Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2023 Jun 7;18(6):e0286811. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286811

Gender-based disparities and biases in science: An observational study of a virtual conference

Junhanlu Zhang 1,*, Rachel Torchet 2, Hanna Julienne 2,3,*
Editor: Andrew R Dalby4
PMCID: PMC10246795  PMID: 37285372

Abstract

Success in STEM (Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math) remains influenced by race, gender, and socioeconomic status. Here, we focus on the impact of gender on question-asking behavior during the 2021 JOBIM virtual conference (Journées Ouvertes en Biologie et Mathématiques). We gathered quantitative and qualitative data including : demographic information, question asking motivations, live observations and interviews of participants. Quantitative analyses include unprecedented figures such as the fraction of the audience identifying as LGBTQIA+ and an increased attendance of women in virtual conferences. Although parity was reached in the audience, women asked half as many questions as men. This under-representation persisted after accounting for seniority of the asker. Interviews of participants highlighted several barriers to oral expression encountered by women and gender minorities : negative reactions to their speech, discouragement to pursue a career in research, and gender discrimination/sexual harassment. Informed by the study, guidelines for conference organizers have been written. The story behind the making of this study has been highlighted in a Nature Career article.

Introduction

Gender equity and diversity are key drivers of scientific productivity and innovation. Indeed, women are more likely to include a sex and gender axis into their research [1] as well as challenge long-standing biased practices [2]. Despite ongoing recent efforts, gender disparities and gender biases continue to plague academia and to prevent researchers from reaching their full scientific potential. Especially in STEM (Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math), women encounter various obstacles to career advancement including : hiring discrimination [3], exacerbate skepticism about their contributions [4, 5], and threatening academic climates [69]. These biases are observed throughout women’s participation in scientific research. For instance, women are disadvantaged in the publication process: women publish less than men [10], are less cited [11], and are less likely to be in the first position among authors who contributed equally [12]. Gender disparities are also noticeable on less externally constrained occasions such as question sessions in academic seminars and conferences with women asking significantly less questions than expected [1318].

Investigators documented this phenomenon in a variety of scientific domains (e.g. genetics [13], neurology [15], hematology [17]) establishing its generality. However, to the extent of our knowledge, no face-to-face interview of conference participants was conducted, leaving the underlying causes unknown. To date, previous study settings were live conferences. The dynamic of gender differences in question-asking behaviors in online conferences could deviate from in-person conferences. For instance, it might be less intimidating to write questions in a chatbox than to stand up and ask a question aloud in front of the audience. This dynamic is important to document since online or hybrid conferences could persist from now on for sanitary, practical, or ecological reasons. The interaction between professional status and gender on question-asking at scientific conferences was only partially investigated so far as it is difficult to know precisely the name and status of askers in in-person conferences. The interplay between these factors could be of interest for several reasons. First, previous reports showed that giving the first question to a student makes the session more welcoming to women [13]. Second, in non-academic settings (United States Senate), a study documented that powerful women speak briefly by fear of backlash while powerful men do speak lengthily [19]. The online setting allows for a finer identification of question askers and to retrieve both gender and professional status. The observation of an online conference might hence shed more light on this power dynamic in STEM.

Here, we aim to document the impact of a virtual setting, gender, and professional status on question-asking behavior during scientific conferences and to provide hints to mitigate the underrepresentation of women and gender minorities amongst question askers. We comprehensively observed the JOBIM (Journées Ouvertes en Biologie et Mathématiques) 2021 conference (S1 Fig). This conference is a convivial, medium scale event (3˜00 to 600 participants) that has been gathering annually the French bioinformatics community for the last 20 years. The JOBIM 2021 conference was for the second time held online due to the COVID-19 pandemic (The JOBIM conference was held online for the first time in 2020). The virtual setting offers us a unique opportunity to understand the differences between in-person and online question-asking from a gender perspective. During the conference registration, we collected demographic data and included the possibility to disclose sexual orientation as an important demographic characteristic often left out in previous studies [20]. We attempt to step out of the historical or typical binary vision of gender by giving the attendees the opportunity to report their self-identified genders with options other than “man” and “woman”. We also investigated the reasons behind women and gender minorities under-representation in question askers by conducting in-depth interviews of participants and a post-conference survey.

We believe that to successfully address women and gender minorities’ (by gender minority we refer to individuals identifying outside of the binary gender categories that are male or female) under-representation at scientific conferences, we need a clear picture of its causes that can only be derived by studying their overall experience. We therefore adopted an evidence-based and mix-method approach to delve into gender-based disparities in science. Being at the interface between hard and soft sciences, this study integrates qualitative and quantitative research methodologies. Considering the complex and intangible nature of gender biases, we believe that bringing in-depth qualitative analysis into the picture is unarguably important, as illustrated by previous work [21]. In this study, we provide an overview of the evolution of the JOBIM conferences demographics across two decades and an analysis of question-asking behavior. We then contextualize these results with the perception of JOBIM participant collected through a post conference survey and interviews. The story behind the making of this study has been highlighted in a Nature Career article [22].

Materials and methods

Demographic analysis of previous conferences

Anonymous data on previous editions on attendees, speakers and keynote speakers was provided by the SFBI. When asked at registration, gender was inferred from civility. For editions where civility was not recorded, gender was inferred by the SFBI from the first name of the participants before the anonymous data was handed to us. Gender most commonly associated with the first name was looked up in a public database of French first name (https://www.data.gouv.fr/en/datasets/liste-de-prenoms/). The quality of such a procedure was assessed using data from the 2021 editions by comparing the gender proportion assessed by this method and the one measured by self-identified gender (S1 Table). While gender could not be identified from the first name in 12.5% of the cases, the inferred gender proportion was still accurate and equivalent to the proportion computed with self-identified gender (two-sample proportion test p-val 0.94, self-identified proportion : 0.508, first name proportion : 0.503).

To assess the proportion of women in committees, the names of committee members were retrieved from the website of past and current editions of JOBIM (from 2015 to 2021). Each committee member was assigned a gender from their first name by looking up the corresponding gender in the first name database. When gender could not be assigned from the first name alone (notably for rare or foreign first name for instance), the gender was assigned by looking up a photograph of the committee member. The gender assigned by this procedure is the gender as perceived by society and might differ from the gender identity.

Registration and post-conference surveys

To understand the factors that potentially contribute to attendees’ question-asking behavior during the conference, we invited all attendees’ to fill out relevant questions through online surveys. Registration survey is a mandatory step when the attendees sign up for the JOBIM conference. In the survey, apart from asking regular questions on the attendees’ demographic profiles including age and country of residence, we also gave the opportunity for the respondents to share personal information on their self-identified gender, preferred pronouns and whether or not they identify with the LGBTQIA+ community. We made sure to acknowledge all survey respondents that sharing their personal information is not a mandatory part of the registration procedure, which means not answering those questions will have no impact on their registration to the conference.

A survey was also sent after the JOBIM 2021 conference. The purpose of the post-conference survey is to collect data related to the attendees’ question-asking behavior during the conference as well as their previous experiences with and general opinions on academic conferences in STEM. Therefore, we divided the post-conference survey into three sections: contact information, question-asking during the JOBIM 2021 conference, and general experiences at conferences (Fig 3). On the 695 participants at JOBIM 2021, 525 consented to the use of their data for this study and 152 answered the post-survey.

Observation of the virtual conference

During the JOBIM 2021 conference, an observational study was conducted to collect data that is relevant to attendees’ question-asking behaviors. Given the fact that the conference took place virtually on the videoconferencing platform Zoom, the research team gathered observational data through : 1) an observation forms filled out by a team of observers and 2) attendance reports generated from Zoom.

The observation team consisted of 8 observers (6 women, 2 men), including researchers from the research team as well as conference attendees who volunteered to support the observational study. Prior to the conference, an online version of the observation form was developed and tested to assist the recording of observational data. During the conference, observers followed a set of guidelines (S1 File) to observe the planned sessions for observation and to fill out the observation form accordingly. The observation form was a key tool to collect data on question-asking behavior, for example, the number of questions asked during each session, type of the questions, and gender of the question askers (gender of the askers was identified by the observers). To minimize the human errors that could potentially occur during observation, each session was observed by at least 2 observers. By the end of each session, observers submitted the observation forms online. Zoom attendance and Q&A report were exported for each session. The gender of question askers was identified using the following procedure: 1) Use self-identifying gender from the registration survey when available, 2) Identified from first name, 3) in last resort, we queried the name of the asker through Google search engine and identified gender from a portrait. Q&A report were manually curated to remove comments containing no question (“thanks”, “clap-clap”, …). In rare occurrences, one question was spread over several postings. In this case the question was counted as 1 item.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis were conducted using R/3.6.3. Underrepresentation of women and gender minorities in question askers compared to attendees was tested using the exact Fisher test. Comparisons of postsurvey answers between genders were performed though a χ2-contingency table test.

For the Poisson regression of the rate of question asking by demographics, we computed the number of questions asked by each attendee by counting their corresponding entries in Zoom’s “Q_n_A” reports and the total time spend in conference by summing up the “Time in Session (minutes)” column in the Zoom’s “Attendee” reports. The rate of question asking was the number of questions asked over the total time spent following the conference. The model retained by the AIC criteria (S2 Table) is

ln(Y)=β0+β1×(Gender=Male)+β2×(Age35)+β3×(ProfessionalStatus=permanent)+ε (1)

where Y is rate of question asking.

Semi-structured and in-depth interviews

Qualitative data was collected through 7 semi-structured and in-depth interviews 2–3 weeks after the JOBIM 2021 conference. he number of interviews we were able to conduct is relatively low (7) and our qualitative observations might not generalize to the complete academic population. Yet, these interviews provide valuable examples of career trajectories impacted by gender and by the LGBTQIA+ status. This contextualization can help generate hypothesis on why women and gender minorities ask less questions than men. To reflect the diversity of the population studied and to respect the intersectional basis of this study, we created a customized interviewee selection procedure based on the principle of quota sampling. Attendees’ age/seniority, gender, nationality, membership of the LGBTQIA+ community as well as activeness (activeness specifically refers to number of questions asked during the conference) during the conference were taken into consideration. Attendees who responded that they were interested in or may be interested in the post-conference interview during the registration process were categorised into the following subgroups: “junior male attendees” who self-identify as male and are under 35 years old, “junior female attendees” who self-identify as female and are under 35 years old, “senior male attendees” who self-identify as male and are above 44 years old, “senior female attendees” who self-identify as female and are above 44 years old, “foreign female attendees” who self-identify as female and are foreign (non-French) nationals; “LGBTQIA+ attendees” who self-reported as members of the LGBTQIA+ community, and “serial question askers” who asked 4 and more than 4 questions during the conference. A random interviewee was selected from each of the subgroups and contacted for the interview. It is worth mentioning that, due to the lack of response from the contacted interviewee candidates, the interviewee of the subgroup “senior male attendees” was identified among the younger age group “between 35 and 44 years old”. All interviewees agreed to attend an one-to-one online interview session on the voluntary basis. Prior to the interview, each interviewee was asked to carefully read and sign the consent form (S2 File) which provides description of the project, detailed information about the interview as well as data privacy and protection policies.

Following the principles of semi-structured interview, we developed an interview guide (S3 File) with the purpose of ensuring the organisation and flexibility of the interviews. The interview guide served as a checklist of topics to be covered and provided a set of interview questions. Although the questions were designed beforehand to guide the conversation, the interviewer prioritized open-ended questions and encouraged two-way communication during the interview in order to collect qualitative and in-depth information. All the interviews were recorded via the video-conference platform (All the interviews were conducted on ZOOM) and the recordings were later used to produced interview transcripts. The interviews were between 33 and 58 minutes.

Qualitative data analysis

To analyse data collected from in-depth interviews, we adopted qualitative coding as the key method to transform data into findings with the support of one of the popular CAQDAS (Computer-assisted Qualitative Data analysis Software), MAXQDA. The process of analysing qualitative data in this study combined inductive and deductive approaches. On the one hand, an initial codebook S3 Table) was developed prior to coding, based on research questions and interview guidelines. Based on the codebook, researchers were able to apply predefined codes to the interview data. On the other hand, line-by-line coding was performed on all interview transcripts. Associating with the constructivist grounded theory, this approach made sure that close attention was paid to all collected data and that researchers had the opportunity to extract as much information as possible from the data. To generate conclusions from the coded data, the initial codes—including parent codes and child codes—were grouped into concepts of higher order named categories (S4 Table). As a result, the categories were developed into themes which represent the significant findings of the study. Developing themes involve a necessary process of comparing identified codes regarding their importance, particularly based on the following two measures: 1) the frequency of a code occurring within one specific information source, and 2) the frequency of the code occurring throughout all information sources.

Ethic statement

Participation in this study was independent of JOBIM 2021 conference attendance. All of the people who took part in the study gave explicit and written consent to the processing of their data for the research project, in particular to the processing of sensitive data within the meaning of article 9 of the GDPR (data concerning sex life and sexual orientation), in accordance with the requirements of the GDPR. The personal and sensitive data collected in the context of this study was deleted once the objectives of the study were fulfilled. The detailled data policy was also available on the project page (https://research.pasteur.fr/en/project/jobim-2021-pilot-project-gender-speaking-differences-in-academia/). The data processing has been entered in the records of Institut Pasteur processing activities with the assistance of the Institut Pasteur data protection officer. This study was declared exempt of ethical concerns by the Institut Pasteur IRB (IRB00006966).

Results

Demographic of current and past JOBIM editions

JOBIM targets young professionals in bioinformatics and is known as a convivial event where attendees participate in a series of scientific talks and social events. A striking feature of JOBIM 2021 conference confirms its popularity among young attendees: based on the registration form, a majority of attendees declared they are under 35 years of age (62%, Fig 1A). Parity between female and male attendees has been reached for age categories under 45 (52% of women) but not among attendees who belong to the older age category (38%, p-val = 0.047, Fig 1A). Concerning gender, the vast majority of attendees identified themselves within the binary gender categories (i.e. ‘Male’ and ‘Female’). However, 13 attendees preferred other categories (1 agender, 4 Non-binaries, 8 prefer not to say). A substantial fraction (9.5%) of participants self-identified as members of the LGBTQIA+ (Fig 1B). Answering Yes to the question ‘Are you a member of the LGBTQIA+ community?’ was more likely in young attendees (Fig 1B). Women attendees were also more likely to identify as members of the LGBTQIA+ community (13% in women versus 5% in men), which is consistent with a previous report stating that lesbian and bisexual women are as likely to graduate and work in STEM (compared to straight women), whereas gay men tend to drop out more than straight men [23].

Fig 1. Demographics of JOBIM conferences in current and past editions.

Fig 1

A) Count bar plot of the number of participants at the JOBIM 2021 conference by age category. Colors represent the count of each gender. B) Count bar plot of the number of participants at the JOBIM 2021 conference by age category. Colors represent the attendee answer to the question: “Do you identify as a member of the LGBTQIA+ community?”. (C-F) The proportion of women with respect to JOBIM edition year in : C) participants, D) contributed speakers, E) organization and program committees (line types represent the type of committee), F) keynote speakers. In plot C, D, E, and F vertical bars represent the standard error on the proportion estimate. Stars indicate a significant deviation from parity.

We equally retrieved the number of participants and their gender from past editions (inferred from first name, Materials and methods). The proportion of women in the audience has increased steadily at a slow pace from the first edition in 2000 to 2019 (Fig 1C). Yet, in all these editions, women were significantly underrepresented in participants compared to the general population. In 2020, JOBIM became virtual due to the COVID-19 pandemics. Simultaneously, the proportion of women increased and was no longer significantly below 50% for the 2021 edition. To disentangle the slow increase of women proportion in the field with the effect of virtual conferences, we compared the proportion of women in the 2 last virtual editions with the 2 last in-person editions. The women proportion difference between virtual editions and recent in-person editions was highly significant (in-person proportion: 40%, virtual proportion : 48%, p-value = 3.4 × 10–4) suggesting that virtual conferences might increase women attendance. The proportion of women in contributed speakers (who submitted an abstract to the conference and were selected for giving an oral presentation.) varies with the year and is not always significantly below 0.5 (Fig 1D). Yet, amongst the 13 JOBIM editions with available data, 6 significantly lacked women contributed speakers. We retrieved the number of posters presented for this edition and we did not observe a gender imbalance (S5 Table). An improving trend is noticeable in the composition of committees and keynote speakers. The proportion of women in the program committee has increased in 2018 and remains stable since (Fig 1E). No “manel” in keynotes was observed since 2010 (Fig 1F). Interestingly, the parity in the program committee and keynote speakers seems to be simultaneous. These tendencies are in line with previous reports [24] showing that the presence of women in committee efficiently diminished the chance of invited speakers being all men.

Written question asking

Throughout the JOBIM 2021 conference, 192 questions where asked through the chatbox during a variety of scientific sessions: 57 questions asked by women, 115 by men, and 20 by anonymous or group attendees. No question was asked by a person from a gender minority (agender, nonbinary, or transgender). Since gender parity was reached among the conference attendees, women were significantly underrepresented in question askers (Exact fisher test p-value : 3.1e-05, Fig 2A and 2B). We noticed a tendency for a stronger underrepresentation in ‘Mini-symposia’, which are series of technical talks featuring invited speakers, and less obvious in the contributed talks, which usually feature more junior speakers (Fig 2A). This imbalance is further reinforced when weighting the questions by their length (Fig 2B). Similarly, previous reports stated that in live conferences men ask more oral questions and slightly longer ones [13, 14]. The proportion of women in sessions did not deviate strongly from parity in attendees (range:43% to 57%) while being under 50% in 14 sessions out of 23 for questions asked (range: 0 to 66%). The number of questions asked by members of the LGBTQIA+ community was slightly lower than expected based on the registration data, yet this tendency was not significant (Exact Fisher test odds ratio : 0.4, p-value: 0.067).

Fig 2. Written question asking during the JOBIM 2021 conference and gender.

Fig 2

A) Proportion of questions asked by women with respect to the proportion of women in attendees for each session. The color and shape of point represent the session type. Horizontal and vertical bars represent the standard error on the proportion in askers and attendees respectively. The black diagonal line represent the expected proportion in askers accounting for the proportion in attendees. B) Barplot of the proportion of women in: 1) attendees, 2) questions asked 3) total number of characters written. C) Count of askers by the total number of question they asked throughout the conference. Colors in barplot represents the gender of the asker.

We leveraged the Zoom exports to compute the number of questions asked by participants throughout the JOBIM 2021 conference (Materials and methods). The vast majority of attendees did not ask questions during the conference (87%). The overrepresentation of men is more pronounced in attendees asking several questions throughout the conference (Fig 2C).

To understand the interplay between professional status, age, gender and being members of the LGBTQIA+ community, we modelled the rate of question asking as a function of these factors using a Poisson regression (Materials and methods). We compared several models including different sets of variables. According to the AIC criteria (Akaike information criterion), the two best models comprised gender, age, and status and the interaction between status and gender as predictors (S2 Table). However, the interaction between status or age and gender was not significant on its own. Hence we focused on the model including age, status and gender. When accounting for age and professional status, the effect of gender remained significant, with men asking twice as many questions as other genders (Table 1). Even though JOBIM is considered as student-friendly conference, the effect of age and professional status weighed heavily on the rate of questions asked with senior academics asking 4.6 times more questions than junior academics. When modulating this effect by gender, senior women and gender minorities would ask 2.3 more questions than junior men, while senior men would ask 9.3 more questions than a junior women or gender minorities. Note that while we did not find an interaction between gender and seniority using Poisson regression, the multiplicative nature of the model implies that seniority for men results in a larger increase of the rate of question asking than in other gender categories (S2 Fig).

Table 1. Model of the rate of question asking by hour as a function of gender, age and professional status.

A Poisson Regression of the rate of question asking as a function of gender, age and professional status was fitted to the data. The exponent of the intercept corresponds to the question-asking rate of an attendee who is a woman or a gender minority, under 35 and have a short-term contract. The exponents of other coefficients give the multiplicative factor to apply to the rate when the condition is verified.

β e β p-value
Intercept -4.34 1.13 × 10−2 -
Gender = Male 0.7 2.01 1.34 × 10−4
Age > 35 0.71 2.02 1.05 × 10−2
Permanent position 0.823 2.28 5.5 × 10−3

Questions asked or read out loud

The results could differ from the preceding section due to a few questions asked orally by the attendees, the questions asked by the chairperson, and the selection effect of the chairperson when reading questions out loud from the chatbox. We counted 257 questions by observing all JOBIM 66 talks. Only 78 questions (30.3%) where asked by women despite a proportion of attendees close to parity (woman proportion : 50.2%). The difference between the two proportions is highly significant (proportion test p-value: 4.44e-10). We did not observe a specific effect of the gender of speakers or chairpersons, contrarily to previous studies (although the sign of the effect was not consistent from one study to another) [13, 14]. The number of chairwomen and chairmen was equivalent in the JOBIM 2021 conference (36 and 37 respectively) and the number of question they asked followed the global trend with chairwomen asking less questions (16 and 31 respectively, OR = 0.53).

The total number of questions asked at the end of a talk depends on the type of session, with keynotes receiving twice as many questions (p-value: 0.01e-2) as mini-symposia talks and contributed talks (S3A Fig). While observing a trend consistent with a previous report [14], we did not find a significant positive effect of the total number of questions asked during the question session on women and gender minority representation (S3B Fig).

Post survey results

Participants shared their thoughts on what factors influenced their question-asking behaviors and on their experiences at scientific conferences in general. Of the 695 registered participants, 144 shared their feedbacks with us through a post-conference survey (Material and methods).

Overall, factors encouraging, discouraging or motivating question asking were considered to be the same by all genders (Fig 3A–3C), which suggests that a welcoming environment would be the same for everyone independently of gender. Top impacting factors are similar research interests between the asker and the speaker, clarity of the talk, and confidence. Note that our results differ from [14] which showed that factor hindering question-asking where rated as more important by women. This might be due to the survey design : [14] used Likert scales (where respondents are asked how much a factor matters using a scale from one to five) whereas we asked respondents to say if a factor mattered or not in a binary way.

Fig 3. Post survey results.

Fig 3

A) fraction of respondent answering these factors encourage them to ask questions, B) fraction of respondent answering these factors discourage them to ask questions, C) fraction of respondent answering these factors motivate them to ask questions, D) Likert scale on the perceived importance of several factors on question asking E) conference experience items significantly differ among genders.

Gender did not impact the perception of what demographic factor was important regarding question-asking Fig 3D. The effects of age and seniority were clearly perceived by respondents with respectively 80% and 94% of respondents selecting “it plays an important role” or “it plays a crucial role”. In contrast, 38% of respondents thought that gender was an important or a crucial factor for question asking (Fig 3D). While none of the respondents thought that women asked more questions than men, a large fraction 50% was “not sure about” which gender ask more questions.

While for most survey items all genders provided similar answers, there were a few notable exceptions (Fig 3E). Women and gender minorities declared to be less motivated to ask questions at conferences in general (Chi-squared test p-value = 6.4e10–3). Women and gender minorities were more likely to declare that they have or might have experienced discrimination or harassment during conferences (answering “Maybe” or “Yes”, Exact Fisher test p-value = 4.0e10–3, OR = 4.6). This observation is coherent with previous reports stating that women experience is overall harsher in STEM than men experience [2527]. This strongly emphasizes the need for guidelines and for a reporting system during conferences.

In-depth interviews : Gender-based differences experienced at scientific conferences

To contextualize quantitative findings we conducted in depth interviews on a representative set of participants. The number of interviews we were able to conduct is relatively low (7) and our qualitative observations might not generalize to the complete academic population. Yet, these interviews provide valuable examples on how gender and LGBTQIA+ status can impact career trajectories which could lead to a lesser comfort in question asking.

We selected an interviewee for each of the 7 classes identified during the quantitative analysis (Material and methods) : “junior male attendees”, “junior female attendees”, “senior male attendees”, “senior female attendees”, “foreign female attendees”, “LGBTQIA+ attendees”, and “serial question askers”. By analyzing the transcripts of 7 interviews, we developed a codebook collecting recurring themes in interviews (Material and methods, S4 Fig, S3 Table).

Findings indicate 4 main recurring topics: 1) women and gender minority attendees report negative experiences based on sexual orientation, gender identity and expression in professional contexts; 2) there is a significant correlation among gender identity, confidence and career advancement; 3) women and gender minorities are more proactive to challenge gender inequalities in the workplace; and 4) women report experiencing gender-based discrimination and harassment during scientific activities, including academic conferences.

Women and gender minority attendees report negative experiences based on sexual orientations, gender identities, and expressions

Negative experiences reported by women and gender minorities during interviews can be grouped into two types—“discouragement” and “professional abilities being undermined” due to their sexual orientation, gender identity, and expression. Three female interviewees stated that they felt discouraged to ask questions in conferences or in the workplace by experiencing or witnessing negative reaction to women’s speeches.

“When she (female team leader) was open to ask questions or even to present, she was very…they (male team leaders) were not nice with her.” (I4—Senior foreign female attendee)

Another female interviewee expressed that she was discouraged to become a scientific researcher already in the early stage of career planning.

“My parents were saying that I should be a teacher instead of a researcher, (this way) I would have more holidays for my children. At the time I thought… wow I don’t want it. I don’t want to my professional to depend on holidays and kids. I want to be a researcher. I must say that… my brother who is younger than me, they (the parents) were not saying that to him.” (I3—Senior female attendee)

Regarding women and gender minority professional abilities being undermined, 2 female and gender minority interviewees indicated that they were either labelled or alienated based on their sexual orientation, gender identity and expression.

“I was often known as the ‘gender nerd’ in my field. I am not sure I want to be that, because sometimes you are perceived to be just that.” (I5—Junior female attendee)

Correlation between gender identity, confidence and career advancement

Qualitative analysis of the interview transcripts suggests a correlation between gender identity, confidence and career advancement. To be specific, 3 female and gender minority interviewees pointed out that their gender identity impacted their level of confidence.

“…That’s also possible (that men speakers are more confident). Maybe they just don’t often have negative reactions to their questions.” (I2—LGBTQIA+ attendee)

In turn, 3 female and gender minority interviewees stated that confidence is a key factor which influences their career advancement.

“Interviewer: what do you consider as the biggest barrier in your career path?.

Interviewee:I think (it’s) self-confidence. Self-confidence is really impacted by how society reflects me as a woman and the ways genders are expressed.” (I5—Junior female attendee)

Furthermore, the findings also show that women and gender minority might improve their level of confidence through self-acceptance. As an example, a gender minority interviewee reported that the reason behind gaining confidence is rather “internal”.

“(I feel more confident in speaking up)…Because I do feel more confident as myself, as my own person. That just makes it easier. (I2—LGBTQIA+ attendee)”

In comparison, 1 male interviewee suggested that his level of confidence has been improved through professional experience.

“Now that I am more senior (in research), I am asking more questions and I am less afraid of asking questions. (It was) something I didn’t do when I was younger. (I1—Senior male attendee)”

These observations correspond in part with studies reporting that women tend to have a lessened self-esteem compared to men, especially during early adulthood, and start recovering partially during their forties [28, 29]. Our observation suggests that the origin of this recovery could be internal rather than external. Confirming these hypotheses would need further investigations.

Women and gender minorities are more proactive to challenge gender inequalities in the workplace

During the analysis of interview data, interviewees displayed various attitudes towards the topic of gender inequality, including conscious, curious, supportive, oblivious, and “active” (i.e. interviewees taking actions or being eager to make a change). Findings show that all interviewees were conscious about the subject of gender inequality either in the workplace or in society. However, female and gender minority interviewees presented a more proactive attitude to tackle gender inequality or more willingness to make a change.

“As a woman, I am happy to be in this field because I can help and to improve (gender inequality). (I4—Senior foreign female attendee)”

Male interviewees stated that they were oblivious to potential issues related to gender inequality or they did not feel concerned by the issues.

“…But maybe I heard it and I don’t remember, because I am a male and it doesn’t impact me. (I1—Senior male attendee)”

These findings are consistent with previous reports stating that men are more often unaware of these inequalities than women and gender minorities and are more skeptical about empirical studies on inequalities [30, 31].

Women are subjected to gender-based discrimination and sexual harassment

Gender-based discrimination and harassment in the workplace (i.e. “gender-based behaviors, policies, and actions that adversely affect work by leading to disparate treatment or creation of an intimidating environment”, and Sexual harassment covers “a spectrum from generalized sexist remarks and behaviors to coercive sexual advances and from unconscious patronization and subtle innuendo to blatant sexual threats” [32], including academic conferences, were one of the most frequently discussed phenomena during the interviews. The subject of discussion was either brought up by the interviewees voluntarily or introduced by the interviewer following the interview guide. All female interviewees stated that they have personally experienced gender-based discrimination and harassment in the past.

“I had a very strange experience of hmm…harassment during my internship. Sexual harassment can happen to men, but I think it happened to me because I am a woman.”(I5—Junior female attendee)

All man, woman, and gender minority interviewees (in total 7 of them) confirmed that they have witnessed or heard about such a phenomenon happening to their fellow female workers. This consensus contrasts with interviewees’ varying degree of awareness regarding the overall topic of gender inequalities discussed in the previous section.

“A senior colleague told me that during her PhD, her PhD advisor said that since we now have female researchers, their jobs as researchers will be disrespected.” (I1—Senior male attendee)

Although gender-based discrimination was found to be the most common type of discrimination against female professionals, 3 interviewees pointed out that sexual harassment also occurred often and incidents were rarely reported or dealt with at an organizational level. It is worth mentioning that the female interviewee of foreign nationality indicated her personal experience of intersectional discrimination, which underlines the particularity of discrimination due to her identities of being a woman as well as being a foreigner.

Discussion

By implementing a quantitative and qualitative observation study on the JOBIM 2021 conference, we established and reproduced several key findings on how gender and sexual identity impact working conditions in STEM and more specifically at scientific conferences. Several indicators of equal gender representation improved significantly between the first edition of JOBIM (2000) and JOBIM 2021. Notably, parity was observed amongst attendees, keynote speakers, the program committee, chairmen and chairwomen of JOBIM 2021. For the first time, we reported the fraction of attendees identifying as LGBTQIA+: 9.5%. When accounting for attendees’ demographics, women, gender minorities and junior academics were still strongly underrepresented in question askers. The qualitative analysis of attendees interviews provides potential explanation mechanisms to this picture.

The virtual setting of JOBIM 2021 was an opportunity to observe if the under-representation of women would persist in different conditions than the traditional live conferences. Interestingly the online format seemed to boost the registration of women, with the conference reaching parity in attendees for the first time in 2021. This increase in women registration might be due to the lesser logistic burden of attending virtual conferences enabling them to navigate, for instance, more easily child care and conference attendance. Indeed, parenthood still impacts female academics more than their male peers [33, 34].

Several factors could have mitigated the lesser number of questions from women at virtual conferences: question asking in written form and the possibility to ask anonymously. Yet, the underrepresentation of women in askers of written questions was similar to previous reports on live conferences [13, 14] and the anonymous questions remained marginal (10 questions overall). The virtual setting enabled us to identify askers more precisely and to count the total number of questions throughout the conference by asker. Interestingly, the vast majority (87%) of attendees did not ask any questions. Attendees asking numerous questions (3 or more throughout the conference) were men at 85.6%. Anecdotally, several observers stated that they were able to recall the name of the “serial askers” at the end of the conference, hinting that recurrent question asking is an efficient way to gain visibility in a field. To clarify if the women underrepresentation was merely a consequence of the leaky pipeline (attrition of women as we climb the hierarchical ladder), we modeled the effect of gender, age, and professional status on the rate of question-asking. Accounting for seniority, gender still impacted the rate of question asking with women and gender minorities asking 50% less questions. Although the impact of seniority might be more understandable than the impact of gender as experience might help to formulate relevant questions, its extent was striking (4.6 times more questions for a senior academic). A pedagogical opportunity might be lost in scientific conferences if junior academics are not comfortable enough to ask questions. Interestingly, encouraging junior academics to ask the first question has been shown to mitigate women under-representation as well [13].

The fraction of members of the LGBTQIA+ community was comparable to the one reported in the French general population (9.5% compared to 8%) which contrast with previous reports stating that LGBTQIA+ community is under-represented in STEM [20, 23, 35]. However, this fraction might be overoptimistic since young attendees seem to be more willing to identify as LGBT and the majority of attendees at JOBIM are under 35. The estimate of the LGBT fraction in the French general population is also subject to controversy and is not, to our knowledge, reported in the scientific literature. The estimate provided here originates from a poll survey (Le regard des français sur l’homosexualité et la place des lgbt dans la société (2019)) conducted by the IFOP (Institut français d’opinion publique). In line with a previous report, more women identify as LGBT in our sample suggesting that gay men are more discriminated against in STEM than lesbian women [23]. While remaining relatively rare to this day with 13 attendees identifying outside of binary categories, the report of diverse gender identities stresses the value of proposing a variety of options other than male and female when collecting gender-related information. Concerning question asking, we report a tendency of LGBTQIA+ to be underrepresented. The underrepresentation of LGBTQIA+ in question asking is coherent with reports underlining that this community is marginalized in STEM [36]. Yet, this effect was no longer significant when accounting for gender and seniority. Due to the scarcity of data (5 questions asked by LGBTQIA+ members), it is unclear if the underrepresentation of LGBT in askers is coufounded by gender and seniority or if it has an effect on its own.

The post survey demonstrated that all genders prefer the same setting (live conferences) and are encouraged to ask questions by the same factors. While attendees noticed accurately the importance of seniority in question asking behavior, they were unaware of the effect of gender indicating the need for communication on this topic. Women and gender minorities reported that they endured more discrimination during conferences and were less motivated to ask questions. Potential underlying factors to these observations were provided by the qualitative analysis of attendee interviews. Among the recurrent themes were “Gender-based negative experience” and “Women are subjected to discrimination & harassment” highlighting the overall more negative experience of women and gender minorities in STEM as reported before [3, 25, 37]. Discouraging reactions to women speech were cited as causes of an hindered motivation to speak. Concerning sexual harassment, incident reported in interviews occurred exclusively to junior women. Although our sample is limited in size, this finding suggests that the power imbalance between junior female academics and senior male academics with a permanent position can be an enabling environment for harassment, which might explain why sexual harassment is higher in academia than in other working environments [37]. Female interviewees and gender minority noticed that their confidence was impacted by gender and in turn impacted ability to speak up. Hence, self-confidence might be the mediator between an unwelcoming environment and lesser ability to speak or self-promote rather than the primal cause. While this effect was not apparent in the post survey, women and gender minorities exhibit a higher degree of information on gender bias and more willingness to take action to correct it in interviews. This is coherent with previous reports stating that men are skeptical about the gender gap in general [14, 30, 31].

We believe that ensuring inclusivity should start already at the stage of research design and implementation; as researchers, we share the responsibility of acknowledging our own biases when conducting studies on such a complex topic. Although question asking is an interesting behavior to observe (public, not externally constrained) it pertains limitations. Indeed only 13% of attendees ask questions, hence the initial sample size of all attendees is shrink down to 101 askers. This limited sample might explain why we could not confirm the underrepresentation of LBGT attendees in question askers. While we present an original first attempt to combine qualitative and quantitative approaches, the number of interviews we were able to conduct was limited (7). Attendees accepting to go through an interview might not be entirely representative of all attendees. Notably, we were not able to interview a senior male attendee due to the lack of positive answers to our solicitations of members of this group. We believe that encouraging the systematic collection of data on gender but also on other axes (LGBTQIA+ belonging, racial, socio-economic) by institutions will help to address these limitations.

Our study documents the persistence of barriers against women and gender minorities in academia and provides hints on the most important problems to address and how to include them. We provide guidelines on the project web page for conference organizers as a first step to improve the current situation. We indeed think that large scientific conferences, by their collective nature, are the ideal opportunity to address the phenomena of gender-based inequity as well as to initiate changes in the STEM field.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Overview of the study with the list of data collected, the main data processing steps and the main analysis steps.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Predicted mean rate for junior and senior academics conditioned by their gender.

By senior, we refer to an attendee older than 35 and with a permanent position. By junior, we refer to an attendee younger than 35 and with a short term contract.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Effect of the length of the question session.

A) Histogram of the number of questions asked at the end of the talk by type of sessions (color coded), B) Total number of questions asked by gender (indicated by the color of the bars) with respect to the total number of questions asked at the end of the talk (used as a proxy for the duration of question session).

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Qualitative analysis of interview transcripts.

Each line of the transcript is assigned to a child code either derived from the research questions or created to represent the recurring topics. When all the transcripts have been processed, the resulting child codes are gathered into broader categories : parent codes. Finally parent codes are summarized in themes.

(TIF)

S5 Fig

(PNG)

S1 File. Observation guidelines and form.

(PDF)

S2 File. Interview consent form.

(PDF)

S3 File. Interview guide.

(PDF)

S1 Table. Confusion matrix between gender identified from first name versus self identified gender.

Each case is the count of attendees belonging to this category.

(XLSX)

S2 Table. AIC by variables included in the Poisson regression.

(XLSX)

S3 Table. Initial codebook.

(XLSX)

S4 Table. Final codebook.

(XLSX)

S5 Table. Number of posters by gender and poster categories.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank the SFBI and, in particular Julien Fumey, for providing data on previous JOBIM editions and collecting data on our behalf for this edition. We would like to thank the Institut Pasteur hub of bioinformatics and Biostastics for supporting this study. We thank Gillian Sandstrom for our fruitful scientific exchanges. We thank the JOBIM organizing committee for authorizing this study and enabling its presentation to the audience during the opening and closing sessions. We thank the team of observers and their invaluable contribution to this study : Elise Jacquemet, Sophie Schbath, Claudia Chica, Nicolas Maillet, Pascal Campagne. We thank Christophe Boetto and Stephan Fischer for their careful proofreading of the manuscript. We thank the reviewer for their thorough review and constructive feedback. Finally, we warmly thank all JOBIM attendees who entrusted us with their personal data, survey respondents and interviewees.

Data Availability

In accordance with GDPR regulation, personal data collected in the context of this study are not publicly available. Aggregated data with accompanying scripts to reproduce the manuscript figures have been made publicly available at: https://gitlab.pasteur.fr/hub/gender_at_jobim_2021. To ensure persistence and citability, an additional copy of aggregated data and scripts has been uploaded on Zenodo under the DOI number 10.5281/zenodo.7923694.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1. Nielsen MW, Andersen JP, Schiebinger L, Schneider JW. One and a half million medical papers reveal a link between author gender and attention to gender and sex analysis. Nature Human Behaviour. 2017;1(11):791–796. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0235-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Shansky RM. Are hormones a “female problem” for animal research? Science. 2019;364(6443):825–826. doi: 10.1126/science.aaw7570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Moss-Racusin CA, Dovidio JF, Brescoll VL, Graham MJ, Handelsman J. Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences. 2012;109(41):16474–16479. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211286109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Terrell J, Kofink A, Middleton J, Rainear C, Murphy-Hill E, Parnin C, et al. Gender differences and bias in open source: Pull request acceptance of women versus men. PeerJ Computer Science. 2017;3:e111. doi: 10.7717/peerj-cs.111 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Van der Lee R, Ellemers N. Gender contributes to personal research funding success in The Netherlands. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(40):12349–12353. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1510159112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Sassler S, Glass J, Levitte Y, Michelmore KM. The missing women in STEM? Assessing gender differentials in the factors associated with transition to first jobs. Social science research. 2017;63:192–208. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2016.09.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Charles M, Bradley K. Indulging our gendered selves? Sex segregation by field of study in 44 countries. American journal of sociology. 2009;114(4):924–976. doi: 10.1086/595942 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Casad BJ, Franks JE, Garasky CE, Kittleman MM, Roesler AC, Hall DY, et al. Gender inequality in academia: Problems and solutions for women faculty in STEM. Journal of neuroscience research. 2021;99(1):13–23. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Ensmenger N. “Beards, sandals, and other signs of rugged individualism”: masculine culture within the computing professions. Osiris. 2015;30(1):38–65. doi: 10.1086/682955 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Van den Besselaar P, Sandström U. Vicious circles of gender bias, lower positions, and lower performance: Gender differences in scholarly productivity and impact. PloS one. 2017;12(8):e0183301. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0183301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Larivière V, Ni C, Gingras Y, Cronin B, Sugimoto CR. Bibliometrics: Global gender disparities in science. Nature News. 2013;504(7479):211. doi: 10.1038/504211a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Broderick NA, Casadevall A. Meta-research: gender inequalities among authors who contributed equally. Elife. 2019;8:e36399. doi: 10.7554/eLife.36399 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Telis N, Glassberg EC, Pritchard JK, Gunter C. Public discussion affects question asking at academic conferences. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2019;105(1):189–197. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2019.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Carter AJ, Croft A, Lukas D, Sandstrom GM. Women’s visibility in academic seminars: Women ask fewer questions than men. PloS one. 2018;13(9):e0202743. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Schmidt SJ, Davenport JRA. Who asks questions at astronomy meetings? Nature Astronomy 2017 1:6. 2017;1:1–2. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Hinsley A, Sutherland WJ, Johnston A. Men ask more questions than women at a scientific conference. PloS one. 2017. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Moazzam S, Onstad L, O’Leary H, Marshall A, Osunkwo I, Du E, et al. Gender differences in question-asking at the 2019 American Society of Hematology Annual Meeting. Blood Advances. 2020;4(21):5473–5479. doi: 10.1182/bloodadvances.2020002714 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Käfer J, Betancourt A, Villain AS, Fernandez M, Vignal C, Marais GAB, et al. Progress and Prospects in Gender Visibility at SMBE Annual Meetings. Genome Biology and Evolution. 2018;10:901–908. doi: 10.1093/gbe/evy056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Brescoll VL. Who takes the floor and why: Gender, power, and volubility in organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly. 2011;56(4):622–641. doi: 10.1177/0001839212439994 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Freeman J. LGBTQ scientists are still left out; 2018. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 21. O’Connell C, McKinnon M. Perceptions of Barriers to Career Progression for Academic Women in STEM. Societies. 2021;11(2):27. doi: 10.3390/soc11020027 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Gulland A. Men dominate conference Q&A sessions-including online ones. Nature. 2022;. doi: 10.1038/d41586-022-04241-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Sansone D, Carpenter CS. Turing’s children: Representation of sexual minorities in STEM. PloS one. 2020;15(11):e0241596. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241596 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Casadevall A, Handelsman J. The presence of female conveners correlates with a higher proportion of female speakers at scientific symposia. MBio. 2014;5(1):e00846–13. doi: 10.1128/mBio.00846-13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Roy MF, Guillopé C, Cesa M, Ivie R, White S, Mihaljevic H, et al. A Global Approach to the Gender Gap in Mathematical, Computing, and Natural Sciences: How to Measure It, How to Reduce It?; 2020. Available from: 10.5281/zenodo.3882609. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Aycock LM, Hazari Z, Brewe E, Clancy KB, Hodapp T, Goertzen RM. Sexual harassment reported by undergraduate female physicists. Physical Review Physics Education Research. 2019;15(1):010121. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevPhysEducRes.15.010121 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Johnson PA, Widnall SE, Benya FF, editors. Sexual Harassment of Women: Climate, Culture, and Consequences in Academic Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2018. Available from: https://www.nap.edu/catalog/24994/sexual-harassment-of-women-climate-culture-and-consequences-in-academic. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Robins RW, Trzesniewski KH, Tracy JL, Gosling SD, Potter J. Global self-esteem across the life span. Psychology and aging. 2002;17(3):423. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.3.423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Robins RW, Trzesniewski KH. Self-esteem development across the lifespan. Current directions in psychological science. 2005;14(3):158–162. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00353.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Handley IM, Brown ER, Moss-Racusin CA, Smith JL. Quality of evidence revealing subtle gender biases in science is in the eye of the beholder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(43):13201–13206. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1510649112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Begeny CT, Ryan MK, Moss-Racusin CA, Ravetz G. In some professions, women have become well represented, yet gender bias persists—Perpetuated by those who think it is not happening. Science Advances. 2020;6(26):eaba7814. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aba7814 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Carr PL, Ash AS, Friedman RH, Szalacha L, Barnett RC, Palepu A, et al. Faculty perceptions of gender discrimination and sexual harassment in academic medicine. Annals of internal medicine. 2000;132(11):889–896. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-132-11-200006060-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Staniscuaski F, Kmetzsch L, Soletti RC, Reichert F, Zandonà E, Ludwig ZM, et al. Gender, race and parenthood impact academic productivity during the COVID-19 pandemic: from survey to action. Frontiers in psychology. 2021;12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Morgan AC, Way SF, Hoefer MJ, Larremore DB, Galesic M, Clauset A. The unequal impact of parenthood in academia. Science Advances. 2021;7(9):eabd1996. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abd1996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Hughes BE. Coming out in STEM: Factors affecting retention of sexual minority STEM students. Science advances. 2018;4(3):eaao6373. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aao6373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Cech E, Waidzunas T. Systemic inequalities for LGBTQ professionals in STEM. Science advances. 2021;7(3):eabe0933. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abe0933 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Witze A. Sexual harassment is rife in the sciences, finds landmark US study. Nature. 2018;558(7710):352–354. doi: 10.1038/d41586-018-05404-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Andrew R Dalby

16 Mar 2023

PONE-D-22-28787Gender-based disparities and biases in science: an observational study of a virtual conferencePLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Julienne,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 30 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Andrew R. Dalby, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.  Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

3. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well.

4. Please upload a new copy of All Figures as the detail is not clear. Please follow the link for more information: " ext-link-type="uri" xlink:type="simple">https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/06/looking-good-tips-for-creating-your-plos-figures-graphics/" https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/06/looking-good-tips-for-creating-your-plos-figures-graphics/

5. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.      

6. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is an important study with useful recommendations.

Authors give quantitative data on an Online/virtual conference with qualitative individual interviews and address gender and sexual orientation. The main weakness is in the numbers of individuals interviewed, and this should be clarified in the data section itself. Still, this is a useful report.

At the top of page 7, authors state:

“women are subjected to gender-based discrimination and harassment during scientific activities, including academic conferences.”

It would be more accurate to state

“women report experiencing gender-based discrimination and harassment during scientific activities, including academic conferences.”

Believing one has experienced discrimination is not the same thing as discrimination. Perceptions may not be reality.

On page 7 what is meant by ‘surrounding work atmosphere.” Can you give some details? examples?

Similarly, how was she “discouraged to become a scientific researcher.” What was said? By whom?

Authors state. On p. “Furthermore, the findings also show that women and gender minority tend to improve their level of confidence through self-acceptance”. What evidence is there for this statement?

At the top of Page 9 authors wrote “Gender-based discrimination and harassment in the workplace6, including academic conferences, were one of the most frequently discussed phenomena during the interviews.” Is that because the interviewers specifically asked about this? Or did interviewees bring it up on their own?

Page 9 authors wrote “All man, woman, and gender minority interviewees confirmed that they have witnessed or heard about such a phenomenon happening to their fellow female workers.” Please add the exact number after the word interviewees, how many people was ‘all’? 6?

Page 10, the numbers here are too low to support this statement

“The fraction of members of the LGBTQIA+ community was higher than in the French general population (9.5% compared to 8%)”. You cannot tell any real difference between 9.5 and 8% with numbers this low.

Minor comments/corrections:

Near the bottom of page 2, Authors have accidentally used the wrong wording here

“We attempt to step out of the non-binary vision of gender by”

From the context, the authors meant to write

“We attempt to step out of the historical or typical binary vision of gender by”

In the intro, when authors describe the “underrepresentation of minorities” do they mean racial/ethnic minorities or are they including women in ‘minorities’ since women are minorities in science. Clarify.

Near the top of page 4, authors state

“Yet, amongst

the 13 JOBIM editions with available data, 6 significantly lacked women contributed speakers.”

I am uncertain what the phrase ‘women contributed speakers’ means. Does this mean that some people are chosen to give oral presentations while others are not? If so the phrasing might be

“Yet, amongst

the 13 JOBIM editions with available data, 6 significantly lacked women who were invited to be speakers.”

On page 4, the authors wrote

“This overall effect was modulated by session type and was

especially stark in 'Mini-symposia' and less obvious in the contributed talks (see Fig. 2A).”

Do the authors mean that mini symposia are more prestigious or have larger audiences than contributed talks? I’m guessing that is what is meant, but I am unsure. Please clarify.

On page 5, I believe the authors mean to write

“Even though JOBIM is considered as

student-friendly conference, the effect of age and professional status weighed heavily on the rate of questions

asked with senior academics asking 4.6 times more questions than junior academics.”

On page 5, authors wrote

“The results could differ from the precedent section due to a few questions asked orally by the attendees, the

questions asked by the chairman, and the selection effect of the chairman when reading questions out loud

from the chatbox.”

I think authors may mean the ‘preceding’ section rather than precedent if they mean the section prior to this one.

Second, the authors use the word ‘chairman’. In the US, we often just say the “chair’ with the same meaning, except it does not mean that the person is a man. In the same paragraph, the authors use chairperson, so I am now confused by what they mean by chairman.

I do not understand the meaning at the bottom of page 5 “we did not find a significant

positive effect of the length of question session on women and gender minority representation (see Fig. S3B).”

page 6 please clarify this “Hence, our results provide a slightly different information: the proportion

of respondents thinking that the factor matters.” Explain exactly what you mean.

At the top of page 7, it should state

“women, sexual and gender minority attendees report negative experiences

based on sexual orientation, gender identity and expression in professional contexts”

Similarly the heading on page 7 should read

Women and gender minority attendees report negative experiences based on sexual orien-

tations, gender identities, and expressions

Page 11.

This is coherent consistent with previous reports stating that men need are skeptical about the gender gap in general[14, 27, 28].

There is a typo on the website under “support minorities in question asking”. The word ‘talking’ is present, when it should be ‘taking”

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jun 7;18(6):e0286811. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286811.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


22 May 2023

Dear Reviewer,

Please find below our point by point answer to your concerns.

With kind regards,

Hanna Julienne, Ph.D.

Statistical Genetics Group,

Institut Pasteur

hanna.julienne@pasteur.fr

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is an important study with useful recommendations.

Authors give quantitative data on an Online/virtual conference with qualitative individual interviews and address gender and sexual orientation. The main weakness is in the numbers of individuals interviewed, and this should be clarified in the data section itself. Still, this is a useful report.

We thank the reviewer for their appreciation of the importance of our work, their thorough review, and pointing out the main limitation of our study.

Concerning the number of interviews conducted, we clarified the scope of the qualitative section by adding the following text in the Material and method “Semi-structured and in-depth interviews” paragraph.

“The number of interviews we were able to conduct is relatively low (7) and our qualitative observations might not generalize to the complete academic population. Yet, these interviews provide valuable examples of career trajectories impacted by gender and by the LGBTQIA+ status. This contextualization can help generate hypotheses on why women and gender minorities ask less questions than men.”

We equally repeated this clarification at the beginning of the result section discussing qualitative findings:

“To contextualize quantitative findings we conducted in depth interviews on a representative set of participants. The number of interviews we were able to conduct is relatively low (7) and our qualitative observations might not generalize to the complete academic population. Yet, these interviews provide valuable examples on how gender and LGBTQIA+ status can impact career trajectories which could lead to a lesser comfort in question asking.”

Throughout the Result section describing qualitative finding we removed intensity adverbs (e.g. strongly, significantly) and modified strong affirmative wording (e.g. change “is associated” to “might be associated”) to avoid any overstatements of our results.

At the top of page 7, authors state:

“women are subjected to gender-based discrimination and harassment during scientific activities, including academic conferences.”

It would be more accurate to state

“women report experiencing gender-based discrimination and harassment during scientific activities, including academic conferences.”

Believing one has experienced discrimination is not the same thing as discrimination. Perceptions may not be reality.

We thank the reviewer for the pertinent remark and suggestion. We updated the manuscript text accordingly.

On page 7 what is meant by ‘surrounding work atmosphere.” Can you give some details? examples?

“Surrounding work atmosphere” refers to the general professional environment of the interviewees. In the following text, one example is given by Interviewee I4 who reported witnessing in her work environment negative reactions received by a female team leader from other male team leaders when posing questions or presenting. We thank the reviewer for pointing out this unclear expression. To avoid potential confusion, we removed “surrounding work atmosphere”. In fact, we think such a modification will not affect the original meaning of the phrase.

Similarly, how was she “discouraged to become a scientific researcher.” What was said? By whom?

We added the supportive evidence by I3 to the text.

Authors state. On p. “Furthermore, the findings also show that women and gender minority tend to improve their level of confidence through self-acceptance”. What evidence is there for this statement?

We thank the reviewer for pointing out that our statement lacked thorough support in the previous version of the manuscript. We substantiated this point by adding supportive evidence formulated by I2 and I1 to the text. We also reformulated the statement to a more hypothetical form “Furthermore, the findings also show that women and gender minorities might improve their level of confidence through self-acceptance.”

We also contextualize this observation with studies on the evolution of self-esteem through life reporting women have lessened self-esteem compared to men:

“These observations correspond in part with studies reporting that women tend to have a lessened self-esteem compared to men, especially during early adulthood, and start recovering partially during their forties (Robins et al, 2002; Robins et al, 2005). Our observation suggests that the origin of this recovery could be internal rather than external. Confirming these hypotheses would need further investigations.”

At the top of Page 9 authors wrote “Gender-based discrimination and harassment in the workplace6, including academic conferences, were one of the most frequently discussed phenomena during the interviews.” Is that because the interviewers specifically asked about this? Or did interviewees bring it up on their own?

We thank the reviewer for raising this valuable question. To answer the question, both situations occurred. In some cases, the interviewer brought the topic up by following the interview guide. Specific examples are given as following:

For example, during the interview with I4, the interviewee commented on the phenomenon of gender discrimination in her current workplace without the subject being mentioned by the interviewer. I4 talked about gender discrimination following the interviewer’s general question about the impact of gender identity on her professional life. Similarly, I5 voluntarily talked about her experience of sexual harassment during the interview. However, it also happened that discussion on such a subject occurred during the interview because the interviewer prompted the topic of gender-based discrimination or harassment. As an example, the interviewer asked the question to I6: “Have you participated in or heard of discussions around gender-related topics? For example, gender-based stereotypes or discrimination.”

To clarify this point, we added the following sentence as a complementary information: “The subject of discussion was either brought up by the interviewees voluntarily or introduced by the interviewer following the interview guide.”

In addition to this point, what we would like to highlight here is the consensus among interviewees that gender-based discrimination and harassment are common phenomena in the workplace. All interviewees, despite the differences in their gender identity and seniority, all reported either hearing about or witnessing such events or experiencing them first hand in their workplace. To this end, we added the following sentence to the text:

“All man, woman, and gender minority interviewees (in total 7 of them) confirmed that they have witnessed or heard about such a phenomenon happening to their fellow female workers. This consensus contrasts with interviewees' varying degree of awareness regarding the overall topic of gender inequalities discussed in the previous section.”

Page 9 authors wrote “All man, woman, and gender minority interviewees confirmed that they have witnessed or heard about such a phenomenon happening to their fellow female workers.” Please add the exact number after the word interviewees, how many people was ‘all’? 6?

We thank the reviewer for the pertinent remark and suggestion. We added the information to the text. In total there were 7 interviewees.

Page 10, the numbers here are too low to support this statement

“The fraction of members of the LGBTQIA+ community was higher than in the French general population (9.5% compared to 8%)”. You cannot tell any real difference between 9.5 and 8% with numbers this low.

We thank the reviewer for noticing this poor wording on our part. Indeed, 8% is included in the confidence interval of our point estimate (9.5%). Hence, the fraction of LGBTQIA+ participant in the JOBIM conference seems comparable to the general French population. We modified the text as follows:

“The fraction of members of the LGBTQIA+ community was comparable to the one reported in the French general population (9.5% compared to 8%), which contrast with previous reports stating that LGBTQIA+ community is under-represented in STEM [20, 22, 34]”

We believe that the rest of the sentence, which contextualizes this percentage against previous reports, remains pertinent.

Minor comments/corrections:

We thank the reviewer for their careful proofreading of the manuscript. We inserted all proposed suggestions.

Near the bottom of page 2, Authors have accidentally used the wrong wording here

“We attempt to step out of the non-binary vision of gender by”

From the context, the authors meant to write

“We attempt to step out of the historical or typical binary vision of gender by”

We thank the reviewer for noticing this error. We inserted the proposed suggestion in our manuscript.

In the intro, when authors describe the “underrepresentation of minorities” do they mean racial/ethnic minorities or are they including women in ‘minorities’ since women are minorities in science. Clarify.

We thank the reviewer for pointing out that minorities could be misinterpreted when referring to women. In sociology, women are frequently referred to as minority not necessarily in terms of number but rather because they have lessened access to power and opportunity than men.

Nonetheless, our usage of minority in the introduction was vague (as it could refer to racial minorities) and more obscuring than enlightening. We replaced “minorities” by “women and gender minority” while keeping a footnote specifying that by gender minority we refer to people identifying outside of the binary categories of male and female.

Near the top of page 4, authors state

“Yet, amongst

the 13 JOBIM editions with available data, 6 significantly lacked women contributed speakers.”

I am uncertain what the phrase ‘women contributed speakers’ means. Does this mean that some people are chosen to give oral presentations while others are not? If so the phrasing might be

“Yet, amongst

the 13 JOBIM editions with available data, 6 significantly lacked women who were invited to be speakers.”

Indeed, “contributed speakers” refers to a person who submitted an abstract to the conference program committee and were selected to give an oral presentation. We clarified this expression by inserting a footnote to define it.

On page 4, the authors wrote

“This overall effect was modulated by session type and was

especially stark in 'Mini-symposia' and less obvious in the contributed talks (see Fig. 2A).”

Do the authors mean that mini symposia are more prestigious or have larger audiences than contributed talks? I’m guessing that is what is meant, but I am unsure. Please clarify.

In the JOBIM conference, mini symposia are a series of technical talks organized around a theme usually featuring mid-career researchers. While it might be of importance, the reason for a stronger underrepresentation of women in question askers in this type of talks is not completely clear at this stage. We modified the text to provide the element of context necessary to understand what “mini symposia” refers to while avoiding overstating the observed tendency.

“We noticed a tendency for a stronger underrepresentation in 'Mini-symposia', which are series of technical talks featuring invited speakers, and less obvious in the contributed talks, which usually feature more junior speakers (Fig. 2A).”

On page 5, I believe the authors mean to write

“Even though JOBIM is considered as

student-friendly conference, the effect of age and professional status weighed heavily on the rate of questions

asked with senior academics asking 4.6 times more questions than junior academics.”

On page 5, authors wrote

“The results could differ from the precedent section due to a few questions asked orally by the attendees, the

questions asked by the chairman, and the selection effect of the chairman when reading questions out loud

from the chatbox.”

I think authors may mean the ‘preceding’ section rather than precedent if they mean the section prior to this one.

Second, the authors use the word ‘chairman’. In the US, we often just say the “chair’ with the same meaning, except it does not mean that the person is a man. In the same paragraph, the authors use chairperson, so I am now confused by what they mean by chairman.

The above suggestions have been inserted.

I do not understand the meaning at the bottom of page 5 “we did not find a significant

positive effect of the length of question session on women and gender minority representation (see Fig. S3B).”

The wording has been clarified to: “we did not find a significant positive effect of the total number of questions asked during the question session on women and gender minority representation”.

page 6 please clarify this “Hence, our results provide a slightly different information: the proportion

of respondents thinking that the factor matters.” Explain exactly what you mean.

We clarified this paragraph by providing a brief description of Likert scale. We removed the sentence point out by the reviewer as we thought it was indeed unclear:

“This might be due to the survey design: used Likert scales (where respondents are asked how much a factor matters using a scale from one to five) whereas we asked respondents to say if a factor mattered or not in a binary way.”

At the top of page 7, it should state

“women, sexual and gender minority attendees report negative experiences

based on sexual orientation, gender identity and expression in professional contexts”

Similarly the heading on page 7 should read

Women and gender minority attendees report negative experiences based on sexual orientations, gender identities, and expressions

We thank the reviewer for pointing out our lack of accuracy in describing the concluding statements. We modified both sentences according to the reviewer’s suggestions.

Page 11.

This is coherent consistent with previous reports stating that men need are skeptical about the gender gap in general[14, 27, 28].

There is a typo on the website under “support minorities in question asking”. The word ‘talking’ is present, when it should be ‘taking”

We thank the reviewer for their careful proofreading. We inserted the proposed suggestions.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response_to_reviewer.pdf

Decision Letter 1

Andrew R Dalby

24 May 2023

Gender-based disparities and biases in science: an observational study of a virtual conference

PONE-D-22-28787R1

Dear Dr. Julienne,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Andrew R. Dalby, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Andrew R Dalby

30 May 2023

PONE-D-22-28787R1

Gender-based disparities and biases in science: an observational study of a virtual conference

Dear Dr. Julienne:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Andrew R. Dalby

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Overview of the study with the list of data collected, the main data processing steps and the main analysis steps.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. Predicted mean rate for junior and senior academics conditioned by their gender.

    By senior, we refer to an attendee older than 35 and with a permanent position. By junior, we refer to an attendee younger than 35 and with a short term contract.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. Effect of the length of the question session.

    A) Histogram of the number of questions asked at the end of the talk by type of sessions (color coded), B) Total number of questions asked by gender (indicated by the color of the bars) with respect to the total number of questions asked at the end of the talk (used as a proxy for the duration of question session).

    (TIF)

    S4 Fig. Qualitative analysis of interview transcripts.

    Each line of the transcript is assigned to a child code either derived from the research questions or created to represent the recurring topics. When all the transcripts have been processed, the resulting child codes are gathered into broader categories : parent codes. Finally parent codes are summarized in themes.

    (TIF)

    S5 Fig

    (PNG)

    S1 File. Observation guidelines and form.

    (PDF)

    S2 File. Interview consent form.

    (PDF)

    S3 File. Interview guide.

    (PDF)

    S1 Table. Confusion matrix between gender identified from first name versus self identified gender.

    Each case is the count of attendees belonging to this category.

    (XLSX)

    S2 Table. AIC by variables included in the Poisson regression.

    (XLSX)

    S3 Table. Initial codebook.

    (XLSX)

    S4 Table. Final codebook.

    (XLSX)

    S5 Table. Number of posters by gender and poster categories.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_to_reviewer.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    In accordance with GDPR regulation, personal data collected in the context of this study are not publicly available. Aggregated data with accompanying scripts to reproduce the manuscript figures have been made publicly available at: https://gitlab.pasteur.fr/hub/gender_at_jobim_2021. To ensure persistence and citability, an additional copy of aggregated data and scripts has been uploaded on Zenodo under the DOI number 10.5281/zenodo.7923694.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES