult female marmoset from the Coquiero Group carefully stalking a dragonfly by dragging her body along the tree branch. She seldom looks away from her target as she approaches. She cocks her head to the side, displaying a putative example of visual parallax, and pauses for 1.25s before initiating an upper body lunge with both hands.
Video S3. Exemplar videos of ‘Capture in Flight’ strategy. Related to Figure 1C. Clip 1 shows an adult male marmoset from the House Group surrounded by several flying beetles while in a stationary position ~4m from the forest floor. His gaze shifts often as he visually tracks multiple beetles that fly into his field of view and attempts several times to capture them using one or two hands. Clip 2 shows an adult male marmoset from the House Group sitting at the very top of the tree 4m high on thin branches. His gaze follows the trajectory of a flying beetle for several seconds, then his gaze breaks from the beetle momentarily, and finally resumes back on the beetle. As the beetle flies within his reach, he strikes downwards using both hands. Juvenile marmoset from the same group also spots a stationary beetle and pauses for a few seconds before capturing it with both hands. Clip 3 shows an adult female from the House Group moving through several thin branches while her gaze is fixated on the flying beetle. Her gaze breaks from her prey as she looks away and back several times to determine her pathway in the canopy before returning to her flying target. She uses both hands to reach out to successfully capture the prey. Clip 4 shows an adult female from the House Group following a flying beetle, looking away and back to the prey, as she advances nonstop through the environment. However, she misses with a two-handed attempt. She quickly spots another flying insect and continues to move downward, extending upside down to quickly capture using both hands.
Highlights.
Marmosets employ active visual strategies for effective prey capture in the wild.
Flexible biomechanical movements are used to maximize the visual field.
Visual behavior flexibly adapted to the challenges of the environment.
Visually-guided feedback of the hands is needed to capture flying prey in 3D.
Acknowledgements.
We thank Drs. T. Perreira, A. Fishbein, D. Grijseels, and A. Lefevre for comments, Dr. G. Baracuhy for permission to conduct research at Baracuhy Biological Field Station and Sarah Mientka for the marmoset illustrations. This work was supported by the NIH (UF1 NS116377) and AFOSR (19RT0316) to CTM.
Footnotes
Declaration of Interests.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References.
- 1.Kleinfeld D, Ahissar E, and Diamond ME (2006). Active sensation: insights from the rodent vibrissa sensorimotor system. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 16, 435–444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Parker PRL, Brown MA, Smear MC, and Niell CM (2020). Movement-Related Signals in Sensory Areas: Roles in Natural Behavior. Trends Neurosci. 43, 581–595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wachowiak M. (2011). All in a Sniff: Olfaction as a Model for Active Sensing. Neuron 71, 962–973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gibson EJ (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception (Houghton Mifflin; ). [Google Scholar]
- 5.Leopold DA, and Park SH (2020). Studying the visual brain in its natural rhythm. Neuroimage 216, 116790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Schroeder CE, Wilson DA, Radman T, Scharfman H, and Lakatos P. (2010). Dynamics of Active Sensing and perceptual selection. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 20, 172–176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Palleroni A., Miller CT., Hauser M., and Marler P. (2005). Predation: Prey plumage adaptation against falcon attack. Nature 434, 973–974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lin H-T, and Leonardo A. (2017). Heuristic Rules Underlying Dragonfly Prey Selection and Interception. Curr. Biol 27, 1124–1137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mischiati M, Lin H-T, Herold P, Imler E, Olberg R, and Leonardo A. (2015). Internal models direct dragonfly interception steering. Nature 517, 333–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Michaiel AM, Abe ET, and Niell CM (2020). Dynamics of gaze control during prey capture in freely moving mice. Elife 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wagner H, Kettler L, Orlowski J, and Tellers P. (2013). Neuroethology of prey capture in the barn owl (Tyto alba L.). J. Physiol. Paris 107, 51–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hoy JL, Yavorska I, Wehr M, and Niell CM (2016). Vision Drives Accurate Approach Behavior during Prey Capture in Laboratory Mice. Curr. Biol 26, 3046–3052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Mitchell JF, Reynolds JH, and Miller CT (2014). Active vision in marmosets: a model system for visual neuroscience. J. Neurosci 34, 1183–1194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Mitchell JF, and Leopold DA (2015). The marmoset monkey as a model for visual neuroscience. Neurosci. Res 93, 20–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mitchell JF, Priebe NJ, and Miller CT (2015). Motion dependence of smooth pursuit eye movements in the marmoset. J. Neurophysiol 113, 3954–3960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Coop SH, Yates JL, and Mitchell JF (2021). Foveal remapping of motion in area MT of the marmoset monkey. J. Vis 21, 2638–2638. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hori Y, Cléry JC, Selvanayagam J, Schaeffer DJ, Johnston KD, Menon RS, and Everling S. (2021). Interspecies activation correlations reveal functional correspondences between marmoset and human brain areas. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hung CC, Yen CC, Ciuchta JL, Papoti D, Bock NA, Leopold DA, and Silva AC (2015). Functional mapping of face-selective regions in the extrastriate visual cortex of the marmoset. J. Neurosci 35, 1160–1172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Schiel N, Souto A, Huber L, and Bezerra BM (2010). Hunting strategies in wild common marmosets are prey and age dependent. Am. J. Primatol 71, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Schiel N, and Souto A. (2017). The common marmoset: An overview of its natural history, ecology and behavior. Dev. Neurobiol 77, 244–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Digby L, and Barreto CE (1998). Vertebrate predation in common marmosets. Neotrop. Primates 6, 124–126. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Souto A, Bezerra BM, Schiel N, and Huber L. (2007). Saltatory search in free-living Callithrix jacchus: Environmental and Age influences. Int. J. Primatol 28, 881–893. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bichot NP, and Schall JD (1999). Saccade target selection in macaque during feature and conjunction visual search. Vis. Neurosci 16, 81–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wang Q, Cavanagh P, and Green M. (1994). Familiarity and pop-out in visual search. Percept. Psychophys 56, 495–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Markle S. (2008). Stick Insects: Masters of Defense (Lerner Publications; ). [Google Scholar]
- 26.Pereira TD., Aldarondo DE., Willmore L., Kislin M., Wang SSH., Murthy M., and Shaevitz JW. (2019). Fast animal pose estimation using deep neural networks. Nat. Methods 16, 117–125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Pereira TD, Shaevitz JW, and Murthy M. (2020). Quantifying behavior to understand the brain. Nat. Neurosci 23, 1537–1549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cartmill M. (2005). New views on primate origins. Evol. Anthropol 1, 105–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Oostwoud Wijdenes L, Brenner E, and Smeets JBJ (2011). Fast and fine-tuned corrections when the target of a hand movement is displaced. Exp. Brain Res 214, 453–462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Goodale MA, Pelisson D, and Prablanc C. (1986). Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature 320, 748–750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Song J-H, Takahashi N, and McPeek RM (2008). Target selection for visually guided reaching in macaque. J. Neurophysiol 99, 14–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Matthis JS, Muller KS, Bonnen KL, and Hayhoe MM (2022). Retinal optic flow during natural locomotion. PLoS Comput. Biol 18, e1009575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.De la Fuente MF, Sueur C, Garber PA, Bicca-Marques JC, Souto A, and Schiel N. (2022). Foraging networks and social tolerance in a cooperatively breeding primate (Callithrix jacchus). J. Anim. Ecol 91, 138–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Caselli C, Ayres PHB, Castro S, Souto A, Schiel N, and Miller CT (2018). The role of extra-group encounters in a Neotropical cooperative breeding primate, common marmosets: Field playback experiments. Anim. Behav 136, 137–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Miller CT.; Gire D; Hoke K; Huk A; Kelley D; Leopold D; Smear M; Theunnisen F; Yartsev M; and Niell C (2022) The Language of the Brain is Natural Behavior. Current Biology 32, R482–R493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Video S1. Exemplar videos of ‘Mouth Capture’ strategy. Related to Figure 1A. Clip 1 shows an adult female marmoset from the House Group follows a scattered trail of ants along the tree trunk/branches and uses her mouth as the primary approach of hunting. Clip 2 shows an adult female marmoset belonging to the House Group tracks ants moving along the tree trunk and uses ‘mouth capture’. Clip 3 shows an adult female marmoset from the House Group searches for ants traveling along multiple branches and continues to move closer to grab with mainly her mouth.
Video S2. Exemplar videos of ‘Stalk/Pause and Lunge’ strategy. Related to Figure 1B. Clip 1 shows an adult male from the Coqueiro Group slowly drags his body while climbing up a tree 6 meters high. He pauses for 1s as his hands slowly synchronize upwards before a high-speed ballistic lunge is achieved to grab the stick bug that is camouflaging with the surrounding environment. Clip 2 shows an adult female belonging to the Coquiero Group quietly stalking a stick bug resting above and moves closer until she is within arm’s reach of quickly seizing the prey using both hands. Clip 3 shows an adult female marmoset from the Coquiero Group carefully stalking a dragonfly by dragging her body along the tree branch. She seldom looks away from her target as she approaches. She cocks her head to the side, displaying a putative example of visual parallax, and pauses for 1.25s before initiating an upper body lunge with both hands.
Video S3. Exemplar videos of ‘Capture in Flight’ strategy. Related to Figure 1C. Clip 1 shows an adult male marmoset from the House Group surrounded by several flying beetles while in a stationary position ~4m from the forest floor. His gaze shifts often as he visually tracks multiple beetles that fly into his field of view and attempts several times to capture them using one or two hands. Clip 2 shows an adult male marmoset from the House Group sitting at the very top of the tree 4m high on thin branches. His gaze follows the trajectory of a flying beetle for several seconds, then his gaze breaks from the beetle momentarily, and finally resumes back on the beetle. As the beetle flies within his reach, he strikes downwards using both hands. Juvenile marmoset from the same group also spots a stationary beetle and pauses for a few seconds before capturing it with both hands. Clip 3 shows an adult female from the House Group moving through several thin branches while her gaze is fixated on the flying beetle. Her gaze breaks from her prey as she looks away and back several times to determine her pathway in the canopy before returning to her flying target. She uses both hands to reach out to successfully capture the prey. Clip 4 shows an adult female from the House Group following a flying beetle, looking away and back to the prey, as she advances nonstop through the environment. However, she misses with a two-handed attempt. She quickly spots another flying insect and continues to move downward, extending upside down to quickly capture using both hands.
Data Availability Statement
All data and original code have been deposited in a Dryad repository (https://doi.org/10.6076/D1P88) and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOI is listed in the key resources table.
Key resources table
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Antibodies | ||
Bacterial and virus strains | ||
Biological samples | ||
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
Critical commercial assays | ||
Deposited data | ||
Data and Software | Dryad | https://doi.org/10.6076/D1P88 |
Experimental models: Cell lines | ||
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
Oligonucleotides | ||
Recombinant DNA | ||
Software and algorithms | ||
Other | ||
All data and original code have been deposited in a Dryad repository (https://doi.org/10.6076/D1P88) and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOI is listed in the key resources table.