Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2023 Jun 2;18(6):e0286746. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286746

Higher operating theatre temperature during burn surgery increases physiological heat strain, subjective workload, and fatigue of surgical staff

Zehra Palejwala 1,*, Karen E Wallman 1,#, Shane Maloney 1,#, Grant J Landers 1,#, Ullrich K H Ecker 2, Mark W Fear 3, Fiona M Wood 4
Editor: Samuel Penna Wanner5
PMCID: PMC10237492  PMID: 37267345

Abstract

Raising the ambient temperature of the operating theatre is common practice during burn surgeries to maintain the patient’s core body temperature; however, the effects of operating in the heat on cognitive performance, manual dexterity, and perceived workload of surgical staff have not been assessed in a real-world context. Therefore, the aim was to assess the real-time impact of heat during burn surgeries on staff’s cognitive function, manual dexterity, and perceptual measures (workload, thermal sensation, thermal comfort, perceived exertion, and fatigue) and physiological parameters (core temperature, heart-rate, fluid loss, and dehydration). Ten burn surgery staff members were assessed in CON (24.0±1.1°C, 45±6% relative humidity [RH]) and HOT (30.8±1.6°C, 39±7% RH) burn surgeries (average 150 min duration). Cognitive performance, manual dexterity, and perceptual measures were recorded pre- and post-surgery, while physiological parameters were recorded throughout surgery. HOT conditions did not significantly affect manual dexterity or cognitive function (p > .05), however HOT resulted in heat strain (increased heart-rate, core temperature, and fluid loss: p < .05), and increased subjective workload, discomfort, perceived exertion, and fatigue compared to CON conditions (p < .05). Cognitive function and manual dexterity were maintained in hot conditions, suggesting that operating in approximately 31°C heat is a safe approach for patient treatment. However, job burnout, which is positively correlated with perceived workload, and the impact of cumulative fatigue on the mental health of surgery staff, must be considered in the context of supporting an effective health workforce.

Introduction

Major burn surgeries (usually ≥ 20% total body surface area [TBSA]) are typically conducted in ambient temperatures of 30–40°C to prevent patients from developing intraoperative hypothermia [1]. This can improve patient outcomes; however, patient outcomes also depend on the cognitive function [2], manual dexterity/technical skills [3], and fatigue levels of surgical teams [4].

Heat exposure can lead to heat and cardiovascular strain [5], and dehydration if fluids are not adequately replaced, all of which can impair physical and cognitive function [69], specifically complex decision-making tasks such as those involved during surgery [10, 11]. Dehydration, heat strain, and cardiovascular strain are further exacerbated when individuals wear personal protective equipment (PPE) in hot ambient conditions [12], which is a major concern for surgery staff.

Perceptions of workload are consistently higher in hot temperatures [13]. In both warm (26°C) and hot (34°C) compared to thermoneutral (19–23°C) operating theatres (OT), the physical, mental, and temporal demand of surgery tasks can increase [2, 14, 15], as well as the surgery team’s subjective discomfort [16]. An increase in perceived workload is correlated with burnout, especially in the health care sector [17]. This is important as burnt-out employees often have poor mental health and an increased risk for cardiovascular disease [18, 19]. In a 2.5 h burn surgery simulation, executive functioning and verbal reasoning were impaired in a hot (34°C) compared to a cooler (23°C) OT [2], while manual dexterity scores tended to be lower [2, 16]. Together, these physical and cognitive effects represent a concern for surgery staff who have their own and the patients’ welfare at risk. However, the impact of the heat on burn surgical teams has not been previously measured in a real-world (not-simulated) context.

Therefore, the aim of this study was to compare the impact of operating in hot ambient conditions (HOT) compared to control conditions (CON) on burn surgery staff, during real-life surgeries. It was hypothesised that operating in a hot theatre would result in heat strain, subsequently impairing manual dexterity and cognitive function, while increasing subjective workload and fatigue.

Materials and methods

Participants

Surgical staff from a burns department were recruited in the winter (June—October 2021; when average maximum ambient temperature was 20°C) to minimise the possibility of acclimatisation/acclimation, for testing in CON and HOT conditions. Descriptive statistics are provided in S1 Table. All staff gave written consent for participation in this study and patients gave either written or verbal consent (written consent was not able to be provided by all patients because of the nature and location of the burn injury). Verbal consent was witnessed by a member of the surgical burns team who then signed and dated the consent form, attesting that the requirements for informed consent were satisfied. Ethical approval was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Western Australia (2020/ET000239) and the South Metropolitan Health Service Human Research Ethics Committee (PRN RGS0000004250).

Experimental design

The staff were assessed in thermoneutral (CON; 24°C) and heated (HOT; 31°C) surgeries, which all commenced between 8:30–9:30am. Ten staff members were recruited for participation in this study, of which seven were tested in both conditions. The remaining three were tested in HOT only. There were 22 observations in the CON condition and 18 in HOT. Testing in the CON condition occasionally included two/three back-to-back cases, with staff remaining in theatre until the conclusion of the final case, to match testing times. No patients became hypothermic during surgery in either the CON or HOT condition. Staff wore the same standard surgical clothing and PPE (scrub gown, gloves, scrub hat, surgical mask) for each trial. S2 Table summarises the testing regime.

Familiarisation session

Staff attended a familiarisation session approximately one week prior to being assessed during surgery, where they were made familiar with the questionnaires and practiced the performance tests to prevent a practice effect from occurring while testing [20]. All staff provided information on weekly heat exposure and physical activity via the International Physical Activity Questionnaire [21], to determine acclimatisation/acclimation status. The average, maximum, outdoor temperature during the testing period was 20°C and no staff had travelled to a warmer climate in recent months prior to testing. Within the four months of testing, staff were exposed to a heated OT on 5 occurrences, with the average duration of exposure being 158 min, equating to a total average of 50 min per week. The average amount of recreational physical activity (not including job-related, indoor physical activity i.e. walking within the hospital) was 5 hours weekly, with most reporting light, as opposed to moderate and high intensity physical activity. Thus, no staff were determined to be acclimatised/acclimated. Female staff provided information on their menstrual cycle and contraception use so to determine differences in menstrual cycle phase during surgery. Height (cm) and body-mass (kg) were recorded and then the staff were familiarised with the testing equipment including heart-rate (HR) monitors (Polar RS400, Finland), digital platform weighing scales (SOEHNLE, Style sense comfort 100, Digital & Anko Glass Electronic), and the refractometer (for determining urine specific gravity; USG: ATAGO MASTER-URC/Na, Tokyo, Japan). Values obtained for USG were classified as ‘well hydrated’ <1.010, ‘minimal dehydration’ = 1.010–1.020, ‘significant dehydration’ = 1.021–1.030 and ‘serious dehydration’ >1.030 [22]. It is important to note that measurement of USG may not reflect plasma osmolality [23], the most efficient measure to assess hydration status, and so the classifications provided may not be accurate in illustrating the extent of hypohydration. Staff were also provided with an ingestible core-temperature (TCORE) pill (CorTemp, HQ Inc., Palmetto, USA).

Protocol

Four to eight hours prior to surgery, staff ingested a pill that objectively measured TCORE [24]. Upon arrival to the hospital, staff provided a urine sample to determine USG. In private, nude body mass was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg using a digital platform scale (details provided above) and following this, staff were fitted with a HR monitor. In the OT, prior to surgery, both performance tests and all questionnaires, except the SURG-TLX, were completed. Staff then exited the OT (~2–3 min) to scrub before beginning surgery, and then fulfilled their usual roles within the OT. An initial (baseline) TCORE and HR measurements were taken as soon as surgery commenced and at 15-min intervals throughout surgery. Once surgery began, staff remained in the OT and did not consume any food/fluids until after the final measures were recorded at the conclusion of the surgery. All questionnaires and performance tasks were re-completed upon completion of surgery, followed by the assessment of nude body to determine fluid loss (pre nude body mass–post nude body mass) and the collection of a final urine sample.

Perceptual questionnaires

Thermal sensation (TS) and thermal comfort (TC) were rated using 20-point scales from ‘very cold’ to ‘very hot’, and ‘very comfortable’ to ‘very uncomfortable’ [25], respectively. A score of 10 indicates optimal thermal sensation and comfort. Perceived exertion (RPE) was rated using the Borg 6-20-point scale which ranges from ‘no exertion at all’ to ‘maximal exertion’ [26]. Perceived workload was assessed using the SURG-TLX [27], a surgery-specific workload measure adapted from the NASA-TLX workload scale [28], which assesses workload over seven domains (mental demand, physical demand, temporal demand, task complexity, situational stress, distractions, and frustration) on a scale of 0 = very low to 100 = very high. With these instruments, a higher score indicates poorer health.

Performance tests

The function of working memory–a core component of executive capacity–was measured by the counting span task (millisecond software) on a laptop [29]. In this test, participants are presented with cards featuring a number of both target dots (green: 3–9) and distractor dots (yellow: 3–9); participants count the number of target dots, press the corresponding key on a keyboard, and remember the count number. After a certain number of cards (starting with a set size of 3 and going up to 7, with two trials per set size), participants recall the counts (i.e., the number of dots they counted for each card) in order, starting with the first card (i.e., serial recall). The test ends when an individual fails to successfully recall the sequence on both trials of a particular set size (i.e., the number of cards presented depends on performance). Measures obtained from this task were the number of correct counting responses and counting latencies (in milliseconds [ms]), number of correct recall responses and recall latencies, and the counting span score (i.e., the highest span level at which participant correctly recalled 2 out of 3 sets). As such, measures included those that relate to basic counting performance as well as those assessing working memory capacity; for this reason, the term ‘cognitive function’ has been used to refer to these measures collectively. The highest number of correct responses that can be achieved for both the counting and recall tasks is 54; the highest possible counting span score is 7.

Manual dexterity was assessed by the Purdue pegboard task (60 s). In this test, participants were required to pick up one pin at a time and place as many pins in the holes of a board in 30 s, starting from the top hole and the dominant hand, progressing to the non-dominant hand once the 30 s were complete [30].

Statistical analysis

Analysis was conducted using R Studio (Version 1.4.1717 for Windows). Linear mixed-models (within and between subjects) were used to assess all dependent variables, across all time points and in both conditions. All outputs were produced by running linear regression models (obtained using the lmer function) with random intercepts for individual participants, through the anova test function. This function removes missing observations, i.e. a complete case analysis was performed. One-way ANOVAs were used to assess differences in surgery duration and TBSA. Follow-up post hoc comparisons using Tukey adjustments were used. Significance was accepted at p ≤ .05. All results presented within the written text and tables are expressed as mean ± SD and all figures are presented as individual data points or mean ± SEM. Cohen’s d effect sizes (ES) with ±95% confidence intervals (CI) were also calculated, with effects ≥ 0.8 representing large, 0.5–0.79 moderate, and ≤ 0.49 small effects, respectively [31]. Only moderate to large ES are reported.

Results

Environmental conditions were 24.0±1.1°C, 45±6% RH for the CON trials, and 30.8±1.6°C, 39±7% RH for the HOT trials. Surgery duration was not different between conditions (CON: 141 ± 50 min, HOT: 158 ± 51 min; p = .287). Burn injury TBSA of patients was not different between conditions (CON: 8±13%, HOT: 20±7%; p = .053). Of the seven females tested, three were post-menopausal, two were using an intrauterine device which meant that their menstrual cycle phase was unidentifiable, one was only tested once in the follicular phase, and one was in the follicular phase during the first testing session and the luteal phase during the second.

Counting task

There was no effect of theatre temperature on counting latency (p = .836); however, the main effect of time on counting latency approached significance (p = .060), meaning that post-surgery, response times tended to be faster than pre-surgery. There was no effect of theatre temperature on number of correct responses (p = .483), and there was no pre-/post-surgery difference on number of correct responses (p = .427). There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on counting latency (p = .203; Fig 1) or the number of correct responses (p = .757; Table 1).

Fig 1. Counting latencies ‘pre’ (A) and ‘post’ (B) surgery for target-dot numbers 3 to 9 in CON (Pre; n = 13, Post; n = 18) and HOT (n = 18) surgeries.

Fig 1

bindicates moderate to large effect size between pre and post in HOT (d = -0.52 to -0.80); cindicates moderate to large effect size between HOT and CON trials at specified target-dot numbers (d = 0.52 to 0.70). Data sets on the x-axis are staggered to prevent overlap of error bars. Each point shows Mean ± SEM.

Table 1. Cognitive scores and number of participants for the counting span task pre and post-surgery in CON and HOT surgeries.

Counting task Recall Task
# of correct responses # of correct responses Counting span score
PRE POST PRE POST PRE POST
CON 45 ± 13 47 ± 12 42 ± 11 41 ± 11 5.6 ± 0.9 5.6 ± 1.2
n 13 18 13 18 21 21
HOT 44 ± 13 49 ± 9 39 ± 14 40 ± 10 5.3 ± 1.5 5.3 ± 1.0
n 18 18 18 18 18 18

All data expressed as Mean ± SD

Recall task

There was no effect of theatre temperature on recall latency (p = .623); however, there was a difference between recall latency pre and post-surgery (p = .045), indicating that response times were faster post-surgery. There was no effect of theatre temperature on number of correct responses (p = .964), and there was no pre-/post-surgery difference on number of correct responses (p = .657). There was no effect of theatre temperature on overall counting span score (p = .998), and no difference in scores pre and post-surgery (p = .990). There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on recall latency (p = .821; Fig 2), number of correct responses (p = .828; Table 1), or overall counting span score (p = .949; Table 1).

Fig 2. Recall latencies ‘pre’ (A) and ‘post’ (B) surgery for serial recall positions 1 to 7 in CON (Pre; n = 13, Post; n = 18) and HOT (n = 18) surgeries.

Fig 2

*indicates response numbers that are significantly different from 1st response (p < .05); c indicates moderate to large effect size between HOT and CON trials at specified set size (d = 0.74 to 0.89). Data sets on the x-axis are staggered to prevent overlap of error bars. Data are noisy at serial recall positions 6 and 7 because only few trials had a set size > 5 (whereas all trials had serial positions 1 and 2 and many trials had positions 3–5) and because not every participant made it to a counting span of 6 or 7. Each point shows Mean ± SEM.

Manual dexterity

There was no effect of theatre temperature on manual dexterity when using the dominant hand (p = .460) or the non-dominant hand (p = .099). There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on manual dexterity in either the dominant hand (p = .428) or the non- dominant hand (p = .949). However, when using the dominant hand there was a difference between manual dexterity pre and post-surgery (p = .015), indicating an improvement over time (S3 Table).

Perceptual responses

There was a significant effect of theatre temperature on TS (p = .002), TC (p < .001), and RPE (p < .001), indicating that staff felt hotter, more uncomfortable, and were exerting themselves more in the heat. For all measures, scores post-surgery were significantly higher than pre-surgery: TS (p < .001), TC (p < .001), and RPE (p < .001; Fig 3). There was an interaction between theatre temperature and time on TS (p = .019), TC (p = .047), and RPE (p < .001). The interaction supported that scores post-surgery were higher in HOT than CON for all perceptual measures (p < .001). It also revealed that ratings of TS and RPE were the same pre-surgery in CON and HOT (p > .924), but a difference in ratings of TC existed before surgery (HOT: 11 ± 3, CON: 8 ± 4; p = .025).

Fig 3. Perceptual responses ‘pre’ and ‘post’ surgery in CON (n = 22) and HOT (n = 18) surgeries; thermal sensation (A), thermal comfort (B), and perceived exertion (C).

Fig 3

*indicates significant difference between conditions pre-surgery (p < .05); ***indicates significant difference between conditions post-surgery (p < .001). Individual and mean data shown.

There was a significant main effect of theatre temperature on all domains of the SURG-TLX questionnaire, indicating that scores were significantly higher in the HOT compared to the CON condition (Fig 4). These domains included self-reported levels of mental demand (p = .001), physical demand (p < .001), temporal demand (p = .007), task complexity (p < .001), situational stress (p < .001), level of distraction (p < .001), and frustration (p < .001).

Fig 4. Scores for each dimension of the task load index in CON (n = 22) and HOT (n = 18) surgeries; mental demand (Men), physical demand (Phy), temporal demand (Tem), task complexity (Task), situation stress (Sit), distractions (Dist), and frustration (Frust).

Fig 4

**indicates significant difference between conditions (p < .01); ***indicates significant difference between conditions (p < .001). Results are presented as mean ± SEM.

Physiological parameters

There was a significant effect of theatre temperature on TCORE (p < .001) and HR (p < .001), while only TCORE increased over time during surgery (p < .001; Fig 5). There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on TCORE (p = .138) or HR (p = .700).

Fig 5. Core temperature (A) and heart rate (B) responses at 15-min intervals in CON (n = 22) and HOT (n = 18) surgeries.

Fig 5

***indicates significant difference between conditions (p < .001); n.b Time points beyond 180 min were removed from the plots as the sample size beyond 180 min was too small (n < 5) to accurately represent the trend in core temperature. Data points are staggered to prevent overlap of error bars. Each point shows Mean ± SEM.

There was a significant effect of theatre temperature on decrease in body-mass in kg (p = .008) and as a % of total body-mass (p < .001), in that the decrease in body-mass over time was greater in the HOT condition (Fig 6). The difference between rate of decrease in body mass between CON and HOT approached significance (p = .052). There was no effect of theatre temperature on USG scores (p = .338); however, scores significantly increased over time (p < .001), indicating a greater degree of dehydration post-surgery compared to pre-surgery. There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on USG scores (p = .138; Fig 6). Post-surgery, USG scores had a tendency to be higher in HOT compared to CON, as shown by a moderate effect size (d = 0.50 [-0.22, 1.15]). The number of staff members in each hydration category pre and post-surgery is provided in S4 Table.

Fig 6. Fluid loss and hydration in CON (n = 22) and HOT (n = 17) surgeries; Decrease in body mass in kg (A), decrease in body mass in % of total body mass (B), rate of decrease in body mass (C), and USG scores (D).

Fig 6

**indicates significant difference between conditions pre-surgery (p < .01); ***indicates significant difference between conditions post-surgery (p < .001); c indicates a moderate effect size between USG scores in HOT and CON ‘post’ surgery (d = 0.50). Individual and mean data shown.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the effects of operating in the heat during real-time burn surgery. There were no statistically significant differences between conditions for any performance variable assessed and in general, these findings did not support our hypothesis that working in the heat would impair manual dexterity and cognitive function. Higher levels of fatigue and subjective workload found in the hot surgeries support our second hypothesis that the heat would negatively affect perceptual responses in staff, most likely due to the higher TCORE, HR, and fluid loss in the HOT condition, either alone or in combination.

Manual dexterity in both hands was similar between conditions. Similarly, researchers have reported no difference in dexterity scores on the O’Conner test when ambient conditions of 20 and 30°C were compared, although a significantly lower score was reported at a lower temperature of 10°C [32], possibly because cold stress, as opposed to heat stress, tends to impair manual dexterity as it decreases maximum voluntary grip strength [33]. Improvements in manual dexterity in the dominant hand over time, as found in this study, were also seen by Palejwala and colleagues [16] who attributed the improvement to various mechanisms such as decreased stiffness of muscle fibres during contraction, and reduced muscle and joint viscous resistance [34, 35].

Latency on the counting task tended to improve over time while latency for the recall task significantly improved over time in both conditions, which may be due to an increase in motor nerve conduction velocity that accompanies an increased TCORE [36], and an increase in arousal via activation of thermoregulatory mechanisms [37]. Scores on the counting span task, accuracy on counting and recall task, counting latency, and recall latency did not differ between conditions. Heat exposure can cause cognitive impairment, but the average TCORE of our staff in the heat did not exceed 38.5°C, the temperature at which cognitive tasks that require working memory tend to become impaired [38]. Cognitive function also may have remained unaffected in the heat because the average % loss in body-mass was less than 2%, the critical level at which impairments to cognitive performance are commonly seen [39]. Specifically, our findings in relation to cognitive accuracy and latency are in contrast to findings of Ward and colleagues [2] who reported impaired accuracy and slowed response times in hot, surgical simulations. However, the ambient temperature in the hot simulations [2] was 34°C compared to the 30.8°C recorded in the current study. It is possible that the stronger heat stimulus in the simulations contributed to the difference, which would indicate that burn surgeries conducted in higher temperatures of up to 40°C [1], a common clinical protocol, could be of concern to the cognitive function of staff. In general, people with a high skill level, who perform tasks that are familiar or autonomous in nature, are able to withstand the effects of heat stress [40] and are therefore less susceptible to interference between stimulus and response [41, 42]. Because surgery staff are highly skilled individuals and are likely to be highly motivated, it is possible that these factors helped maintain their performance on the cognitive tasks despite increased perceptual responses to the heat. Heated surgeries of longer duration, where TCORE and fluid loss are likely to increase, may impair cognitive performance.

Ratings of TS, TC, and RPE were all higher post-surgery in the HOT compared to CON condition, and these findings are supported by the literature providing evidence for perceptual measures being affected by TCORE, HR, and fluid loss [6, 4345], all of which were significantly higher in the HOT condition. As expected, the perceived workload was significantly higher in the HOT compared to the CON condition, which aligns with previous findings that subjective workload was greater during surgical simulations in a hot, compared to cooler environment [2]. An increase in perceived workload has been correlated with burnout, especially in the health care sector [17], which is of great importance as burnt out employees often have poor mental health and an increased risk for cardiovascular disease [18, 19]. However, it is important to note that if heated burn surgeries are not a frequent occurrence, staff may not experience the burnout-related effects of continuously working in a thermally stressful environment.

Notably, the physical demand of the surgical task, i.e., how physically fatiguing the procedure was, was higher in the HOT compared to CON theatre. Increased levels of fatigue reported during surgery could be of significant consequence in the health care industry, since fatigue is associated with an increased risk of medical errors [46, 47], carelessness among health care workers [48], and impairment to physical and mental performance during simulated medical work [49]. The level of distractions reported was also higher in the HOT compared to the CON theatre. Previously, health care workers reported that wearing PPE made them feel hot and uncomfortable at work [50], and this discomfort, coupled with the heat stress, could distract staff from their tasks. Distractions in an OT are common, but when exacerbated by heat stress, they can have a cumulative effect and possibly impact on staffs’ vigilance and in turn impair operative performance [51]. In the OT, distractions and frustration can negatively affect technical performance, with staff feeling clumsy, shaky, less dexterous, and making mistakes including badly placed stitches [52]. This can have serious implications on surgical procedures and therefore patient outcomes.

Limitations

Limitations of this study include the small sample size, and that staff were recruited from the same hospital, which introduces sampling error and reduces generalisability. Food and fluid intake as well as activities undertaken the night before surgery were not controlled for, as some staff were on call the night before, but this accurately reflects the real-world environment and job demands in health care. Exposure time in the OT was less than previous simulation time [2] in which performance differences were found, indicating that longer, real-time surgeries in the heat may lead to performance decrements. Further, the complexity of surgeries in the environmental conditions was not matched (surgeries conducted in the heat are more time sensitive in nature due to the difficulty in controlling patients body temperature [53]), which adds bias to the perception of workload, however surgery duration and TBSA was similar between conditions.

Practical implications

The maintenance of cognitive function and manual dexterity in the heat demonstrates that burn surgery staff can maintain their working memory function and manual dexterity despite the effects of heat stress, however heat exposure can increase mental workload [13]. Long-term, continuous work with a high mental workload is correlated with cumulative fatigue and job burnout, especially in the health care sector [17], which may impact the workforce. Alleviating symptoms of heat strain in burn surgery staff should be a priority and could be achieved by taking small breaks during surgery, using underbody warming devices for the patient (warming mattresses) as opposed to heating the OT, or cooling technologies for staff. For example, head cooling caps [54] and cooling vests [55] have been found to lower perceptual heat strain and may be able to do the same for surgery staff working in the heat, thereby lowering their mental workload.

Conclusions

This study showed that cognitive function and manual dexterity was maintained while operating in the heat, however subjective workload and fatigue increased, possibly due to heat strain. Our results suggest that it would be beneficial to consider fatigue/the physical demand of tasks and mental workload in the work design for major burns (heated surgeries). Future research should 1. build on this study and assess cumulative fatigue in burn surgery staff over a longer period of time, and 2. find the optimal temperature for burns OTs in which performance can be maintained while considering factors to lower the level of heat strain and workload of staff.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Participant numbers and demographic information for CON and HOT surgery conditions (mean ± SD).

(PDF)

S2 Table. Assignment of participants to surgeries (Sx) in both environmental conditions.

(PDF)

S3 Table. Scores for the Purdue pegboard task pre and post-surgery in CON (n = 22) and HOT (n = 18) surgeries for the dominant and non-dominant hand.

All data expressed as Mean ± SD.

(PDF)

S4 Table. Number and percentage of participants n (%) in each hydration status category ‘pre’ and ‘post’ surgery, in CON and HOT surgeries.

(PDF)

S1 File. Core temperature and heart rate data.

(XLSX)

S2 File. USG and sweat loss data.

(XLSX)

S3 File. Pegboard data.

(XLSX)

S4 File. Cognitive scores data.

(XLSX)

S5 File. Cognitive latency scores.

(XLSX)

S6 File. Perceptual responses data.

(XLSX)

S7 File. Temperature and humidity data.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the staff at Fiona Stanley Hospital for their participation, and statistician Martin Firth for his consultation.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.

Funding Statement

Funding - This study was funded by the Fiona Wood Foundation. Funding was received by ZP. There was no grant number. URL: https://www.fionawoodfoundation.com/ The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analyses, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Rizzo JA, Rowan MP, Driscoll IR, Chan RK, Chung KK. Perioperative temperature management during burn care. J Burn Care Res. 2017;38(1):e277–e283. doi: 10.1097/BCR.0000000000000371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ward MK, Yam CMH, Palejwala Z, Wallman K, Taggart SM, Wood FM, et al. An experimental simulation of heat effects on cognition and workload of surgical team members. Ann Surg. 2021;274(5):e395–e402. doi: 10.1097/SLA.0000000000004598 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kennedy-Metz LR, Wolfe HL, Dias RD, Yule SJ, Zenati MA. Surgery task load index in cardiac surgery: Measuring cognitive load among teams. Surg Innov. 2020;27(6):602–607. doi: 10.1177/1553350620934931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Porwal AC, Mathew BC, Abhishek P. Surgeon fatigue: A factor in intraoperative complications in high volume tertiary eye care centre. Indian J Ophthalmol. 2021;69(6):1634–1635. doi: 10.4103/ijo.IJO_3578_20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bouchama A, Abuyassin B, Lehe C, Laitano O, Jay O, O’Connor FG, et al. Classic and exertional heatstroke. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2022;8(1):8. doi: 10.1038/s41572-021-00334-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cian C, Koulmann N, Barraud PA, Raphel C, Jimenez C, Melin B. Influence of variations in body hydration on cognitive function: Effect of hyperhydration, heat stress, and exercise-induced dehydration. J Psychophysiol. 2000;14(1):29–36. doi: 10.1027//0269-8803.14.1.29 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gaoua N, Racinais S, Grantham J, El Massioui. Alterations in cognitive performance during passive hyperthermia are task dependent. Int J Hyperthermia. 2011;27(1):1–9. doi: 10.3109/02656736.2010.516305 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gregson W, Drust B, Batterham A, Cable NT. The effects of pre-warming on the metabolic and thermoregulatory responses to prolonged submaximal exercise in moderate ambient temperatures. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2002;86(6):526–533. doi: 10.1007/s00421-002-0580-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kraft JA, Green JM, Bishop PA, Richardson MT, Neggers YH, Leeper JD. Impact of dehydration on a full body resistance exercise protocol. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2010;109(2):259–267. doi: 10.1007/s00421-009-1348-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bandelow S, Maughan R, Shirreffs S, Ozgunen K, Kurdak S, Ersoz G, et al. The effects of exercise, heat, cooling and rehydration strategies on cognitive function in football players. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2010;20(Suppl 3):148–160. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01220.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Crebbin W, Beasley SW, Watters DAK. Clinical decision making: how surgeons do it. ANZ J Surg. 2013;83(6):422–428. doi: 10.1111/ans.12180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wang F, Gao C, Kuklane K, Holmer I. Effects of various protective clothing and thermal environments on heat strain of unacclimated men: the PHS (predicted heat strain) model revisited. Ind Health. 2013;51(3):266–274. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.2012-0073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kimura T, Takemura N, Nakashima Y, Kobori H, Nagahara H, Numao M, et al. Warmer environments increase implicit mental workload even if learning efficiency is enhanced. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00568 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Berg RJ, Inaba K, Sullivan M, Okoye O, Siboni S, Minneti M, et al. The impact of heat stress on operative performance and cognitive function during simulated laparoscopic operative tasks. Surgery. 2015;157(1):87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.surg.2014.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hakim M, Walia H, Dellinger HL, Balaban O, Saadat H, Kirschner RE, et al. The effect of operating room temperature on the performance of clinical and cognitive tasks. Pediatr Qual Saf. 2018;3(2):e069–e069. doi: 10.1097/pq9.0000000000000069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Palejwala Z, Wallman K, Ward MK, Yam C, Maroni T, Parker S, et al. Effects of a hot ambient operating theatre on manual dexterity, psychological and physiological parameters in staff during a simulated burn surgery. PLoS One. 2019;14(10): e0222923. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0222923 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bria M, Baban A, Dumitrascu DL. Systematic review of burnout risk factors among European healthcare professionals. Cogn Brain Behav. 2012;16(3):423–452. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Maslach C. Job burnout: New directions in research and intervention. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2003;12(5):189–192. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01258 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Melamed S, Shirom A, Toker S, Berliner S, Shapira I. Burnout and risk of cardiovascular disease: evidence, possible causal paths, and promising research directions. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(3):327–353. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Saldaris JM, Landers GJ, Lay BS. Enhanced decision making and working memory during exercise in the heat with crushed ice ingestion. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2019;15(4):503–510. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2019-0234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lee PH, Macfarlane DJ, Lam TH, Stewart SM. Validity of the international physical activity questionnaire short form (IPAQ-SF): A systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2011;8:115. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Casa DJ, Armstrong LE, Hillman SK, Montain SJ, Reiff RV, Rich BSE, et al. National athletic trainers’ association position statement: fluid replacement for athletes. J Athl Train. 2000;35(2):212–224. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pereira ER, de Andrade MT, Mendes TT, Ramos GP, Maia-Lima A, Melo ES, et al. Evaluation of hydration status by urine, body mass variation and plasma parameters during an official half-marathon. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. 2017;57(11):1499–1503. doi: 10.23736/S0022-4707.16.06836-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Byrne C, Lim CL. The ingestible telemetric body core temperature sensor: A review of validity and exercise applications. Br J Sports Med. 2007;41(3):126–133. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2006.026344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gaoua N, Grantham J, Racinais S, El Massioui F. Sensory displeasure reduces complex cognitive performance in the heat. J Environ Psychol. 2012;32(2):158–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.01.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Borg GA. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1982;14(5):377–381. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wilson MR, Poolton JM, Malhotra N, Ngo K, Bright E, Masters RSW. Development and validation of a surgical workload measure: the surgery task load index (SURG-TLX). World J Surg. 2011;35(9):1961–1969. doi: 10.1007/s00268-011-1141-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hart SG, Staveland LE. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. Adv Psychol. 1988;52:139–183. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62386-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Conway ARA, Kane MJ, Bunting MF, Hambrick DZ, Wilhelm O, Engle RW. Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychon Bull Rev. 2005;12(5):769–786. doi: 10.3758/bf03196772 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tiffin J, Asher EJ. The Purdue pegboard: norms and studies of reliability and validity. J Appl Psychol. 1948;32(3):234–247. doi: 10.1037/h0061266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. New York: Routledge; 1988. 10.4324/9780203771587 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dizmen C, Man KSS, Chan AHS. The effect of temperature on manual dexterity, reaction time, and optimum grip-span. In: 2015 Proceedings of the International MultiConference of Engineers and Computer Scientists, 2015 March 18–20, Hong Kong.
  • 33.Chi CF, Shih YC, Chen WL. Effect of cold immersion on grip force, EMG, and thermal discomfort. Int J Ind Ergon. 2012;42(1):113–121. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2011.08.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Asmussen E, Boje OVE. Body temperature and capacity for work. Acta Physiol Scand. 2008;10:1–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1945.tb00287.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Bishop D. Warm up I: potential mechanisms and the effects of passive warm up on exercise performance. Sports Med. 2003;33(6):439–454. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200333060-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Karvonen J, Lemon PWR. Importance of warm-up and cool down on exercise performance. Med Sport Sci. 1992;35:189–214. doi: 10.1159/000421152 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hancock PA, Vasmatzidis I. Effects of heat stress on cognitive performance: the current state of knowledge. Int J Hyperthermia. 2003;19(3):355–372. doi: 10.1080/0265673021000054630 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hocking C, Silberstein RB, Lau WM, Stough C, Roberts W. Evaluation of cognitive performance in the heat by functional brain imaging and psychometric testing. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol. 2001;128(4):719–734. doi: 10.1016/s1095-6433(01)00278-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Grandjean AC, Grandjean NR. Dehydration and cognitive performance. J Am Coll Nutr. 2007;26(Suppl 5):549S–554S. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2007.10719657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hüttermann S, Memmert D. Does the inverted-U function disappear in expert athletes? An analysis of the attentional behavior under physical exercise of athletes and non-athletes. Physiol Behav. 2014;131:87–92. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.MacLeod H, Cooper S, Bandelow S, Malcolm R, Sunderland C. Effects of heat stress and dehydration on cognitive function in elite female field hockey players. BMC Sports Sci Med Rehabil. 2018;10:1–13. doi: 10.1186/s13102-018-0101-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Simmons SE, Saxby BK, McGlone FP, Jones DA. The effect of passive heating and head cooling on perception, cardiovascular function and cognitive performance in the heat. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2008;104(2):271–280. doi: 10.1007/s00421-008-0677-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Grego F, Vallier JM, Collardeau M, Rousseu C, Cremieux J, Brisswalter J. Influence of exercise duration and hydration status on cognitive function during prolonged cycling exercise. Int J Sports Med. 2005;26(1):27–33. doi: 10.1055/s-2004-817915 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Malcolm RA, Cooper S, Folland JP, Tyler CJ, Sutherland C. Passive heat exposure alters perception and executive function. Front Physiol. 2018;9:585–585. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00585 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Russo LN, Rosalky DS, Robertson RJ, Rittenberger JC, Hostler D. Core temperature, thermal Sensation, and thermal comfort among firefighters during a live fire evolution. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46:689. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000495541.62896.57 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Baldwin DC, Daugherty SR. Sleep deprivation and fatigue in residency training: results of a national survey of first- and second-year residents. Sleep. 2004;27(2):217–223. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.2.217 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gawande AA, Zinner MJ, Studdert DM, Brennan TA. Analysis of errors reported by surgeons at three teaching hospitals. Surgery. 2003;133(6):614–621. doi: 10.1067/msy.2003.169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Papp KK, Stoller EP, Sage P, Aikens JE, Owens J, Avidan A, et al. The effects of sleep loss and fatigue on resident-physicians: a multi-institutional, mixed-method study. Acad Med. 2004;79(5):394–406. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200405000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Barker LM, Nussbaum MA. The effects of fatigue on performance in simulated nursing work. Ergonomics. 2011;54(9):815–829. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2011.597878 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Davey SL, Lee BJ, Robbins T, Randeva H, Thake CD. Heat stress and PPE during COVID-19: impact on healthcare workers’ performance, safety and well-being in NHS settings. J Hosp Infect. 2021;108:185–188. doi: 10.1016/j.jhin.2020.11.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Arora S, Sevdalis N, Nestel D, Woloshynowych M, Darzi A, Kneebone R. The impact of stress on surgical performance: a systematic review of the literature. Surgery. 2010;147(3):318–330. doi: 10.1016/j.surg.2009.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wetzel CM, Kneebone RL, Woloshynowych M, Nestel D, Moorthy K, Kidd J, et al. The effects of stress on surgical performance. Am J Surg. 2006;191(1):5–10. doi: 10.1016/j.amjsurg.2005.08.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Mullhi R, Ewington I, Chipp E, Torlinski T. A descriptive survey of operating theatre and intensive care unit temperature management of burn patients in the United Kingdom. Int J Burns Trauma. 2021;11(3):136–144. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Coelho LGM, Ferreira-Junior JB, Williams TB, Maia-Lima A, Borba DA, Silva CD, et al. Head pre-cooling improves 5-km time-trial performance in male amateur runners in the heat. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2021;31(9):1753–1763. doi: 10.1111/sms.13985 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.de Korte JQ, Bongers CCWG, Catoire M, Kingma BRM, Eijsvogels TMH. Cooling vests alleviate perceptual heat strain perceived by COVID-19 nurses. Temperature. 2022;9(1):103–111. doi: 10.1080/23328940.2020.1868386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Samuel Penna Wanner

13 Apr 2023

PONE-D-23-05820Higher operating theatre temperature during burn surgery increases subjective workload and fatigue as a result of heat strainPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Zehra Palejwala,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Three experts in the field reviewed the present study. The manuscript provides novel and exciting data and may be suitable for publication in PLOS One. However, it can still be improved (some issues should be clarified) before it is ready for publication. Although the reviewers did not indicate flaws or methodological issues that cannot be corrected in the revised manuscript, they have presented suggestions/corrections to all sections: from the title to the conclusions. Please take every comment very seriously and resubmit a better manuscript. This academic Editor is looking forward to receiving a revised and improved version of the manuscript for further analysis.

Please submit your revised manuscript by June 16th, 2023. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Samuel Penna Wanner, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

Once you have amended this/these statement(s) in the Methods section of the manuscript, please add the same text to the “Ethics Statement” field of the submission form (via “Edit Submission”).

For additional information about PLOS ONE ethical requirements for human subjects research, please refer to http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research.

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

I have nothing to add to the reviewers' comments.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors completed attractive work overall, with the aims and methods section being interesting. My comments are all minimal. However, I would like them to answer them. Thanks for the invitation to review this interesting work.

Reviewer #2: Palejwala and colleagues aimed to assess the real-time impact of heat during burn surgeries on staff’s cognitive performance, manual dexterity, and perceptual measures (workload, thermal sensation, thermal comfort, perceived exertion, and fatigue) and physiological parameters (core temperature, heart rate, fluid loss, and dehydration).

The results are quite relevant, mainly because (according to the authors) the impact of heat on burn surgical teams has not been previously measured in a real-world context. There are some questions that should be elucidated and corrected.

I have listed minor points aimed at improving the quality of the manuscript as follows:

1. The authors should detail more about “All staff provided information on previous heat exposure (i.e., recent holidays in hot climates and weekly, surgical heat exposure) and none were determined to be acclimatized/acclimated”. This simple information is not able to refute a hypothesis of acclimatized/acclimated participants. For example, aerobic fitness can acclimatize/acclimate humans. In this sense, is fundamental to expose a table with the sample characteristics (age, weight, level of aerobic fitness or amount of weekly physical activity, experience time as a surgeon, etc.).

2. “An increase in perceived workload is correlated with burnout, especially in the health care sector [14]”. Are acutes perceived workloads able to cause burnout?

3. Why do you use “hot” and “thermoneutral”, and not “heat”, “warm” and “temperate” environments?

4. Why were three participants only tested in HOT?

5. How did you measure the body-mass? Insert this information in the methods section.

6. Page 3, line 65. Please Insert reference.

7. Page 5, line 129. Please Insert a reference for this method.

Reviewer #3: Dr. Zehra Palejwala and colleagues assessed the impact of environmental heat stress during burn surgeries on staff’s cognitive performance, manual dexterity, perceptual measures (workload, thermal sensation, thermal comfort, perceived exertion, and fatigue), and physiological parameters (body core temperature, heart rate, fluid loss, and dehydration). The authors reported that the performance variables were unaffected in the heat. However, they observed higher levels of fatigue and subjective workload in hot surgeries, suggesting that environmental heat stress negatively affected perceptual responses in staff, most likely due to the higher body core temperature, heart rate, and fluid loss in the hot condition, either alone or in combination.

The topic addressed in the manuscript interests healthcare workers and researchers investigating the quality of life/burnout in this population. The current study advances previous literature because it investigates the impact of environmental heat stress on performance and psychophysiological responses in real-world (not simulated) contexts. The manuscript is well written and has adequate size. The methods are adequate to investigate the research problem. The results supporting the conclusions are presented in good figures and tables. Despite these strong points, the authors should address some issues before the manuscript is ready for publication.

Please see my suggestions to improve the manuscript below.

Major points

1. Title, lines 2 and 3. The authors indicated increased perceived workload and fatigue resulted from augmented heat strain. Because of the experimental design, the statement indicating a cause-effect relationship between heat strain and perceived workload/fatigue seems too strong. Moreover, the title should indicate whether the changes were assessed in patients or surgeons. Please consider the following alternative title: "Higher operating theatre temperature during burn surgery increases the surgical staff's physiological heat strain and subjective workload and fatigue".

2. The authors should be less emphatic in their conclusions in the abstract and regular text. For example, the experiments in the heat were conducted at the lower end of the recommended ambient temperature range for major burn surgeries (i.e., 30°C to 40°C). Therefore, the authors should exercise caution to state that "operating in the heat is a safe approach for patient treatment" (line 54) because this may not be true for surgeries at 35°C.

2.1. Abstract, lines 54 to 55. The information about job burnout was poorly contextualized in this section and appeared first in conclusion. To amend this issue, the authors may want to mention that job burnout is positively correlated with the perceived workload.

2.2. Lines 381 and 382. Please consider replacing “heat stress” with “heat strain” and insert the word “possibly” before “due to”.

3. Did the patients become hypothermic during surgeries under control conditions? This information should be inserted in the revised manuscript to ensure researchers took proper ethical care.

4. Did the different scales used in the current study provide overlapping information? For example, what are the differences between data provided by Borg's scale, the 20-point visual-analogue scale for determining "physical fatigue", and the physical demand domain of the perceived workload scale?

4.1. Results, lines 237 and 239. The fact that overlapping information exists was evidenced at the end of the following sentence: “There was a significant effect of theatre temperature on TS (p = .002), TC (p < .001), RPE (p < .001), and fatigue (p < .001), indicating that staff felt hotter, more uncomfortable, more fatigued, and were exerting themselves more in the heat (Fig 3)”.

4.2. Panels C and D in Figure 3 are very similar, thus basically providing the same information.

4.3. If these scales provide overlapping information, please consider removing one from the manuscript. For example, I would suggest removing the 20-point visual-analogue scale for determining "physical fatigue", because it was initially used to measure mental fatigue (lines 147 and 148).

5. While reading the Results section, I noticed that the authors could clearly describe the main effect of time or the main effect of ambient temperature. However, I missed the information about the time x ambient temperature interactions. This issue is evident when analyzing data presented in figures and Table 1. Therefore, please include the information about interactions in the revised manuscript.

6. Although the figures were well elaborated, they can still be improved in several ways. First, most font sizes should be increased, especially in Figures 3, 4, and 6. Second, the white space between panels should be reduced in all figures. For example, the letters indicating the panels can be placed closer to or even inside these panels. Third, PLoS One is a journal published online, and it does not charge an additional tax to publish color figures. The manuscript will benefit from using blue symbols for the control condition and red symbols for the hot condition.

6.1. Figure 4. The authors may want to present a panel with the overall score for the task load index (or workload), thus reducing the white space in this figure.

Minor points

1. Abstract, line 38. When mentioned for the first time, it may be interesting to emphasize that authors are referring to the term "body core temperature". After that, writing only "core temperature" or using the corresponding abbreviation is okay. This suggestion is also valid for the regular text (line 126).

2. Abstract, line 46. Please define the meaning of the “RH” abbreviation before its first appearance.

3. Introduction, line 82. Please consider adding “not simulated” in the following sentence to improve clarity: “measured in a real-world (not simulated) context”.

4. Methods. Comparisons in lines 104 to 108 should be moved to the Results section. If the authors decide to maintain this information in the methods (although not ideal), please explain the meaning of the data (i.e., mean ± SEM) and include the statistical test used to generate the p-values reported.

5. Methods, line 107. The average total body surface area was less than 20% (i.e., 8 ± 13%) during surgery in control conditions. This means that surgery was not necessarily a major burn surgery. Please amend the sentence in line 384.

6. Results, lines 190 to 193. To improve the description, the authors should mention the menstrual phase of the following two women surgeons: “… one was only tested once, and one was in the same menstrual cycle phase during testing sessions”.

7. Table 1, lines 209 to 211. Please consider replacing “49 ± 9.0” with “49 ± 9”.

8. Discussion, line 306. Please indicate the number of the reference instead of the publication year.

9. Discussion, line 376. Please consider citing the following reference – doi: 10.1080/23328940.2020.1868386 – or any other relevant reference.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Alexandre SR Hudson

Reviewer #3: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Reviewer comments.docx

PLoS One. 2023 Jun 2;18(6):e0286746. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286746.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


29 Apr 2023

General comments for revision submission

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Response:

Submission guidelines including style requirements, file naming and author affiliations have been thoroughly checked and we believe the manuscript adheres to all guidelines.

Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

Response:

The ethics statement on page4, from lines 92-96 of the materials and methods section has been amended and now reads: “All staff gave written consent for participation in this study and patients gave either written or verbal consent (written consent was not able to be provided by all patients because of the nature and location of the burn injury). Verbal consent was witnessed by a member of the surgical burns team who then signed and dated the consent form, attesting that the requirements for informed consent were satisfied. Ethical approval was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Western Australia (2020/ET000239) and the South Metropolitan Health Service Human Research Ethics Committee (PRN RGS0000004250).”

Once you have amended this/these statement(s) in the Methods section of the manuscript, please add the same text to the “Ethics Statement” field of the submission form (via “Edit Submission”).

Response:

The appropriate text has been added to the ‘ethics statement’ field of the submission form.

Reviewer # 1

GENERAL COMMENTS

The authors completed attractive work overall, with the aims and methods section being interesting. My comments are all minimal.

Response:

We thank the reviewer for their positive appraisal and valuable comments. We have done our best to address each comment below and revise the manuscript accordingly. Amended sentences appear highlighted in red in the revised manuscript.

SPECIFIC COMMENTS

I see cognitive performance as a complex phenomenon. Thus, analyses involving three cognitive domains provide limited explanations for something as complex as the regulation of cognitive performance. If you don't have a justification, consider using executive function performance and not cognitive performance.

Response:

We agree that working memory, assessed in our study, is a cognitive domain rather than something that completely encompasses cognitive performance, however some of the measures from the counting-span task are simple counting scores, latencies, and counting dots, which do not measure executive function in a meaningful way. Thus, we have used the term “cognitive function” as a better umbrella term for counting and serial recall performance (while making no claim that cognitive function was assessed in an exhaustive manner). Additional information has been inserted in the manuscript to provide clarity in the cognitive task used and what it assessed. The manuscript on lines 176-178 now reads: “As such, measures included those that relate to basic counting performance as well as those assessing working memory capacity; for this reason, the term ‘cognitive function’ has been used to refer to these measures collectively.”

Line 156. Working memory is not a component of executive function? Please be more explicit.

Response:

The sentence on line 164 has been modified accordingly, avoiding the term executive function, and it now reads “The function of working memory – a core component of executive capacity – was measured by the counting span task (millisecond software) on a laptop.”

Line 99. I am not native to an English-speaking country, but if there is no specific reason, consider using CON and not CONTROL.

Response:

We have changed the term “CONTROL” to “CON” throughout the manuscript. These changes have occurred on lines 42, 48, 91, 102, 105, 106, 108, 201, 203, 204, 226 (table legend & table), 264, 265, 300, 304, 354, 366, 371. The term “CONTROL” was also changed to “CON” in all figures, figure legends, and supplementary files.

Line 59. Workload and fatigue already appear in the title. Consider defining other keywords. Perhaps this increases the chances of your article being found in a database.

Response:

Thank you for this observation. On line 55, the keywords “workload” and “fatigue” have now been replaced with alternative terms; “surgery task load index” and “physical demand.”

Line 109. Considering the results of this study, I would suggest head cooling during burn surgery. Perhaps you should know about a recent work on the topic (doi: 10.1111/sms.13985).

Response:

We agree and thank you for providing the resource above. In the practical implications section, where there is mention of cooling technologies for staff, the use of head cooling and a cooling vest (as per an additional comment from a reviewer) has been suggested. On lines 399-404, the section now reads: “Alleviating symptoms of heat strain in burn surgery staff should be a priority and could be achieved by taking small breaks during surgery, using underbody warming devices for the patient (warming mattresses) as opposed to heating the OT, or cooling technologies for staff. For example, head cooling caps [54] and cooling vests [55] have been found to lower perceptual heat strain and may be able to do the same for surgery staff working in the heat, thereby lowering their mental workload”.

Line 129. Consider using TCORE. I think it improves visualization.

Response:

Throughout the manuscript the term Tcore has been altered to read TCORE. These changes have occurred on lines 136, 139, 145, 287, 288, 289, 321, 333, 336, 352, and 356.

Line 136. Why was water not provided ad libitum for the medical team?

Response:

To clarify, water was not provided ab libitum for the medical team as fluids were not permitted in the operating theatre. However, if the team desired water, they were able to ‘scrub out’, leave the operating theatre and consume fluids. It is not common practice to exit the theatre during a procedure and all bar one chose to refrain from drinking to minimise bathroom breaks and avoid having to scrub out.

Line 139. Just a suggestion. You could have corrected the BM pre- and post-test for the volume of water ingested (Please note the question before), the clothes you wore, and the weight of the experimental instruments.

Response:

Thank you for your comment. Only one participant in one trial consumed fluids during surgery and this amount was recorded and accounted for in the body mass calculations. In no other trials were any fluids consumed after the initial body mass measurement was taken and before the final one was taken. On lines 140-142, within the protocol section of the methods, it is stated that measurements were taken with participants in the nude, in private, therefore the weight of clothes was not considered in the calculation. The same experimental instruments were used pre- and post- surgery for everyone to account for the weight of experimental instruments.

Line 176. Why didn't you use a more up-to-date version of R Studio for Windows (e.g., version 2022.07.2)? I think this may have reflected in the quality of the figures. Use colors and the ggplot2 package.

Response:

All figures were created using GraphPad Prism 5 for windows, version 5.00, 2007. Initially, all figures were saved as resolution 300 dpi but have now been exported as resolution 600 dpi to improve the quality.

Line 182. I'm not a statistician, but I was concerned about Tukey's use of post hoc. I think some variables have a coefficient of variation (CV) ≥ 15%. Tukey's post hoc is very rigorous; consider using a less rigorous one for variables with a high CV. I consider in my post hoc choices the number of treatments and the CV of the variable. Thus, I minimize the chances of type I and II errors (DOI: 10.1177/0013164488483001; ISBN: 9788587144522).

Table. Determining the post-hoc test

Variable instability CV Number

of treatments Post hoc test

Low ≤15% ≤5 Tukey, SNK or Scheffé

Low ≤15% >5 SNK or Tukey

Medium 15 a 30% ≤4 Student's t test

Medium 15 a 30% ≥5 SNK or Duncan

High ≥30% ≤4 Student's t test

High ≥30% >4 Duncan

Response:

Thank you for providing the resource above. We have spoken to a statistician at the university who has informed us that the “Tukey” post hoc method is an appropriate test to use for our data analysis. For this reason, we have left the manuscript as is, with the Tukey post-hoc test.

Line 189. If you don't have a specific reason, consider using relative humidity instead of Relative Humidity.

Response:

On line 43 in the abstract, the term “Relative Humidity” has now been corrected to “relative humidity”. Following an additional observation from one of the reviewers, the term relative humidity was not defined prior to its abbreviation in the abstract of the manuscript and so the term “relative humidity” has been inserted in the abstract and removed from the results section, where it was first written as “Relative Humidity”.

Line 198. If you don't have a cause in mind, think about using Figure rather than Fig.

Response:

The term “Fig” has been used throughout the manuscript, rather than “Figure”, in accordance with the PLOS One manuscript body and formatting guidelines.

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf

Lines 183, 207, and 228. Are all results shown expressed as mean ± SD or mean ± SEM?

Response:

The data in the written text and tables are presented as mean ± SD throughout the manuscript however in the figures it is presented as mean ± SEM due to standard deviations being large and overlapping figure data points. In all figure legends we have noted whether or not data are presented as mean ± SD, mean ± SEM, or whether all individua data points are plotted. In the statistical analysis section of ‘Materials and methods’ we have provided clarity on line 194-195 by noting that “All results presented within the written text and tables are expressed as mean ± SD and all figures are presented as individual data points or mean ± SEM”.

Line 279. I am aware of the study by Casa et al. (2000); however, you could be a little more cautious in stating that individuals have significant dehydration (consider using hypohydration). Measurement of urine specific gravity may not reflect plasma osmolality, the most efficient measure to assess hydration status. Perhaps you should know and consider the article by Pereira et al. (2017) (DOI: 10.23736/S0022-4707.16.06836-5).

Response:

Thank you for providing the resource above. Within the materials and methods section, after the mention of USG measurement and classifications, a sentence citing the reference above has been added, to exercise caution in the way hydration is classified. The section from lines 132-135 now reads: “Values obtained for USG were classified as ‘well hydrated’ <1.010, ‘minimal dehydration’=1.010-1.020, ‘significant dehydration’=1.021-1.030 and ‘serious dehydration’ >1.030 [22]. It is important to note that measurement of USG may not reflect plasma osmolality [23], the most efficient measure to assess hydration status, and so the classifications provided may not be accurate in illustrating the extent of hypohydration.

In supplemental table 4, which provides the number of staff members in each hydration category, a footnote reading “It is important to note that measurement of USG may not reflect plasma osmolality, the most efficient measure to assess hydration status, and so the classifications provided may not be accurate in illustrating the extent of hypohydration” has been added.

Lines 394 to 532. Important current references in the area of thermoregulation and areas similar to this study were not presented. You only presented three references from the last three years. Please consider citing these: DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0274584; DOI: 10.1038/s41572-021-00334-6; DOI: 10.1177/1553350620934931; DOI: 10.1308/rcsann.2020.7001.

Response:

Thank you for providing the resources above. The following references, DOI: 10.1038/s41572-021-00334-6 and 10.1177/1553350620934931, have been cited in the revised manuscript in the introduction. On lines 61-65, the section now reads: “This can improve patient outcomes; however patient outcomes also depend on the cognitive function [2], manual dexterity/technical skills [3], and fatigue levels of surgical teams [4]. Heat exposure can lead to heat and cardiovascular strain [5], and dehydration if fluids are not adequately replaced, all of which can impair physical and cognitive function [6-9]”.

Reviewer # 2

GENERAL COMMENTS

Palejwala and colleagues aimed to assess the real-time impact of heat during burn surgeries on staff’s cognitive performance, manual dexterity, and perceptual measures (workload, thermal sensation, thermal comfort, perceived exertion, and fatigue) and physiological parameters (core temperature, heart rate, fluid loss, and dehydration). The results are quite relevant, mainly because (according to the authors) the impact of heat on burn surgical teams has not been previously measured in a real-world context. There are some questions that should be elucidated and corrected

Response:

We thank the reviewer for their positive appraisal and valuable comments. We have done our best to address each comment below and revise the manuscript accordingly. Amended sentences appear highlighted in red in the revised manuscript.

SPECIFIC COMMENTS

The authors should detail more about “All staff provided information on previous heat exposure (i.e., recent holidays in hot climates and weekly, surgical heat exposure) and none were determined to be acclimatized/acclimated”. This simple information is not able to refute a hypothesis of acclimatized/acclimated participants. For example, aerobic fitness can acclimatize/acclimate humans. In this sense, is fundamental to expose a table with the sample characteristics (age, weight, level of aerobic fitness or amount of weekly physical activity, experience time as a surgeon, etc.)

Response:

We agree with this comment and have provided additional information in the revised manuscript to support the hypothesis that staff were not acclimatised/acclimated. Data were collected in the winter months when outdoor ambient temperatures reached a maximum of 20°C, specifically to minimise the possibility of acclimatisation. This has now been clarified in the manuscript on lines 90-91, under the subheading ‘participants’ in the ‘materials and methods’, which now reads: “Surgical staff from a burns department were recruited in the winter (June - October 2021; when average maximum ambient temperature was 20°C) to minimise the possibility of acclimatisation/acclimation, for testing in CON and HOT conditions”.

In the ‘familiarisation’ section of ‘materials and methods’ additional information regarding weekly heat exposure has been provided. Lines 115-124 now read: “All staff provided information on weekly heat exposure and physical activity via the International Physical Activity Questionnaire [21], to determine acclimatisation/acclimation status. The average, maximum, outdoor temperature during the testing period was 20°C and no staff had travelled to a warmer climate in recent months prior to testing. Within the four months of testing, staff were exposed to a heated OT on 5 occurrences, with the average duration of exposure being 158 min, equating to a total average of 50 min per week. The average amount of recreational physical activity (not including job-related, indoor physical activity i.e. walking within the hospital) was 5 hours weekly, with most reporting light, as opposed to moderate and high intensity physical activity. Thus, no staff were determined to be acclimatised/acclimated”.

“An increase in perceived workload is correlated with burnout, especially in the health care sector [14]”. Are acutes perceived workloads able to cause burnout?

Response:

Acute periods of a high workload in isolation would not be sufficient to cause burnout, but chronic exposure to these acute periods are what accumulate and cause burnout (doi: 10.2147/LRA.S240564). Long-term, continuous work with a high mental workload is correlated with job burnout, specifically in the health care sector .

Why do you use “hot” and “thermoneutral”, and not “heat”, “warm” and “temperate” environments?

Response:

Thank you for your question. In the most recent journal articles (DOI: 10.1097/sla.0000000000004598 and https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0222923) assessing heat strain in burn surgery staff, the term “hot” for the operating theatre was used and the environmental conditions in this study were similar to those in the aforementioned articles. Thus, the term “hot” was used to maintain consistency with the literature. We chose to use the term “thermoneutral” as the temperature in our control condition fell within the temperatures classified as the thermoneutral ambient temperature range for someone who is lightly clothed, which is ~21.5-25°C (DOI: 10.4161/temp.29702).

Why were three participants only tested in HOT?

Response:

The three participants who were tested in HOT only were initially tested in the HOT theatre and were to be tested in the CONTROL condition however rising COVID cases, and subsequent snap lockdowns, prevented the research team from entering the theatre to collect CONTROL data for said individuals. As such, their data were still included in the analysis and a mixed model code was used to account for the unmatched data.

How did you measure the body-mass? Insert this information in the methods section.

Response

The required information has now been added to the methods section, within the protocol write up on lines 141-142. The sentence now reads “In private, nude body mass was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg using a digital platform scale (details provided above)”, as the details of the weighing scales were included in the section titled “familiarisation session.

Page 3, line 65. Please Insert reference.

Response:

Thank you for this observation. References for the statement “patient outcomes also depend on the cognitive function, manual dexterity and fatigue levels of surgical teams” have been inserted and the statement from lines 61-63 now reads “patient outcomes also depend on the cognitive function [2], manual dexterity/technical skills [3], and fatigue levels of surgical teams [4].”

Page 5, line 129. Please Insert a reference for this method.

Response:

Thank you for this observation. On line 139, a reference has been added at the end of the statement and the sentence now reads “Four to eight hours prior to surgery, staff ingested a pill that objectively measured TCORE [24]”.

Reviewer # 3

GENERAL COMMENTS

Dr. Zehra Palejwala and colleagues assessed the impact of environmental heat stress during burn surgeries on staff’s cognitive performance, manual dexterity, perceptual measures (workload, thermal sensation, thermal comfort, perceived exertion, and fatigue), and physiological parameters (body core temperature, heart rate, fluid loss, and dehydration). The authors reported that the performance variables were unaffected in the heat. However, they observed higher levels of fatigue and subjective workload in hot surgeries, suggesting that environmental heat stress negatively affected perceptual responses in staff, most likely due to the higher body core temperature, heart rate, and fluid loss in the hot condition, either alone or in combination. The topic addressed in the manuscript interests healthcare workers and researchers investigating the quality of life/burnout in this population. The current study advances previous literature because it investigates the impact of environmental heat stress on performance and psychophysiological responses in real-world (not simulated) contexts. The manuscript is well written and has adequate size. The methods are adequate to investigate the research problem. The results supporting the conclusions are presented in good figures and tables. Despite these strong points, the authors should address some issues before the manuscript is ready for publication.

Response:

We thank the reviewer for their positive appraisal and valuable comments. We have done our best to address each comment below and revise the manuscript accordingly. Amended sentences appear highlighted in red in the revised manuscript.

MAJOR COMMENTS

Title, lines 2 and 3. The authors indicated increased perceived workload and fatigue resulted from augmented heat strain. Because of the experimental design, the statement indicating a cause-effect relationship between heat strain and perceived workload/fatigue seems too strong. Moreover, the title should indicate whether the changes were assessed in patients or surgeons. Please consider the following alternative title: "Higher operating theatre temperature during burn surgery increases the surgical staff's physiological heat strain and subjective workload and fatigue".

Response:

Thank you for your suggestion. We agree that the manuscript title would be improved with minor revisions and so the title has been altered to read “Higher operating theatre temperature during burn surgery increases physiological heat strain, subjective workload, and fatigue of surgical staff.”

The authors should be less emphatic in their conclusions in the abstract and regular text. For example, the experiments in the heat were conducted at the lower end of the recommended ambient temperature range for major burn surgeries (i.e., 30°C to 40°C). Therefore, the authors should exercise caution to state that "operating in the heat is a safe approach for patient treatment" (line 54) because this may not be true for surgeries at 35°C.

Response:

In the abstract, on line 50, the statement “suggesting that operating in the heat is a safe approch for patient treatment” has been altered to exercise caution and now reads “suggesting that operating in approximately 30°C heat is a safe approach for patient treatment”.

Abstract, lines 54 to 55. The information about job burnout was poorly contextualized in this section and appeared first in conclusion. To amend this issue, the authors may want to mention that job burnout is positively correlated with the perceived workload.

Response:

On line 51, The conclusion section of the abstract has been amended to contextualise the statement regarding burnout. The sentence now reads: “However, job burnout, which is positively correlated with perceived workoad, and the impact of cumulative fatigue on the mental health of surgery staff, must be considered in the context of supporting an effective health workforce”.

Lines 381 and 382. Please consider replacing “heat stress” with “heat strain” and insert the word “possibly” before “due to”.

Response:

In the conclusion, on lines 408- 409, the statement “due to heat stress” has been revised to “possibly due to heat strain” and the manuscript now reads “This study showed that cognitive function and manual dexterity was maintained while operating in the heat, however subjective workload and fatigue/the physical demand of tasks increased, possibly due to heat strain”

Did the patients become hypothermic during surgeries under control conditions? This information should be inserted in the revised manuscript to ensure researchers took proper ethical care.

Response:

Thank you for your question. After consulting with the surgical team, we have been informed that no patients became hypothermic during surgery in either environmental condition. In the section on experimental design, following surgery information, a statement from lines 107-108 that reads “No patients became hypothermic during surgery in either the CON or HOT condition” has been added.

Did the different scales used in the current study provide overlapping information? For example, what are the differences between data provided by Borg's scale, the 20-point visual-analogue scale for determining "physical fatigue", and the physical demand domain of the perceived workload scale?

Response:

Thank you for this question. To clarify, Borg’s scale for rating of perceived exertion is different from physical demand, which is answered by the SURG-TLX questionnaire. Borg’s scale provides a measure of how hard it feels that the body is working and encompasses effort, exertion, breathlessness as well as fatigue, to give an overall estimate of intensity of physical activity whereas the SURG-TLX domain of physical demand provides information on how physically fatiguing an individual feels a procedure or task was. The 20-point visual analogue scale however provides overlapping information with the physical demand domain of the SURG-TLX questionnaire and thus has been removed from the revised manuscript

Results, lines 237 and 239. The fact that overlapping information exists was evidenced at the end of the following sentence: “There was a significant effect of theatre temperature on TS (p = .002), TC (p < .001), RPE (p < .001), and fatigue (p < .001), indicating that staff felt hotter, more uncomfortable, more fatigued, and were exerting themselves more in the heat (Fig 3)”.

Response:

Thermal sensation and thermal comfort, although similar, provide different information. For example, thermal sensation relates to information from the skin surface where changes are dependent on the external environment whereas thermal comfort refers to the state of mind of an individual when expressing satisfaction or dissatisfaction with the surrounding environment. Both measures have been used in conjunction in previous studies (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jobe.2017.02.004). As noted above, fatigue and perceived exertion also provide different information and complement each other in describing how an individual feels. A recent article (https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2021.735565) that discusses fatigue and perceived exertion confirm that they are related but are independent measures. The article states that “Although RPE [is] considered a measure of exercise intensity, recent studies suggest that [it] could be affected by other factors, i.e., duration of the session or fatigue”

Panels C and D in Figure 3 are very similar, thus basically providing the same information. If these scales provide overlapping information, please consider removing one from the manuscript. For example, I would suggest removing the 20-point visual-analogue scale for determining "physical fatigue", because it was initially used to measure mental fatigue (lines 147 and 148).

Response:

We agree that there is an overlap of information. To prevent overlap, reuslts from the 20-point visual-analogue scale (panel D) have been removed from the manuscript. Amendments have been made to the revised manuscript wherever there is mention of fatigue, to clarify that statements are made based on the SURG-TLX results and not from the 20-point visual-analogue scale.

From the materials and methods section, the following sentences have been removed: “Physical fatigue was measured using a modified, 0-20 point visual-analogue scale, initially used to measure mental fatigue. The scale ranges from ‘no fatigue, full energy levels’ to ‘extremely fatigued.’”

Within the results section, scores, main effects and interactions for fatigue (from the 20-point visual analogue scale, have been removed.

The discussion on responses from the 20-point visual analogue scale for fatigue has now been removed from the section that addresses individual perceptual responses and has been moved to the section of the discussion that addresses the SURG-TLX questionnaire, under the heading/domain of ‘physical demand’ Lines 365-370 now read: “Notably, the physical demand of the surgical task, i.e., how physically fatiguing the procedure was, was higher in the HOT compared to CON theatre. Increased levels of fatigue reported during surgery could be of significant consequence in the health care industry, since fatigue is associated with an increased risk of medical errors [46, 47], carelessness among health care workers [48], and impairment to physical and mental performance during simulated medical work [49]”.

Line 409 of the manuscript now reads: “Our results suggest that it would be beneficial to consider fatigue/the physical demand of tasks and mental workload in the work design for major burns (heated surgeries)”.

While reading the Results section, I noticed that the authors could clearly describe the main effect of time or the main effect of ambient temperature. However, I missed the information about the time x ambient temperature interactions. This issue is evident when analyzing data presented in figures and Table 1. Therefore, please include the information about interactions in the revised manuscript.

Response:

All information about interactions has now been included in the revised manuscript. The following additions have been made;

On page 9, lines 213-214, a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on counting latency has been added and the section now reads: There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on counting latency (p = .203; Fig 1).

On page 9, lines 216-217, a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on the number of correct responses for the counting task has been added and the section now reads: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on the number of correct responses (p = .757; Table 1)”.

On page 10, lines 232-233, a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on recall latency has been added and the section now reads: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on recall latency (p = .757; Fig 2)”.

On page 10, lines 235-236, a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on number of correct responses for the recall task has been added and the section now reads: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on number of correct responses (p = .828; Table 1)”.

On page 10, lines 238-239, a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on overall counting span score has been added and the section now reads: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on overall counting span score (p = .949; Table 1)”.

On page 11, lines 252-254, a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on manual dexterity has been added and the section now reads: “There was no interatcion between theatre temperature and time on manual dexterity in either the dominant hand (p = .428) or the non-dominant hand (p = .949)”.

On page 11, lines 262-266, a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on perceptual responses has been added and the section now reads: “There was an interaction between theatre temperature and time on TS (p = .019), TC (p = .047), and RPE (p < .001), which indicated similar perceptual responses for TS and RPE in CON and HOT pre-surgery (p > .924), but a higher TC score pre-surgery in HOT (11 ± 3) than CON (8 ± 4; p = .025)”.

On page 12, lines 288-289 a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on core temperature and heart rate has been added and the section now reads: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on TCORE (p = .138) or HR (p = .700)”.

On page 13, lines 302-303 a sentence regarding the interaction between time and theatre temperature on USG scores has been added and the section now reads: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on USG scores (p = .138)”.

Although the figures were well elaborated, they can still be improved in several ways. First, most font sizes should be increased, especially in Figures 3, 4, and 6. Second, the white space between panels should be reduced in all figures. For example, the letters indicating the panels can be placed closer to or even inside these panels. Third, PLoS One is a journal published online, and it does not charge an additional tax to publish color figures. The manuscript will benefit from using blue symbols for the control condition and red symbols for the hot condition.

Response:

We agree that the figures could be improved and so they have been amended accordingly. Font size in all figures has been increased. The letters indicating panels have now been placed inside the figures, thus reducing white space on all figures. The scatter plots have been modified to aligned dot plots for visual aesthetic purposes and colours have been added to all figures (blue symbols for the CON condition and red for the HOT condition).

Figure 4. The authors may want to present a panel with the overall score for the task load index (or workload), thus reducing the white space in this figure.

Response:

Thank you for your suggestion. We agree that the figure could be improved and so the mean scores for each dimension of the task load index have been presented in a new figure, reducing the white space. The figure legend, on line 280, for this figure has been altered to read “Scores for each dimension of the task load index in CON (n=22) and HOT (n=18) surgeries; mental demand (Men), physical demand (Phy), temporal demand (Tem), task complexity (Task), situation stress (Sit), distractions (Dist), and frustration (Frust). **indicates significant difference between conditions (p < .01); ***indicates significant difference between conditions (p <.001). Results are presented as mean ± SEM”.

MINOR COMMENTS

Abstract, line 38. When mentioned for the first time, it may be interesting to emphasize that authors are referring to the term "body core temperature". After that, writing only "core temperature" or using the corresponding abbreviation is okay. This suggestion is also valid for the regular text (line 126).

Response:

In the abstract, the first sentence from lines 35-36 now reads: “Raising the ambient temperature of the operating theatre is common practice during burn surgeries to maintain the patient’s core body temperature”.

Abstract, line 46. Please define the meaning of the “RH” abbreviation before its first appearance.

Response:

Thank you for this observation. In the abstract, upon first appearance of the abbreviation RH (line 43), the term “relative humidity” has been inserted.

Introduction, line 82. Please consider adding “not simulated” in the following sentence to improve clarity: “measured in a real-world (not simulated) context”.

Response:

The sentence in the introduction on line 80 has now been altered to read: “measured in a real-world (not simulated) context”.

Methods. Comparisons in lines 104 to 108 should be moved to the Results section. If the authors decide to maintain this information in the methods (although not ideal), please explain the meaning of the data (i.e., mean ± SEM) and include the statistical test used to generate the p-values reported.

Response:

We agree and have moved the aforementioned statements regarding duration of surgery and patient TBSA to the results section and have clarified the meaning of the data by re-wording the sentence. The first paragraph of the results section from lines 201-204 now reads: “Environmental conditions were 24.0±1.1°C, 45±6% RH for the CON trials, and 30.8±1.6°C, 39±7% RH for the HOT trials. Surgery duration was not different between conditions (CON: 141 ± 50 min, HOT: 158 ± 51 min; p = .287). Burn injury TBSA of patients was not different between conditions (CON: 8±13%, HOT: 20±7%; p = .053).” The statistical test used to generate the results is in the statistical analysis write up, before the mention of p values regarding surgery duration and TBSA.

Methods, line 107. The average total body surface area was less than 20% (i.e., 8 ± 13%) during surgery in control conditions. This means that surgery was not necessarily a major burn surgery. Please amend the sentence in line 384.

Response:

Thank you for your comment. For clarification, the major burns are those conducted in the heat and are typically ≥ 20% TBSA. This has been noted in the introduction. The first sentence of the introduction, page 3 line 59, has now been amended to read: “Major burn surgeries, usually ≥ 20% total body surface area). The burns conducted in the CON condition are not major burns, they are the minor elective surgeries performed on burns usually < 20% TBSA. The sentence on line 410 is referring to the HOT surgeries, which are major burns. The term ‘heated surgeries’ has been added for clarity on line 410 and the sentence from lines 409-410 now reads: “Our results suggest that it would be beneficial to consider fatigue/the physical demand of the task and mental workload in the work design for major burns (heated surgeries)”.

Results, lines 190 to 193. To improve the description, the authors should mention the menstrual phase of the following two women surgeons: “… one was only tested once, and one was in the same menstrual cycle phase during testing sessions”.

Response:

Upon re-examination of the data, we have identified that one participant may have been in a different menstrual cycle phase during her testing sessions and so this has been added in to the text. The female participant who was tested once only was identified to be in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle. All information has been added to the written text and the results section from lines 204-208 and the section now reads: “Of the seven females tested, three were post-menopausal, two were using an intrauterine device which meant that their menstrual cycle phase was unidentifiable, one was only tested once in the follicular phase, and one was in the follicular phase during the first testing session and the luteal phase during the second”.

Table 1, lines 209 to 211. Please consider replacing “49 ± 9.0” with “49 ± 9”.

Response:

The text in the table (lines 226-227) has been adjusted accordingly to read “49 ± 9”.

Discussion, line 306. Please indicate the number of the reference instead of the publication year.

Response:

Thank you for this observation. On line 328, the reference has been added. The sentence now reads: “Improvements in manual dexterity in the dominant hand over time, as found in this study, were also seen by Palejwala and colleagues [16]...”

Discussion, line 376. Please consider citing the following reference – doi: 10.1080/23328940.2020.1868386 – or any other relevant reference.

Response:

Thank for providing the resource above. The reference has been incorporated into the discussion session, following the mention of cooling technologies for staff. Another relevant reference discussing head cooling has also been added to this section. The section on lines 399-404 now reads: “Alleviating symptoms of heat strain in burn surgery staff should be a priority and could be achieved by taking small breaks during surgery, using underbody warming devices for the patient (warming mattresses) as opposed to heating the OT, or cooling technologies for staff. For example, head cooling caps [54] and cooling vests [55] have been found to lower perceptual heat strain and may be able to do the same for surgery staff working in the heat, thereby lowering their mental workload”.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Samuel Penna Wanner

19 May 2023

PONE-D-23-05820R1Higher operating theatre temperature during burn surgery increases physiological heat strain, subjective workload, and fatigue of surgical staffPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Zehra Palejwala,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The authors have addressed all the comments made by the reviewers satisfactorily. Indeed, the authors were highly responsive to these comments. Thank you! As a result, the revised manuscript was much improved compared to its first version. However, a few minor modifications should be made before the study is ready for publication; please see the Academic Editor’s comments at the end of this letter.

Please submit your revised manuscript by June 9th, 2023. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Samuel Penna Wanner, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Additional Editor Comments:

Minor points should be addressed before the manuscript is ready for publication. If the authors address these points (they will do), the manuscript will be accepted without an additional round of external reviews.

1) Abstract, line 50. Please consider referring to the HOT condition as approximately 31�C heat, not 30�C.

2) Methods, line 145. Please consider replacing “TCORE and HR measurement was taken as soon” with “TCORE and HR measurements were taken as soon”.

3) Please describe the results in a better way. For example, in the paragraph between lines 211 and 217, the authors could merge the two sentences about interaction into one sentence. Suggestion: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on counting latency (p = .203; Fig 1) or time on the number of correct responses (p = .757; Table 1)”.

3.1) Similarly, in the paragraph between lines 230 and 239, the authors could merge the three sentences about interaction into only one sentence. Suggestion: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on recall latency (p = .821; Fig 2), number of correct responses (p = .828; Table 1), or overall counting span score (p = .949; Table 1).

4) Results, lines 262 to 266. It is challenging to understand the information in the following sentence: “There was an interaction between theatre temperature and time on TS (p = .019), TC (p = .047), and RPE (p < .001), which indicated similar perceptual responses for TS and RPE in CON and HOT pre-surgery (p > .924), but a higher TC score pre-surgery in HOT (11 ± 3) than CON (8 ± 4; p = .025).” Please amend this sentence to improve clarity. The authors may want to focus on post-surgery rather than on pre-surgery data when describing the findings concerning these three perceptual variables. In the case of TC, it is also valid to comment that a difference already exists before surgery.

5) Please indicate where the Discussion section starts. The revised manuscript lacks the heading of the discussion section.

6) The figures are not shown in an appropriate order in the pdf file of the revised manuscript. For example, figure 6 comes before figures 1 and 2. Please amend this issue to avoid mistakes in the manuscript that will be published.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I was very pleased with the authors' review of the article and their responses to my questions. Congratulations on the nice article!

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: The authors addressed my comments satisfactorily. Congratulations!

Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript.

Kind Regards.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Alexandre SR Hudson

Reviewer #3: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jun 2;18(6):e0286746. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286746.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


21 May 2023

GENERAL COMMENTS

The authors have addressed all the comments made by the reviewers satisfactorily. Indeed, the authors were highly responsive to these comments. Thank you! As a result, the revised manuscript was much improved compared to its first version. However, a few minor modifications should be made before the study is ready for publication; please see the Academic Editor’s comments at the end of this letter.

Reviewer #1: I was very pleased with the authors' review of the article and their responses to my questions. Congratulations on the nice article!

Reviewer #3: The authors addressed my comments satisfactorily. Congratulations! Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript. Kind Regards.

Response: We thank the reviewers for their positive feedback on the manuscript.

JOURNAL REQUIREMENTS:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Response:

The reference list has been reviewed and is complete and correct. No retracted papers have been cited in the mnuscript.

MINOR REVISIONS

Abstract, line 50. Please consider referring to the HOT condition as approximately 31�C heat, not 30�C.

Response:

Lines 49-51 of the abstract now read: “Cognitive function and manual dexterity were mainained in hot conditions, suggesting that operating in approximately 31�C heat is a safe approach for patient treatment.

Methods, line 145. Please consider replacing “TCORE and HR measurement was taken as soon” with “TCORE and HR measurements were taken as soon”.

Response:

Line 145 of the methods section now reads: “TCORE and HR measurements were taken as soon as surgery commenced...”

Please describe the results in a better way. For example, in the paragraph between lines 211 and 217, the authors could merge the two sentences about interaction into one sentence. Suggestion: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on counting latency (p = .203; Fig 1) or time on the number of correct responses (p = .757; Table 1)”.

Response:

Lines 215-217 of the results section now read: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on counting latency (p = .203; Fig 1) or the number of correct responses (p = .757; Table 1)”.

Similarly, in the paragraph between lines 230 and 239, the authors could merge the three sentences about interaction into only one sentence. Suggestion: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on recall latency (p = .821; Fig 2), number of correct responses (p = .828; Table 1), or overall counting span score (p = .949; Table 1).

Response:

Lines 235-237 of the results section now read: “There was no interaction between theatre temperature and time on recall latency (p = .821; Fig 2), number of correct responses (p = .828; Table 1), or overall counting span score (p = .949; Table 1).

Results, lines 262 to 266. It is challenging to understand the information in the following sentence: “There was an interaction between theatre temperature and time on TS (p = .019), TC (p = .047), and RPE (p < .001), which indicated similar perceptual responses for TS and RPE in CON and HOT pre-surgery (p > .924), but a higher TC score pre-surgery in HOT (11 ± 3) than CON (8 ± 4; p = .025).” Please amend this sentence to improve clarity. The authors may want to focus on post-surgery rather than on pre-surgery data when describing the findings concerning these three perceptual variables. In the case of TC, it is also valid to comment that a difference already exists before surgery.

Response:

Thank you for your comment. To clarify, the post-surgery perceptual responses have been explained in detail in respect to the main effect for time. A sentence has been added to the discussion of the interaction to explain the post-surgery comparison between CON and HOT. The statement regarding the interaction discusses pre-surgery as that is where the significant post-hocs were found. To improve clarity, lines 260-264 of the manuscript now read: “There was an interaction between theatre temperature and time on TS (p = .019), TC (p = .047), and RPE (p < .001). The interaction supported that scores post-surgery were higher in HOT than CON for all perceptual measures (p < .001). It also revealed that ratings of TS and RPE were the same pre-surgery in CON and HOT (p > .924), but a difference in ratings of TC existed before surgery (HOT: 11 ± 3, CON: 8 ± 4; p = .025)”.

Please indicate where the Discussion section starts. The revised manuscript lacks the heading of the discussion section.

Response:

The heading “Discussion” has now been added to line 314 of the manuscript, to indicate where the discussion section starts.

The figures are not shown in an appropriate order in the pdf file of the revised manuscript. For example, figure 6 comes before figures 1 and 2. Please amend this issue to avoid mistakes in the manuscript that will be published.

Response:

The figures have now been uploaded in the appropriate order in the PDF file of the revised manuscript.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers 22.05.23.docx

Decision Letter 2

Samuel Penna Wanner

23 May 2023

Higher operating theatre temperature during burn surgery increases physiological heat strain, subjective workload, and fatigue of surgical staff

PONE-D-23-05820R2

Dear Dr. Zehra Palejwala,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Samuel Penna Wanner, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

The authors have satisfactorily addressed all six comments made by this Academic Editor. As a result, the revised manuscript is ready for publication. Congratulations on the excellent study!

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Samuel Penna Wanner

25 May 2023

PONE-D-23-05820R2

Higher operating theatre temperature during burn surgery increases physiological heat strain, subjective workload, and fatigue of surgical staff

Dear Dr. Palejwala:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Samuel Penna Wanner

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Participant numbers and demographic information for CON and HOT surgery conditions (mean ± SD).

    (PDF)

    S2 Table. Assignment of participants to surgeries (Sx) in both environmental conditions.

    (PDF)

    S3 Table. Scores for the Purdue pegboard task pre and post-surgery in CON (n = 22) and HOT (n = 18) surgeries for the dominant and non-dominant hand.

    All data expressed as Mean ± SD.

    (PDF)

    S4 Table. Number and percentage of participants n (%) in each hydration status category ‘pre’ and ‘post’ surgery, in CON and HOT surgeries.

    (PDF)

    S1 File. Core temperature and heart rate data.

    (XLSX)

    S2 File. USG and sweat loss data.

    (XLSX)

    S3 File. Pegboard data.

    (XLSX)

    S4 File. Cognitive scores data.

    (XLSX)

    S5 File. Cognitive latency scores.

    (XLSX)

    S6 File. Perceptual responses data.

    (XLSX)

    S7 File. Temperature and humidity data.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Reviewer comments.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers 22.05.23.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES