Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Jun 7.
Published in final edited form as: J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2001 Feb;22(1):19–26. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200102000-00003

Preschool Language Outcomes of Children With History of Bronchopulmonary Dysplasia and Very Low Birth Weight

LYNN T SINGER 1, A CAROL SIEGEL 2, BARBARA LEWIS 3, SUZANNE HAWKINS 4, TOYOKO YAMASHITA 5, JILL BALEY 6
PMCID: PMC10246333  NIHMSID: NIHMS1902104  PMID: 11265919

Abstract

A prospective follow-up of very low birth weight (VLBW) infants with and without bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD) and term control infants was conducted. The effects of BPD and VLBW on speech-language development and specific language impairment at 3 years of age were investigated, controlling for the effects of sociodemographic and other medical risk factors. Groups were compared on cognitive and speech-language outcomes using the Battelle Language and Bayley Mental Scales of Infant Development. Children with a history of BPD had lower receptive language skills than VLBW children without BPD, who in turn had lower receptive skills than term children. Children with a history of BPD also had lower expressive skills than the two comparison groups, whereas VLBW children without BPD did not differ in expressive language from term children. When IQ score was controlled, children with BPD demonstrated specific language impairment in receptive language. The presence of patent ductus arteriosis (PDA) was the best predictor of language deficits and the combined occurrence of PDA and BPD resulted in differentially lower language scores. Neurologic complications, low socioeconomic status, and minority race were also significant predictors of language delay. The findings emphasize the importance of considering both medical and sociodemographic factors in evaluating the risk of VLBW infants for poorer speech-language outcomes.

Keywords: very low birthweight, bronchopulmonary dysplasia, patent ductus arteriosis, development, speech and language


Recent progress in medical technology and neonatal intensive care is responsible for dramatic decreases in mortality and morbidity for very low birth weight (VLBW) infants (i.e., infants born weighing less than 1500 g). An estimated 20% to 40% of VLBW survivors are considered language delayed or impaired as toddlers and young children.1,2 Although VLBW, in and of itself, does not necessarily result in language delays, it is associated with a number of risk factors that can exert both generalized and specific effects on speech and language development.3,4 School age findings of language impairment in children with VLBW are equivocal. Whereas some studies report continuing deficits in language comprehension, production, and speech abilities,3,5,6 others report either no differences between VLBW and control children7-9 or diminishing delays over time.10 A related issue that has not been well addressed is whether the language delays seen in VLBW samples reflect generalized IQ deficits or more specific language impairments, reflecting disability above and beyond the effects of IQ alone.3,4

In recent years, it has become clear that VLBW infants are not a homogeneous group and therefore not at equal risk for developmental delays. Rather, the differential effects of medical risk factors, such as intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH),11 respiratory distress, and intrauterine growth retardation, have been recognized as important in generating more accurate estimates of risk status among VLBW infants. However, many outcome studies of VLBW fail to consider the number and type of medical complications that may be important predictors of language delay.

Factors associated with VLBW birth that may affect speech and language functioning include prenatal sociodemographic factors (such as socioeconomic status, race, and maternal education),12,13 birth status factors (such as gestational age, birth weight, and neurosensory deficits of hearing and vision),12,13 and perinatal and postnatal neurological and medical complications secondary to disruptions in oxygen regulation and metabolic processes associated with VLBW birth.14,15 This last category of factors includes hypoxic-ischemic events that may cause specific brain damage to language areas, such as IVH and persistent dilation and enlargement of the lateral ventricles. It also includes other brain events and chronic medical conditions that may cause more generalized, diffuse cerebral damage.

Several studies have concluded that IVH is a risk factor for language delays.14,15 Left ventricular damage in particular may be responsible for the delay in expressive language observed at 1 to 2 years of age in VLBW infants.16 Vocabulary development and verbal reasoning in 3-year-old VLBW children have been related to the presence and severity of IVH.17 Ischemic lesions developing in the periventricular region, periventricular leukomalacia (PVL), often occur with IVH in VLBW infants.18 At 5 years of age, only VLBW children with IVH or PVL had language and memory deficits in comparison with full-term children.19

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD) is a significant medical complication of prematurity that has not been well investigated in relation to language outcomes in VLBW children. BPD is a chronic lung disease that develops after preterm neonates with immature lungs are treated with artificial ventilation and supplemental oxygen for severe respiratory distress. Since its initial description,20 BPD has become the leading cause of lung disease in United States infants, with over 7,000 new cases diagnosed annually.21

BPD is associated with a number of risk factors that may affect language development, including an increased incidence of growth failure and cardiopulmonary and central nervous system complications that can affect sensory, motor, and/or cognitive aspects of speech-language outcomes.22-26 Sauve and Singhal27 observed a higher rate of hearing loss in BPD (3.8%) versus VLBW controls without BPD (1.7%). Perlman and Volpe28 described a movement disorder, characterized by rapid, random, jerky movements involving the neck, limbs, trunk, and oral-buccal-lingual structures that may be seen in infants with severe BPD. This may be associated with early articulatory delay and may affect motor aspects of speech. Furthermore, BPD is associated with repeated episodes of hypoxia during sleep and feeding, which may result in diffuse cerebral damage that may affect speech-language development in a global way.24,25

Northway20 originally speculated that BPD could have widespread neurological consequences. More recently, Volpe29 observed that chronic episodic hypoxia in BPD might lead to nonprogressive white matter injury and cortical loss, affecting a wide range of intellectual functions. Nevertheless, only a few studies have considered the effects of BPD on speech-language outcomes. Sauve and Singhal27 compared VLBW infants with BPD with preterm controls and found a greater frequency of language deficits at 30 months. Similarly, at school age, children with a history of BPD were found to have more receptive vocabulary deficits than preterm and term controls, but expressive language was not evaluated, and it was not clear whether language delays were specific or reflected more general cognitive delays.30

The present study is part of a larger investigation of the effects of BPD on child development.22 The first question of the current study is whether BPD is associated with negative effects on language development at age 3 years, beyond the effects of sociodemographic and other medical risk factors, and the second, whether language delays, if they occur, are reflections of overall developmental delay or whether they represent specific language impairments occurring over and above the effects of IQ. Finally, among VLBW infants, which medical and sociodemographic factors are most predictive of language delay?

METHODS

Subjects

All infants with very low birth weights and bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD) admitted to the neonatal intensive care units of hospitals in the Cleveland region were eligible for the study and were prospectively, consecutively recruited. For the approximately four-county region, all infants with BPD were cared for in the neonatal intensive care units (NICUS) of the three participating hospitals that had the only Level 3 NICUS in the region, providing an exhausting regional sample. BPD infants were preterm, less than 1500 g at birth, requiring supplementary oxygen for more than 28 days with radiographic evidence of chronic lung disease.20 A partial stratification sampling strategy was adopted to enroll adequate numbers of subjects without socioeconomic disadvantage or severe neurologic risk so that these factors could be investigated in data analyses. Infants diagnosed with BPD who were free of neurologic problems other than grades I to II intraventricular hemorrhage, and who were not socially disadvantaged (i.e., Hollingshead classification IV and V), were exhaustively recruited. The remaining infants were randomly recruited by approaching the family of the next available infant diagnosed with BPD who could be accommodated in the follow-up schedule. Parents of infants with BPD were approached by a research assistant in the NICU as soon as possible after the diagnosis of BPD was made by the attending physician.

For each infant with BPD, the next born very low birth weight (VLBW) comparison infant without BPD of the same race and socioeconomic status, born during the same time period, was recruited. Term infants were recruited from the newborn nurseries. Information about the study and return-addressed postcards were provided to all mothers in the nurseries. For each BPD infant enrolled, the next term infant equivalent in race and socioeconomic status with a returned postcard indicating parental willingness to participate was recruited, providing eligibility criteria were met. Infants with major congenital malformations or drug exposure, or whose mothers had a major psychiatric or physical illness, human immunodeficiency virus, or mental retardation, or who lived more than 2 hours driving distance from the facility, were excluded. VLBW infants without BPD were preterm, less than 1500 g birth weight, requiring oxygen supplementation for less than 14 days. Term infants had no diagnosed medical illnesses or abnormalities at birth, were more than 36 weeks gestational age, and more than 2500 g birth weight for singleton infants.

During the recruitment period, 250 infants with BPD were identified, of whom 89 were excluded (35 for drug/alcohol exposure, 21 for all other exclusions, 33 who could not be accommodated into the testing schedule and who were, by definition, all of lower socioeconomic status, on public assistance), leaving 161 eligible VLBW infants with BPD. Twenty (12%) refused the study, 14 (9%) died, and 5 (3%) were unable to be contacted. Of 122 enrolled infants, 7 died after enrollment, 1 withdrew before 8 months of age, 4 were lost to follow-up, and 12 did not come for the visit, leaving 98 (89%) at 3 years of age.

Of 214 VLBW infants without BPD, 24 were excluded for drug/alcohol exposure, 34 for oxygen supplementation for 21 to 28 days, and 46 for all other exclusions, leaving 110 eligible VLBW infants without BPD, 8 (7%) of whom were unable to be contacted and 18 (16%) who refused the study. Of 84 recruited, 2 withdrew and 1 was lost. One infant died at 2 years and 10 did not come for the visit, leaving 70 (80%) at 3 years of age. Of 123 term infants, 6 withdrew, 5 were lost, 16 did not come for the visit, and 95 (85%) were seen at 3 years of age. Groups did not differ by gender, race, socioeconomic status, maternal education, or marital status of parents. At follow-up at 3 years, group demographic and medical characteristics did not differ significantly from the sample recruited at birth.

The following neonatal medical and demographic information was obtained from the hospital chart: infant gestational age (based on both the date of the last menstrual period and the Ballard examination),31 birthweight (g), length (cm), head circumference (cm), and Apgar scores at 1 and 5 minutes. The presence or absence of respiratory distress syndrome, BPD, patent ductus arteriosis (PDA), necrotizing enterocolitis (demonstrated, with or without surgery), septicemia, retinopathy of prematurity, and failed hearing screening was recorded, as well as the number of days of ventilator support and of supplemental oxygen, and peak bilirubin levels. Also noted was the presence or absence of the following neurologic abnormalities: minor neurologic malformations, seizures, echodense lesions, porencephaly, hydrocephaly, ventriculoperitoneal shunt, meningitis, and periventricular leukomalacia.

Cranial ultrasound studies were performed and reviewed by board-certified radiologists, typically at 3, 10, and 28 days, as well as before discharge. A system for rating the severity of intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) was devised based on the extent of the lesion. A score of 0 was used to indicate no hemorrhage. Identifiable lesions were graded on a scale from 1 to 4, based on the criteria of Papile.32 At least one ultrasound study was available for all infants; ratings were based on the most severe lesion diagnosed. A total neurologic risk score was calculated for the above neurologic risk factors, wherein 0 was used to indicate absence and 1 was used to indicate presence.

Procedures

All children recruited at birth were scheduled for the 3-year follow-up visit, with ages corrected for prematurity, at which time language assessments were completed in one or two sessions at the behavioral laboratory of the Department of Pediatrics. Families were reimbursed for transportation to the assessment and given a stipend of $25. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the hospitals which participated, and maternal informed consent was obtained for all subjects.

The Bayley Scales of Infant Development II33 and the Communication Domain Subscale of the Battelle Developmental Inventory34 were administered. The Mental Development Index (MDI) of the Bayley Scales reflects overall cognitive development with a standard score that has a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. The Scales are normed for children up to 42 months of age. The Battelle provides a standardized measure of developmental skills in five functional domains for children from birth to age 8 years. Items of the Communication subscale in the receptive subdomain measure discrimination, recognition, and understanding of sounds, words, and gestures, whereas items in the expressive subdomain assess their production and use in communication. Receptive, expressive, and total communication scores were converted to developmental quotients (DQs) for comparison with Bayley standard scores. Battelle DQs have a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15.

Data Analysis

Student’s t tests (for continuous data), χ2 (for categorical data), or test of proportions, with correction for continuity for proportions, were employed to compare groups on demographic and medical birth characteristics, and to compare the VLBW groups in incidence of medical complications associated with preterm birth.

To assess the effects of BPD and VLBW birth on language outcomes, one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAS) were used to compare the three groups. To assess whether BPD and VLBW birth were related to specific language impairment above and beyond the effects of IQ, analyses of covariance (ANCOVAS) were conducted, using the Bayley MDI as a covariate. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were used to test the hypothesis that BPD predicted poorer language outcomes after controlling for the effects of socioeconomic (SES) and other medical risk factors. Stepwise multiple regression (MAXR) was used to understand prediction on the basis of cumulative risk and to measure the total predictive power of BPD in comparison with other perinatal risk factors. For regression analyses, only the two VLBW groups were considered because there were no medical risk factors in the term group. All medical and sociodemographic risk factors, i.e., race, socioeconomic status, multiple birth, gestational age, birth weight, neurologic risk score, presence of septicemia, patent ductus arteriosis (PDA), retinopathy of prematurity, BPD, necrotizing enterocolitis, and IVH, as well as peak bilirubin, were included in these analyses.

RESULTS

As previously described,22 infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD) were smaller and more immature than very low birth weight (VLBW) and term control infants (Table 1). The BPD group had a higher neurologic risk summary score, attributable to a higher incidence of neonatal seizures and intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) but did not differ in the occurrence of other neurologic complications of prematurity (Table 2). The BPD group had a higher incidence of retinopathy of prematurity (ROP), patent ductus arteriosis (PDA), and septicemia, and lower peak bilirubin levels. There were no differences in the incidence of failed hearing screenings as measured by brain stem-evoked response recordings at birth.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic Characteristics at Birth

VLBW with BPD
(n = 122)
VLBW (n = 84)
Term (n = 123)
F p
Mean SD % Mean SD % Mean SD %
Birthweight (g) 956 248 1252 178 3451 526 1633 .001*
Gestational age (wk) 27 2 30 2 40 1 1416 .001*
Social class 3.5 1 3.6 1 3.6 1 .7 .71
Total oxygen (days) 106 149 5 5 0 0 44.5 .001**
Race (white) 55 48 51 1.1 .58
Gender (male) 52 43 50 1.9 .39
Multiple birth 21 43 10 31.5 .001***

VLBW, very low birth weight; BPD, bronchopulmonary dysplasia.

*

BPD < VLBW < Term, p < .05

**

BPD and VLBW > Term, p < .05

***

VLBW > BPD and Term, p < .05.

Table 2.

Medical and Neurologic Complications by Group

BPD
VLBW
χ 2 p
Mean SD % Mean SD %
Minor neurologic malformations 1 1 0.1 1.00
Seizures 7 0 5.7 .02
Porencephaly 3 4 0.0 1.00
Hydrocephalus 9 5 1.3 .28
Ventriculoperitoneal shunt 3 1 0.4 .64
shunt
Meningitis 2 0 1.4 .51
Intraventricular hemorrhage 43 18 13.9 .01
  Grades I–II 28 14 5.3 .02
  Grades III–IV 15 4 6.8 .01
Periventricular leukomalacia 8 7 0.2 .78
Neurologic risk scorea 1.3 2 .58 1 3.34b .001
Necrotizing enterocolitis 4 6 .4 .74
Retinopathy of prematurity 43 4 39.2 .001
Patent ductus arteriosis 56 18 29.7 .001
Hearing abnormal 16 11 .7 .52
Peak bilirubin 8.3 3 9.1 3 1.8b .05
Septicemia 47 24 11.2 .001

BPD, bronchopulmonary dysplasia; VLBW, very low birth weight.

a

Neurologic risk score is a summary variable, calculated by summarizing the number of zeros and ones, where zero was used for absence and one for presence of the above listed neurological risk factors. The neurological risk score could range from 0–8 for an individual infant.

b

t value.

At 3 years of age, children with history of VLBW with BPD achieved language standard scores that were significantly lower than the other two groups on receptive, expressive, and total communicative competence scores. VLBW children without BPD performed significantly better than BPD children, but were significantly lower than matched term children on receptive skills. They did not differ from term children in expressive or total communication domains. A high percentage of all children had both receptive and expressive language scores that fell within the impaired range of functioning (developmental quotient [DQ] < 85) (Table 3). However, children with a history of BPD were significantly more likely to score in the impaired range than VLBW and term children. BPD was related to lower receptive language scores at age 3 years, even after controlling for the confounding effect of lower IQ, indicating specific language impairment in this domain. The effect of BPD on expressive and total communication quotients, however, was no longer significant after controlling for IQ.

Table 3.

Language, Mental, and Motor Outcomes at Three Years

BPD (n = 90)
VLBW (n = 65)
Term (n = 91)
F/x2 p
Mean SD % Mean SD % Mean SD %
Battelle Scale
 Receptive DQ 84.7 17 91.5 17 97.3 18 11.1 <.02*
 Expressive DQ 92.8 22 99.7 17 101.3 20 4.3 <.02**
 Communication DQ 89.4 21 96.4 19 99.8 20 5.9 <.005**
 Receptive DQ < 85 49 34 30 3.7 <.05**
 Expressive DQ < 85 44 25 25 4.1 <.05**
 Communication DQ < 85 43 31 28 2.3 NS
Bayley Scales (n = 98) (n = 70) (n = 95)
 MDI 83.7 24 90.0 16 96.4 12 11.2 <.0001*
 PDI 84.1 28 97.4 19 102.8 14 17.6 <.0001**

BPD, bronchopulmonary dysplasia; VLBW, very low birth weight; DQ, developmental quotient; MDI, Mental Development Index; PDI, Psychomotor Development Index; NS, not significant.

*

BPD < VLBW < Term, p < .05

**

BPD < VLBW and Term, p < .05.

Hierarchical stepwise multiple regression analyses allowed the assessment of the relationship of BPD to language measures after adjusting for the confounding factors of sociodemographic and medical risk. Because these regression results did not differ among the three domains of the Battelle, only the Total Communication DQ is described in further analyses. Results showed that BPD no longer predicted 3-year Battelle scores after controlling for other medical risk factors associated with BPD.

Rather, presence of PDA, lower socioeconomic status, minority race, and higher neurologic risk were the primary factors predicting language outcome. Table 4 presents the rank order listing of risk factors in the order of magnitude of effect and the number of DQ points lowered by the risk factor. Presence of PDA accounted for a 13-point decrement; minority race, a 6-point decrement; lower socioeconomic status, a 5-point decrement; and higher neurologic risk, a 5-point decrement.

Table 4.

The Best Four-Variable Model for Predicting the Effects of Medical and Demographic Factors on Communicative Competence

Criterion Communication DQ betaa Unstandardized R R2b F p
Step 1 Patent ductus arteriosis −.31 −13.14 .09 16.1 .001
Step 2 Socioeconomic status −.25 −4.58 .15 14.0 .001
Step 3 Neurologic risk −.19 −5.42 .19 11.8 .001
Step 4 Racec −.15 −6.27 .21 9.4 .001
a

Standardized regression coefficient.

b

Cumulative R2.

c

No other risk factor added significantly to the prediction model. These included multiple birth, necrotizingenterocolitis, septicemia, peak bilirubin, retinopathy of prematurity, gestational age, and birthweight.

Because PDA was the best single predictor of 3-year language scores and occurred more frequently in the children with BPD at birth (56%) in comparison with the VLBW controls (18%), the possibility that PDA might differentially affect language outcome within the BPD group was tested. When we compared BPD and VLBW infants with and without PDA on language outcomes, controlling for IQ, BPD was found to exacerbate the effects of PDA on language. Children with BPD and a history of PDA had a 14-point decrement in language score (Table 5) compared with a 7-point decrement in VLBW children with PDA but without BPD. Both BPD- and non-BPD-VLBW children without a history of PDA had equivalent scores.

Table 5.

Communication Developmental Quotients of BPD and VLBW Children With and Without a History of PDA

BPD
VLBW
Mean SD Mean SD
+ PDA 83.6 19 89.4 20
− PDA 97.8 19 96.8 19

BPD, bronchopulmonary dysplasia; VLBW, very low birth weight; PDA, patent ductus arteriosis.

Within the BPD group, there was a trend for the presence of PDA to be associated with a greater duration of time dependent on oxygen and more severe BPD (r [88] = .19; p < .08). However, the presence of PDA had a detrimental effect on language, even for children within the VLBW group, all of whom were on oxygen for less than 14 days.

DISCUSSION

The findings of this study support previous research suggesting that children with very low birth weight (VLBW) and with a history of bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD) are at risk for specific receptive language delays over and above speech/language delays because of lower general intelligence.27,30 Children with a history of BPD exhibited receptive language delays at 3 years of age, after the variables of IQ, race, gender, socioeconomic status, maternal education, and marital status of the parents were controlled, and were more likely than other VLBW children to fall within the impaired range of language functioning, whereas VLBW children without BPD were not different from term controls. Unlike previous studies, the current study did not find hearing loss to be more frequent in the BPD group. Thus, deficits in receptive language cannot be attributed to lower hearing acuity. However, as of yet, measures of central auditory processing in children with BPD have not been examined. The receptive language deficits observed in this sample may be related to auditory processing difficulties. The presence of BPD was, however, associated with an increase in the presence of neurologic risk factors and an increase in patent ductus arteriosis (PDA), which were the major contributors to speech-language delays. PDA and the presence of neurologic problems were the only neonatal medical risk factors to predict language development when the relative contributions of all factors were considered. As has been found in previous studies, neurologic complications were associated with poorer language outcomes.11,13-15 The neurologic factors that were more likely to be found in the BPD group included intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) and seizures, both of which were related to poorer speech language outcomes in prior studies.11,14,15,18

Previous studies have suggested that motor delays in children with BPD may affect the articulatory/motor components of speech development.27,28 Thus, the differences in expressive language skills between children with BPD and children without BPD may be partially mediated by poorer motor skills.

An unexpected finding was that the presence of PDA was the best predictor of language deficits and that the combined occurrence of PDA and BPD resulted in differentially lower language scores. In contrast, the presence of PDA was not found to be a significant risk factor in the same population when cognitive and motor outcomes of children with BPD and VLBW were evaluated at the same age,22 and thus, suggests an effect specific to language functions. The mechanisms by which a history of PDA in VLBW children may impact later language skills are unclear and the subject of speculation. PDA results in alterations in blood flow to many different organ systems and is associated with the development of bronchopulmonary dysplasia.35-38 Thus, PDA may be a marker for the most severe cases of BPD.39 Indomethacin administration, associated with treatment of BPD, has been shown to increase oxygen and surfactant requirement35 and may exacerbate the hypoxic effects of BPD. However, the negative effects of PDA on language development were also found in non-BPD VLBW children, although to a lesser extent. Anatomically, the laryngeal nerve, important in language development, crosses the ductus arteriosis and may be negatively affected by PDA. Developmentally, the presence of PDA may also affect vagal tone, an autonomic measure of cardiac efficiency. Vagal tone, as measured by respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), has been associated with measures of physiological self-regulation in early infancy and is thought to play an important role in regulatory processes via communication with brain stem nuclei implicated in attention and communication.40

Lower socioeconomic status and minority race also predicted poorer language outcomes although caution regarding interpretation of the relationship between race and scores on the language measure should be taken. African-American children from low socioeconomic status families often use language systems that differ from standard American English, which place them at a disadvantage on standardized tests of language development.41,42 The fact that a large percentage of all groups of children scored one standard deviation or more below the mean on this test supports this view and also reflects the large proportion of lower socioeconomic families found disproportionately in VLBW cohorts. Moreover, because race and socioeconomic factors covary in the United States, it is difficult to specify the extent to which they are independently associated with developmental performance.43 Family income and parental educational level have been shown to be more significant influences on development than race or ethnicity.44 In the present study, however, whereas minority race and low socioeconomic status were confounded, medical and neurologic complications were not associated with socioeconomic status or race.

The findings of this study also suggest that medical factors associated with BPD have a greater impact on receptive than on expressive language skills. Significant differences between groups were found on the receptive language quotient but not on the expressive language quotient after controlling for IQ, supporting a previous study of school age VLBW children.30 Several explanations may be proposed for the discrepancy between receptive and expressive language scores. First, the language test itself may be more sensitive to receptive language difficulties than expressive problems. The scores of all three groups reflected poorer performance on the receptive subscale than the expressive. A second explanation may be that receptive and expressive language skills differ in the underlying genetic, neurological, and cognitive processes that they use and that both BPD and receptive language processes may share similar genetic bases.36 Third, receptive language abilities may be more closely tied to auditory processing skills and the speed of the processing of sensory input, than are expressive language abilities.40,45 The effects of BPD and other concomitant sequelae of prematurity, such as IVH, seizures, and hypoxia, may have specific negative effects on language comprehension, whereas the effects of BPD on expressive language may be mediated through the effects of BPD on overall intelligence.

This study highlights the need to consider both the medical complications of prematurity and the sociodemographic factors in the follow-up of VLBW infants.22,46 Neurologic complications and a history of BPD and PDA conferred substantial additional risk for language delays in VLBW children. As has been found for other developmental outcomes,22 severity of medical complications rather than lower birth weight and shorter gestation, per se, predicted outcomes within the VLBW cohort.

Early speech and language surveillance of VLBW children with these neurologic and medical complications is indicated, especially when combined with risk from low socioeconomic status and minority status. Early intervention programs should focus on the development of receptive language skills, auditory processing abilities, and speech-sound production, as well as more general language, cognitive, and motor development. Future prospective longitudinal studies that follow children with a history of BPD into early school age are needed to explore how early medical complications of prematurity interact with environmental risk factors to impact language development. Delineating relationships between specific medical complications of prematurity and specific child outcomes may lead to better identification of VLBW children who are at greater risk for later learning problems and may illuminate the biological mechanisms implicated in the adverse effects of VLBW and prematurity.

Acknowledgments.

This work was supported by Grants MCJ 390592 and MCJ 39017 from the Maternal and Child Health Bureau (Title V, Social Security Act) Health Resources and Services Administration and the NIH Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (HL38193). Thanks are extended to the participating families and to Sarah Fulton, Marilyn Davillier, Peggy Bruening, Karen Sofranko, and Jie Huang for research assistance.

Contributor Information

LYNN T. SINGER, Department of Pediatrics, Divisions of Behavioral Pediatrics and Psychology, Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine

A. CAROL SIEGEL, Department of Pediatrics, Divisions of Behavioral Pediatrics and Psychology, Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine

BARBARA LEWIS, Department of Pediatrics, Divisions of Behavioral Pediatrics and Psychology, Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine

SUZANNE HAWKINS, Department of Pediatrics, Divisions of Behavioral Pediatrics and Psychology, Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine

TOYOKO YAMASHITA, Department of Critical Care, Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine

JILL BALEY, Department of Neonatology, Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine, Cleveland, Ohio

REFERENCES

  • 1.Casiro OG, Moddemann DM, Stanwick RS, Cheang MS, Cowan H: The national history and predictive value of early language delays in very low birthweight infants. Early Hum Dev 26:45–50, 1991 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Weisglas-Kuperus N, Baerts W, DeGraff MA, van Zanten GA, Sauer PJ: Hearing and language in preschool very low birthweight children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 26:129–140, 1993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Aram D, Hack M, Hawkins S, Weissman B, Borawski-Clark E: Very low birthweight children and speech and language development. J Speech Hear Res 34:1169–1179, 1991 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Tomblin JB, Smith E, Zhang X: Epidemiology of specific language impairment: Prenatal and perinatal risk factors. J Commun Disord 30:325–343, 1997 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hirata T, Epcar J, Walsh A, et al. : Survival and outcome of infants 510–750 grams: A six year experience. J Pediatr 102:741–748, 1983 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Eilers B, Desai N, Wilson M, Cunningham M: Classroom performance and social factors of children with birthweights of 1,250 grams or less: Follow-up at 5 to 8 years of age. Pediatrics 77:203–208, 1986 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kenworthy O, Bess F, Stahlman M, Lindstrom D: Hearing, speech, and language outcome in infants with extreme immaturity. Am J Otolaryngol 8:419–425, 1987 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lloyd BW, Wheldall K, Perks D: Controlled study of intelligence and school performance of very low-birthweight children from a defined geographical area. Dev Med Child Neurol 30:36–42, 1988 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Michelsson K, Lindahl E, Parre M, Helenius M: Nine year follow up of infants weighing 1,500 grams or less at birth. Acta Paediatr Scand 73:835–841, 1984 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rickards AL, Ford GW, Kitchen WH, Doyle LW, Lissenden JV, Keith CG: Extremely-low-birthweight infants: Neurological, psychological, growth, and health status beyond five years of age. Med J Aust 147:476–481, 1987 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sostek A, Smith Y, Katz K, Grant T: Developmental outcome of preterm infants with intraventricular hemorrhage at one and two years of age. Child Dev 58:779–786, 1987 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.McAllister L, Masel C, Tudehope D, O’Callaghan M, Mohay H, Rogers Y: Speech and language outcomes 3 years after neonatal intensive care. Eur J Disord Commun 28:369–382, 1993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.McAllister L, Masel C, Tudehope D, O’Callaghan M, Mohay H, Rogers Y: Speech and language outcomes in preschool-aged survivors of neonatal intensive care. Eur J Disord Commun 28:383–394, 1993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Janowsky J, Nass R: Early language development in infants with cortical and subcortical perinatal brain injury. J Dev Behav Pediatr 8:3–7, 1987 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bendersky M, Lewis M: Early language ability as a function of ventricular dilation associated with intraventricular hemorrhage. J Dev Behav Pediatr 11:17–21, 1990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Byers-Brown B, Bendersky M, Chapman T: The early utterances of preterm infants. Br J Disord Commun 21:307–319, 1986 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Grunau RV, Kearney SM, Whitfield MF: Language development at three years in preterm children of birthweight below 1000 g. Br J Disord Commun 25:173–182, 1990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Trounce JQ, Rutter N, Levene GI: Periventricular leukomalacia and intraventricular hemorrhage in the preterm neonate. Arch Dis Child 61:1196–1202, 1986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Frisk V, Whyte H: The long-term consequences of periventricular brain damage on language and memory. Dev Neuropsychol 10:313–333, 1994 [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Northway W, Rosan R, Porter D: Pulmonary disease following respiratory therapy of hyaline membrane disease. N Engl J Med 276:3557–3560, 1967 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Northway W: Bronchopulmonary dysplasia: Then and now. Arch Dis Child 65:1076–1081, 1990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Singer LT, Yamashita TS, Lilien L, Collin M, Baley J: A longitudinal study of infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia and very low birthweight. Pediatrics 100:987–993, 1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Singer L, Davillier M, Preuss L, Hawkins S, Yamashita T, Baley J: Feeding interactions in infants with very low birthweight and bronchopulmonary dysplasia. J Dev Behav Pediatr 11:17–21, 1995 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Singer L, Martin R, Hawkins S, Benson-Szekely L, Yamashita T, Carlo W: Oxygen desaturation complicates feeding in infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia after discharge. Pediatrics 90:380–384, 1992 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Garg M, Kurzner S, Bautista D, Keens T: Clinically unsuspected hypoxia during sleep and feeding in infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Pediatrics 81:635–642, 1988 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kurzner S, Garg M, Bautista D: Growth failure in infants with BPD: Nutrition and elevated resting metabolic expenditure. Pediatrics 1:379–384, 1988 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sauve R, Singhal N: Long term morbidity of infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Pediatrics 76:725–733, 1985 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Perlman J, Volpe J: Movement disorder of premature infants with severe bronchopulmonary dysplasia: A new syndrome. Pediatrics 84:215–218, 1989 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Volpe J: Neurology of the Newborn, 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA, W.B. Saunders, Co., 1992 [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Robertson CT, Ethes PC, Goldson E, Kyle JM: Eight-year school performance, neurodevelopmental and growth outcome of neonates with bronchopulmonary dysplasia: A comparative study. Pediatrics 89:365–372, 1992 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ballard J, Novak K, Driver M: A simplified assessment of fetal maturation of newly born infants. J Pediatr 95:769–774, 1979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Papile L, Burnstein J, Burnstein R, Koffler H: Incidence and evolution of subependynal and intraventricular hemorrhage: A study of infants with birthweights less than 1,500 grams. J Pediatr 92:529–534, 1978 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bayley N: Bayley Scales of Infant Development. San Antonio, TX, Psychological Corporation, Harcourt, Brace, & Co., 1993 [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Newborg J, Stock J, Winek C, Guidubaldi J, Svinicki J: The Battelle Developmental Inventory. Allen, TX, DLM Teaching Resources, 1993 [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yaseen H, Alumram K, Ali H, Rustrum M, Danvich M, Fararidy A: Effects of early indomethacin administration on oxygenation and surfactant requirement in low birthweight infants. J Trop Pediatr 43:42–46, 1997 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Parker RA, Lindstrom DP, Cotton RB: Evidence from twin study implies possible genetic susceptibility to bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Semin Perinatol 20:206–209, 1996 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gonzalez A, Sosendo IR, Chandar J, Hammler H, Claure N, Bancalari E: Influence of infection on patent ductus arteriosis and chronic lung disease in premature infants weighing 1000 grams or less. J Pediatr 128:470–478, 1996 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Zenerdo V, Trevisanuto D, Dini C, et al. : Silent patent ductus arteriosus and bronchopulmonary dysplasia in low birthweight infants. J Perinat Med 23:493–499, 1995 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Farstad T, Brattid D: Pulmonary effects of closure of patent ductus arteriosus in premature infants with severe respiratory distress syndrome. Eur J Pediatr 153:903–905, 1994 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Porges SW: Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Physiological basis, quantitative methods, and clinical implications, in Grossman P, Janssen K, Vaitl D (eds): Cardiorespiratory and Cardiosomatic Psychophysiology. New York, NY, Plenum Press, 1986, pp 101–115 [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Craig HK: The challenges of conducting language research with African-American children, in Kamhi AG, Pollack KE, Harris JL (eds): Communication Development and Disorders in African-American Children. Baltimore, MD, Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc., 1996 [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stockman IJ: Phonological development and disorders in African American children, in Kamhi AG, Pollack KE, Harris JL (eds): Communication Development and Disorders in African American Children. Baltimore, MD, Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc., 1996 [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Huston AC, McLoyd VC, Garcia-Coll C: Children and poverty: Issues in contemporary research. Child Dev 65:275–282, 1994 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Klebanov PR, Brooks-Gunn J, McCarton C, McCormick MC: The contribution of neighborhood and family income to developmental test scores over the first three years of life. Child Dev 69:1420–1436, 1998 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Tallal P, Miller S, Fitch RH: Neurobiological basis of speech: A case for the preeminence of temporal processing. Ann N Y Acad Sci 682:27–47, 1996 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lewis M, Bendersky M: Cognitive and motor differences among low birthweight infants: Impact of intraventricular hemorrhage, medical risk, and social class. Pediatrics 83:187–192, 1989 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES