Abstract
Objective
In Alzheimer's disease (AD), the presence of circadian dysfunction is well‐known and may occur early in the disease course. The melanopsin retinal ganglion cell (mRGC) system may play a relevant role in contributing to circadian dysfunction. In this study, we aimed at evaluating, through a multimodal approach, the mRGC system in AD at an early stage of disease.
Methods
We included 29 mild–moderate AD (70.9 ± 11 years) and 26 (70.5 ± 8 years) control subjects. We performed an extensive neurophtalmological evaluation including optical coherence tomography with ganglion cell layer segmentation, actigraphic evaluation of the rest‐activity rhythm, chromatic pupillometry analyzed with a new data‐fitting approach, and brain functional MRI combined with light stimuli assessing the mRGC system.
Results
We demonstrated a significant thinning of the infero‐temporal sector of the ganglion cell layer in AD compared to controls. Moreover, we documented by actigraphy the presence of a circadian‐impaired AD subgroup. Overall, circadian measurements worsened by age. Chromatic pupillometry evaluation highlighted the presence of a pupil‐light response reduction in the rod condition pointing to mRGC dendropathy. Finally, brain fMRI showed a reduced occipital cortex activation with blue light particularly for the sustained responses.
Interpretation
Overall, the results of this multimodal innovative approach clearly document a dysfunctional mRGC system at early stages of disease as a relevant contributing factor for circadian impairment in AD providing also support to the use of light therapy in AD.
Introduction
Circadian rhythms are characterized by a period of about 24 h, synchronized to the light–dark cycle by the projections of melanopsin retinal ganglion cells (mRGCs) to the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) through the retinohypothalamic tract. 1 These photoreceptors are a small subgroup of intrinsically photosensitive RGCs, particularly responsive to blue light and mediating mainly the non‐image forming functions of the eye, including pupillary light reflex (PLR) and sleep regulation. 2 , 3 , 4 Light is the main “zeitgeber” for circadian photoentraiment. Circadian rhythm dysfunction is commonly observed in aging and is particularly prominent in Alzheimer's disease (AD) 5 , 6 impacting on sleep quality, cognition, mood, and longevity. 7 , 8 Moreover, mounting evidence indicates that the occurrence of sleep disturbances leads to ß‐amyloid deposition 9 and that circadian dysfunction is a risk factor for dementia. 10 The mRGC functions and their impact on circadian rhythms have been extensively investigated over the last two decades taking advantage of animal models, which allow for their genetic manipulations. 11 However, major difficulties are encountered in evaluating their function in humans, in particular in the context of neurodegenerative disorders such as AD, due to the limited availability of in vivo investigative tools.
In vivo optical coherence tomography (OCT) and post mortem studies on retina and optic nerves demonstrated an age‐related loss of RGCs including also mRGCs. 12 , 13 , 14 In AD patients, it has been reported the occurrence of optic neuropathy and SCN degeneration, 15 , 16 , 17 as well as of mRGC loss. 18 Moreover, ß‐amyloid deposition as extracellular plaques or affecting directly mRGCs with intracellular and axonal inclusions have been documented in AD post mortem retinas. 18 , 19
Chromatic pupillometry, allowing to assess the mRGC contribution to PLR, has been applied to evaluate this system in several neurodegenerative disorders including hereditary optic neuropathies, glaucoma, Parkinson's disease, isolated Rapid Eye Movement sleep Behaviour Disorder, 20 , 21 and AD. 22 , 23
Blue light is particularly effective in stimulating mRGCs and previous brain functional magnetic resonance (fMRI) studies conducted in controls, people suffering seasonal affective disorder (SAD) and blind subjects, demonstrated the presence of activation/deactivation of specific cerebral areas crucial for cognitive functions using light stimuli tasks designed to stimulate mRGCs. 24 , 25 , 26 , 27 , 28 , 29
In this study, we aimed at expanding on our previous investigation by evaluating in vivo the mRGC system in relation to circadian rhythms through a novel multimodal approach including cognitive testing, neurophthalmological assessment and OCT measures, chromatic pupillometry, actigraphy, and brain fMRI studies in a cohort of 29 mild–moderate AD patients compared to 26 controls. We also evaluated the mRGC system in 3 centenarians.
Materials and Methods
In this study, we prospectively screened at the IRCCS Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, Bologna, Italy, 33 AD patients and 42 controls. All participants signed institutional review board–approved consent forms. The local Ethical Committee approved the study (#CE 16032, Comitato Etico Interaziendale Imola‐Bologna). We also included 3 centenarians (104 years M, 101 years F and 103 years F), who underwent the neuropthalmological evaluation, pupillometry, and rest‐activity assessment by actigraphy.
For AD, the inclusion criterion was AD diagnosis at mild–moderate stage of disease 30 (MMSE between 11 and 25) according to IWG‐2 criteria, 31 whereas for controls the absence of cognitive dysfunction as evaluated by cognitive tests.
Exclusion criteria for patients and controls were as follows: (1) spherical or cylindrical refractive errors more than 3 or 2 diopters, (2) presence of posterior pole pathology including age‐related macular degeneration and known optic neuropathies including open‐angle glaucoma, (3) ocular pressure more than 20 mmHg, (4) severe lens opacity, (5) retinal detachment, (6) vascular retinal pathology including diabetic retinopathy, (7) shift‐workers in the last year, (8) travels through more than one time zone during the last 3 months, and (9) contraindications for MRI.
Exclusion criteria for controls included (1) excessive daytime sleepiness as assessed by the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS score > 10), (2) presence of poor sleep quality as determined by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index Questionnaire (PSQI index ≥7), 28 and (3) abnormal scores (≥11) on the 21‐item Beck Depression Inventory 32 and the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory–Y. 33
Neurophthalmological evaluation
Neurophthalmological evaluations included best corrected visual acuity (VA) by Snellen's chart, color vision tests (Ishihara test), slit‐lamp biomicroscopy, Goldman applanation tonometry, color fundus photography, and OCT (DRI Triton, Topcon, Tokyo, Japan). OCT protocols included the evaluation of peripapillary retinal nerve fiber (RNFL) thickness and ganglion cell layer (GCL) segmentation analysis of the macula (GCL is defined as the thickness from the inner boundary of the GCL to the outer boundary of the Inner Plexiform Layer). 23
Cognitive testing
All AD patients and control subjects underwent an extensive neuropsychological assessment. In particular, global cognition was evaluated by Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) 34 and Brief Mental Deterioration Battery (BMDB). 35 , 36 All tests were standardized for age and years of education in the Italian population.
Actigraphy
Rest‐activity rhythm was evaluated using a wrist actigraph (MotionWatch8, CamNtech Ltd, Fenstanton, UK) worn on the non‐dominant wrist for 7 consecutive days. Subjects were instructed to wear the device continuously over the 24 h and to complete a daily sleep‐log to track: bed and raise times, daytime naps, and periods of device removal. Subjects with <5 consecutive days of recording were excluded from analysis.
All weekly participants' MotionWatch8 files were checked for missing data. Missing values were edited as follows: missing values of <1 h were not edited; between 06:00 and 23:00 h, any missing activity data ≥1 h, but ≤3 h in total were replaced with the daily mean activity value; between 06:00 and 23:00 h, any days with missing activity data >3 h were discarded. Actigraphy recordings were analyzed using the Motionware software v.1.1.15a (CamNtech Ltd) to assess sleep and NPCRA (Non‐Parametric Circadian Rhythm Analysis) measures. 37 , 38 In particular, we included non‐parametric measures of circadian rhythms [IS, Interdaily Stability, range 0–1; IV, Intradaily Variability, range: 0–2; RA, Relative Amplitude, (M10 − L5)/(M10 + L5), range 0–1; M10, Most 10 average, activity during most 10 active hours; L5, Least 5 average, activity for least 5 active hours]. The following estimated sleep measures were also considered: SE (Sleep Efficiency, %); TST (Total Sleep Time, minutes); TIB (Time In Bed, hours); AWT (Actual Wake Time); SL (Sleep Latency, minutes); TAS (Total Activity Score); and FI (Fragmentation Index) (https://www.camntech.com/motionware‐software/). Circadian and sleep parameters were used for comparisons between AD patients and controls, and for correlations with clinical data.
Moreover, to assess group differences in circadian motor activity profile, we processed the time‐series of raw motor activity data through functional linear modeling (FLM). 39 The seven‐day of motor activity data were averaged into a single 24‐h activity profile and fitted using a Fourier expansion model with n = 19 basis permutations. Groups' differences in motor activity were assessed through non‐parametric permutation F‐test.
Pupillometry
Chromatic pupillometry methods are detailed in Romagnoli et al. 23 Briefly, a Ganzfeld ColorDome full‐field stimulator (Espion V6, ColorDome Desktop Ganzfeld; Diagnosys LLC, Lowell, MA, USA) was used, and subjects were dark‐adapted for 10 min before undergoing the following conditions:
rod‐condition: low luminance 1 s blue flash (0.001 cd/m2, 472 nm) under dark‐adaption;
mRGC‐condition: high luminance 1 s blue flash (450 cd/m2, 472 nm) under dark‐adaption;
cone‐condition: 1 s red flash (10 cd/m2, 632 nm) presented against 6 cd/m2 rod‐suppressing blue‐adapting field.
Data were analyzed offline using custom MATLAB scripts (MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA) as reported in details in Romagnoli et al. 23 In addition, we complemented this quantitative report with parameters describing the PLR entity and its dynamics, tailored on individual stimulations. For the rod‐condition, we computed the contraction onset timing (the latency relative to the stimulus delivery needed for the pupil to start constriction, that is, to detach from the baseline level), the average slope (computed as the ratio between the peak amplitude and the duration of the contraction) parameters (Fig. 1A) and the parameters (see Fig. 1B) ideally fitting the contraction time course to a negative exponential curve whose equation can be summarized in y = A – B * e (−lambda * x).
Regarding the mRGC‐condition, the exponential dynamics of PIPR was characterized by using the MATLAB Curve FITTING Toolbox (exp1 function, MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA) and deriving the exponential coefficient (the global rate constant of the exponential modeled PLR curve) and AUC 5–7 s (the area under the curve over a 5–7 s time interval from the light‐stimulus offset) (Fig. 1C,D).
Brain fMRI
Brain fMRI methods can be found detailed in Evangelisti et al. 29
Before the MR acquisitions, participants underwent a dark adaption period of 1 h and a training session for the cognitive task.
MR acquisitions were performed with a 1.5T system (GE Signa HDx 15), equipped with an 8‐channel phased array coil. The protocol included fMRI (gradient‐echoplanar sequence, slice thickness 4 mm, resolution 1.875 × 1.875 mm, repetition time TR = 3000 ms) and high‐resolution T1‐weighted images (fast spoiled gradient echo sequence, TR = 12.4 ms, TE = 5.2 ms, inversion time TI = 600 ms, voxel 1 × 1 × 1 mm3).
As for the fMRI paradigms (Fig. 2), for the cognitive stimulation was administrated the auditory Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT). 40 It consisted in the presentation of a series of sounds separated by intervals between 1 and 9 s, and participants were required to respond to each sound as quickly as possible by pressing a button. Overall, 10 blocks were presented, separated by rest interval of 15 s, for a total duration of ~10 min. This paradigm was aimed to evaluate cognitive brain responses of sustained attention. The task was also acquired in combination with a simultaneous light stimulation (visual cognitive paradigm, duration ~20 min) consisting of an alternation of blue and red illumination periods of 50 s and darkness (duration from 20 to 30 s). This paradigm allowed the evaluation of the effects on brain responses of the interactions between light stimulation and cognitive task. Finally, the pure visual paradigm was meant to investigate the possible role of mRGCs in a pure visual setting. Participants were exposed to blue or red lights for periods of 10 s separated with 5 s of darkness, with a random color alternation, for a total duration of ~10 min.
Monochromatic light stimulation was achieved with a metal‐free purpose‐built optic fiber combined with narrow (FWHM 10 nm) interference band‐pass filters (blue 480 nm, red 620 nm).
The image analyses were carried out using the FSL software (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl).
After image pre‐processing, single subject brain responses were estimated with a General Linear Model. For each paradigm, the events of interest were the following: for the “Cognitive” paradigm, the stimuli of the PVT paradigm were described with stick functions; for the “Visual” paradigm, the periods of lighting with blue and red were modeled with blocks and the switching on/off of the lighting with sticks; for the “Visual/Cognitive” paradigm, the stimuli of the PVT paradigm were described as sticks and the periods of blue and red were modeled with blocks. The activation maps were linearly registered to the T1‐weighted structural images, and then non‐linearly aligned to the standard space of the MNI template.
Statistical analysis
Between group analyses were performed to compare demographics, OCT, actigraphy, and pupillometry outcomes. Individual patient's data were compared by using chi‐square, independent sample t‐, and the Mann–Whitney U‐tests. Normality of all continuous variables was checked by using Shapiro–Wilk and Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Spearman correlation coefficients were computed to measure the degree of association between OCT/actigraphy/pupillometry findings or clinical variables (disease duration, MMSEc), Bonferroni's correction method was used, and two‐sided p‐values are presented. Statistical analyses were carried out using R software (version 4.0.0) and IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 20.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, N.Y., USA) software.
OCT
Between groups (AD versus controls) analyses to compare OCT outcomes were performed by following the “all‐eyes approach”. 41 OCT variables were compared by using linear mixed‐effect model (LMM) with the visual outcome as the dependent variable, the group as independent variable, under a compound symmetry covariance structure and with a patient random effect, while adjusting for age‐decade and gender. 42 Alternatively, for OCT outcomes with skewed distribution, we applied the Clustered Wilcoxon rank sum test using Rosner‐Glynn‐Lee method. 43 Moreover, p‐value for Group×Age decade interaction term was calculated with log‐likelihood ratio test (LR test) comparing nested LMMs with and without the interaction term, and stratified β coefficients (95% confidence interval, 95% CI) for the variables turning out to be effect modifiers (interaction term p‐value <0.20).
Actigraphy
Comparison of actigraphy measures between AD and control groups were performed through independent t‐ and Mann–Whitney U‐ tests. Moreover, p‐value for Group × Age decade interaction term was calculated with LR test comparing nested linear models with and without the interaction term, and stratified β coefficients (95% CI) for the variables turning out to be effect modifiers (interaction term p‐value <0.20).
Pupillometry
For all conditions (rod/mRGC/cone), pupillometry metrics were obtained by their individual average values. Statistical analyses included Wilcoxon signed‐rank or independent t‐tests and aimed to compare variables among groups (AD/controls).
Brain fMRI
Group comparisons were performed by non‐parametric permutation statistics. For the visual paradigm, comparisons were made within the primary visual cortex (based on the Juelich histological atlas 44 ), while for cognitive and visual/cognitive ones, comparisons were made within the entire gray matter. Statistical inferences were made from statistical maps corrected for multiple comparisons (FWE, Family Wise Error, p < 0.05).
Demographics of participants recruited for fMRI examinations were compared between the two groups using non‐parametric testing. To compare the performance in the cognitive task during MRI acquisitions in different light conditions, a Friedman test was performed.
The fMRI results were correlated, by non‐parametric Spearman test, with the data related to the neuropsychological evaluation, with the measurements of RNFL, with pupillometry and actigraphic measures.
Results
According to inclusion and exclusion criteria, 13/42 controls and 4/33 AD were excluded and 3 controls dropped‐out after the screening visit (Fig. 3). Ultimately, 26 controls and 29 AD were included in the study.
Demographic and main clinical data of the examined subjects are reported in Table 1.
Table 1.
Controls | Alzheimer's | p‐value | |
---|---|---|---|
N | 26 (47.3%) | 29 (52.7%) | |
Gender | |||
Male | 9 (34.6%) | 15 (51.7%) | 0.2 |
Female | 17 (65.4%) | 14 (48.3%) | |
Age (mean ± SD), years | 70.5 ± 8 | 70.9 ± 11 | 0.89 |
Age decade | |||
50–59 years | 3 (11.5%) | 6 (20.7%) | 0.32 |
60–69 years | 9 (34.6%) | 4 (13.8%) | |
70–79 years | 9 (34.6%) | 12 (41.4%) | |
≥80 years | 5 (19.3%) | 7 (24.1%) | |
MMSEc | 27.9 ± 1.4 | 20.2 ± 4.2 | p < 0.0001 |
Disease duration, years | – | 3.9 ± 2.8 | – |
Optical coherence tomography evaluation
According to exclusion criteria, OCT scans of 4 controls were excluded from analysis. Therefore, OCT data of control (n = 22) and AD (n = 29) subjects are reported in Table 2. RNFL thickness was not different between AD and controls. Only the GCL thickness of the IT sector was significantly reduced in AD patients compared to controls (p = 0.034). All other GCL measurements were not significantly different between groups.
Table 2.
Controls | Alzheimer's | p‐value 1 | p‐value 2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
N, subjects | 22 (43.1%) | 29 (56.9%) | ||
N, eyes | 40 | 51 | ||
n, Right eyes | 18 | 27 | ||
n, Left eyes | 22 | 24 | ||
RNFL, AVG μm | 105.4 ± 2.1 | 106.6 ± 1.8 | 0.99a | 0.94 |
RNFL, T μm | 77.8 ± 1.95 | 76.2 ± 1.7 | 0.16a | 0.14 |
RNFL, S μm | 127.9 ± 2.98 | 131.4 ± 2.6 | 0.32a | 0.89 |
RNFL, N μm | 82.1 ± 3.0 | 83.4 ± 2.6 | 0.13b | 0.81 |
RNFL, I μm | 134 ± 3.3 | 135.4 ± 2.9 | 0.92a | 0.82 |
GCL, AVG μm | 70.4 ± 1.15 | 69.4 ± 0.95 | 0.40a | 0.16 |
GCL, ST μm | 71 ± 1.1 | 69.3 ± 0.9 | 0.10a | 0.20 |
GCL, S μm | 69.6 ± 1.2 | 68.8 ± 0.97 | 0.51a | 0.035 |
GCL, SN μm | 72 ± 1.3 | 71.8 ± 1.1 | 0.74a | 0.15 |
GCL, IN μm | 71.1 ± 1.3 | 70.1 ± 1.1 | 0.52a | 0.48 |
GCL, I μm | 66.9 ± 1.2 | 66 ± 1.0 | 0.13b | 0.30 |
GCL, IT μm | 72 ± 1.3 | 70.6 ± 1.1 | 0.034b | 0.55 |
p‐value referred to: a “Group” predictor of linear mixed‐effect model (LMM, maximum likelihood method, random intercept, compound symmetry) for OCT variables normally distributed; bComparison of OCT variables not‐normally distributed by using Clustered Wilcoxon rank sum test, Rosner–Glynn–Lee method.
p‐value referred to likelihood‐ratio test comparing the goodness of fit of two competing LMM models (with and without interaction term Group × Age decade) for each OCT parameters; variables highlighting a significant effect modifier are those with p‐value for interaction < 0.20.
The Group × Age decade interaction term analysis revealed significant differences between groups for the following OCT parameters: temporal RNFL thickness, and average, superior and supero‐nasal GCL thickness (Table 2). Specifically, looking at β coefficients stratified by group (AD/control), we found for controls a significant decrease for the average, superior and supero‐nasal GCL thickness for the 60–69, 70–79, and over 80 decades if compared to the decade 50–59, while in AD we observed a significant decrease only for the average GCL thickness in the comparison between the decade over 80 and 50–59 (Fig. 4).
Actigraphy
Actigraphic data were available for 22 controls and 27 AD. We found a significant difference between controls and AD for the following actigraphic parameters: TST (p = 0.01), TIB (p = 0.001), AWT (p = 0.04), SL (p = 0.04) and TAS (p = 0.04) (Fig. 5, panels D, H–K).
Overall, even if there were no significant differences, AD patients showed higher variability for circadian measures compared to controls, with some subjects clearly distinct from the control group. The circadian‐impaired patients were defined as cases with circadian values >2 standard deviations (SD) from the mean of controls, as previously proposed 45 (Fig. 5, red plots in panels A–L).
Considering the Group × Age decade interaction term analysis, we found a significant difference between groups for RA, L5, and TAS (Fig. 5, panels M–O). Specifically, if we look at the single decades, for RA a significant difference was evident in the comparison between the decades 60–69 and 70–79 versus 50–59 in controls and between the over 80 versus 50–59 in AD (Fig. 5M), for L5 in the comparison between 70–79 versus 50–59 in controls and over 80 versus 50–59 in AD (Fig. 5N), for TAS in the comparison between over 80 versus 50–59 in AD (Fig. 5O).
Moreover, AD patients presented significantly lower motor activity from 24:00 to 2:00 (global test of significance), from 11:00 to 14:00 (point‐wise test of significance), and from 18:00 to 21:00 (point‐wise test of significance) compared to controls (Fig. 5, panels P,Q).
We also evaluated the actigraphic recordings of the 3 centenarians which did not significantly differ from the controls in the decade 80–90 (data not shown). Furthermore, SE was significantly higher in the centenarians compared to AD in the group over 80s (p = 0.03), as well as AWS (p = 0.02) and TAS (p = 0.005) were significantly reduced in centenarians compared to AD in the group over 80s.
Pupillometry
Pupillometry recordings included in the final analysis, according to inclusion/exclusion criteria, were available for 22 controls and 26 AD. Under rod‐condition, AD patients showed a significantly delayed onset of transient PLR response [contraction onset timing (ms): AD = 362 (50); Control = 323 (24); p = 0.001] as well as a significantly lower average slope [average slope (1/ms): AD = 2.8 × 10−4 (1 × 10−4); Control = 3.4 × 10−4 (5.4 × 10−5); p = 0.035], and also a significantly reduced |B| [|B|: AD = 0.24 (0.08); Control = 0.29 (0.08); p = 0.034] indicative of a lower exponential decay amplitude. Under mRGC‐condition, no significant differences were observed among AD and control groups. Finally, we evaluated the PLR in the 3 centenarians demonstrating a significant reduction only for the PIPR parameter (p = 0.008) in the comparison with controls in the 80–90 decade (data not shown).
Correlations between actigraphic and pupillometric variables
For each group (control/AD), a Spearman correlation coefficient was computed to assess the relationship between actigraphy/pupillometry/clinical variables (disease duration and MMSEc), the latter only for AD group.
In controls, there was a strong, negative correlation between RA and contraction onset timing (rod‐condition) with r = −0.84 (CI 95% = −0.94 to −0.60, p adjusted = 0.001), and a strong, positive correlation between M10 and the slope (rod‐condition) of PLR with r = 0.83 (CI 95% = 0.57–0.94, p adjusted = 0.002). The same bivariate relationships were lost in AD group (data not shown). In AD, there was only a moderate, negative correlation between TIB and |B| with r = −0.65 (CI 95% = −0.84 to −0.32, p adjusted = 0.048) (Supplementary Materials).
Functional MRI
Among the 55 subjects enrolled in the study, 7 participants were not available to perform the fMRI. From the remaining 48, 14 subjects were not included because of claustrofobia or not‐compatible MRI implanted devices. For 34 participants (14 AD and 20 controls), the fMRI acquisitions were completed but 6 were excluded for suboptimal image quality.
The final dataset for fMRI studies consists of 12 AD (mean age 66.8 ± 10.0 years, 8 males, all right‐handed and with a mean MMSEc score of 22.4 ± 5.5) and 16 controls (mean age 72.1 ± 8.2 years, 5 males, all right‐handed and with a mean MMSEc score of 29.0 ± 0.9). The two groups differed significantly, as expected, for the cognitive level assessed with MMSEc (p‐value 0.001), while there was no statistically significant difference for age nor gender (p = 0.271 and p = 0.141, respectively).
Visual stimulation
Control subjects showed primary occipital cortex activation in response to both transient and sustained (10 s and 50 s) stimuli for both red and blue light without significant differences between the two light conditions. Instead, AD patients showed reduced cortical activations to blue light compared to red light in response to transient and sustained (10 s) stimuli. Moreover, when considering only activations under blue light, they did not show significant responses to sustained stimuli (10 s and 50 s) (Fig. 6, panel A). In the AD group, the reduced cortical activations to blue light compared to red light in response to transient stimuli (blue < red) showed significant positive correlations with disease duration (r = 0.689; p = 0.013) and negative correlations with the inferior RNFL thickness at OCT (r = 0.806; p = 0.008). Instead, in the control group, the sustained response to blue light positively correlated with some actigraphic parameters (i.e., RA, r = 0.606; p = 0.029 and SE, r = 0.589; p = 0.042).
Cognitive activation
Cerebral responses to PVT, in both groups, involved brain regions associated with sustained attention; however, these responses were significantly reduced in AD patients compared to controls within occipital, temporal, insular, parietal, and cerebellar cortices (Fig. 6, panel B).
Reaction time in AD was overall longer compared to controls even though this difference was not significant due to large variability of responses, especially in the AD group. Moreover, the lower brain responses for AD compared to controls correlate with performance obtained in different cognitive tasks, such as MMSEc, those evaluating executive functions, memory and language abilities (p < 0.001).
Visual and cognitive stimuli interaction
The effect of blue light compared to red light on brain responses during the cognitive task was more evident in cerebral regions involved in visual functions (such the intracalcarine and occipital lateral cortex) and in brain regions crucial for vigilance and sustained attention (such as the frontal pole, superior and middle frontal gyri, middle temporal gyrus) (Fig. 6, panel C). Even if not statistically significant, in AD patients there seem to be a tendency toward an improvement of the cognitive performances under light stimulation in particular with blue light ([mean ± SD], median, AD: RTblue [775 ± 784, 444] ms, RTred [1113 ± 1813, 468] ms, RTdark [1376 ± 2293, 532] ms; HC: RTblue [438 ± 118, 432] ms, RTred [427 ± 116, 419], RTdark [447 ± 132, 455] ms) (Fig. 6, panel D).
Brain activations for the contrast PVTblue > PVTred in the comparison HC > AD the cingulate gyrus correlate with the SE (r = −0.776; p = 0.005) in AD patients.
Discussion
In this study, we evaluated in vivo the mRGC‐system through a multimodal approach including OCT, actigraphy, chromatic pupillometry, and fMRI in a cohort of mild–moderate AD patients demonstrating that mRGCs are affected in AD, as highlighted by actigraphic, pupillometry, and fMRI findings. Each approach provides a specific layer of information that deserves a comment.
First, the OCT analysis failed to show significant differences between AD and controls except for the infero‐temporal GCL thickness. This sector corresponds to the supero‐nasal sector of the optic nerve, in line with previous results pointing to a preferential loss of the magnocellular component of the optic nerve in AD. 15 , 19 , 46 Thus, the absence of significant RNFL differences suggests that in this group of mild AD with short disease duration an early pathology of the RGC cell body may occur before axonal damage becomes evident. Differently, in another cohort of AD, we have previously shown a significant thinning of the RNFL, mostly in the superior sector of the optic nerve. 18 This discrepancy is most probably explained by the longer disease duration and the greater disease severity of this AD cohort and different OCT technologies used. 18 Similarly to our results, other OCT studies failed to document significant differences in terms of RNFL between AD and controls. 47 , 48 We also found a significant decline of the average GCL thickness by age in both control and AD groups, which, remarkably, in controls was always significant between the 50–59 decade as compared to all the other decades, whereas in AD only in the comparison between the decades 50–59 and over 80s. Thus, in controls there is a major age‐related decline starting after 60 years of age, whereas in AD this is already evident in the 50–59 decade. In general, however, OCT measurements are age and disease severity‐related and only indirectly may be linked to mRGC pathology, if we consider that mRGCs represent only about 1% of total RGCs.
Second, the actigraphic recordings showed a significant difference between controls and AD for the TST, TIB, SL, and TAS. The increased TST found in the AD group is in line with previous findings. 49 However, considering the circadian parameters (RA, IS, and IV), the two groups did not significantly differ even though the presence of a subgroup of circadian‐impaired AD patients was evident, as previously reported. 18 We also demonstrated a decline of actigraphic parameters by age in both controls and AD for RA, L5, and TAS. Overall, actigraphic results confirm the presence of circadian and sleep dysfunction in at least a subgroup of AD patients at early stage of disease, and these abnormalities worsened with aging. Remarkably, when we evaluated the circadian parameters in the 3 centenarians, we found that some of the sleep parameters were better in centenarians than in AD patients in the 80–90 decade, as well as we observed the absence of significant worsening in centenarians in comparison with the controls from the same decade. This supports the concept of a successful aging in this group of super‐controls, which may be contributed by the maintenance of circadian synchronization possibly due to mRGC‐dependent circadian photoentrainment. 50
Third, in relation to pupillometry studies, we recently published a preliminary study demonstrating a significant reduction of the peak amplitude for the rod‐condition in AD compared to controls as well as a significant correlation with age only in the AD group and a significant increase of the PIPR variance in the melanopsin protocol in the same AD patients here investigated. 23 We interpreted these results, considering the retinal circuitry connecting mRGCs with rods, 51 as a sign of ongoing pathology affecting mRGC dendrites, thus the connections between mRGCs with classical photoreceptors, 23 as supported by the occurrence of dendropathy affecting mRGCs in post mortem AD retinas. 18 Overall, these findings are compatible with mRGC pathology that does not affect yet the cell body but only the dendritic connections. 51 In the current study, using a curve fitting approach we calculated the contraction onset timing and the average slope of the PLR, and using the exponential fitting of the constriction phase of the PLR we calculated lambda and |B| parameters of the PLR. Under rod‐condition, AD patients showed a significantly delayed onset of transient PLR response as well as a significantly lower average slope, and also a significantly reduced |B| indicative of a lower exponential decay amplitude. These pupillometric results overall reinforce and are in line with our previous findings 23 again pointing toward an early dendropathy affecting mRGCs. In the 3 centenarians, we found a significant reduction only of the PIPR, which might suggest a loss of mRGCs with aging, as previously reported. 14 , 52
The significant correlation emerging between pupillometric and actigraphic measurements in controls argues in favor of a cross‐validity of these two approaches in capturing the mRGC‐mediated non‐visual functions, further validating the sensitivity of these tools to explore the mRGC system in vivo.
As final approach, brain fMRI results revealed reduced occipital cortex responses to blue light compared to red light stimulation in AD compared to controls, in particular for the sustained responses to light. This result further complements in vivo the mRGC loss previously reported in post mortem studies, 18 as well as the pupillometry and actigraphic results. 23 Interestingly, the reduced brain response to blue light compared to red light is correlated with disease duration in AD. As expected, the activation of brain areas is significantly lower in the psychomotor vigilance test in AD patients, mainly within temporal, insular, parietal, and cerebellar cortices, and the performances are worse in AD. When the cognitive task is combined with the light stimulation, performances in AD patients tend to be slightly better under the blue light and this stimulation is associated with the activation of not only brain areas involved in visual processing but also brain areas involved in vigilance and sustained attention, such as medial frontal cortex, medial frontal gyrus, and cingulate gyrus, where the activation is reduced compared to controls. Taken together, these findings might support the role of mRGCs in mediating brain cognitive responses, as previously described in healthy subjects and Leber's hereditary optic neuropathy patients. 24 , 29 At the same time occipital, frontal and temporal regions show higher brain activity under blue versus red light stimulation in AD compared to controls, indicating a possible compensatory effect.
Overall, the findings of our multimodal approach point to a dysfunctional mRGC system since the early stages of AD. Considering also the results of previous studies, we envisage a specific pathology affecting the mRGC cell body, reflected at initial stages by the dendropathy involving the connections with rods, detected by pupillometry, as well as by the reduction of the GCL thickness. Only at later stages, this mRGC pathology evolves into axonal damage, included in the general RNFL thinning. The dysfunction of the mRGC system ultimately contributes to the well‐known circadian and sleep disturbances reported in AD, documented by actigraphic studies. The timing of this process may vary in subgroups of AD patients, as reflected in the great variability observed for the multiple readouts that we used in our multimodal approach. Our findings may also support the use of light therapy as a counteracting measure for circadian dysfunction in AD aimed at stimulating the still functional mRGCs. 53 Finally, mRGC function, as studied in vivo by multiple approaches providing different layers of information, may be proposed as a potential biomarker useful for stratifying the risk of disease conversion from MCI to AD and to identify subgroup of AD patients who are at increased risk to develop circadian and sleep disturbances.
This study has some limitations which include the limited number of patients enrolled, the absence of complete data for all of the patients examined and the inclusion of mild cases, which was necessary for compliance in running the tests.
In conclusion, the use of such a multimodal approach for investigating the mRGC system, particularly in neurodegenerative disorders characterized by circadian and sleep dysfunction, is here proposed as an effective tool for evaluating in vivo the mRGC‐dependent occurrence of circadian dysfunction, and biomarking patients for risk stratification.
Author Contributions
CL, MM, RL, and VC contributed to the conception and design of the study; CL, MM, MR, MSM, SE, MD, SC, CB, CT, GV, MC, PD, MF, PA, CZ, and FB contributed to the acquisition and analysis of data; CL, MM, MR, SE, SC, CT, GV, AS, MF, PA, PB, RL, CT, RL, and VC contributed to drafting the text or preparing the figures.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare no conflicts of interest related to this manuscript.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
This work has been supported by the Italian Ministry of Health Young Researcher Project Grant (GR‐2013‐02358026 to CL). We are deeply grateful to patients and their caregivers for participating to the study. We also thank Prof. Piero Parchi for performing the CSF examination of the AD subjects included, Dr. Lidia Di Vito and Dr. Giulia Amore for their support in the initial evaluation of the patients, Prof. Gaetano Cantalupo and Dr. Jason Park for support in developing and setting the chromatic pupillometry protocol, Dr. Vincenzo Allegri for performing amyloid PET in AD patients, Prof. Claudio Franceschi for his support in the recruitment of centenarians, Dr. Maria Lunardelli and Dr. Maria Macchiarulo for the support in the recruitment of AD patients and Prof. Alfredo Sadun for his advice and fruitful discussions. This work has been also supported by Bibliosan. Open access funding provided by BIBLIOSAN.
Funding Statement
This work was funded by Italian Ministry of Health grant GR‐2013‐02358026.
Data Availability Statement
For original data, please contact Chiara La Morgia (chiara.lamorgia@unibo.it).
References
- 1. Hannibal J, Hindersson P, Knudsen SM, Georg B, Fahrenkrug J. The photopigment melanopsin is exclusively present in pituitary adenylate cyclase‐activating polypeptide‐containing retinal ganglion cells of the retinohypothalamic tract. J Neurosci. 2002;22(1):RC191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Berson DM, Dunn FA, Takao M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science. 2002;295(5557):1070‐1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Hattar S, Liao HW, Takao M, Berson DM, Yau KW. Melanopsin‐containing retinal ganglion cells: architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity. Science. 2002;295(5557):1065‐1070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Aranda ML, Schmidt TM. Diversity of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: circuits and functions. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2021;78(3):889‐907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Oosterman JM, van Someren EJ, Vogels RL, Van Harten B, Scherder EJ. Fragmentation of the rest‐activity rhythm correlates with age‐related cognitive deficits. J Sleep Res. 2009;18(1):129‐135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Wu YH, Swaab DF. Disturbance and strategies for reactivation of the circadian rhythm system in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Sleep Med. 2007;8(6):623‐636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Froy O. Circadian rhythms, aging, and life span in mammals. Physiology (Bethesda). 2011;26(4):225‐235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Li P, Gao L, Gaba A, et al. Circadian disturbances in Alzheimer's disease progression: a prospective observational cohort study of community‐based older adults. Lancet Healthy Longev. 2020;1(3):e96‐e105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Ozcan GG, Lim S, Leighton P, Allison WT, Rihel J. Sleep is bi‐directionally modified by amyloid beta oligomers. eLife. 2020;9:e53995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Tranah GJ, Blackwell T, Stone KL, et al. Circadian activity rhythms and risk of incident dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older women. Ann Neurol. 2011;70(5):722‐732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Do MTH. Melanopsin and the intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: biophysics to behavior. Neuron. 2019;104(2):205‐226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Johnson BM, Miao M, Sadun AA. Age‐related decline of human optic nerve axon populations. Age. 1987;10(1):5‐9. [Google Scholar]
- 13. Feuer WJ, Budenz DL, Anderson DR, et al. Topographic differences in the age‐related changes in the retinal nerve fiber layer of normal eyes measured by stratus optical coherence tomography. J Glaucoma. 2011;20(3):133‐138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. La Morgia C, Ross‐Cisneros FN, Sadun AA, et al. Melanopsin retinal ganglion cells are resistant to neurodegeneration in mitochondrial optic neuropathies. Brain. 2010;133(Pt 8):2426‐2438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Hinton DR, Sadun AA, Blanks JC, Miller CA. Optic‐nerve degeneration in Alzheimer's disease. N Engl J Med. 1986;315(8):485‐487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Harper DG, Stopa EG, Kuo‐Leblanc V, et al. Dorsomedial SCN neuronal subpopulations subserve different functions in human dementia. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 6):1609‐1617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Chan VTT, Sun Z, Tang S, et al. Spectral‐domain OCT measurements in Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Ophthalmology. 2019;126(4):497‐510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. La Morgia C, Ross‐Cisneros FN, Koronyo Y, et al. Melanopsin retinal ganglion cell loss in Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol. 2016;79(1):90‐109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Koronyo Y, Biggs D, Barron E, et al. Retinal amyloid pathology and proof‐of‐concept imaging trial in Alzheimer's disease. JCI Insight. 2017;2(16):e93621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. La Morgia C, Carelli V, Carbonelli M. Melanopsin retinal ganglion cells and pupil: clinical implications for neuro‐ophthalmology. Front Neurol. 2018;9:1047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. La Morgia C, Romagnoli M, Pizza F, et al. Chromatic pupillometry in isolated rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Mov Disord. 2022;37(1):205‐210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Oh AJ, Amore G, Sultan W, et al. Pupillometry evaluation of melanopsin retinal ganglion cell function and sleep‐wake activity in pre‐symptomatic Alzheimer's disease. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(12):e0226197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Romagnoli M, Stanzani Maserati M, De Matteis M, et al. Chromatic pupillometry findings in Alzheimer's disease. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Vandewalle G, Schmidt C, Albouy G, et al. Brain responses to violet, blue, and green monochromatic light exposures in humans: prominent role of blue light and the brainstem. PLoS ONE. 2007;2(11):e1247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Vandewalle G, Maquet P, Dijk DJ. Light as a modulator of cognitive brain function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(10):429‐438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Vandewalle G, Archer SN, Wuillaume C, et al. Effects of light on cognitive brain responses depend on circadian phase and sleep homeostasis. J Biol Rhythms. 2011;26(3):249‐259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Vandewalle G, Collignon O, Hull JT, et al. Blue light stimulates cognitive brain activity in visually blind individuals. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013;25(12):2072‐2085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Daneault V, Hebert M, Albouy G, et al. Aging reduces the stimulating effect of blue light on cognitive brain functions. Sleep. 2014;37(1):85‐96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Evangelisti S, La Morgia C, Testa C, et al. Brain functional MRI responses to blue light stimulation in Leber's hereditary optic neuropathy. Biochem Pharmacol. 2021;191:114488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Perneczky R, Wagenpfeil S, Komossa K, Grimmer T, Diehl J, Kurz A. Mapping scores onto stages: mini‐mental state examination and clinical dementia rating. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(2):139‐144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, et al. Advancing research diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease: the IWG‐2 criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(6):614‐629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4:561‐571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Spielberger CDVP, Barker LR, Donham GW, Westberry LG. The factor structure of the state‐trait anxiety inventory. In: Sarason IG, Spielberger CD, eds. Stress and Anxiety. Vol 7. Hemisphere Publishing; 1980:95‐109. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini‐mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189‐198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Gallassi R, Lenzi P, Stracciari A, et al. Neuropsychological assessment of mental deterioration: purpose of a brief battery and a probabilistic definition of “normality” and “non‐normality”. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1986;74(1):62‐67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Gallassi R, Oppi F, Poda R, et al. Are subjective cognitive complaints a risk factor for dementia? Neurol Sci. 2010;31(3):327‐336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. van Someren EJ, Hagebeuk EE, Lijzenga C, et al. Circadian rest‐activity rhythm disturbances in Alzheimer's disease. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40(4):259‐270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Goncalves BS, Adamowicz T, Louzada FM, Moreno CR, Araujo JF. A fresh look at the use of nonparametric analysis in actimetry. Sleep Med Rev. 2015;20:84‐91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Wang J, Xian H, Licis A, et al. Measuring the impact of apnea and obesity on circadian activity patterns using functional linear modeling of actigraphy data. J Circadian Rhythms. 2011;9(1):11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Drummond SP, Bischoff‐Grethe A, Dinges DF, Ayalon L, Mednick SC, Meloy MJ. The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep. 2005;28(9):1059‐1068. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Armstrong RA. Statistical guidelines for the analysis of data obtained from one or both eyes. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2013;33(1):7‐14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Ying GS, Maguire MG, Glynn R, Rosner B. Tutorial on biostatistics: linear regression analysis of continuous correlated eye data. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2017;24(2):130‐140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Rosner B, Glynn RJ, Lee ML. Extension of the rank sum test for clustered data: two‐group comparisons with group membership defined at the subunit level. Biometrics. 2006;62(4):1251‐1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Eickhoff SB, Paus T, Caspers S, et al. Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. Neuroimage. 2007;36(3):511‐521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Hatfield CF, Herbert J, van Someren EJ, Hodges JR, Hastings MH. Disrupted daily activity/rest cycles in relation to daily cortisol rhythms of home‐dwelling patients with early Alzheimer's dementia. Brain. 2004;127(Pt 5):1061‐1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. La Morgia C, Di Vito L, Carelli V, Carbonelli M. Patterns of retinal ganglion cell damage in neurodegenerative disorders: parvocellular vs magnocellular degeneration in optical coherence tomography studies. Front Neurol. 2017;8:710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Sanchez D, Castilla‐Marti M, Rodriguez‐Gomez O, et al. Usefulness of peripapillary nerve fiber layer thickness assessed by optical coherence tomography as a biomarker for Alzheimer's disease. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):16345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. den Haan J, Verbraak FD, Visser PJ, Bouwman FH. Retinal thickness in Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2017;6:162‐170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Hooghiemstra AM, Eggermont LH, Scheltens P, van der Flier WM, Scherder EJ. The rest‐activity rhythm and physical activity in early‐onset dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2015;29(1):45‐49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Shen J, Tower J. Effects of light on aging and longevity. Ageing Res Rev. 2019;53:100913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Hannibal J, Christiansen AT, Heegaard S, Fahrenkrug J, Kiilgaard JF. Melanopsin expressing human retinal ganglion cells: subtypes, distribution, and intraretinal connectivity. J Comp Neurol. 2017;525(8):1934‐1961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Esquiva G, Lax P, Perez‐Santonja JJ, Garcia‐Fernandez JM, Cuenca N. Loss of melanopsin‐expressing ganglion cell subtypes and dendritic degeneration in the aging human retina. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Mitolo M, Tonon C, La Morgia C, Testa C, Carelli V, Lodi R. Effects of light treatment on sleep, cognition, mood, and behavior in Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2018;46(5–6):371‐384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
For original data, please contact Chiara La Morgia (chiara.lamorgia@unibo.it).