Abstract
The primary somatosensory cortex (S1) is a hub for body sensation of both innocuous and noxious signals, yet its role in somatosensation versus pain is debated. Despite known contributions of S1 to sensory gain modulation, its causal involvement in subjective sensory experiences remains elusive. Here, in mouse S1, we reveal the involvement of cortical output neurons in layers 5 (L5) and 6 (L6) in the perception of innocuous and noxious somatosensory signals. We find that L6 activation can drive aversive hypersensitivity and spontaneous nocifensive behavior. Linking behavior to neuronal mechanisms, we find that L6 enhances thalamic somatosensory responses, and in parallel, strongly suppresses L5 neurons. Directly suppressing L5 reproduced the pronociceptive phenotype induced by L6 activation, suggesting an anti-nociceptive function for L5 output. Indeed, L5 activation reduced sensory sensitivity and reversed inflammatory allodynia. Together, these findings reveal a layer-specific and bidirectional role for S1 in modulating subjective sensory experiences.
Subject terms: Neural circuits, Thalamus, Cortex, Translational research
How the brain controls pain perception remains elusive. Here, authors show that layers 5 and 6 of the somatosensory cortex suppress or enhance nociception through cell-type-specific cortical and corticothalamic interactions.
Introduction
Flexible modulation of sensory gain is a canonical thalamocortical circuit function, serving to sharpen sensory information streams en route to the cortex1–3 and thereby adaptively drive perception and behavior4. Cortical output pathways from layers 5 and 6 (L5 and L6) are in key positions to control sensory gain via their influence on diverse subcortical structures5,6. The emerging consensus is that L6 corticothalamic (L6-CT) neurons of primary sensory cortices control sensory gain by both recruiting cortical inhibitory neurons7–9 and modulating multiple cellular and circuit mechanisms in the thalamus1,10–14. These L6-CT functions have been evaluated from the standpoint of gain control as beneficial to sensory processing, attention, and perception1,10,11,13,15. However, such powerful sensitivity tuning could give rise to a maladaptive mismatch between cortical gain and the sensory environment, for example, by promoting hypersensitivity to peripheral sensory stimuli. Such a potentially detrimental facet of L6-CT gain modulation could be particularly pertinent to sensory circuits involved in pain, as these circuits link sensory and affective functions to generate both healthy and pathological pain perceptions16–18.
Primary somatosensory cortex (S1) receives both innocuous and noxious signals via thalamocortical (TC) inputs from the ventral posterolateral (VPL) thalamus, and is, therefore, a primary junction at which body sensations could be amplified into pain or, conversely, painful signals could be suppressed17,19. In turn, S1’s output is broadcast to thalamic nuclei via the L6-CT pathway and to several subcortical targets via the L5 pathway6,20. Thus, S1 could potentially modulate ascending pain signals at various subcortical points, including VPL, which is tightly linked to different pain states21–28. Indeed, tantalizing yet heterogeneous reports that broad manipulations of S1 alter sensory and affective pain responses29–33 have challenged the view that S1 generates strictly sensory-discriminative aspects of pain16,34–37. However, the prevalent use of bulk manipulations and lesions of S138,39 has led to contrasting views on the role of S1 in pain processing40,41. Therefore, dissecting the cell-type-specific contributions of distinct S1 output layers to pain perception is required to disambiguate potential opposing effects which may underlie these controversies.
We hypothesized a link between S1 gain modulation and the subjective dimensions of sensory experiences, which may contribute to the generation of painful sensations. To test this hypothesis, we probed how L6-CT neurons in S1 modulate sensitivity to innocuous and noxious stimuli in mice, using sensory and affective behavioral read-outs of pain, targeted optogenetics, and electrophysiology in the VPL-S1 TC system.
Here, we report that L6-CT—but not L5—activation triggers mechanical hypersensitivity and negative affect (i.e., aversion) in behaving mice, suggesting that L6-CT gain amplification can contribute to functionally adverse outcomes in sensory processing. Probing the neural basis of this effect with in vivo electrophysiology demonstrated that L6-CT activation enhances TC signaling at the level of VPL and superficial layers of S1. In parallel, L6-CT stimulation largely silences L5 neurons suggesting that the pronociceptive effect of L6-CT results from both enhancements of thalamocortical gain and simultaneous suppression of L5. Indeed, direct optogenetic inhibition of L5 elicited mechanical hypersensitivity, negative affect (i.e., aversion), and excitation of L6. Conversely, L5 activation had antinociceptive effects, namely a decrease in mechanical sensitivity in naive animals, and in animals with pharmacologically-induced inflammatory allodynia, affective L5-stimulation-seeking behavior, and a complete reversal of mechanical hypersensitivity. Together, these findings reveal bidirectional control over somatosensation by two discrete S1 cortical output pathways, suggesting future avenues for treating pain-related and sensory processing disorders.
Results
The Ntsr1-Cre mouse line is a useful model to study L6-CT pathways in different sensory cortices, including S1 cortex7,8,10,13,14,20,42. To assess the role of L6-CT neurons in modulating sensory gain and nociception, we expressed channelrhodopsin-2-EYFP (ChR2) or EGFP in L6-CT neurons of the right S1 hindlimb cortex (S1HL) of Ntsr1-Cre mice via stereotaxic injections of AAV-DIO-ChR2-EYFP (L6-ChR2 mice) or AAV-DIO-EGFP (L6-EGFP mice). Transgene expression in L6-ChR2 and L6-EGFP mice was restricted to L6 neurons in S1HL and their corticothalamic axons/terminals (Fig. 1a; L6-EGFP Supplementary Fig. 1b). Extracellular silicon-probe recordings in anesthetized L6-ChR2 mice validated optotagging of L6-CT neurons in S1HL (Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 2b, c), and demonstrated persistent drive of L6-CT spiking activity during 5 s laser stimuli (Fig. 1c) in a laser intensity-dependent manner (Fig. 1d).
Optogenetically-evoked L6-CT activity in the S1HL cortex elicits spontaneous nocifensive behavior
We asked whether L6-CT activation in the S1HL cortex has any measurable effect on spontaneous behavior in the absence of peripheral sensory stimulation. In awake, freely moving animals implanted with a fiber optic above S1HL (Fig.1e), optogenetic activation of L6-CT neurons elicited vigorous lifting and shaking of the contralateral hindlimb in a laser intensity-dependent manner (Fig. 1f and Supplementary video 1). At maximum intensity (318 mW/mm2), the majority of animals showed paw lifting and limb shaking (9/11 and 6/11, respectively), while EGFP-expressing control animals did not show any responses to the laser. The manifestation of these vigorous nocifensive responses, specifically to the contralateral hindlimb, suggests that L6-CT activation in S1HL evokes a nociceptive experience originating from the hindlimb, even in the absence of peripheral sensory stimulation.
Optogenetically-evoked L6-CT activity in the S1HL cortex induces hypersensitivity, exacerbates inflammatory allodynia, and induces aversion
The ability of L6-CT neurons to elicit spontaneous nocifensive behavior (Fig. 1e) suggests that this pathway’s activity is sufficient to induce a state of hypersensitivity and, contrary to a strict sensory discriminatory role, implicates S1 in regulating emotional-affective aspects of pain. We tested these hypotheses by assessing mechanical and noxious heat sensitivity and aversion as a function of L6-CT activation.
To quantify mechanical sensitivity, we used the von Frey test43,44 to measure hindpaw withdrawal probabilities in response to mechanical stimulation over a range of stimulation forces (0.04–2 g), with and without optogenetic stimulation of L6-CT neurons in the contralateral S1HL (Fig. 2a, b). Using optogenetic laser intensities below the spontaneous paw lifting threshold (see Methods), L6-CT activation increased mechanical sensitivity over nearly the entire range of tested mechanical forces. Increased sensitivity to low-pressure punctate stimuli (0.04–0.4 g)45 suggests an allodynic effect of L6-CT activity in S1 (i.e., a nociceptive response to typically innocuous stimuli), while increased sensitivity to nociceptor-activating mechanical forces (0.6–2 g)46 represents a hyperalgesic effect. In addition to increasing mechanical sensitivity, L6-CT stimulation similarly amplified responses to noxious heat stimulation of the hindpaw (Supplementary Fig. 3). Mechanical and heat sensitivity remained unchanged in control animals (Ntsr1-Cre mice expressing EGFP in L6-CT; Supplementary Figs. 3, 4).
Does the L6-CT pathway also interact with a preexisting pain condition? To address this, we repeated von Frey experiments in a well-established inflammatory pain model induced by injection of Complete Freund’s adjuvant (CFA)47 in the left hindpaw, which induces allodynia and hyperalgesia (Supplementary Fig. 5). L6-CT activation in contralateral S1HL further increased mechanical sensitivity (Fig. 2c) in contrast to L6-EGFP controls in which sensitivity remained unchanged (Supplementary Fig. 4b, d). Taken together, L6-CT activation induced mechanical hyperalgesia and allodynia in naive animals, and furthermore exacerbated hyperalgesia and allodynia in an inflammatory model (Fig. 2b, c).
Because L6-CT activation can elicit a seemingly distressful and aversive experience (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Video 1), we sought to directly test the link between L6-CT activity in S1HL and negative affect by employing a real-time aversion paradigm32. Here, mice (L6-ChR2 and L6-EGFP controls) freely roamed a two-chamber setup during five baseline sessions to establish individual chamber preferences. In subsequent conditioning sessions, preferred chambers were paired with laser stimulation of the right S1HL cortex (schematic in Fig. 2e). Changes in the time spent in the preferred chamber were used to quantify the aversive effects of the stimulation. L6-ChR2 but not L6-EGFP control mice spent significantly less time in the laser-paired chamber relative to the time spent in the same chamber during the baseline session (i.e., without optogenetic stimulation) (Fig. 2f, g). The preference index for the paired chamber (PI) also dropped in L6-EGFP mice, indicating that the laser itself is optically aversive. However, the laser effect was significantly stronger in L6-ChR2 mice (Fig. 2h), who developed an immediate and persistent avoidance of the stimulated chamber not observed in L6-EGFP controls (Supplementary Fig. 6). Together, these data show the potential of L6-CT-mediated changes in thalamocortical activity to cause hypersensitivity, amplified nociception, and aversion.
Neuronal circuit mechanisms underlying the pronociceptive effect of L6-CT activation
We hypothesized that the pronociceptive effect of L6-CT stimulation during behavior arises from enhanced sensory signal flow through the thalamus and, in addition, might also involve cortico-cortical components, given that L6-CT neurons recruit local and translaminar cortical inhibitory neurons8,9,42. To assess the corticothalamic and cortico-cortical effects of L6-CT, we recorded thalamic and translaminar S1 single-unit spiking with silicon probes (Fig. 3a schematic and Supplementary Fig. 7) in combination with optogenetics and plantar mechanical stimulation in anesthetized Ntsr1-Cre mice expressing ChR2 in L6-CT neurons in S1HL cortex.
We assessed single-unit spiking in three stimulation conditions (Fig. 3a): (1) Laser (L) stimulation of L6-CT neurons, (2) Mechanical (M) stimulation of the hindpaw, and (3) combined mechanical and laser (ML) stimulation. After pooling recordings across animals, we first screened for units with spiking responses significantly modulated relative to baseline in any of the three stimulus conditions (see Methods). The majority of units met this criterion (742/1018 in VPL and 751/842 in S1HL) and the remainder were not analyzed further. The response of each unit was further quantified as mean firing rate (r̄) measured in a 1.5 s window after stimulus onset and modulation index (MI, −1 to 1) calculated by comparing stimulus-evoked spiking to baseline spiking. For VPL units, we also calculated burst probability (BP, 0–1), the ratio of burst events out of all observed responses, and response probability (RP, 0–1) per stimulus presentation.
L6-CT activation enhances VPL output to the cortex
L6-CT stimulation enhanced VPL spiking in the absence of sensory stimulation (Fig. 3b–d, f, g), and both VPL r̄L and RPL increased with stimulation strength (Supplementary Fig. 8a and Fig. 3g). L6-CT stimulation also decreased VPL bursting in favor of tonic spiking patterns (Supplementary Fig. 8b and Fig. 3h, i), in line with VPL depolarization during sustained CT activation as demonstrated in the whisker and visual thalamus10,14,48. These increases in both RP per trial and tonic spiking support a scenario in which L6-CT enhances VPL responses to somatosensory stimuli, as a mechanism for amplified nociception observed in the behavior (Fig. 2). To test this hypothesis, we next assessed whether L6-CT activation at moderate laser intensities altered encoding of mechanical stimuli in a paired stimulation protocol.
Laser stimulation (L) of L6-CT and mechanical stimulation of the paw (M) alone modulated sizable fractions of VPL units (74 and 33%, L and M, respectively, Fig. 3b) with substantial overlap between the populations (69% of M-sensitive responded to L, 42% of L-sensitive responded to M, Fig. 3b), demonstrating that our probes targeted the paw-sensitive region of VPL and that S1 CT projections from ChR2-expressing L6 neurons overlapped with this region (probe location in Supplementary Fig. 7). Paw stimulation alone evoked time-varying responses (Fig. 3d), with either phasic responses to initial changes in pressure (dP/dt) during stimulus onset, or less often, sustained responses throughout the duration of the stimulus. The direction of modulation by M relative to baseline was largely positive, in line with the established role of VPL as the main TC path for somatosensory information.
How does L6-CT activation modulate paw responses in VPL? Pairing L6-CT stimulation with paw stimulation (ML) enhanced the VPL representation of mechanical stimuli at the population and single-unit levels. In the ML condition, the fraction of responsive VPL units (61%) increased relative to both M and L conditions (33 and 54%, p < 0.001, McNemar’s test)—including 12% of previously silent neurons which responded only to this combined condition. At the single-unit level, combined ML increased r̄, MI, and RP relative to both M and L control conditions (Fig. 3e and Supplementary Fig. 8). Estimating from the mean spiking rate and response probability observed across animals, CT activation approximately doubled mechanically-evoked thalamic output on the level of single VPL units (~2 AP/stim vs. 1.5 AP/stim vs 1 AP/stim, for ML, L, and M conditions, respectively). Aside from enhancing VPL response magnitude and probability, CT activation altered VPL spike timing statistics by shifting mechanically-evoked VPL spiking from burst to tonic mode (Fig. 3h, i). This effect depended on baseline firing mode and indeed served to normalize firing modes across units: (1) For tonic units (low BP), L6-CT activation shifted ISIs to lower values (Fig. 3i, dashed lines), reflecting increased firing rates. (2) In contrast, bursty (high BP) units were shifted to tonic mode, as CT activation decreased the fraction of short ISIs (Fig. 3i, solid lines). Thalamic tonic firing patterns likely depolarize cortical targets more effectively because they are less affected by the frequency-dependent depression at TC synapses49.
In a separate set of experiments, we found that another thalamic target of S1 L6-CT, the posterior medial thalamic nucleus (POm), was more weakly and heterogeneously modulated by L6 activation, with lower median MI compared to VPL (Supplementary Fig. 9). Furthermore, the vast majority of POm units did not show significant mechanical responses, preventing further consideration of L6-CT modulation of POm mechanical responses under these experimental conditions.
Taken together, these findings suggest that the pronociceptive effect of L6-CT activation involves a large thalamic signaling component via VPL: under conditions of increased L6-CT activity, baseline feedforward thalamocortical signaling to the cortex is increased, suggesting a likely mechanism for the spontaneous pain-like behaviors, as well as enhanced thalamocortical representation of sensory stimuli during paw stimulation underlying hypersensitivity.
Layer-specific modulation of S1 stimulus-evoked responses by L6-CT
As VPL is the primary thalamic route for somatosensory information to S1HL, we hypothesized that enhancement of thalamic responses by L6-CT could lead to concomitantly enhanced responses in the S1HL cortex. Additionally, L6-CT activity could alter S1HL responses more directly through cortical inhibitory networks. We therefore recorded depth-resolved spiking in S1HL layers 2–6 (Methods) during the paired stimulation protocol (Fig. 4a). The layer and cell-type-specific responses to mechanical, L6-CT, and paired stimulation for the entire pooled population of S1 neurons is summarized in Fig. 4b–e.
Consistent with direct optogenetic control of the L6 CT pathway shown in Fig. 1, the majority (74%) of L6 neurons (Fig. 4b–e, blue shading) were driven in the L condition, with spiking time-locked to the duration of the laser stimulus. In contrast, L6 units had weak mechanical responses: only a small fraction (15%) of L6 units were significantly modulated in M, with modest MIM corresponding to low r̄M and low spontaneous spiking. This is consistent with weak and heterogeneous sensory modulation of L6 neurons reported in other sensory cortices50,51.
We next examined the effect of L6-CT activation on other cortical layers. The degree of suppression and/or enhancement of neuronal responses by L6-CT was highly layer-specific, showing distinct effects on L5 versus L2/3 and L4 (Fig. 4b–f; see Supplementary Table 2 for comparisons across layers).
The most striking intracortical effect of L6-CT stimulation was a nearly complete abolishment of spiking in L5 units (Fig. 4b–e, red shading). While 56% of L5 units responded to paw stimulation, these responses largely disappeared with L6-CT stimulation. L6-CT stimulation also strongly suppressed spontaneous L5 spiking (69% negatively modulated, Fig. 4b, c). This suppressive effect of L6-CT on L5 has been described in the whisker cortex52 and is mediated by feedforward inhibition7–9,42. We conclude that while L6-CT stimulation increases VPL output to the cortex (Fig. 3), this increased TC input is not sufficient to overcome the effect of intracortical feedforward inhibition on the L5 population.
In contrast, the effect of L6-CT activation on upper layers (L2/3 and L4) was weaker and more heterogeneous, for both spontaneous activity and modulation of mechanically-evoked responses (Fig. 4b–e, gray shadings). For both L2/3 and L4, L6-CT activation alone (L) enhanced and (suppressed) largely comparable fractions of units: L2/3: 52% (28%), L4: 60% (31%). Focusing on the subset of L2/3 and L4 neurons that were sensitive to paw stimulation (92 and 73%, respectively; Fig. 4c), both L2/3 and 4 units showed increased r̄ML relative to r̄M (Supplementary Fig. 8e), but the differences between M and ML conditions were not significant at the population level for either r̄ (Supplementary Fig. 8e) or MI (Fig. 4d). In sum, L6-CT activation weakly enhanced mechanical-evoked spiking of some units in superficial layers, consistent with increased VPL output (Fig. 3), and inhibited other units, consistent with L6-CT feedforward inhibition. This moderate net enhancement of superficial layers may contribute to L6-CT-mediated behavioral hypersensitivity (Fig. 2) during the processing of somatosensory stimuli.
Given the marked suppressive effect of L6-CT stimulation on L5 – particularly in contrast to the relatively mild enhancement of superficial layers—and the increasingly established role for L5 neurons in perception as well as pathologies such as chronic pain (reviewed in53), we reasoned that the L6–L5 functional interaction may contribute—in synergy with enhanced VPL-S1 TC signaling—to the modulation of pain-like behaviors. To test this hypothesis, we employed cell-type-specific optogenetic control of L5 neurons in behaving mice and studied the function of L5 activation and suppression on sensitivity, analgesia, and aversion.
L5 inhibition in S1HL reproduces the pronociceptive phenotype of L6-CT stimulation, while L5 stimulation is antinociceptive
Because L6-CT stimulation induced hypersensitivity and, at the same time, strongly suppressed L5 neurons, we asked whether the L6-CT behavioral modulation could be in part due to the suppression of L5. We, therefore directly suppressed L5 using the inhibitory opsin stGtACR254 (characterization in Supplementary Fig. 10) in the Rpb4-Cre line (Fig. 5). We found that suppression of L5 leads to a net enhancement of L6 in S1HL in awake animals (10% suppressed vs. 42% activated, Fig. 5d), in close agreement with recent findings from the primary auditory cortex55. On the behavioral level, direct optogenetic suppression of L5 neurons had similar effects as L6-CT stimulation, namely increased mechanical sensitivity (Fig. 5e) and aversion in the CPA paradigm (Fig. 5f, g, heat sensitivity and CPP in Supplementary Fig. 11). Thus, L5 inhibition reproduces the hypersensitivity and aversion phenotype we observed for L6-CT stimulation.
Based on the pronociceptive effect of L5 inhibition, we next reasoned that stimulation of L5 in S1HL might have an opposite, anti-nociceptive effect. To directly test this possibility, we next optogenetically stimulated L5 neurons in S1HL in channelrhodopsin-2-EYFP (“L5-ChR2”) or EGFP (“L5-EGFP”) expressing Rbp4-Cre mice (Fig. 6a; for EGFP-controls, see Supplementary Fig. 1a). Silicon-probe recordings in anesthetized L5-ChR2 mice validated optogenetic drive of short-latency and low-jitter spiking L5 activity (Fig. 6b) that persisted throughout the 5 s laser stimulus (Fig. 6c).
As with the L6-CT pathway, we first assessed whether L5 activation in the S1HL cortex affected spontaneous behavior in fiber-implanted, freely moving animals. At all tested laser intensities (0–318 mW/mm²), none of the L5-ChR2 animals showed optogenetically-elicited nocifensive behavior, in stark contrast to the observations in the L6-ChR2 animals (Fig. 6d and Supplementary Video 2). L5 activation in S1HL also did not induce an aversive phenotype (Supplementary Fig. 12). How does optogenetic control of L5 modulate paw sensitivity to acute stimulation? Quantification of mechanical sensitivity using the von Frey test revealed that L5 activation significantly decreased mechanical sensitivity over nearly the entire range of tested mechanical forces (Fig. 6e), while L5 suppression increased mechanical sensitivity (Fig. 5). Sensitivity remained unchanged in L5-EGFP control animals (Supplementary Fig. 4a).
These effects in naive mice suggest an anti-nociceptive function of the L5 pathway. To test this directly, we repeated the mechanical sensitivity measurements in the CFA inflammatory pain model. In CFA-injected L5-ChR2 animals, laser stimulation of the S1HL decreased mechanical sensitivity (Fig. 6f), an effect not present in L5-EGFP control animals (Supplementary Fig. 4c). Furthermore, we observed that L5 stimulation in CFA mice completely reverses mechanical sensitivity back to pre-CFA sensitivity (Fig. 6f, g), suggesting that L5 stimulation can ameliorate inflammatory pain. To further explore this possibility, we conducted real-time place preference tests, in which mice freely explored two chambers, of which the non-preferred chamber was longitudinally paired with optogenetic stimulation of S1HL. L5-ChR2 mice, but not L5-EGFP controls, showed real-time conditioned place preference (CPP) for the stimulation-paired chamber (Fig. 6h–j). Thus, L5 stimulation reduces sensitivity in naive animals and causes a place preference in the inflammation model, while suppression of L5 causes hypersensitivity and aversion.
S1 bidirectionally modulates paw sensitivity via L6-CT and L5
We next directly compared the effects of L5 and L6-CT stimulation across animals and stimulation forces, illustrating that L5 reduces sensitivity and L6-CT increases sensitivity for a given mechanical stimulation force (Fig. 7a).
As a single readout for overall mechanical sensitivity, we computed areas under the curves (AUC) from all mechanical sensitivity measurements, which allowed us to directly compare the modulatory functions of cortical pathway-specific optogenetic stimulation on sensitivity in the naive and CFA-inflamed mouse models. Compared to baseline levels (Laser off), high AUC values correspond to hypersensitivity and were caused by L6-CT stimulation and by paw inflammation in CFA animals. In contrast, low AUC values indicate reduced sensitivity and were caused by L5 stimulation (Fig. 7b), demonstrating that L6-CT and L5 in S1 can modulate paw sensitivity in opposite directions (Fig. 7c). In summary, this bidirectional modulation of mechanical sensitivity by S1HL output pathways complements the affective behavioral results obtained in this study (see overview in Supplementary Table 3), together supporting pronociceptive effects following L6-CT activation and antinociceptive effects following L5 activation.
Discussion
Pain is an inherently context-dependent phenomenon. Competing needs, psychosocial factors, and cross-modal sensory interactions can exacerbate or ameliorate the subjective experience of pain16,56,57, and such contextual effects point to central neural mechanisms for “top-down” control over pain perception. Here, we investigated how two distinct top-down pathways from the S1HL cortex modulate somatosensation in behaving mice (Fig. 8), revealing that L6-CT activation not only increases somatosensory sensitivity but also evokes aversion and nocifensive behavior even in the absence of peripheral stimulation, whereas L5 activation has an opposing, antinociceptive effect. This bidirectional control of pain-related behaviors was effective in both naive conditions and inflammatory pain models.
The marked aversive effects of L6-CT activation were unexpected, as existing research has primarily studied L6-CT in purely sensory processing contexts and left unresolved how L6-CT functions link to subjective sensory experiences and to behavior. It is broadly established that L6-CT pathways endow sensory systems with “beneficial” information processing functions, such as controlling sensory gain and tuning selectivity7,8,11,52,58 and mediating burst-tonic transitions in the thalamus10,12,59. Our observation that S1HL L6-CT activation can cause aversive hypersensitivity reveals a nociceptive potential for S1 L6-CT to gain control. Therefore, these results expand the function of L6-CT, suggesting that, contingent upon L6-CT output level, gain control follows a functional continuum spanning both increased sensitivity and aversive hypersensitivity—even exacerbating preexisting hypersensitivity in inflammatory pain and triggering spontaneous pain-like behavior.
What are the possible circuit mechanisms underlying L6-CT-mediated hypersensitivity? To assess corticothalamic1,10,14 and cortico-cortical effects8,9,42 of L6-CT activation, we conducted silicon-probe recordings in VPL and S1 to measure how L6-CT activity modulates thalamocortical representations of mechanical stimuli. We found that L6-CT enhances VPL spiking and modulates S1 in a layer-specific manner, with the most drastic effect being the suppression of L5. This dual effect of L6-CT to suppress L5 while enhancing the thalamus has been described in the whisker TC system, but not in the context of behavior or pain52. Notably, our VPL recordings revealed several parallel mechanisms favoring increased thalamocortical sensory transmission. L6-CT activation not only enlarged the fraction of sensory-encoding units in VPL, but also increased spike output on the level of individual units, and in parallel, both favored tonic spiking mode and inhibited non-responding units. Another thalamic target of L6-CT is POm20 which has been implicated in pain processing60. L6-CT stimulation also modulated POm activity, albeit more weakly and heterogeneously compared to VPL. Taken together, the thalamic effects of L6-CT activation predict an enhanced signal-to-noise ratio of sensory output to the cortex. Consistent with an increase in thalamic signaling, at the cortical level, L4 and L2/3 responses to mechanical stimuli increased. L6-CT recurrent excitation via the thalamus likely contributes to the net enhancement of L2/3 and L461 and we cannot fully resolve the recurrent component in L2/3 and L4 in the present study. Nevertheless, this enhancement of the VPL – L4 – L2/3 axis suggests that L6-CT can amplify sensory transmission through the canonical circuitry of the lemniscal pathway of the lateral pain system, which carries body signals to S134. Furthermore, at the same time as sensory transmission to S1 is amplified, antinociceptive L5 output from the cortex is suppressed.
The near complete suppression of L5 neurons’ spontaneous and sensory-evoked spiking likely comes about from L6-CT recruitment of inhibitory circuits, as demonstrated in the barrel and visual cortices. Based on this work, excitatory effects of L6-CT neurons are via direct glutamatergic synapses, while the suppression of L5 is mediated by feedforward inhibition via L6-CT’s selective recruitment of deep-layer, fast-spiking parvalbumin-positive inhibitory neurons7–9,42,52.
We hypothesized that the L6-CT-mediated suppression of L5 contributes to increased nociception. Consistent with this hypothesis, we found that direct optogenetic suppression of L5 increased sensitivity and caused place aversion. Conversely, L5 stimulation reduced mechanical sensitivity, reversed inflammatory allodynia, and caused place preference in animals with pharmacologically-induced inflammatory allodynia (for a summary of the outcomes of the main behavioral manipulations, see Supplementary Table 3). Together these findings suggest that the pronociceptive function of L6-CT results from combined suppression of L5 and enhancement of thalamocortical excitability via enhancing the VPL – L4 – L2/3 axis (Fig. 8).
These results potentially have clinical value, such that positive effects on pain states may be achieved by preventing L6-CT hyperactivity and/or by enhancement of L5 activity in the S1 cortex. Based on the corticothalamic anatomy of the S1 limb area, this antinociceptive action of L5 is unlikely to directly involve VPL, which does not receive L5 input20, but may rather be mediated by descending modulation of subcortical L5 target circuits (Fig. 8)—e.g., in POm, zona incerta, periaqueductal gray, or spinal cord—which have been associated with pain60,62,63. Untangling the downstream effects of L5 stimulation may generate highly specific interventional strategies for pain management.
Behavioral determinants and circuit mechanisms controlling L6-CT output pathways are only beginning to be studied, e.g., refs. 64,65, let alone those that potentially lead to L6 hyperactivity or synchronization. Interestingly, there appears to be a strong link between pain states and increased S1 activity66,67 and cortical synchronization31,32,68. Optogenetic stimulation tends to strongly activate and synchronize neuronal activity69, an effect which likely helped to uncover—but also likely exaggerated—adverse effects of L6-CT gain control and antinociceptive effects of L5 as described in this study. Nonetheless, these results demonstrate the potential of cortical output pathways to turn sensory signals into painful experiences or dampen nociception, even though it is currently not known what drives L6-CT neurons under physiological or pathological conditions61.
While our study did not directly focus on the interactions between L6 and L5, our data concurs with recent research in the auditory cortex that showed that optogenetic L5 stimulation inhibited L655. In line with this L5 feedforward suppression of L6, we find that L5 inhibition increases L6 spiking. In turn, L6 stimulation inhibits L5, as shown here and in previous work7,52. Generalizing these results suggests that these two distinct cortical output layers may ‘compete’ with one another, and that the result of this competition influences sensory perception, including pain sensitivity and affect. Changes to either L5 or L6-CT would not only alter perception directly—with respect to a single cortical output pathway—but would also tip the balance between cortical output pathways through mutual inhibition. For example, in the context of the present study, L6-CT is pronociceptive directly through increased sensory gain from the periphery, and indirectly via its inhibition of the antinociceptive L5 pathway.
In conclusion, this study reveals dual modulation of sensitivity and nociception via two specific populations of cortical output neurons in S1, underscoring the potential of cortical output pathways as therapeutic targets. The role of S1 in pain has been debated35, as targeting S1 has been shown to both beneficially alter pain trajectories but also enhance nociceptive responses29,30,32,70,71. Moreover, previous causal studies affected bulk S1 activity across all layers and cell types and were thereby unable to functionally disambiguate the opposing roles of the two cortical output pathways, as revealed in this study. Understanding how the balance of corticofugal output is maintained in health and altered in disease is a promising direction for future studies.
Methods
Ethics statement
All experimental procedures were approved by the local governing body (Regierungspräsidium Karlsruhe, Germany, approval numbers: 35-9185.81/G-29/16, 35-9185.81/G-70/21, T-39-20, and 35-9185.82/A-8/20) and performed according to their ethical guidelines.
Animals
Mice (male and female, 7–16 weeks of age) were housed with food and water ad libitum on a 12 h light/dark cycle (housing conditions 20–22 °C, 40–65% humidity).
Mouse lines
Layer 6 optogenetic stimulation
“Ntsr1-Cre” (B6.FVB(Cg)-Tg/(Ntsr1-cre)GN220Gsat/Mmucd)
“Ntsr1-Cre-ChR2-EYFP”; crossbreed between “Ntsr1-cre” x “Ai32” (B6.FVB(Cg)-Tg/(Ntsr1-cre)GN220Sat/Mmucd x B6.129S-Gt(ROSA)26Sortm32(CAG-COP4*H134R/EYFP).
Layer 5 optogenetic stimulation
“Rbp4-Cre” (B6.FVB/CD1-Tg(Rbp4-cre)KL100Gsat/Mmucd)
Virus injection and optical fiber implantation
Ntsr1-Cre and Rbp4-Cre mice were stereotaxically injected with either an excitatory opsin (AAV2-EF1a DIO-ChR2(H134R)-EYFP-WPRE-pA, 5.7 × 10¹² vg/ml, Zürich vector core), an inhibitory opsin (AAV1-hSyn-SIO-stGtACR2-FusionRed, 5 × 1011 vg/ml, Addgene) or a control virus (AAV2-hSyn-DIO-EGFP, 100 µL at titer ≥3 × 10¹² vg/mL, Addgene). A subset of the optogenetic L6-CT experiments was done in Ntsr1-Cre-ChR2-EYFP mice, which were not virus injected. We observed no differences in any of the measurements and pooled Ntsr1-Cre-ChR2-EYFP mice and virus-injected Ntsr1-Cre experiments. Virus expression time was between 3-4 weeks.
For the injection and implantation, mice were placed in a stereotaxic frame (Kopf Instruments) and anesthetized with 1.2–2.0 vol% isoflurane in medical oxygen at a flow rate of 0.8 L/min, while keeping body temperature at 39 °C. Carprofen (CP-Pharma) was administered subcutaneously (5 mg/kg) and Lidocaine (Xylocaine 1%, Aspen Pharma) was injected under the scalp and around fixation ear bars for local anesthesia. Eyes were covered with Bepanthen ointment (Bayer) to prevent eye drying during the surgery. After ensuring the absence of tail and toe pinch reflexes, the skin was opened with a midline incision, the periosteum and aponeurotic galea was removed to visualize anatomical reference points (bregma and lambda), and the head was aligned to the stereotaxic frame. Small craniotomies were drilled above the S1HL area and viral particle solutions were injected into two sites within S1HL with calibrated glass micropipettes (Blaubrand; IntraMARK) at the following coordinates relative to bregma (AP, ML) and pia mater (DV):
First injection: ML = +1.4 mm, AP = −0.46 mm; sec injection: ML = +1.5 mm, AP = −0.94 mm. Ntsr1-Cre mice were injected at a depth of −0.9 and −1.0 mm, while Rbp4-Cre mice were injected at a depth of −0.7 and −0.8 mm (100 nl at each depth, followed by a waiting period of 10 min before relocating the injecting pipette).
Chronic optical fiber implants (200 µm diameter, numerical aperture of 0.39, Thorlabs GmbH) were placed on the dura above the S1HL (ML = +1.5 mm, AP = −0.94 mm), and the ceramic ferrule encapsulating the optical fiber was fixed to the skull with dental cement. To minimize laser light leakage, the cement was colorized in black and the mating sleeve used during the experiments was covered with black tape. Mice were kept between three and four weeks for optimal viral expression before experiments.
Histology and immunohistochemistry
The mice were exposed to a lethal dose of Ketamine (120 mg/kg) and Xylazine (20 mg/kg) and transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. The brains were sectioned with a vibratome (Thermo Scientific Microm HM 650 V) at a thickness of 80 µm. Selected sections were stained with DAPI, mounted with Mowiol, and imaged with an epifluorescent microscope (Leica DM6000).
Behavior
Behavioral experiments were conducted during the light cycle and experimenters were blinded to the experimental identity of the animals.
Optogenetic stimulation
The implanted fiber was coupled to an optical patch cord (Thorlabs GmbH) attached to a laser output module (473 nm) (Shanghai Laser Optics Century Co., Ltd.). The laser power at the fiber tip was measured with a power energy meter (Thorlabs GmbH). Irradiance values for layers 5 and 6 were estimated based on previous measurements in mammalian brain tissue72. For 10 mW measured at the fiber tip (fiber NA = 0.39; fiber radius = 100 µm), the irradiance is 318.18 mW/mm², which corresponds to 3.47 mW/mm² at the level of L5 (0.75 mm cortical depth) and 1.54 mW/mm² at the level of L6 (1 mm cortical depth).
Laser protocols
Single laser pulse trials were 5 s long and consisted of continuous laser stimulation. Mechanical and thermal stimuli were applied during laser pulses. Optical stimulation blocks were interspersed with at least 30 s non-stimulation blocks.
Measurements to determine individual paw lifting laser intensity thresholds
Laser stimuli (5 s continuous) were applied at 10, 8, 6, 4, 2, 0 mW (318.18 254.55, 190.91, 127.27, 63.64, and 0 mW/mm²) five times at each intensity and paw lifting probabilities were calculated for each intensity (Fig. 1e). For each mouse, we determined the highest intensity resulting in a lack of paw lifting and used this individual laser intensity during sensitivity measurements (mechanical and thermal).
Inflammatory pain model
About 20 µl of Complete Freund’s adjuvant (CFA) was subcutaneously injected into the left hindpaw under anesthesia using isoflurane. Behavioral experiments with the CFA cohorts (von Frey and CPP) were carried out one day after the CFA injection.
von Frey test
Mice were habituated to the von Frey test chamber twice a day for 1 h for three consecutive days without von Frey filament stimulation. Each von Frey test session started with an acclimatization period of 15 min. Mechanical sensitivity was quantified as the probability of paw withdrawal in response to the application of von Frey filaments (Aesthesio Precise Tactile Sensory Evaluator, Ugo Basile S.R.L.) to the plantar surface of the left hindpaw (contralateral to the stimulated HL cortex). Eight filaments (0.04–2.0 g of force) were applied five times each in ascending order, with at least 30 s delay between applications. 100% withdrawal probability was reached when one filament provoked a withdrawal response in all five trials. In this case, measurements were stopped and sensitivity for filaments with greater forces was not tested. Mechanical sensitivity was first measured in the absence of laser stimulation (baseline) and after ~1 h in the homecage, the test was repeated with laser stimulation (5 s per trial) of either S1HL L6-CT or L5 pathways. The mechanical stimulus was applied within 1 s after the onset of the 5 s laser stimulation. Withdrawal responses were only considered during the 5 s laser stimulation period. Another round of tests (baseline and laser) was conducted 1 day after the subcutaneous injection of CFA in the left hindpaw.
Thermal test
Thermal sensitivity was tested using the Hargreaves setup (Ugo Basile Inc., Italy) equipped with an infrared heat laser (Model 37370-001, Ugo Basile). The heat laser was aimed at the plantar surface of the left hindpaw and produced radiant heat of increasing intensity. The intensity level was set to 35 and the cut-off time to 20 s to avoid damage to the paw. Three heat stimulation trials were applied alone, and then in the presence of optogenetic laser stimulation (238.64 mW/mm²), with 3 min of recovery time between trials. The paw withdrawal latency was measured per trial.
Conditioned place preference/aversion (CPP/CPA) test
The setup for this paradigm consisted of two chambers (each 15 cm × 15 cm) separated by a neutral chamber (8 cm × 8 cm). One chamber contained walls with vertical stripes and a cherry scent, while the other chamber contained walls with horizontal stripes and a honey scent. The paradigm consisted of one (CPP) or five (CPA) baseline sessions followed by two conditioning sessions, each lasting 20 min. Prior to each session, animals were lightly anesthetized with isoflurane and were attached to an optic fiber cable, at which point they were restricted to the neutral chamber using removable wall slides. Once recovered from anesthesia, the wall slides were removed and the session was started. During baseline sessions, no optogenetic stimulus was administered. During the conditioning sessions, optogenetic stimulation (8 mW, 254.55 mW/mm2, 20 Hz) was administered when the animal entered the chamber in which it spent less time (in the case of CPP) or more time (in the case of CPA) in the last baseline session. Pulsed stimulation at 20 Hz was chosen to reduce phototoxic effects during prolonged stimulation required for the paradigm. AnyMaze software (Version 7.1, Stoelting Co., Ireland) was used to track the animals’ position and time spent per chamber for every session. To assess performance outcomes, a comparison was made between the time spent in the stimulation-paired chamber during the last conditioning session and the time spent in that same chamber during the last baseline session. Preference indices (PI) were computed using the following formula: (time in paired chamber – time in unpaired chamber)/(time in paired chamber + time in unpaired chamber).
Statistics and data analysis
All behavioral data are expressed as the mean ± the standard error, and were analyzed using SPSS (Version 28.0.1.0) and R Studio (Version 4.2.0). Unless stated otherwise, two-way ANOVA for repeated measures with Bonferroni tests for multiple comparisons were used. A p value less than 0.05 was considered to be significant. Microscopy images were edited using Fiji/Image J (Version 1.53c). Schematics and figures were created in Affinity Designer (Version 1.10.6), GraphPad Prism (Version 9.1.1), and Matlab 2022a.
In vitro slice electrophysiology
Preparation of acute brain slices
Ntsr1-Cre mice (n = 3) were stereotaxically injected with an inhibitory opsin (AAV1-hSyn-SIO-stGtACR2-FusionRed, 5 × 1011 vg/ml, Addgene) see “Virus injection and optical fiber implantation” 6 weeks prior to the recordings. Acute brain slices containing the S1HL region were prepared as follows. Isoflurane-anesthetized mice were decapitated, their brain was removed and trimmed, and the forebrain containing the S1HL region was placed in oxygenated (95% O2 and 5% CO2) ice-cold, cutting solution containing (mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 3 MgCl2, 0.1 CaCl2, 25 glucose, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 0.4 ascorbic acid, 3 myo-inositol, 2 Na-pyruvate, and 25 NaHCO3 (pH 7.4). Then, 170-mm-thick slices were cut with a Leica vibratome (VT1200S) and kept for 20 min in warm (36 °C), oxygenated (ACSF solution that contained (in mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1 MgCl2, 2 CaCl2, 25 glucose, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 0.4 ascorbic acid, 3 myo-inositol, 2 Na-pyruvate, and 25 NaHCO3 pH 7.4. Then, the brain slices were moved to oxygenated ACSF maintained at room temperature for at least 1 h before performing electrophysiological recordings.
Optogenetic stimulation and electrophysiological recordings from S1HL L6-CT-stGtACR2 neurons
After recovery, S1HL-containing slices were placed on an RC-27 chamber (Sutter Instruments) mounted under BX51 upright microscope (Olympus), equipped with DIC and fluorescent capabilities. Slices were maintained at 24 ± 1 °C using a dual TC344B temperature control system (Sutter Instruments). S1HL slices were continuously perfused with oxygenated (95%O2/ 5%CO2) ASCF solution containing (in mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 0.1 MgCl2, 4 CaCl2, 25 glucose, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 0.4 ascorbic acid, 3 myo-inositol, 2 Na-pyruvate, 25 NaHCO3, pH 7.4, and 315 mOsm. Cells were approached and patched under DIC, using 3.0 ± 0.5 MegaOhm glass pipettes (WPI, Inc), pulled with a PC10 puller (Narishige, Japan). Recording pipettes were filled with a current-clamp internal solution containing (in mM): 125 K-gluconate, 20 KCl, 10 HEPES, 0.5 EGTA, 4 ATP-Magnesium, 0.3 GTP-Sodium, 10 Na-Phosphocreatine, osmolarity: 312 mOsmol; pH 7.2 adjusted with KOH. In all recordings, we used a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Axon Instruments, Inc) controlled by Clampex 10.1 and Digidata 1440 digitizer (Molecular Devices, Inc). Detection and analysis of current-clamp recordings was done with Clampfit 10.1.
To assess the impact of stGtACR2 activation on L6-CT neuron activity, cells expressing red fluorescence were approached and recorded in either loose cell-attached or in whole-cell current-clamp configuration. Cells displaying spontaneous spikes in cell-attached mode, were challenged with pulses of blue light (480 nm, 5 s) generated via a CoolLED illumination system (pE-300) controlled by a TTL pulse. In whole-cell current-clamp mode, cells were maintained at near resting potentials (~−70 mV) or at more depolarized potentials (~−40 mV) via direct current injection through the patch pipette. Two experiments were performed in the current-clamp mode. First, to assess the impact of stGtACR2 activation on membrane potential and spontaneous spiking activity, cells were challenged with long (5 s) pulses of blue light (480 nm), similar to the cell-attached experiments described above. Second, to determine if stGtACR2 activation leads to changes in the input resistance of L6 neurons, cells maintained at near resting potentials were stimulated with square pulses of current (500 ms, from −100 to +300 pA, 20 pA steps), and the amplitude of membrane potential changes before, during, and after blue light activation, was determined.
In vivo electrophysiology
Anesthetized in vivo electrophysiology
Mice were anesthetized using urethane (1.4 g/kg, i.p.) and maintained using an oxygen-isoflurane mixture (0.2%). The mice were fixed with ear bars and the skull was leveled. A craniotomy was performed above the recording site, and a well was cemented (Paladur) and filled with isotonic Ringer’s solution. Sharpened 64-channel silicon probes (impedance ~50 kOhm) (Cambridge Neurotech) were inserted into the S1HL cortex (ML = +1.67 mm, AP = −1.0 mm, DV = −1.4 mm), VPL (ML = +1.8, AP = −1.3, DV = −4.5), or POm (ML = +1.2 mm, AP = −1.7 to −2.4 mm, DV = −3.4 mm, 4 shank probe) using a micromanipulator (Luigs Neumann 3-axis Motor), moving at ~2 μm/s. The probes were docked to a connector (ASSY-77) with an adapter (A64-Om32x2 Samtec) that was, in turn, connected to an RHD2164 headstage amplifier chip (Intan technologies). Signals were amplified and digitized at a sampling rate of 30,030 Hz via an RDH2000 Intan evaluation board using USB 2.0 interface. An Intan Talker module (Cambridge Electronic Devices, Cambridge, UK) was for data acquisition with Spike2 (v9.06) software.
Awake in vivo electrophysiology
Mice between 8–12 weeks (n = 4) were recorded on a cylindrical treadmill consisting of a 15 cm diameter foam roller mounted on a custom-built low friction rotary metal axis, attached to two vertical posts. Ntsr1-Cre and Rbp4-Cre mice were stereotaxically injected with an inhibitory opsin (AAV1-hSyn-SIO-stGtACR2-FusionRed, 5 × 1011 vg/ml, Addgene) see “Virus injection and optical fiber implantation”) 2–3 weeks prior to the recordings. At least a week before recording, a polycarbonate two-winged head plate was cemented onto the skull with dental cement (Super-Bond, Sun Medical Co. LTD). A rubber ring was cemented around the craniotomy to create a small ringer reservoir for the reference electrode. Mice were allowed to recover from the surgery for 2 days and, over the next 4 days, were habituated to the cylindrical treadmill in the Faraday cage. Animals were head-fixed on the treadmill apparatus with the head plate. Habituation sessions lasted for ~60 min, during which mice freely walked on the cylindrical treadmill and were fed sweetened condensed milk as a reward. A maximum of 24 h prior to recording, a craniotomy was performed over the injection sites. The rubber well was then covered with silicone elastomer (Kwik-Cast, World Precision Instruments) until the experiment. During the recording session, the protective silicone was removed and an acute silicone optrode (H3, Cambridge Neurotech) was lowered into the S1HL. The activity was recorded using the apparatus and software described in the section “Anesthetized in vivo electrophysiology”.
Mechanical stimulation during anesthetized in vivo electrophysiology
Target regions were identified by brushing or pressing the hindpaw with a brush or cotton swab and assessment of evoked activity from the Spike2 visual interface readout. Mechanical stimulation was automated by a stimulation protocol prepared in Spike2 through interface hardware (Power1401, Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) through a stepper controller, which initiated a motor (Mercury Step C-663 Stepper Motor Controller, PIMikroMove Version 2.25.2.0). Nine von Frey filaments glued (Pattex Sekundenkleber) to a force sensor (Single Tact miniature force sensor) attached to a motor, so the TTL pulse would apply these von Frey filaments to the paw, and pressure information would be relayed to the Intan board. A total of 9 g (9 × 1 g per filament) pressure was delivered for a 5 s duration every 60 s (based on the protocol in ref. 73).
Optogenetic stimulation during anesthetized and awake in vivo electrophysiology
An optical fiber (Thorlabs GmbH, NA = 0.22; radius = 52.5 µm) was positioned ~0.5 mm perpendicular above the craniotomy. Laser power densities overlapped with the behavioral experiments (~0.5–27 mW at fiber tip corresponding to 57.72−3105.34 mW/mm² and 0.26−14.16 mW/mm² at the level of L672). Light pulses were initiated automatically by an Omicron Light-Hub2 (Wavelength = 488 nm) with a stimulation protocol prepared in Spike2 through interface hardware (Power1401, Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). Trial repetitions per condition were 31–52 trials for VPL recordings and 32–35 trials for S1 recordings, and 22–26 trials for POm.
Stimulation protocol during anesthetized and awake in vivo electrophysiology
Experiments consisted of three stimulus conditions of 5 s duration each: mechanical, mechanical + optogenetic laser, and optogenetic laser. The time interval between mechanical and mechanical + optogenetic laser conditions was 60 s, otherwise, the time interval between conditions was 30 s (Fig. 3a). The stimulation in awake recordings consisted only of 5 s laser stimuli every 15 s.
Spike sorting
Voltage data were band-pass filtered upon acquisition (500–5000 Hz). Spike2 data files (.smrx) were converted into binary files. The file conversion consisted in reading the electrophysiology channels in the.smrx file, transforming them back into uint16 values from the 16-bit depth analog-to-digital (ADC), and writing them in the resulting.bin file.
Spike sorting was performed semi-automatically using Matlab-based Kilosort 2.574 and resulting clusters curated in Phy2 (https://github.com/cortex-lab/phy). Single units with <0.5% refractory period (1 ms) violations and a baseline spike rate >0.1 Hz were accepted for further analysis. Kilosort 2.5 spike-sorting parameters are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1.
Parameter | Value | Parameter | Value |
---|---|---|---|
ops.fs | 3.003003003003003e + 04 | ops.sigmaMask | 30 |
ops.fshigh | 600 | ops.ThPre | 8 |
ops.Th | [10 2] | ops.sig | 20 |
ops.lam | 10 | ops.nblocks | 0 |
ops.AUCsplit | 0.7 | ops.spkTh | −6 |
ops.minFR | 1/50 | ops.reorder | 1 |
ops.momentum | [20 400] | ops.nskip | 35 |
ops.GPU | 1 | ops.Nfilt | 1024 |
ops.nfilt_factor | 4 | ops.ntbuff | 64 |
ops.NT | 64*1024+ ops.ntbuff | ops.whiteningRange | 32 |
ops.nSkipCov | 25 | ops.scaleproc | 200 |
ops.nPCs | 6 | ops.useRAM | 0 |
Classification of putative cell types
Thalamic units
VPL units were defined as follows. First, probe location within VPL was confirmed histologically (Supplementary Fig. 7): For each recording, we first identified the dorsolateral range of channels which contained units with significant responses to M or ML. All units within that range were assigned to VPL. POm units were defined by the location of the probe, which was composed of four shanks aligned along the anterior/posterior axis, with the shanks distributed in 250 µm intervals. The anterior shank of the probe was stereotaxically targeted to POm based on the following coordinates: (ML = +1.2, AP = −1.7, DV = −3.4).
Cortical units
Silicon-probe recording channels were registered to histological layer borders to assign each unit a cortical depth and layer. Layer borders in the S1HL cortex were estimated histologically based on soma sizes and densities using DAPI signals (examples in Figs. 1, 6). Putative L6-CT and L5 units were isolated based on low latency (<9.5 ms) and low-jitter (<3 ms L5, <2 ms L6-CT) responses to laser light pulses (Supplementary Fig. 2). Putative FS-like units were identified by their peak-to-second trough latency below 215 µs75, and were removed from the optotagged populations. All other electrophysiological analysis included putative FS and RS units. The identity of optotagged units was validated by plotting these units by depth along the cortical axis, demonstrating that optotagged L5 and L6-CT neurons are located at the expected cortical depths in layers 5 and 6, respectively (Figs. 1, 6).
Spike train analysis
After spike-sorting, spike times were aligned to stimulus onsets and segregated into stimulation conditions using custom Matlab 2022a scripts. For cleaner visualization in Figs. 3, 4, peristimulus time histograms (bin size = 20 ms) were smoothed using a Matlab port (https://github.com/iandol/spikes/tree/master/Bars) of the Bayesian Adaptive Regression Splines technique76 using default parameters except for prior_id = ‘POISSON’. All further calculations were done on unsmoothed data.
Response windows
We considered spike counts and timing within a time window of 1.5 s after the stimulus onset + 500 ms to account for delayed activation of the mechanical stimulus relative to the command signal and baseline activity was taken as 1.5 s before stimulus onset (Figs. 3a, 4a). Laser-only trials were also sampled during the same windows to discard transient effects at stimulus onset (e.g., PSTH peak in Fig. 4d, L6) and to enable comparison between L and ML conditions.
Unit modulation
Units were considered modulated in a given stimulus condition if a significant difference was measured between baseline and stimulus-evoked spiking either in terms of absolute spike counts (p ≤ 0.05, signed-rank test for paired baseline and stimulus-evoked trials) or spike timing detected by using the ZETA test77.
Response parameters
We compared stimulus-evoked changes in single-unit activity across L, M, and ML conditions using spike counts and interspike interval (ISI) statistics to capture both overall spike output and spike timing. The mean spiking rate (r̄) was calculated as the mean spike count per trial, divided by the response window duration of 1.5 s. Response probability (RP) was calculated as (trial count with at least one spike)/total trial count, measured within the 1.5 s response window.
To calculate burst probability (BP) for single units, spikes preceded by an interspike interval of less than 5 ms were considered part of a burst. BP was then calculated as total burst events/(total burst events + total single spike events). Trends reported were relatively insensitive to a burst cut-off ISI of up to 10 ms. Modulation index (MI) was calculated as MI = (r̄stimulus − r̄baseline)/(r̄stimulus + r̄baseline) as in ref. 55. Data were presented as (first quartile, median, and third quartile) or mean ± SEM.
Statistical analysis of spike train data
All statistical analysis was done in Matlab 2022a or R, using built-in or custom-written functions. Unless otherwise stated, data were analyzed by two-way repeated measures ANOVA with a Bonferroni test. See Supplementary Table 1 for exact statistical tests and test outputs (F- and p-values). Paired MI and r̄ data across conditions (per region) were analyzed using the Friedman test, followed by a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. MI and r̄ comparisons across regions (Supplementary Table 2) was done with a mixed-model ANOVA followed by a rank-sum test for pairwise differences. Comparisons of the proportion of either positively, negatively, or unmodulated units per stimulation condition were made by a Χ2 proportions test, followed by the Marascuillo procedure for multiple comparisons. Statistical differences in the proportions of responsive units across conditions were assessed using McNemar’s test in the case of paired data or a two-proportions Χ2 test in the case of unpaired data (custom written), e.g., between cortical layers.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
The authors are grateful for technical assistance from Manfred Oswald and Zheng Gan, and software development and support from Liam Keegan (Scientific Software Center, Heidelberg University). Funding was provided by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG): Collaborative Research Center 1158 (A.G. and R.A.M.: project B10; R.K.: project B01, B06; T.K.: project B08, C.A.: project S02), DFG individual grant (GR3757/3-1, A.G., E.I.-C., J.M.-C.), the Chica and Heinz Schaller Stiftung and the Brigitte-Schlieben-Lange Programm by the Ministry of Science, Research and the Arts Baden-Württemberg (R.A.M.). C.A. is also supported by the Chica and Heinz Schaller Stiftung, the NARSAD Young Investigator award 2019, and a 2021 Fritz Thyssen grant. The authors acknowledge support by the state of Baden-Württemberg through bwHPC and the German Research Foundation (DFG) through grant no INST 39/963-1 FUGG (bwForCluster NEMO) and the data storage service SDS@hd supported by the Ministry of Science, Research and the Arts Baden-Württemberg (MWK) and the German Research Foundation (DFG) through grant INST 35/1503-1 FUGG. For the publication fee, we acknowledge financial support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft within the funding program “Open Access Publikationskosten“ as well as by Heidelberg University.
Source data
Author contributions
Conceptualization and project administration: A.G. and R.A.M., Methodology: S.A.-G., J.M.-C., L.L.T., S.K., R.K., and T.K., Investigation: K.Z., R.F., A.J.G., J.B., and C.A., Data analysis and analysis tools: K.Z., R.F., A.J.G., J.B., E.I.-C., R.A.M., and C.A., Visualization: K.Z., J.B., R.F., A.J.G, R.A.M., and A.G., Funding acquisition and supervision: A.G., R.A.M., R.K., and T.K., Writing—review and editing: K.Z., R.F., A.J.G., J.B., E.I.-C., J.M.-C., R.K., T.K., C.A., R.A.M., and A.G.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Hiroaki Wake, Zhi Zhang and the other, anonymous, reviewer for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Data availability
All the data are in the manuscript or in the supplementary material. Source data is provided as a Source Data file with this paper and under the following link: 10.11588/data/D2O0JZ78. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
Code necessary to reproduce the Matlab-generated figures in this study are provided and maintained at https://github.com/rebecca-mease/Ziegler_et_al_202379.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Katharina Ziegler, Ross Folkard, Antonio J Gonzalez.
These authors jointly supervised this work: Rebecca A Mease, Alexander Groh.
Contributor Information
Rebecca Audrey Mease, Email: beckin@gmail.com.
Alexander Groh, Email: groh@uni-heidelberg.de.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-023-38798-7.
References
- 1.Born, G. et al. Corticothalamic feedback sculpts visual spatial integration in mouse thalamus. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 1711–1720 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2.Wang Q, Webber RM, Stanley GB. Thalamic synchrony and the adaptive gating of information flow to cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:1534–1541. doi: 10.1038/nn.2670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Mukherjee, A., Lam, N. H., Wimmer, R. D. & Halassa, M. M. Thalamic circuits for independent control of prefrontal signal and noise. Nature10.1038/s41586-021-04056-3 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 4.Aru J, Suzuki M, Larkum ME. Cellular mechanisms of conscious processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020;24:814–825. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Usrey, W. M. & Sherman, S. M. Corticofugal circuits: communication lines from the cortex to the rest of the brain. J. Comp. Neurol.10.1002/cne.24423 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 6.Shepherd, G. M. G. & Yamawaki, N. Untangling the cortico-thalamo-cortical loop: cellular pieces of a knotty circuit puzzle. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.10.1038/s41583-021-00459-3 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 7.Olsen SR, Bortone DS, Adesnik H, Scanziani M. Gain control by layer six in cortical circuits of vision. Nature. 2012;483:47–52. doi: 10.1038/nature10835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bortone DS, Olsen SR, Scanziani M. Translaminar inhibitory cells recruited by layer 6 corticothalamic neurons suppress visual cortex. Neuron. 2014;82:474–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Frandolig JE, et al. The synaptic organization of layer 6 circuits reveals inhibition as a major output of a neocortical sublamina. Cell Rep. 2019;28:3131–3143.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mease RA, Krieger P, Groh A. Cortical control of adaptation and sensory relay mode in the thalamus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:6798–6803. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1318665111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hasse JM, Briggs F. Corticogeniculate feedback sharpens the temporal precision and spatial resolution of visual signals in the ferret. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:E6222–E6230. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1704524114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Spacek MA, et al. Robust effects of corticothalamic feedback and behavioral state on movie responses in mouse dLGN. Elife. 2022;11:e70469. doi: 10.7554/eLife.70469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Guo W, Clause AR, Barth-Maron A, Polley DB. A corticothalamic circuit for dynamic switching between feature detection and discrimination. Neuron. 2017;95:180–194.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Crandall SR, Cruikshank SJ, Connors BW. A corticothalamic switch: controlling the thalamus with dynamic synapses. Neuron. 2015;86:768–782. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ahrens S, et al. ErbB4 regulation of a thalamic reticular nucleus circuit for sensory selection. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:104–111. doi: 10.1038/nn.3897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kuner R, Kuner T. Cellular circuits in the brain and their modulation in acute and chronic pain. Physiol. Rev. 2021;101:213–258. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00040.2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Prescott SA, Ma Q, De Koninck Y. Normal and abnormal coding of somatosensory stimuli causing pain. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:183–191. doi: 10.1038/nn.3629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baliki MN, Apkarian AV. Nociception, pain, negative moods, and behavior selection. Neuron. 2015;87:474–491. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Vierck CJ, Whitsel BL, Favorov OV, Brown AW, Tommerdahl M. Role of primary somatosensory cortex in the coding of pain. Pain. 2013;154:334–344. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Guo K, Yamawaki N, Barrett JM, Tapies M, Shepherd GMG. Cortico-thalamo-cortical circuits of mouse forelimb S1 are organized primarily as recurrent loops. J. Neurosci. 2020;40:2849–2858. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2277-19.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Patel R, Dickenson AH. Neuronal hyperexcitability in the ventral posterior thalamus of neuropathic rats: modality selective effects of pregabalin. J. Neurophysiol. 2016;116:159–170. doi: 10.1152/jn.00237.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Iwata M, et al. High-frequency stimulation in the ventral posterolateral thalamus reverses electrophysiologic changes and hyperalgesia in a rat model of peripheral neuropathic pain. Pain. 2011;152:2505–2513. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.LeBlanc BW, et al. Thalamic bursts down-regulate cortical theta and nociceptive behavior. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:2482. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02753-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Song Y, et al. Feedforward and feedback pathways of nociceptive and tactile processing in human somatosensory system: a study of dynamic causal modeling of fMRI data. Neuroimage. 2021;234:117957. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lenz FA, Kwan HC, Dostrovsky JO, Tasker RR. Characteristics of the bursting pattern of action potentials that occurs in the thalamus of patients with central pain. Brain Res. 1989;496:357–360. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91088-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gerke MB, Duggan AW, Xu L, Siddall PJ. Thalamic neuronal activity in rats with mechanical allodynia following contusive spinal cord injury. Neuroscience. 2003;117:715–722. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00961-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hains BC, Saab CY, Waxman SG. Changes in electrophysiological properties and sodium channel Nav1.3 expression in thalamic neurons after spinal cord injury. Brain. 2005;128:2359–2371. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wang J-Y, et al. Corticofugal influences on thalamic neurons during nociceptive transmission in awake rats. Synapse. 2007;61:335–342. doi: 10.1002/syn.20375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Xiong W, et al. Enhancing excitatory activity of somatosensory cortex alleviates neuropathic pain through regulating homeostatic plasticity. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:12743. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12972-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Monconduit L, Lopez-Avila A, Molat J-L, Chalus M, Villanueva L. Corticofugal output from the primary somatosensory cortex selectively modulates innocuous and noxious inputs in the rat spinothalamic system. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:8441–8450. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1293-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gross J, Schnitzler A, Timmermann L, Ploner M. Gamma oscillations in human primary somatosensory cortex reflect pain perception. PLoS Biol. 2007;5:e133. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tan LL, et al. Gamma oscillations in somatosensory cortex recruit prefrontal and descending serotonergic pathways in aversion and nociception. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:983. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-08873-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Okada T, et al. Pain induces stable, active microcircuits in the somatosensory cortex that provide a therapeutic target. Sci. Adv. 2021;7:eabd8261. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abd8261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Groh A, Krieger P, Mease RA, Henderson L. Acute and chronic pain processing in the thalamocortical system of humans and animal models. Neuroscience. 2018;387:58–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tan LL, Kuner R. Neocortical circuits in pain and pain relief. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2021;22:458–471. doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00468-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rainville P, Duncan GH, Price DD, Carrier B, Bushnell MC. Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science. 1997;277:968–971. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bushnell MC, et al. Pain perception: is there a role for primary somatosensory cortex? Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1999;96:7705–7709. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.7705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Uhelski ML, Davis MA, Fuchs PN. Pain affect in the absence of pain sensation: evidence of asomaesthesia after somatosensory cortex lesions in the rat. Pain. 2012;153:885–892. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Tan LL, et al. A pathway from midcingulate cortex to posterior insula gates nociceptive hypersensitivity. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:1591–1601. doi: 10.1038/nn.4645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Fields HL. Pain and the primary somatosensory cortex. Pain. 2012;153:742–743. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.01.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Canavero S, Bonicalzi V. Role of primary somatosensory cortex in the coding of pain. Pain. 2013;154:1156–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.02.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kim J, Matney CJ, Blankenship A, Hestrin S, Brown SP. Layer 6 corticothalamic neurons activate a cortical output layer, layer 5a. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:9656–9664. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1325-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Deuis JR, Dvorakova LS, Vetter I. Methods used to evaluate pain behaviors in rodents. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2017;10:284. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2017.00284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bradman MJG, Ferrini F, Salio C, Merighi A. Practical mechanical threshold estimation in rodents using von Frey hairs/Semmes–Weinstein monofilaments: towards a rational method. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2015;255:92–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Boada MD, Woodbury CJ. Myelinated skin sensory neurons project extensively throughout adult mouse substantia gelatinosa. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:2006–2014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5609-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Boada, M. D., Danilo Boada, M. & Jeffery Woodbury, C. Physiological properties of mouse skin sensory neurons recorded intracellularly in vivo: temperature effects on somal membrane properties. J. Neurophysiol.98, 668–680 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 47.Fehrenbacher, J. C., Vasko, M. R. & Duarte, D. B. Models of inflammation: Carrageenan- or complete Freund’s adjuvant (CFA)-induced edema and hypersensitivity in the rat. Curr. Protoc. Pharmacol. Chapter 5, Unit5.4 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 48.Kirchgessner MA, Franklin AD, Callaway EM. Distinct “driving” versus “modulatory” influences of different visual corticothalamic pathways. Curr. Biol. 2021;31:5121–5137.e7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bruno RM, Sakmann B. Cortex is driven by weak but synchronously active thalamocortical synapses. Science. 2006;312:1622–1627. doi: 10.1126/science.1124593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.de Kock CPJ, Bruno RM, Spors H, Sakmann B. Layer- and cell-type-specific suprathreshold stimulus representation in rat primary somatosensory cortex. J. Physiol. 2007;581:139–154. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.124321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Vélez-Fort M, et al. The stimulus selectivity and connectivity of layer six principal cells reveals cortical microcircuits underlying visual processing. Neuron. 2014;83:1431–1443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Pauzin FP, Krieger P. A corticothalamic circuit for refining tactile encoding. Cell Rep. 2018;23:1314–1325. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.03.128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Mease RA, Gonzalez AJ. Corticothalamic pathways from layer 5: emerging roles in computation and pathology. Front. Neural Circuits. 2021;15:88. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.730211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Mahn M, et al. High-efficiency optogenetic silencing with soma-targeted anion-conducting channelrhodopsins. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:4125. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06511-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Onodera K, Kato HK. Translaminar recurrence from layer 5 suppresses superficial cortical layers. Nat. Commun. 2022;13:1–16. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-30349-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Alhadeff AL, et al. A neural circuit for the suppression of pain by a competing need state. Cell. 2018;173:140–152.e15. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.02.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Zhou W, et al. Sound induces analgesia through corticothalamic circuits. Science. 2022;377:198–204. doi: 10.1126/science.abn4663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Sillito AM, Jones HE, Gerstein GL, West DC. Feature-linked synchronization of thalamic relay cell firing induced by feedback from the visual cortex. Nature. 1994;369:479–482. doi: 10.1038/369479a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kirchgessner MA, Franklin AD, Callaway EM. Context-dependent and dynamic functional influence of corticothalamic pathways to first- and higher-order visual thalamus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2020;117:13066–13077. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2002080117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Park A, Li Y, Masri R, Keller A. Presynaptic and extrasynaptic regulation of posterior nucleus of thalamus. J. Neurophysiol. 2017;118:507–519. doi: 10.1152/jn.00862.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Harris KD, Shepherd GMG. The neocortical circuit: themes and variations. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:170–181. doi: 10.1038/nn.3917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Liu Y, et al. Touch and tactile neuropathic pain sensitivity are set by corticospinal projections. Nature. 2018;561:547–550. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0515-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Moreno-Lopez, Y., Bichara, C., Delbecq, G., Isope, P. & Cordero-Erausquin, M. The corticospinal tract primarily modulates sensory inputs in the mouse lumbar cord. Elife10, e65304 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 64.Clayton KK, et al. Auditory corticothalamic neurons are recruited by motor preparatory inputs. Curr. Biol. 2021;31:310–321.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.10.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Dash S, Autio DM, Crandall SR. State-dependent modulation of activity in distinct layer 6 corticothalamic neurons in barrel cortex of awake mice. J. Neurosci. 2022;42:6551–6565. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2219-21.2022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Quiton RL, Masri R, Thompson SM, Keller A. Abnormal activity of primary somatosensory cortex in central pain syndrome. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;104:1717–1725. doi: 10.1152/jn.00161.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Chang Y, et al. Spatiotemporal characteristics of pain-associated neuronal activities in primary somatosensory cortex induced by peripheral persistent nociception. Neurosci. Lett. 2008;448:134–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.08.090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Ploner M, Sorg C, Gross J. Brain rhythms of pain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;xx:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Adesnik H. Layer-specific excitation/inhibition balances during neuronal synchronization in the visual cortex. J. Physiol. 2018;596:1639–1657. doi: 10.1113/JP274986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Moseley GL, Flor H. Targeting cortical representations in the treatment of chronic pain: a review. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2012;26:646–652. doi: 10.1177/1545968311433209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Cichon J, Blanck TJJ, Gan W-B, Yang G. Activation of cortical somatostatin interneurons prevents the development of neuropathic pain. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:1122–1132. doi: 10.1038/nn.4595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Deisseroth, K. Predicted irradiance values: model based on direct measurements in mammalian brain tissue. Preprint at https://web.stanford.edu/group/dlab/cgi-bin/graph/chart.php (2012).
- 73.Hohmann AG, Martin WJ, Tsou K, Walker JM. Inhibition of noxious stimulus-evoked activity of spinal cord dorsal horn neurons by the cannabinoid WIN 55,212-2. Life Sci. 1995;56:2111–2118. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(95)00196-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Steinmetz, N. A. et al. Neuropixels 2.0: a miniaturized high-density probe for stable, long-term brain recordings. Science372, eabf4588 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 75.Schmitt LI, et al. Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature. 2017;545:219–223. doi: 10.1038/nature22073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Wallstrom G, Liebner J, Kass RE. An implementation of Bayesian adaptive regression splines (BARS) in C with S and R wrappers. J. Stat. Softw. 2008;26:1–21. doi: 10.18637/jss.v026.i01. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Montijn JS, et al. A parameter-free statistical test for neuronal responsiveness. Elife. 2021;10:e71969. doi: 10.7554/eLife.71969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Ziegler, K. et al. Data for “Primary somatosensory cortex bidirectionally modulates sensory gain and nociceptive behavior in a layer-specific manner [research Data]”. heiDATA. 10.11588/DATA/D2O0JZ (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 79.Mease, R., Folkard, R. & Gonzalez, A. J. rebecca-mease/Ziegler_et_al_2023: Ziegler_et_al_2023. Zenodo10.5281/zenodo.7920058 (2023).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All the data are in the manuscript or in the supplementary material. Source data is provided as a Source Data file with this paper and under the following link: 10.11588/data/D2O0JZ78. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code necessary to reproduce the Matlab-generated figures in this study are provided and maintained at https://github.com/rebecca-mease/Ziegler_et_al_202379.