Abstract
Background:
Intermittent Explosive Disorder (IED) is a disorder primarily of aggression, defined by recurrent behavioral outbursts out of proportion to provocations or stressors. IED first appears in childhood and adolescence. This study examines the underlying childhood environment of those with IED, particularly familial and school-related factors.
Methods:
Adult participants from a larger study completed diagnostic assessments and a battery of self-report measures. Group assignment was based on the assessment: 1) IED diagnosis; 2) non-IED psychiatric diagnosis; and 3) no significant psychiatric history. Groups were compared on factors of parental demographics, intrafamilial aggression, lifetime syndromal and personality diagnoses, neurodevelopmental and learning difficulties, childhood peer relationships, and juvenile legal issues.
Results:
Significant patterns emerged specific to IED for not being raised by both parents, greater physical aggression to participant, and greater degree of fighting with peers by age ten.
Limitations:
The retrospective, and cross-sectional, nature of the study, which prevent the making of causal inferences, and the basic nature of the questions asked of participants which limit a more nuanced interpretation of the data. A further limitation is bias associated with self-reported responses.
Conclusions:
Results suggest the prevalence childhood adversaries may be linked with IED; the childhood environment of those with IED likely is substantially more tumultuous than individuals with or without other psychiatric disorders.
Keywords: IED, Aggression, Parental characteristics, Behavioral issues
1. Introduction
Intermittent Explosive Disorder (IED) is a disorder primarily of human aggression, defined by recurrent behavioral outbursts out of proportion to aggravating provocations or stressors. Aggression is a behavior that conveys human anger; however, in IED, this anger is out of proportion to the provocation and is not explained by another psychiatric condition. The DSM-5 [1] criteria of IED includes 1) frequent minor outbursts such as verbal tirades or minor physical aggression (at least twice a week on average, over three or more months [Criterion A1] or 2) infrequent major outbursts that result in physical harm to other or property (minimum of three major outbursts in a single year) [Criterion A2], all of which are not better explained by a different medical or mental disorder.
The lifetime prevalence of IED in the US is approximately 4% lifetime [2] and individuals with IED have a poorer quality of life and greater psychosocial impairment compared with others without IED [3,4]. In addition, individuals with IED are more likely to have adverse health outcomes, including hypertension, coronary heart disease, and stroke [5]. The course of IED is waxing and waning, typically becoming obvious in childhood/adolescence and extending through adult life [6].
The prevalence of IED in adolescents is about twice that in adults, at nearly 8%, and IED has a mean age of onset at about 11 years of age [7]. Given this, it is likely that a variety of conditions and adverse events in childhood influence the development of IED. Such conditions and events would include household and parental characteristics as well as a history of experiencing and witnessing violent behavior during childhood. Specifically, childhood abuse has been associated with more aggressive personality traits including impulsivity and aggression [8], both of which are definitionally associated with IED. Additionally, physical abuse during childhood has been shown to be independently associated with IED, and both this physical abuse in addition to emotional abuse in childhood has been shown to independently predict aggression [9].
In addition to environmental factors, concrete neurobiological changes associated with childhood trauma have been shown to contribute to the development of aggression. For example, chronic hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activation from childhood stresses can result in the accelerated loss of hippocampal neurons, lags in myelination development, neurogenesis inhibition, and changes synaptic pruning [10]. Additionally, studies have shown that maltreated children have smaller intracranial and cerebral volumes, smaller mid-sagittal areas, and smaller middle/ posterior corpus callous regions [11]. Genetic susceptibility also plays a role in the development of aggressive personality traits. Monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A) is an enzyme involved in the deamination of dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine; MAO-A genetic deficiencies are linked with higher rates of aggression. Moreover, children with low MAO-A activity are more likely to develop antisocial behaviors [12].
Histories of environmental conflict in childhood may lead to learned aggressive behavior. Early physical abuse biases social information processing: individuals experiencing such early abuse are more likely to perceive benign acts as hostile with disinhibition of emotional and behavioral regulation [13]. Data from the NCS-R study revealed IED was associated with exposure to childhood trauma at a rate of 51.3% [14]. Furthermore, previous studies have shown that parental bonding is lowest among participants with IED, even compared to other psychiatric comorbidities [15].
To further explore conditions and childhood adversities of those with IED, this study focused on specific psychosocial aspects of IED participant households and childhoods. We specifically investigate the parent marital status, participant adoption status, intrafamilial physical aggression witnessed and experienced, frequency of contact with non-primary caregiver (in cases of parental separation), syndromal and personality psychopathology, neurodevelopmental and learning difficulties, behavioral issues at school, peer relationships, and juvenile legal concerns of participants with IED. We then compared these factors between participants with IED, those with a non-IED psychiatric diagnosis, and those without significant psychopathology.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Eleven-hundred-forty-five (n = 1145) adult individuals participated in this study. All participants were physically healthy and were systematically evaluated regarding aggressive and other behaviors as part of a larger program designed to study correlates of impulsive aggressive, and other personality-related, behaviors in human participants. Participants were recruited through public service announcements, and newspaper advertisements seeking out individuals who: (a) reported psychosocial difficulty related to anger or, (b) had little evidence of psychopathology. All participants signed the informed consent document approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the Biological Sciences Division of the University of Chicago (IRB Protocol#: 10375; Approval Date: June 3, 2002).
2.2. Assignment of diagnoses
Psychiatric diagnoses were made according to DSM-5 criteria [1] using information from: (a) the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Diagnoses (SCID-I) [16] for syndromal (formally Axis I) disorders and the Structured Interview for the Diagnosis of Personality Disorder [17] for personality (formally Axis II) disorders; (b) clinical interview by a research psychiatrist; and (c) review of all other available clinical data. Research diagnostic interviews were conducted by individuals with a masters, or doctorate, degree in Clinical Psychology. Inter-rater reliabilities across disorders were good to excellent (mean kappa of 0.84 ± 0.05; range: 0.79 to 0.93). Final diagnoses were assigned by team best-estimate consensus procedures involving research psychiatrists and clinical psychologists [18]. Finally, participants with life history of bipolar disorder, schizophrenia (or other psychotic disorder), current substance abuse, or intellectual disability, were excluded from study.
After diagnostic assignment, 425 participants had no evidence of any psychiatric diagnosis (Healthy Controls: HC); 215 participants met criteria for a lifetime diagnosis of a Syndromal Psychiatric Disorder or Personality Disorder (Psychiatric Controls: PC), and 478 participants met criteria for a DSM-5 diagnosis of intermittent explosive disorder. Of the 693 PC/IED participants, most (81.7%) reported: a) history of formal psychiatric evaluation and/or treatment (64.5%) or, b) history of behavioral disturbance during which the participant, or others, thought they should have sought mental health services but did not (17.2%). Syndromal and Personality Disorder diagnoses are listed in Table 1.
Table 1.
Demographic, functional, and psychometric characteristics of participants.
Healthy controls | Psychiatric controls | Intermittent explosive disorder | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n =452) | (n = 215) | (n = 478) | p | Group differences | |
Demographics | |||||
Age* | 33.0 ± 11.5 | 33.1 ± 10.4 | 35.9 ± 10.9 | < 0.001a | HC = PC < IED |
Gender (% Male) | 45.40% | 44.70% | 43.70% | = 0.882b | HC = PC = IED |
Race (% White) | 61.50% | 54.90% | 40.00% | < 0.001b | HC = PC > IED |
SES Score* | 47.0 ± 11.0 | 42.3 ± 14.0 | 38.9 ± 13.0 | < 0.001a | HC > PC > IED |
Psychosocial Function | |||||
GAF Score* | 82.5 ± 4.8 | 60.2 ± 8.3 | 54.9 ± 7.9 | < 0.001a | HC > PC > IED |
Q-LES-Q Score* | 49.5 ± 6.5 | 41.4 ± 9.7 | 36.4 ± 9.7 | < 0.001a | HC > PC > IED |
Psychometric Variables | |||||
Aggression: LHA | 4.7 ± 3.4 | 8.3 ± 9.4 | 17.8 ± 4.4 | < 0.001 | HC < PC < IED |
Aggression: BPA | 27.4 ± 8.0 | 31.4 ± 9.6 | 45.6 ± 11.7 | < 0.001 | HC < PC < IED |
Impulsivity: LHIB | 29.1 ± 17.8 | 43.5 ± 17.9 | 54.4 ± 18.3 | < 0.001 | HC < PC < IED |
Impulsivity: BIS-11 | 56.2 ± 9.1 | 65.6 ± 11.6 | 69.4 ± 11.7 | < 0.001 | HC < PC < IED |
State Depression | 3.0 ± 7.1 | 12.2 ± 11.6 | 16.7 ± 12.5 | < 0.001 | HC < PC < IED |
State Anxiety | 2.4 ± 3.6 | 7.6 ± 8.3 | 9.6 ± 8.7 | < 0.001 | HC < PC < IED |
Note. Means ± SD based on raw data; statistics based on one-way ANCOVA (age, sex, ethnicity, and SES score as covariates).
Mean ± Standard Deviation.
2.3. Assessment of parental/home characteristics and neuro-behavioral/learning/behavioral issues of participants during childhood/adolescence
Prior to the SCID/SIDP interview, assessors completed a structured interview including questions about the home environment and about childhood/adolescent history of neurobehavioral, learning, and behavioral issues. The questions asked are discussed below.
2.3.1. Parental characteristics
This section included questions as to whether: a) participant was adopted or not (Yes/No), b) parents were ever married (Yes/No), c) parents stayed together or not (stayed together/separated or divorced), d) age of participant when parented separated or divorced, e) both parents raised participant throughout most of childhood and, if not, who raised the participant, f) if parents separated/divorced, how often did participant see mother and father (daily to weekly/less than weekly), g) participant lost anyone of significance through death and whether this occurred before the participant was 15 years of age (Yes/No), h) parent’s occupation and highest level of education to determine socio-economic status by method of Hollingshead, h) parents hit participant during childhood/adolescence (hitting leading to bruising/hospital treatment, etc. vs. no/only physical punishment), i) parents hit participant’s sibling (s) during childhood/adolescence (hitting leading to bruising, hospital treatment, etc. vs. no/only physical punishment).
2.3.2. Neuro-behavioral and learning issues during childhood/adolescence
This section included questions as to whether participant had: a) history of sleepwalking before age of 15 (Yes/No), b) history of stuttering/stammering before age of 15 (Yes/No), c) history of enuresis after age of 10 (Yes/No), d) history of difficulty with learning in school (Yes/No), e) being held back in grade (Yes/No).
2.3.3. Behavioral issues during childhood/adolescence
This section included questions as to whether participant had a history of behavioral problems in school: a) that led to suspensions or expulsions from school (Yes/No), b) history of getting into a lot of fights before the age of 10 (Yes/No), c) history of being rejected by peers before age of 10, d) history of arrest before the age of 15 (Yes/No).
2.4. Assessment of aggression, impulsivity, and related behaviors
Psychometric assessment of aggression, impulsivity, and related behaviors for participants included the Life History of Aggression (LHA; [19] and Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ) [20] for aggression; Life History of Impulsive Behavior (LHIB) [21] and Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) [22] for impulsivity; and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-2) [23] and Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) [24] for state depression and state anxiety, respectively.
2.5. Statistical analysis
Comparisons of between-group variables were performed by Chi-square, analysis of variance/covariance (ANCOVA; MANCOVA). Other analyses involved binary logistic regression with relevant covariates as necessary. A two-tailed alpha value of 0.05 was used to denote statistical significance for all analyses.
3. Results
3.1. Sample characteristics
The groups did not differ in adoption status with only 2.3% to 2.7% of all participants, across the groups, reporting that they had been adopted (similar to the proportion in the general U.S. population: “Research: Adoption Facts.” Evan B. Donaldson Adoption Institute. 2013. Accessed: September 17, 2013). IED participants were modestly older than HC or PC participants, less likely to be white, and had a lower mean socioeconomic score than both HC and PC (PC < HC), but were similar in distribution in biological sex (Table 1). IED participants had the lowest scores for GAF and Q-LES-Q; PC participant scores for both were lower than those for HC participants (Table 1). As expected, IED participants had the highest scores on the aggression, impulsivity, and state depression/anxiety scores followed by PC, then HC participants (Table 1). Finally, IED and PC participants only differed in life history of a Substance Use Disorder, Conduct Disorder, and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (IED > PC) after correction for multiple comparisons (Table 2).
Table 2.
Psychiatric diagnoses among participants.
Psychiatric controls (n = 215) | Intermittent explosive disorder (n = 478) | p | Group differences* | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Current Disorders: | ||||
Any Depressive Disorder | 46 (21.4%) | 114 (23.8%) | = 0.478 | PC = IED |
Any Anxiety Disorder | 83 (38.6%) | 142 (29.7%) | = 0.023 | PC = IED |
Any Stress and Trauma Disorder | 28 (13.0%) | 87 (18.2%) | = 0.090 | PC = IED |
Any Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder | 8 (3.7%) | 21 (4.4%) | = 0.683 | PC = IED |
Any Eating Disorder | 11 (5.1%) | 33 (6.9%) | = 0.372 | PC = IED |
Any Non-IED Impulse Control Disorder | 0 (0.0%) | 12 (2.5%) | = 0.019 | PC = IED |
Conduct Disorder | 4 (1.9%) | 27 (5.7%) | = 0.025 | PC = IED |
Oppositional Defiant Disorder | 1 (0.5%) | 11 (2.3%) | = 0.085 | PC = IED |
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder | 6 (2.8%) | 37 (7.8%) | = 0.012 | PC = IED |
Lifetime Disorders: | ||||
Any Depressive Disorder | 126 (58.6%) | 325 (68.0%) | = 0.016 | PC = IED |
Any Anxiety Disorder | 99 (46.0%) | 182 (38.1%) | = 0.048 | PC = IED |
Any Substance Use Disorder | 57 (26.5%) | 206 (43.1%) | < 0.001 | PC < IED* |
Any Stress and Trauma Disorder | 40 (16.7%) | 134 (26.6%) | = 0.003 | PC = IED |
Any Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder | 15 (7.0%) | 30 (6.3%) | = 0.729 | PC = IED |
Any Eating Disorder | 32 (14.9%) | 66 (13.8%) | = 0.707 | PC = IED |
Any Non-IED Impulse Control Disorder | 5 (2.3%) | 28 (5.9%) | = 0.043 | PC = IED |
Conduct Disorder | 15 (6.9%) | 131(27.5%) | < 0.001 | PC < IED* |
Oppositional Defiant Disorder | 10 (4.6%) | 75 (15.7%) | < 0.001 | PC < IED* |
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder | 17 (7.9%) | 73 (15.3%) | < 0.007 | PC = IED |
p < 0.05 after correction for multiple comparisons (uncorrected p < 0.003).
3.2. Socio-economic status of parents during childhood/adolescence (Table 1)
Self-reported SES scores of parents were lowest among IED participants (36.8 ± 13.0), compared to HC (41.5 ± 12.3) or PC (40.8 ± 13.4) participants, which also differed from each other (F[21018] = 15.51, p < 0.001) such that PC participants’ self-reported parental SES scores were lower than HC participants’. SES scores of parents correlated with those of the adult participants to a moderate degree (r = 0.36, p < 0.001); these correlations did not differ by participant group.
3.3. Marital status of parents (Table 3)
Table 3.
Parental variables among participants.
HC | PC | IED | Adjusted OR | Adjusted OR | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 452) | (n = 215) | (n = 478) | IED vs ALL | IED vs PC | |
Both | |||||
Biological Parents Married to Each Other | 410 (90.7%) |
190 (88.4%) |
387 (81.0%) |
0.69 (0.47–1.01) (p = 0.057) |
0.65 (0.39–1.10) (p = 0.106) |
Raised by Both Parents | 361 (79.9%) |
158 (73.5%) |
277 (57.9%) |
0.52 (0.39–0.69) (p < 0.001) |
0.54 (0.37–0.79) (p = 0.002) |
Raised by One | |||||
Biological Parent Parental Separation | 135 (29.9%) |
92 (42.8%) |
250 (52.3%) |
1.64 (1.26–2.13) (p < 0.001) |
1.30 (0.92–1.83) (p = 0.140) |
Age at Separation (± SD) | 9.6 ± 6.5 | 10.6 ± 6.1 | 9.2 ± 6.9 | N/A | N/A |
Frequent Contact - Mother | 125 (92.6%) |
83 (90.2%) |
215 (86.0%) |
0.43 (0.24–0.75) (p = 0.003) |
0.58 (0.28–1.19) (p = 0.134) |
Frequent Contact - Father | 57 (42.2%) |
36 (39.1%) |
32 (32.4%) |
0.60 (0.45–0.80) (p < 0.001) |
0.69 (0.47–0.99) (p = 0.050) |
Note. HC = Healthy Controls; PC = Psychiatric Controls; IED = Intermittent Explosive Disorder.
IED participants did not differ significantly from the two control groups in the proportion of parents who were formally married, though IED study participants were significantly less likely to be raised by both parents than either PC or HC participants. IED participants were more likely to report that their parents had been separated or divorced than HC participants but did not differ from PC participants on this item; the age at such separation did not differ among the groups.
3.4. Parental aggression towards each other (Table 4)
Table 4.
Aggression in household during childhood/adolescence.
HC | PC | IED | Adjusted OR | Adjusted OR | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 452) | (n = 215) | (n = 478) | IED vs ALL | IED vs PC | |
Intimate | |||||
Partner Violence Parents Hit Each Other | 8 (1.8%) |
21 (9.8%) |
73 (15.3%) |
2.95 (1.85–4.71) (p < 0.001) |
1.45 (0.85–2.47) (p = 0.174) |
Father Hits Mother | 7 (1.5%) |
17 (7.9%) |
64 (13.4%) |
3.11 (1.88–5.16) (p < 0.001) |
1.54 (0.86–2.75) (p = 0.146) |
Mother Hits Father | 2 (0.4%) |
7 (3.3%) |
28 (5.9%) |
3.48 (1.58–7.65) (p = 0.002) |
1.60 (0.68–3.82) (p = 0.285) |
Parents Hits/Beats Severely | |||||
Participant | 47 (10.6%) |
54 (25.8%) |
207 (45.3%) |
3.56 (2.68–4.73) (p < 0.001) |
1.97 (1.37–2.84) (p < 0.001) |
Participant’s Siblings |
45 (10.1%) |
53 (25.4%) |
161 (35.2%) |
2.40 (1.79–3.22) (p < 0.001) |
1.28 (0.88–1.85) (p = 0.199) |
Note. Aggression in household during childhood/adolescence was determined when the participant indicated the behavior occurred “often/frequently.” HC = Healthy Controls; PC = Psychiatric Controls; IED = Intermittent Explosive Disorder.
IED participants were more likely to report that their parents “frequently/often” hit each other during their childhood/adolescence compared with PC or HC participants. Fathers were more likely to hit mothers for both IED and PC participants, who did not differ from each other on this item; this was also true for mothers hitting fathers.
3.5. Parental aggression towards proband and siblings (Table 4)
IED participants were more likely to report that their parents “frequently/often” beat them during childhood/adolescence compared with PC or HC participants. The level of aggression from parents towards siblings, however, was similar between IED and PC participants, which was significantly more likely compared with HC participants.
3.6. Relationship with neuro-behavioral and learning difficulty issues (Table 5)
Table 5.
Neuro-behavioral and learning issues in childhood/adolescence.
HC | PC | IED | Adjusted OR | Adjusted OR | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 452) | (n = 215) | (n = 478) | IED vs ALL | IED vs PC | |
History of Sleepwalking (Before Age 15) | 37 (8.2%) | 22 (10.2%) | 62 (13.0%) | 1.67 (1.12–2.49) (p = 0.016) |
1.46 (0.86–2.48) (p = 0.161) |
History of Stuttering (Before Age 15) | 22 (4.9%) | 25 (11.6%) | 49 (10.3%) | 1.25 (0.81–1.94) (p = 0.318) |
0.77 (0.46–1.31) (p = 0.337) |
History of Enuresis (After Age 5) | 33 (7.3%) | 48 (22.3%) | 87 (18.2%) | 1.35 (0.98–1.91) (p < 0.087) |
0.68 (0.45–1.02) (p = 0.062) |
Learning Difficulties | 23 (5.1%) | 42 (19.5%) | 111 (23.2%) | 2.42 (1.71–3.43) (p < 0.001) |
1.18 (0.78–1.78) (p = 0.424) |
Hels Back in Grade | 20 (4.4%) | 18 (8.4%) | 52 (10.9%) | 1.36 (0.86–2.17) (p = 0.190) |
1.10 (0.61–1.99) (p = 0.740) |
Note. HC = Healthy Controls; PC = Psychiatric Controls; IED = Intermittent Explosive Disorder.
IED participants were more likely to report history of sleepwalking or stuttering before age of 15, or enuresis after the age of 5, compared with HC participants. PC participants reported a greater history enuresis compared with IED participants but only at a trend level of statistical significance. IED and PC participants also reported a greater history of learning difficulty in school compared with HC participants. Despite this, the three groups did not significantly differ in being held back in grade.
3.7. Relationship with behavioral issues in school (Table 6)
Table 6.
Behavioral issues in childhood and adolescence.
HC | PC | IED | Adjusted OR | Adjusted OR | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 452) | (n = 215) | (n = 478) | IED vs ALL | IED vs PC | |
Rejection by Peers (Before Age 10) |
21 (4.6%) | 78 (36.3%) |
198 (41.4%) |
3.85 (2.87–5.16) (p < 0.001) |
1.28 (0.91–1.81) (p = 0.151) |
Fighting with Peers (Before Age 10) |
10 (2.2%) |
21 (9.8%) |
127 (26.6%) |
6.45 (4.21–9.89) (p <0.001) |
3.09 (1.87–5.11) (p < 0.001) |
Arrest by Police (Before Age 18) |
12 (2.7%) |
20 (9.3%) |
101 (21.1%) |
4.63 (2.99–7.15) (p < 0.001) |
2.51 (1.49–4.25) (p < 0.001) |
Note. HC = Healthy Controls; PC = Psychiatric Controls; IED = Intermittent Explosive Disorder.
IED participants were more likely to report history of “fighting with peers” before age of 10, as well as “arrest byt police” during adolescence, compared with PC and HC participants. Despite this, IED participants did not differ from PC participants in reporting “rejection by peers” before the age of 10; both groups were significantly different on this item compared with HC participants.
3.8. Effect of lifetime substance use disorder (SUD), conduct disorder (CD), and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) on findings (Supplemental Tables)
Because PC and IED study participants differed significantly in the lifetime prevalence of SUD, CD, and ODD (IED > PC), and each is associated with increased aggression, we added these three disorders to the statistical models for the four variables with elevated and statistically significant adjusted odds ratios for IED vs. PC. IED continued to have elevated and statistically significant adjusted odds ratios for these variables in all participants, but not when only IED and PC participants were examined. In the former comparison the adjusted odds ratio for both IED and CD, but not for SUD or ODD, were significantly elevated. In the latter comparison, adjusted odds ratio for IED was not significantly elevated for “Arrest by Police” though it was for CD.
4. Discussion
Analysis of these data reveals significant differences in several areas including parental SES, the identity of the individual who raised the study participant, parental aggression towards the participant, and childhood history of fighting with peers in study participants with IED compared with both healthy controls and psychiatric controls. Overall, these results suggest that the presence of childhood adversity is linked with the presence of IED and that the childhood environment of those with IED is substantially more tumultuous compared to both healthy individuals and individuals with non-aggressive psychiatric disorders.
The combination of lower parental SES and greater intrafamilial violence in participants with IED can be used to infer important details about their childhood environment. For one, lower SES suggests that participants were more likely to grow up in a financially insecure household, leading to uncertainty and stressors that can facilitate aggressive behavior (e.g., family dysregulation, parental depression; 25). In turn, these stressors may partially account for why the parents of such individuals behave more aggressively on a regular basis. This notion of frequent intrafamilial violence is consistent with research that suggests those with IED are more likely to have immediate family members with IED [26]. This learned pattern of aggressive behavior, in part, translates to environments outside the household (such as at school) as participants with IED reported they were more likely to fight with their peers, peer rejection, and/or juvenile arrest records compared to participants without a psychiatric diagnosis.
That said, aggressive behavior is also under genetic influence as well as the influence of injury that can result when engaging in aggressive behavior (e.g., traumatic brain injury). Twin studies clearly report substantial genetic influence underlying aggressive [27,28] and impulsive [29] behaviors while case-control studies report increased aggressive behavior among those with traumatic head injury even if it is of mild severity [30]. Thus, there may be no less than three routes to aggressive behavior: through behaviors that are inherited, behaviors that are learned, and that may be a consequence of being aggressive.
Another notable finding is that IED participants were more likely to be raised by a single parent. When combined with the previously described risk factors (e.g., lower SES, intrafamilial violence, peer rejection), it can be inferred that IED participants also lacked protective factors in childhood. For example, within single-parent, lower SES households, the parent is more likely to have a full-time job and less time to spend with their children during the day. Thus, IED participants may have received less supportive quality time with their parent, and their parent may have had less time and resources to identify and address behavioral issues during the participant’s childhood compared to healthy control participants [25,31]. Further, compared with healthy study participants, study participants with IED were less likely to have frequent contact with their parent who was not the primary caregiver, compounding the potential lack of parental support. Although differences in parental separation, contact with the non-primary caregiving parent, sleepwalking, and learning difficulties were found between IED and healthy participants, such differences were not found between IED and participants with non-aggressive psychopathology. Thus, these factors were more likely associated with psychopathology in general rather than with aggressive behavior.
While study participants with IED differed in many respects from study participants with non-aggressive psychopathology, the two groups did not differ on variables related to peer rejection and to poor academic function. Given the fact that study participants with IED reported greater incidence of fighting with peers, the similarity in report of peer rejection was surprising. However, there are several paths to peer rejection besides aggressive behavior. Similarly, poor academic function is not specifically related to aggressive behavior even though it can contribute to limited academic achievement.
This study has both strengths and weaknesses. Among its strengths include the relatively large sample and the systematic evaluation of its study participants in terms of diagnoses and behavioral assessments. Among its weaknesses, include the retrospective nature of the study, which makes it subject to recall bias; the cross-sectional nature of the study, which prevents the making of causal inferences; the relatively simple, though fundamental, questions posed to the study participants, which prevent more nuanced interpretation of the data; and the fact that individuals with some disorders, such as bipolar disorder and active substance use disorder, were not included in this study. Future research may seek to include those with the excluded psychopathologies and make similar comparisons to examine whether childhood environments are associated with IED above and beyond other diagnoses with elevated rates of aggression.
5. Conclusion
The disproportionately aggressive nature of IED lends little surprise to the idea that individuals with IED have had (and/or report) conflict-ridden childhoods. These results suggest that interventions that more strongly support higher-need families may be warranted to reduce the development of IED and its behaviors.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This research was supported in part by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health: RO1 MH60836, RO1 MH66984, RO1 104673 (Dr. Coccaro) and the Pritzker-Pucker Family Foundation (Dr. Coccaro).
Role of funding sources
Funding sources has no role in study design or in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data or in the writing of this report.
Footnotes
Declaration of Competing Interest
Dr. Coccaro reports being a member and consultant to the Scientific Advisory Boards of Azevan Pharmaceuticals, Inc., Avanir Pharmaceuticals, Inc., and Boerhinger Ingelheim Pharmaceuticals, Inc. Drs. Shevidi and Timmins have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2023.152367.
References
- [1].American Psychiatric Association. Task force on DSM-5: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- [2].Coccaro EF, Lee RJ. Disordered aggression and violence in the United States. J Clin Psychiatry 2020;81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Rynar L, Coccaro EF. Psychosocial impairment in DSM-5 intermittent explosive disorder. Psychiatry Res 2018;264:91–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Kulper DA, Kleiman EM, McCloskey MS, Berman ME, Coccaro EF. The experience of aggressive outbursts in intermittent explosive disorder. Psychiatry Res 2015; 225:710–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].McCloskey MS, Kleabir K, Berman ME, Chen EY, Coccaro EF. Unhealthy aggression: intermittent explosive disorder and adverse physical health outcomes. Health Psychol 2010;29:324–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Coccaro EF. Intermittent explosive disorder as a disorder of impulsive aggression for DSM-5. Am J Psychiatry 2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Hwang I, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC. Intermittent explosive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012;69:1131–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Brodsky BS, Oquendo M, Ellis SP, Haas GL, Malone KM, Mann JJ. The relationship of childhood abuse to impulsivity and suicidal behavior in adults with major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:1871–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Fanning JR, Meyerhoff JJ, Lee R, Coccaro EF. History of childhood maltreatment in intermittent explosive disorder and suicidal behavior. J Psychiatr Res 2014;56: 10–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Sapolsky RM. Stress and plasticity in the limbic system. Neurochem Res 2003;28: 1735–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].De Bellis MD. Developmental traumatology: the psychobiological development of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and policy. Dev Psychopathol 2001;13:539–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt T, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science. 2002;297:851–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Dodge KA, Coie JD, Pettit GS, Price JM. Peer status and aggression in boys’ groups: developmental and contextual analyses. Child Dev 1990;61:1289–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Nickerson A, Aderka IM, Bryant RA, Hofmann SG. The relationship between childhood exposure to trauma and intermittent explosive disorder. Psychiatry Res 2012;197:128–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Lee R, Meyerhoff J, Coccaro EF. Intermittent explosive disorder and aversive parental care. Psychiatry Res 2014;220:477–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].First MB, Williams JBW, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. Structured clinical interview for DSM-5 - research version. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- [17].Pfohl B, Blum N, Zimmerman M. University of Iowa Dept. of psychiatry: Structured interview for DSM personality disorder: SIDP. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- [18].Coccaro EF, Nayyer H, McCloskey MS. Personality disorder-not otherwise specified evidence of validity and consideration for DSM-5. Compr Psychiatry 2012;53: 907–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Coccaro EF, Berman ME, Kavoussi RJ. Assessment of life history of aggression: development and psychometric characteristics. Psychiatry Res 1997;73:147–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol 1992;63: 452–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Coccaro EF, Schmidt-Kaplan CA. Life history of impulsive behavior: development and validation of a new questionnaire. J Psychiatr Res 2012;46:346–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Patton J, Stanford M, Barratt E. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol 1995;51:768–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Beck AT, Brown GK. Beck depression inventory-II (BDI-II). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- [24].Beck AT, Steer RA. Beck anxiety inventory manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace and Company; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- [25].Lee CS, Lee J, August GJ. Financial stress, parental depressive symptoms, parenting practices, and children’s externalizing problem behaviors: underlying processes. Family Relations 2011;60:476–90. [Google Scholar]
- [26].Coccaro EF. A family history study of intermittent explosive disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2010;44:1101–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Coccaro EF, Bergeman CS, McClearn GE. Heritability of irritable impulsiveness: a study of twins reared together and apart. Psychiatry Res 1993;48:229–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Miles DR, Carey G. Genetic and environmental architecture of human aggression. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;72:207–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Seroczynski AD, Bergeman CS, Coccaro EF. Etiology of the impulsivity/aggression relationship: genes or environment? Psychiatry Res 1999;86:41–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Mosti C, Coccaro EF. Mild traumatic brain injury and aggression, impulsivity, and history of other- and self-directed aggression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2018;30:220–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Kramer-Kuhn AM, Farrell AD. The promotive and protective effects of family factors in the context of peer and community risks for aggression. J Youth Adolesc 2016;45:793–811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.