Skip to main content
Public Health Nutrition logoLink to Public Health Nutrition
. 2012 Nov 5;17(2):324–337. doi: 10.1017/S136898001200482X

Predictors of high-energy foods and beverages: a longitudinal study among socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents

Lena D Stephens 1,*, Sarah A McNaughton 1, David Crawford 1, Kylie Ball 1
PMCID: PMC10282232  PMID: 23122445

Abstract

Objective

While socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents tend to have poor dietary intakes, some manage to eat healthily. Understanding how some disadvantaged adolescents restrict high-energy foods and beverages may inform initiatives promoting healthier diets among this population. The present investigation aimed to: (i) identify disadvantaged adolescents’ high-energy food and beverage intakes; and (ii) explore cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between intrapersonal, social and environmental factors and disadvantaged adolescents’ high-energy food intakes.

Design

Longitudinal online surveys were completed at baseline (2004–2005) and follow-up (2006–2007), each comprising a thirty-eight-item FFQ and questions examining intrapersonal, social and environmental factors.

Setting

Thirty-seven secondary schools in metropolitan and non-metropolitan Victoria, Australia.

Subjects

Of 1938 adolescents aged 12–15 years participating at both time points, 529 disadvantaged adolescents (whose mothers had low education levels) were included in the present investigation.

Results

At baseline and follow-up, respectively 32 % and 39 % of adolescents consumed high-energy foods less frequently (≤2 high-energy food meals/week); 61 % and 65 % consumed high-energy beverages less frequently (≤1 time/d). More girls than boys had less frequent high-energy food intakes, and baseline consumption frequency predicted consumption frequency at follow-up. Adolescents with less frequent consumption of high-energy foods and beverages seldom ate fast food for main meals, reported reduced availability of high-energy foods at home and were frequently served vegetables at dinner.

Conclusions

Nutrition promotion initiatives could help improve disadvantaged adolescents’ eating behaviours by promoting adolescents and their families to replace high-energy meals with nutritious home-prepared meals and decrease home availability of high-energy foods in place of more nutritious foods.

Keywords: Adolescents, Nutrition, Behaviour, Socio-economic position


Good nutrition is important for preventing several diet-related diseases( 1 ). Since dietary habits and preferences developed in childhood and adolescence may be maintained into adulthood( 2 , 3 ), altering diet-related behaviours early in life, such as during adolescence( 4 , 5 ), is important for disease prevention. Many high-energy foods and beverages are nutrient-poor and often sources of high levels of saturated fat, sugar and salt( 6 ), which have been linked to overweight and obesity, dyslipidaemia, hypertension, hyperglycaemia and insulin resistance( 7 ). Increased intakes of high-energy foods and beverages also tend to displace consumption of more nutritious foods including fruit, vegetables and milk( 8 , 9 ), further impacting on health.

Adolescents tend to consume diets that are at odds with recommendations for health, with a high proportion consuming high-energy foods and beverages daily( 10 , 11 ). Further, adolescents’ intake of high-energy foods tends to increase as they mature, resulting in even poorer dietary quality( 12 14 ). Socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents are at greater risk for consuming a poorer diet than more advantaged adolescents, consuming high-energy foods and beverages more frequently( 12 , 15 ). For example, one study of adolescents from all levels of socio-economic position reported that of those who consumed high-energy foods ≥3–4 times/week, 61 % were disadvantaged, and 56 % of adolescents consuming high-energy beverages ≥5–6 times/week were disadvantaged( 15 ). In order to improve such eating behaviours, a better understanding of the factors influencing adolescent nutrition is required.

Although socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents are at greater risk for consuming a poor diet, some manage to consume a more favourable diet( 16 18 ). Understanding factors that enable disadvantaged adolescents to protect their dietary quality, including consuming fewer high-energy foods and beverages, can help inform nutrition promotion initiatives aiming to improve the dietary intakes of disadvantaged adolescents.

Social ecological models consider the connections between individuals and their environments, or ecologies( 19 , 20 ), across intrapersonal, social and environmental domains and provide useful frameworks for identifying factors associated with eating behaviours. Several factors have previously been shown to be associated with dietary intakes among adolescents. Self-efficacy, perceived importance of health behaviours, taste preferences, food-related behaviours (e.g. meal frequency, snacking) and barriers (e.g. cost, time, inconvenience)( 21 ) are key intrapersonal determinants of adolescents’ dietary intakes. Social factors associated with dietary intake include adolescents’ interactions with family, friends and peers( 22 24 ). Parenting style, role modelling of eating behaviours, reinforcement, perceived norms and cultural factors may also be involved( 25 ). Finally, environmental influences on adolescent eating behaviour include food availability, accessibility and affordability at home, school and within the local neighbourhood( 25 27 ).

Several intrapersonal, social and environmental factors have previously been associated with socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents’ frequent intakes of vegetables and fruit. For example, we previously reported that greater perceived importance of health and frequently being served vegetables with dinner were associated with frequent vegetable and fruit intakes( 28 ). Among disadvantaged boys, friends’ support for healthy eating was associated with frequent vegetable consumption. Less stringent adherence to family meal-time rules, including whether the adolescent was allowed to buy whatever was liked at fast-food places among both sexes and being expected to eat all foods among boys, was associated with frequent vegetable and fruit consumption; however, the opposite was observed when girls were expected to eat all foods served( 28 ).

To our knowledge, predictors of less frequent consumption of high-energy foods and beverages among disadvantaged adolescents have not been examined. Therefore the aims of the present investigation were: (i) to identify disadvantaged adolescents’ high-energy food and beverage intakes; and (ii) drawing on Social Ecological Theory, to explore cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between intrapersonal, social and environmental factors and disadvantaged adolescents’ high-energy food and beverage intakes.

Methods

Participants and setting

The present investigation draws on a sub-sample of 529 socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents with longitudinal data from the YEP Study, an online food habits survey conducted in thirty-seven secondary schools in metropolitan and non-metropolitan regions of Victoria, Australia. The baseline phase was conducted in 2004–2005, and adolescents were followed up in 2006–2007( 14 , 28 , 29 ).

At baseline, invitations to participate were open to all co-educational government and Catholic secondary schools that included Years 7–12 and had >200 enrolments located in metropolitan Melbourne and non-metropolitan Gippsland, east of Melbourne, Australia. Of seventy schools that met these criteria, twenty metropolitan and seventeen non-metropolitan schools (thirty-seven in total) agreed to participate. All students (n 9842) from Year 7 (aged 12–13 years) and Year 9 (aged 14–15 years) were invited to participate. Written informed consent was received from adolescents’ parents, and the survey was completed during class time by 3264 sociodemographically diverse secondary students (n 2010 in Year 7, n 1254 in Year 9; response rate at baseline 33·2 %). Schools that participated in the baseline YEP survey were contacted again in 2006 to indicate their interest in continuing involvement in the YEP Study. Schools were sent a list of adolescents for whom parental consent had been granted at baseline and teachers invited adolescents to complete the online follow-up survey. Of 3264 adolescents who completed the baseline YEP survey in 2004–2005, 1938 completed the 2006–2007 YEP follow-up survey (59 % response rate). Of 1287 socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents who completed the baseline YEP survey, 708 (55 %) also completed the follow-up survey. At baseline, a survey assessing sociodemographics of the parent and their partner, including highest level of education, and additional questions about their adolescents’ eating patterns was mailed out to those parents who had given informed consent for their adolescents to participate. Of parents sent a parental survey, 1622 (64 % of parents who initially indicated their interest to participate; representing 50 % of parents for the whole YEP adolescent sample) returned a completed survey.

In the present investigation, only data from socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents who had non-missing data for all variables of interest were included in the analyses (n 529). When adolescents who had non-missing data were compared with those with missing data (n 782) across the measures included in the present investigation, few statistically significant (P ≤ 0·01) differences in these variables existed between groups. When compared with adolescents who had incomplete data, adolescents with complete data rarely bought food/drink from the school canteen, rarely bought food/drink on the way to/from school, had smaller amounts of spending money, perceived greater maternal role modelling of healthy behaviours, were always expected home for dinner and had greater availability of nutritious food at home. The remaining thirty-two variables did not differ between the groups. Where differences did exist, they were relatively small in magnitude.

Socio-economic position was defined based on maternal highest level of education (self-reported at baseline): ‘low’, mother completed up to Year 10 of high school; ‘medium’, mother completed Year 12 high school and/or a technical or trade school certificate/apprenticeship; and ‘high’, mother completed a university or tertiary qualification. The study was approved by Deakin University's Ethics Committee, the Victorian Department of Education and Training, and the Catholic Education Office (EC 227–2003).

Measures

Outcome variables

The online baseline and 2-year follow-up surveys each included a thirty-eight-item FFQ, comprising twenty-seven food items and eleven beverage items, based on food intake questions recommended by the Australian Food and Nutrition Monitoring and Surveillance Unit( 30 ) and those used in the 1995 National Nutrition Survey( 31 ). These food and beverage items were based on those most commonly consumed by individuals aged 12 years and older( 31 ). Adolescents were asked to indicate on a seven-point scale (scored 1–7) the frequency with which they had eaten each food item in the previous month. Scores representing equivalent daily frequencies for high-energy beverages were converted from monthly frequencies as follows: ‘not in the last month’ (scored 0·00 – i.e. consumed zero times daily), ‘several times per month’ (0·07), ‘once a week’ (0·14), ‘a few times a week’ (0·36), ‘most days’ (0·71), ‘once per day’ (1·00) and ‘several times per day’ (2·50). As the FFQ did not include portion size, calculation of serving size was not possible. Therefore each FFQ response scale was converted to weekly (high-energy food) or daily (high-energy beverages) equivalent frequencies separately at baseline and at follow-up. High-energy food monthly frequencies were converted to equivalent weekly frequencies as follows: ‘not in the last month’ (scored 0·0 – i.e. consumed 0 times weekly), ‘several times per month’ (0·50), ‘once a week’ (1·00), ‘a few times a week’ (2·00), ‘most days’ (4·00), ‘once per day’ (7·00) and ‘several times per day’ (14·00). Scores representing equivalent daily frequencies for high-energy beverages were converted from monthly frequencies as follows: ‘not in the last month’ (scored 0·00 – i.e. consumed zero times daily), ‘several times per month’ (0·07), ‘once a week’ (0·14), ‘a few times a week’ (0·36), ‘most days’ (0·71), ‘once per day’ (1·00) and ‘several times per day’ (2·50). This approach is based on the Victorian Cancer Council FFQ User Guide( 32 ) conversion of FFQ response categories to daily equivalent frequencies, a methodology commonly used to rank individuals’ dietary intakes( 33 , 34 ).

The ‘high-energy food’ group included ‘hot chips, French fries, wedges, fried potato’, ‘fish or seafood (from a fish and chip shop)’, ‘pizza’, ‘pies, pastries, sausage rolls’ and ‘fast foods (e.g. McDonalds, KFC)’. Foods purchased at fast-food restaurants average ~1100 kJ per 100 g( 35 , 36 ). ‘High-energy beverages’ included regular (i.e. not diet/low-calorie) cordial (a sweetened, flavoured, concentrated syrup mixed with water to taste), regular soft drink, energy drinks (e.g. V*, Red Bull ) and sports drinks (e.g. Gatorade , Powerade § ).

Among socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents, baseline and follow-up consumption frequencies were dichotomized as follows: less frequent consumption was defined as ≤2 times/week for high-energy food meals and as ≤1 time/d for high-energy beverages.

Predictor variables

Survey items were developed to assess intrapersonal, social and environmental factors drawn from Social Ecological Theory and hypothesized to influence adolescent eating behaviours( 19 , 20 ). The YEP surveys were pilot-tested among twenty adolescents, with survey items modified slightly for clarity based on adolescents’ feedback prior to being administered to the larger sample. The present investigation included a number of intrapersonal, social and environmental measures from the baseline survey (Table 1). Scales were created by summing categorical-response items measuring a particular construct. For example, three items measuring the perceived importance of health behaviours were summed to give a composite score. Cronbach's α coefficients were calculated for all summed scales used in the present investigation (Table 1).

Table 1.

Intrapersonal, social and environmental measures from the baseline YEP adolescent survey

Measure Item type Range Cronbach's α Source
Intrapersonal measures
Skipped meals frequency Three individual items: Individual items 1–5 N/A Original
‘Over the past month, about how often have you…?’
‘…skipped breakfast?’
‘…skipped lunch?’
‘…skipped dinner?’
Meals eaten alone Two individual items: Individual items 1–5 N/A Original
‘Over the past month, about how often have you…?’
‘…eaten breakfast on your own?’
‘…eaten dinner on your own?’
Fast food eating behaviours Four individual items: Individual items 1–5 N/A Original
‘Over the past month, about how often have you…?’
‘…eaten fast food or takeaway for breakfast?’
‘…bought fast food or takeaway for lunch?’
‘…eaten fast food or takeaway for dinner at home?’
‘…eaten dinner at a fast-food place (like McDonalds, Pizza Hut)?’
School eating behaviours Four individual items examining school-time eating behaviours: Individual items 1–5 N/A Adapted from Cleland et al.( 53 )
‘About how often do you…?’
‘…buy foods or drinks from the school canteen/tuck shop?’
‘…leave the school grounds during school (e.g. at recess or lunchtime) to buy food or drinks?’
‘…buy food or drinks on the way to or from school?’
‘About how often do you buy foods or drinks from vending machines at school?’ 1–6
Perceived importance of health ‘How important are the following to you?’ Scale 3–12 0·74 Original
behaviours ‘Eating healthy food’
‘Limiting the amount of “junk food” you eat’
‘Exercising and staying fit’
Self-efficacy (for fruit or energy-dense food) ‘If you wanted to, how confident (sure) are you that you could eat more fruit…?’ (or ‘…could cut down on junk food…?’) Scale 3–12 0·84 (fruit) Adapted from Kremers et al.( 54 ) and Neumark-Sztainer et al.( 55 )
‘…when you're hanging out with friends?’
‘…when you're at school?’ 0·82 (energy-dense
‘…when you're at home?’ food)
Spending money ‘In a typical week, about how much money do you have available to spend on yourself (e.g. from pocket money, a part-time job)?’ Individual item 1–6 N/A Original
Social measures
Family support for healthy eating ‘During the past year, about how often have your family (parents/brothers or sisters) said or done this?’ Scale 5–15 0·76 Adapted from Sallis et al.( 56 )
‘…made you feel good about the way you eat?’
‘…eaten healthy foods with you?’
‘…encouraged you not to eat “junk food” when you felt like it?’
‘…encouraged you to eat healthy foods?’
‘…encouraged you to try new foods?’
Friends’ support for healthy eating The same set of five questions about family support was repeated to assess support for healthy eating from friends Scale 5–15 0·78 Adapted from Sallis et al.( 56 )
Mother's role modelling of ‘My mother…’ Scale 4–12 0·71 Original
healthy eating ‘…eats healthy food’
‘…limits the amount of “junk food” she eats’
‘…eats vegetables most days’
‘…eats fruit most days’
Father's role modelling of healthy eating The same set of four questions about mother's role modelling was repeated to assess role modelling by the father Scale 4–12 0·75 Original
Friends’ role modelling of healthy eating The same set of four questions about mother's role modelling was repeated to assess role modelling by friends Scale 4–12 0·76 Original
Meal-time atmosphere Two individual items: Individual items 1–4 N/A Adapted from Fulkerson
‘The evening meal is an unpleasant time for my family’ et al.( 57 )
‘The evening meal is a time when my family really talks and catches up with each other’
Family meal-time rules Eight individual items: Individual items 1–4 N/A Adapted from Fulkerson
‘I eat whatever I like at home’ et al.( 57 )
‘During meal times, I'm allowed to put the TV on’
‘At meal times I have to follow certain rules (e.g. not talking with my mouth full)’
‘I'm expected to be home for dinner unless otherwise arranged’
‘I'm expected to have good manners at the dinner table (e.g. handling food politely – using my knife and fork properly)’
‘I'm expected to eat all the foods served even if I don't like them’
‘It's OK for me to make something else to eat if I don't like the food being served for dinner’
‘I'm always allowed to buy whatever I want from fast-food places’
Environmental measures
Home access to food (nutritious food or energy-dense food) Two individual items: Individual items 1–4 N/A Adapted from Neumark-Sztainer et al. ( 55 )
‘There is plenty of food at home’
‘Vegetables are served at dinner’
Home availability of food (nutritious food or high-energy food) ‘About how often are the following foods available in your home?’ Scale 2–8 0·75 Adapted from Campbell et al.( 58 ) and Neumark-
Nutritious food: Sztainer et al.( 55 )
‘fruit’ and ‘vegetables’
Energy-dense food: Scale 5–20 0·75
‘cakes/doughnuts/biscuits’
‘potato chips or other salty snack foods’
‘chocolate or other lollies’
‘soft drink’ and ‘sports drinks or energy drinks’
Perception of school canteen ‘How would you rate your school canteen for…?’ Scale 5–25 0·81 Original
‘…buying fresh foods (e.g. fruit)?’
‘…buying prepared foods (e.g. sandwiches, salads)?’
‘…value of food (e.g. price)?’
‘…quality of food (e.g. freshness)?’
‘…speed of service (time to get served)?’
Neighbourhood availability of ‘Are there fast-food places near where you live?’ Scale 4–15 0·70 Original
energy-dense food Summed together with:
‘Are there…’
‘…places to buy snacks near where you live (e.g. ice creams, lollies, soft drinks, cakes, potato crisps)?’
‘…fast-food places near your school?’
‘…places to buy snacks near your school (e.g. ice creams, lollies, soft drinks, cakes, potato crisps)?’

N/A, not applicable.

Covariates

Past research has demonstrated that sociodemographic characteristics including sex( 11 , 21 ), age( 11 , 21 ) and region of residence( 10 ) are associated with adolescent diet. These data were gathered in the baseline survey and were considered as covariates in the present investigation.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using the Stata statistical software package version 11. Descriptive statistics were used to describe sociodemographic characteristics of participating adolescents (n 529) and to examine the proportions of adolescents eating fewer high-energy foods and beverages at baseline and at follow-up. Associations between each sociodemographic characteristic (sex, age and region of residence) and less frequent intakes at each time point were identified in bivariable logistic regression analyses, and only those sociodemographic characteristics significantly (P ≤ 0·01) associated with the dietary outcome were adjusted for in further bivariable and multivariable analyses. The more stringent criterion of using a P value of ≤0·01 (rather than P ≤ 0·05) was applied for determining statistical significance as the relatively large sample size used and large number of tests conducted in the present investigation increased the likelihood of a Type I error.

Spearman non-parametric correlation coefficients were calculated to indicate co-linearity between predictor variables. Two predictor variables were considered to be co-linear if P ≥ 0·4, indicating a moderate correlation( 37 ). Of those two co-linear predictor variables, only the predictor variable most strongly associated with either of the two food group outcomes was included in further analyses. Therefore, due to co-linearity, the ‘Self-efficacy for fruit’ and ‘Perceived importance of healthy behaviours’ scales and the ‘Expected to follow certain meal-time rules’ and ‘Expected to be home for dinner’ items were excluded from analyses.

Bivariable logistic regression analyses were used to examine associations between baseline predictor variables and less frequent consumption of each food group outcome at baseline. Similarly, bivariable logistic regression analyses adjusted for baseline consumption frequency were used to identify baseline predictors of less frequent consumption at follow-up. The reference categories chosen for each predictor variable were selected to facilitate the simplest interpretability of results. Statistically significant (P ≤ 0·01) factors identified in bivariable analyses were then entered into multivariable logistic regression analyses, which included adjustment for baseline frequency of intake. All models were also adjusted for relevant covariates. As the YEP Study involved recruitment of a sample of adolescents clustered by school, potential clustering effects by school in regression models were adjusted for clustering by using the ‘cluster’ command in Stata to generate robust standard errors.

Results

Adolescents were sociodemographically diverse (Table 2). Among socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents included in the present investigation, 32 % consumed fewer high-energy foods (≤2 high-energy meals/week) at baseline, increasing to 39 % at follow-up. Sixty-one per cent of disadvantaged adolescents consumed fewer high-energy beverages (≤1 time/d) at baseline, increasing to 65 % at follow-up.

Table 2.

Sociodemographic characteristics of socio-economically disadvantaged Australian adolescents and proportions consuming fewer high-energy foods and beverages at baseline and follow-up (n 529)

Sociodemographic characteristics n %
Total sample 529 100
Sex
Boys 227 43
Girls 302 57
Age group
Year 7 357 67
Year 9 172 33
Region of residence
Metropolitan 370 70
Non-metropolitan 159 30
Baseline less frequent high-energy food intake† 171 32
Follow-up less frequent high-energy food intake† 208 39
Baseline less frequent high-energy beverage intake‡ 321 61
Follow-up less frequent high-energy beverage intake‡ 345 65

†Less frequent intake defined as consumption of high-energy food meals ≤2 times/week.

‡Less frequent intake defined as consumption of high-energy beverages ≤1 time/d.

Bivariable logistic regression analyses were performed to identify statistically significant (P ≤ 0·01) covariates and predictor variables which were then entered into multivariable logistic regression models (data not shown). Multivariable logistic regression model findings from cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses are detailed below.

Cross-sectional associations between predictor variables and less frequent consumption of high-energy foods

After controlling for all bivariably associated predictor variables in multivariable analysis, two intrapersonal and one environmental predictor variables remained significantly associated with less frequent high-energy food intake (≤2 high-energy meals/week) at baseline among socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents (Table 3). Adolescents who had consumed high-energy food for dinner at home twice or less in the month preceding the baseline survey had nearly five times greater odds of eating fewer high-energy foods than adolescents who consumed high-energy foods at home more frequently. Similarly, adolescents who had not consumed fast food for dinner at a fast-food restaurant in the past month had two times greater odds of having less frequent intakes than adolescents who ate fast-food meals in this setting more often. Each unit increase on the ‘home availability of high-energy food’ scale was associated with a 13 % decrease in the odds of having less frequent intake, i.e. adolescents who reported less high-energy food available at home had greater odds of eating fewer high-energy foods compared with those who reported greater availability of those foods.

Table 3.

Odds ratios and 95 % confidence intervals of consuming fewer high-energy foods and beverages at baseline among socio-economically disadvantaged Australian adolescents identified in multivariable logistic regression analysis (n 529)

Less frequent intake (%) Frequent intake (%) OR 95 % CI P value
Less frequent high-energy food intake at baseline†
n 171 358
Intrapersonal factors
Skipped meals frequency
Skipped lunch
Every day/most days 7 14 1·00 Ref.
Once/twice a week 12 15 1·69 0·68, 4·18 0·25
Once/twice a month 17 22 1·38 0·61, 3·14 0·43
Not in last month 64 49 1·89 0·77, 4·67 0·16
Fast-food eating behaviours
Ate fast food for breakfast
Every day/most days/once/twice a week/once/twice a month 11 24 1·00 Ref.
Not in last month 89 76 1·46 0·72, 2·94 0·28
Ate fast food for lunch
Every day/most days/once/twice a week/once/twice a month 36 60 1·00 Ref.
Not in last month 64 40 1·20 0·69, 2·06 0·51
Ate fast food for dinner at home
Every day/most days/once/twice a week 9 44 1·00 Ref.
Once/twice a month/not in last month 91 56 4·94 2·63, 9·27 <0·001*
Ate fast food for dinner at a fast-food restaurant
Every day/most days/once/twice a week/once/twice a month 25 52 1·00 Ref.
Not in last month 75 48 2·09 1·23, 3·55 0·008*
School eating behaviours
Bought food/drink from school canteen
Every day/most days/sometimes 39 62 1·00 Ref.
Hardly ever/never 61 38 1·53 1·08, 2·18 0·02
Left school ground to buy food/drink
Every day/most days/ sometimes/hardly ever 8 20 1·00 Ref.
Never 92 80 1·32 0·67, 2·58 0·41
Bought food/drink on way to/from school
Every day/most days/sometimes/hardly ever 23 48 1·00 Ref.
Never 77 52 1·59 1·06, 2·39 0·03
Self-efficacy for decreasing intakes of energy-dense food 1·10 1·02, 1·18 0·02
Mean 9·34 8·20
sd 2·13 2·43
Spending money
$AUD ≥30/week/$AUD 20–29/week 12 18 1·00 Ref.
$AUD 10–19/week 16 21 1·28 0·52, 3·16 0·58
$AUD 5–9/week 27 28 0·91 0·44, 1·89 0·80
$AUD <5/week 21 20 0·94 0·38, 2·31 0·88
None 24 13 1·55 0·54, 4·48 0·41
Social factors
Meal-time atmosphere
Evening meal – unpleasant for family
Always/usually 8 15 1·00 Ref.
Sometimes 21 19 1·46 0·59, 3·60 0·41
Never 71 66 1·01 0·49, 2·11 0·97
Family meal-time rules
Allowed to buy whatever is liked at fast-food places
Always 10 20 1·00 Ref.
Usually 26 27 1·44 0·60, 3·44 0·40
Sometimes/never 64 53 1·41 0·61, 3·26 0·41
Environmental factors
Home access to food
Vegetables served at dinner
Never/sometimes/usually 28 46 1·00 Ref.
Always 72 54 1·90 1·15, 3·13 0·02
Home availability of high-energy food 0·87 0·79, 0·95 0·004*
Mean 10·9 12·8
sd 2·50 2·96
Less frequent high-energy beverage intake at baseline‡,§
n 321 208
Sociodemographic characteristics
Sex
Boys 37 51 1·00 Ref.
Girls 63 49 1·58 1·20, 2·09 0·002*
Intrapersonal factors
Skipped meals frequency
Skipped breakfast
Every day/most days 15 23 1·00 Ref.
Once/twice a week 11 15 1·14 0·62, 2·09 0·67
Once/twice a month 18 13 2·07 1·08, 3·97 0·03
Not in last month 56 49 1·52 0·96, 2·41 0·07
Fast-food eating behaviours
Ate fast food for breakfast
Every day/most days/once/twice a week/once/twice a month 15 27 1·00 Ref.
Not in last month 85 73 1·36 0·81, 2·27 0·23
Ate fast food for dinner at home
Every day/most days/once/twice a week 24 45 1·00 Ref.
Once/twice a month/not in last month 76 55 1·46 0·90, 2·37 0·12
Ate fast food for dinner at a fast-food restaurant
Every day/most days/once/twice a week/once/twice a month 36 55 1·00 Ref.
Not in last month 64 45 1·62 0·96, 2·75 0·07
School eating behaviours
Bought food/drink from school canteen
Every day/most days/sometimes 47 65 1·00 Ref.
Hardly ever/never 53 35 1·47 0·95, 2·26 0·08
Self-efficacy for decreasing intakes of energy-dense food 1·02 0·93, 1·12 0·72
Mean 8·88 8·10
sd 2·24 2·55
Social factors
Family meal-time rules
Allowed to make something else for dinner
Always 11 17 1·00 Ref.
Usually 18 18 1·96 0·95, 4·01 0·07
Sometimes 43 47 1·31 0·61, 2·80 0·48
Never 28 18 1·64 0·76, 3·55 0·20
Allowed to buy whatever is liked at fast-food places
Always 13 22 1·00 Ref.
Usually 26 28 1·14 0·71, 1·84 0·57
Sometimes/never 61 50 1·16 0·68, 1·97 0·58
Environmental factors
Home access to food
Vegetables served at dinner
Never/sometimes/usually 34 49 1·00 Ref.
Always 66 51 1·73 1·16, 2·58 0·009*
Home availability of high-energy food 0·84 0·78, 0·91 <0·001*
Mean 11·4 13·4
sd 2·67 3·00
Neighbourhood availability of high-energy food 0·98 0·89, 1·07 0·59
Mean 8·19 8·74
sd 2·06 2·09

Ref., reference category.

*P ≤ 0·01.

†Less frequent intake defined as consumption of high-energy food meals ≤2 times/week at baseline.

‡Less frequent intake defined as consumption of high-energy beverages ≤1 time/d at baseline.

§Model adjusted for covariate ‘sex’.

After controlling for all predictor variables bivariably associated with consuming fewer high-energy beverages (≤1 time/d) at baseline including the covariate ‘sex’, only three factors remained statistically significant in multivariable analysis (Table 3). Disadvantaged adolescent girls had 58 % greater odds of consuming fewer high-energy beverages than boys. Adolescents who reported always being served vegetables at dinner had 73 % greater odds of consuming fewer high-energy beverages when compared with adolescents who were served vegetables less often. Each unit increase on the ‘home availability of high-energy food’ scale was associated with a 16 % decrease in the odds of consuming fewer high-energy beverages.

Longitudinal predictors of less frequent consumption of high-energy foods

Baseline high-energy food intake frequency, sex and one intrapersonal variable remained significant predictors of less frequent high-energy food consumption at follow-up among disadvantaged adolescents (Table 4). Baseline high-energy food intake frequency strongly predicted high-energy food intake frequency at follow-up, i.e. adolescents who ate fewer high-energy foods at baseline had two times greater odds of eating fewer high-energy foods at follow-up when compared with adolescents who frequently ate high-energy foods at baseline. Girls had 57 % greater odds of eating fewer high-energy foods at follow-up when compared with boys. Adolescents who had not consumed fast food for breakfast in the month preceding the baseline survey had nearly three times greater odds of eating fewer high-energy foods at follow-up when compared with adolescents who ate fast food for breakfast more frequently.

Table 4.

Longitudinal predictors and odds ratios and 95 % confidence intervals of consuming fewer high-energy foods and beverages at follow-up among socio-economically disadvantaged Australian adolescents identified in multivariable logistic regression analysis (n 529)

Less frequent intake (%) Frequent intake (%) OR 95 % CI P value
Less frequent high-energy food intake at follow-up†,‡
n 208 321
Dietary factors
Baseline high-energy food intake frequency
Frequent intake at baseline 52 78 1·00 Ref.
Less frequent intake at baseline 48 22 2·02 1·42, 2·89 <0·001*
Sociodemographic characteristics
Sex
Boys 35 48 1·00 Ref.
Girls 65 52 1·57 1·19, 2·08 0·002*
Intrapersonal factors
Fast-food eating behaviours
Ate fast food for breakfast
Every day/most days/once/twice a week/once/twice a month 9 27 1·00 Ref.
Not in last month 91 73 2·80 1·52, 5·17 0·002*
Ate fast food for dinner at home
Every day/most days/once/twice a week 21 40 1·00 Ref.
Once/twice a month/not in last month 79 60 1·50 1·05, 2·16 0·03
School eating behaviours
Bought food/drink from school vending machines
Every day/most days/sometimes 9 19 1·00 Ref.
Hardly ever 16 15 1·69 0·90, 3·19 0·10
Never/no vending machine 75 66 1·82 1·09, 3·04 0·02
Self-efficacy for decreasing intakes of energy-dense food 1·09 1·02, 1·16 0·02
Mean 9·11 8·22
sd 2·14 2·49
Environmental factors
Home availability of high-energy food 0·93 0·86, 1·00 0·04
Mean 11·4 12·7
sd 2·69 3·01
Less frequent high-energy beverage intake at follow-up§,∥
n 345 184
Dietary factors
Baseline high-energy beverage intake frequency
Frequent intake at baseline 27 62 1·00 Ref.
Less frequent intake at baseline 73 38 3·15 2·19, 4·54 <0·001*
Sociodemographic characteristics
Sex
Boys 35 57 1·00 Ref.
Girls 65 43 2·18 1·44, 3·29 0·001*
Intrapersonal factors
Fast-food eating behaviours
Ate fast food for dinner at home
Every day/most days/once/twice a week 35 57 1·00 Ref.
Once/twice a month/not in last month 65 43 1·44 1·02, 2·05 0·04
Environmental factors
Home availability of high-energy food 0·88 0·83, 0·94 <0·001*
Mean 11·6 13·3
sd 2·76 3·00

Ref., reference category.

*P ≤ 0·01

†Less frequent intake defined as consumption of high-energy food meals ≤2 times/week at follow-up.

‡Model adjusted for covariate ‘sex’ and baseline high-energy food intake frequency.

§Less frequent intake defined as consumption of high-energy beverages ≤1 time/d at follow-up.

∥Model adjusted for covariate ‘sex’ and baseline high-energy beverage intake frequency.

Baseline high-energy beverage intake frequency, sex and one environmental factor predicted less frequent high-energy beverage consumption. Adolescents who consumed fewer high-energy beverages at baseline had more than three times greater odds of consuming fewer high-energy beverages at follow-up. Girls had more than twice the likelihood of consuming fewer high-energy beverages at follow-up when compared with boys. For each unit increase on the ‘home availability of high-energy food’ scale, disadvantaged adolescents’ odds of drinking fewer high-energy beverages decreased by 12 %.

Discussion

Some socio-economically disadvantaged adolescents managed to consume fewer high-energy foods and beverages. The present study identifies cross-sectional associations between intrapersonal, social and environmental factors and consumption of fewer high-energy foods and beverages; and longitudinal determinants of adolescents’ less frequent high-energy food consumption.

Past research supports the observation that some disadvantaged adolescents managed to consume fewer high-energy foods. Among disadvantaged American adolescents, 56 % of boys and 58 % of girls consumed fast-food meals two times or less in the week preceding the Project EAT follow-up survey( 22 ). These proportions are greater than observed in the present investigation, which may be explained by methodological differences between the two studies.

Proportions of disadvantaged (defined by family affluence) European adolescents who consumed high-energy beverages less than daily ranged from 50 % to 94 %; when parental occupation was used to define socio-economic position, proportions ranged from 47 % to 91 %( 38 ). Although the cut-off for less frequent high-energy beverage consumption (≤1 time/d) in our study was less stringent than that used by Vereecken et al. ( 38 ) (less than daily), proportions of disadvantaged adolescents who drank fewer high-energy beverages (61 % at baseline, 65 % at follow-up) were comparable to those reported in the European study. Findings from the present investigation support the observation that a proportion of disadvantaged adolescents manage to consume high-energy foods and beverages relatively less frequently.

The observation in the present investigation that disadvantaged adolescents’ consumption of high-energy foods decreased over time is unexpected, as it is at odds with findings that the quality of adolescents’ diets tends to decline as they mature( 12 14 , 39 ). It could be that messages about healthy eating are leading to secular improvements in Australian adolescents’ diets, as evidenced in the wider population( 40 , 41 ). For example, in 2008 fewer Australian adolescent girls consumed fast food at least once weekly (18·4 %) compared with 2003 (34·6 %)( 40 ). Secular changes in Australian adolescents’ diets require further investigation, particularly consumption of high-energy foods and beverages.

The present study showed that less frequent consumption of fast-food main meals, frequently being served vegetables with dinner and reduced home availability of high-energy foods were associated with less frequent high-energy food consumption among disadvantaged adolescents. Less frequent fast-food consumption for breakfasts and dinners at home or at fast-food restaurants was associated with consumption of fewer high-energy foods at both time points. Previously, adolescents’ regular fast-food meal consumption was shown to be positively associated with greater intakes of those foods( 42 ), resulting in poor diet( 8 , 9 , 43 ). Among disadvantaged adolescents, fat avoidance behaviours including eating bread, rolls or muffins without butter or margarine and replacement behaviours including ordering pasta or pizza without meat sauce or meat toppings significantly predicted low fat intakes( 18 ).

Lower home availability of high-energy foods predicted consumption of fewer high-energy foods and beverages. Similarly, frequently being served vegetables with dinner was associated with less frequent high-energy beverage consumption. Adolescents who consumed fewer high-energy foods may consume home-prepared meals more regularly, although this requires further investigation. Greater home availability of high-energy beverages was associated with increased consumption of such drinks( 44 , 45 ), and home availability of high-energy foods predicted US adolescent boys’ and girls’ higher fast-food intake( 12 , 22 ) and girls’ high-energy beverage intake( 46 ). Previously, disadvantaged adolescents were more likely to have greater home availability of high-energy beverages, while less often reporting having vegetables always served at dinner when compared with more advantaged adolescents( 29 ). These findings suggest that despite increasing autonomy and greater influences outside the home impacting on adolescents’ eating behaviours as adolescents mature, the home environment remains important for supporting healthy eating behaviours. Health promotion initiatives could provide education and assistance for disadvantaged adolescents and their families to develop budgeting and cooking skills related to purchase and preparation of quick, easy and nutritious meals (e.g. serving vegetables with dinner) in place of high-energy foods and beverages at home.

In the present investigation, baseline frequency of high-energy food and beverage intakes strongly predicted follow-up intake frequency, reflecting tracking of less frequent high-energy food consumption throughout adolescence. Kelder et al. found that adolescents who measured high on a given health behaviour still ranked highly for that measure 6 years later and those who ranked low remained low( 2 ), a pattern that has emerged in several other related studies( 47 49 ). These findings suggest that while nutrition promotion messages and strategies should be employed throughout adolescence to aid disadvantaged adolescents in avoiding high-energy foods and beverages, particular emphasis on such initiatives during early adolescence is warranted.

The present findings suggest that selected intrapersonal (particularly behavioural factors) and environmental (particularly within the home) factors may be beneficial foci of nutrition promotion initiatives aiming to improve disadvantaged adolescents’ diets. There also remain aspects of social factors that were not measured in the present investigation, e.g. increasing research about peer social networks. However, relatively few factors from across all domains, but particularly few environmental factors, strongly predicted disadvantaged adolescents’ less frequent high-energy food consumption. Different factors were cross-sectionally and longitudinally associated with less frequent consumption of high-energy foods, suggesting that different factors may be important for promoting healthy eating among younger and older adolescents, or over the shorter v. longer term.

Limitations of the present study should be acknowledged. Dietary intake data were based on a self-reported thirty-eight-item FFQ and serving sizes could not be determined. However, while FFQ may provide less detailed data than food diaries or repeated recall methodologies, research has shown that this methodology presents low burden to participants, and is appropriate for ranking participants’ intakes and examining associations with independent variables( 50 ). It is possible that some adolescents may have misunderstood the categories or descriptions of foods or beverages in the FFQ resulting in categorical bias; however, the questions and food categories have been used previously with participants of this age group in national surveys( 31 , 51 ). Also, additional types of high-energy foods and beverages consumed by adolescents may not have been represented in the FFQ resulting in an underestimation of the frequency of adolescents’ consumption of such foods. While BMI is a potentially important covariate that could be associated with dietary intake, height and weight data were not gathered from participants in the YEP Study, and therefore the effects of BMI could not be accounted for in the present investigation. Correlates were only assessed at baseline to increase participant response at follow-up. Income or household economic status was not assessed in the YEP Study, and while measures of paternal education and combined parental education were collected in the YEP Study, the majority (84%) of parental sociodemographic data were provided by mothers and therefore paternal education was only available for a small number of adolescents (n 263). Further, no significant associations between paternal education level and adolescent diet have been found in past research( 10 , 21 , 52 ).

The YEP Study response rate was not high, perhaps due to participant absenteeism on the days the surveys were conducted and the use of an active consent method, but none the less the sample was relatively sociodemographically diverse. Although there were some differences between adolescents with complete and missing data, these were generally small and existed only for six of thirty-eight variables. While some disadvantaged adolescents managed to consume fewer high-energy foods, achieving this as defined in the present study does not reflect achieving dietary recommendations. Finally, analyses could not be stratified by sex due to sample size constraints.

There are several strengths of the present investigation. Data were drawn from a large sample of sociodemographically diverse disadvantaged adolescents, and as the YEP sample incorporated two age cohorts, analyses included adolescents across a wide age range. A comprehensive model based on Social Ecological Theory was used to examine a range of factors associated with less frequent consumption. Factors supporting disadvantaged adolescents’ healthy eating may be more readily adopted by families living in similar contexts. Finally, the longitudinal design of the study allowed examination of temporally appropriate associations.

Based on our findings, health promotion programmes targeting disadvantaged adolescents and their families could focus on educating and assisting such families to develop budgeting and food preparation skills related to the preparation of quick, easy and nutritious meals at home in place of high-energy foods and beverages such as fast-food meals. Emphasis could also be placed on supporting healthy eating behaviours among boys and the suggested health promotion messages could be implemented during early adolescence.

Acknowledgements

Sources of funding: The study was funded by the Australian Research Council (ID: DP0452044) and the William Buckland Foundation. S.A.M. is supported by an Australian Research Council Future Fellowship (ID: 323519), D.C. is supported by a Victorian Health Promotion Foundation Senior Research Fellowship and K.B. is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council Senior Research Fellowship (ID: 479513). Conflicts of interest: The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare. Authors’ contributions: D.C. and K.B. designed the research; D.C. and K.B. conducted the research; L.D.S. analysed the data. All authors contributed to manuscript preparation and approved the manuscript. Acknowledgements: The authors thank Dr Nick Andrianopoulos for advice regarding statistical analyses.

Footnotes

*

Frucor Beverages Ltd, Auckland, New Zealand.

Red Bull GmbH, Fuschl am See, Salzburg, Austria.

Quaker Foods, a Division of Pepsico Beverages and Food, Purchase, NY, USA.

§

The Coca-Cola Company, Atlanta, GA, USA.

References

  • 1. World Health Organzation (2003) Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. Joint WHO/FAO Expert Consultation. WHO Technical Report Series no. 916. Geneva: WHO. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Kelder SH, Perry CL, Klepp KI et al. (1994) Longitudinal tracking of adolescent smoking, physical activity, and food choice behaviors. Am J Public Health 84, 1121–1126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Bertheke Post G, de Vente W, Kemper HC et al. (2001) Longitudinal trends in and tracking of energy and nutrient intake over 20 years in a Dutch cohort of men and women between 13 and 33 years of age: The Amsterdam Growth and Health Longitudinal Study. Br J Nutr 85, 375–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Gaziano JM (1998) When should heart disease prevention begin? N Engl J Med 338, 1690–1692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Lake AA, Mathers JC, Rugg-Gunn AJ et al. (2006) Longitudinal change in food habits between adolescence (11–12 years) and adulthood (32–33 years): The ASH30 Study. J Public Health (Oxf) 28, 10–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Meneton P, Lafay L, Tard A et al. (2009) Dietary sources and correlates of sodium and potassium intakes in the French general population. Eur J Clin Nutr 63, 1169–1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Dwyer JT, Evans M, Stone EJ et al. (2001) Adolescents’ eating patterns influence their nutrient intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 101, 798–802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Larson NI, Story M, Wall M et al. (2006) Calcium and dairy intakes of adolescents are associated with their home environment, taste preferences, personal health beliefs, and meal patterns. J Am Diet Assoc 106, 1816–1824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Bowman SA, Gortmaker SL, Ebbeling CB et al. (2004) Effects of fast-food consumption on energy intake and diet quality among children in a national household survey. Pediatrics 113, 112–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Shi Z, Lien N, Kumar BN et al. (2005) Socio-demographic differences in food habits and preferences of school adolescents in Jiangsu Province, China. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, 1439–1448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Rangan AM, Randall D, Hector DJ et al. (2008) Consumption of ‘extra’ foods by Australian children: types, quantities and contribution to energy and nutrient intakes. Eur J Clin Nutr 62, 356–364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Larson NI, Neumark-Sztainer DR, Story MT et al. (2008) Fast food intake: longitudinal trends during the transition to young adulthood and correlates of intake. J Adolesc Health 43, 79–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Nelson MC, Neumark-Sztainer D, Hannan PJ et al. (2009) Five-year longitudinal and secular shifts in adolescent beverage intake: findings from project EAT (Eating Among Teens)-II. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 308–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Pearson N, MacFarlane A, Crawford D et al. (2009) Family circumstance and adolescent dietary behaviours. Appetite 52, 668–674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Arcan C, Kubik MY, Fulkerson JA et al. (2009) Sociodemographic differences in selected eating practices among alternative high school students. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 823–829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Magarey A, Daniels LA & Smith A (2001) Fruit and vegetable intakes of Australians aged 2–18 years: an evaluation of the 1995 National Nutrition Survey data. Aust N Z J Public Health 25, 155–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Di Noia J, Schinke SP, Prochaska JO et al. (2006) Application of the transtheoretical model to fruit and vegetable consumption among economically disadvantaged African-American adolescents: preliminary findings. Am J Health Promot 20, 342–348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Di Noia J, Contento IR & Schinke SP (2008) Fat avoidance and replacement behaviors predict low-fat intake among urban African American adolescents. Nutr Res 28, 358–363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Bronfenbrenner U (1979) The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Bandura A (1986) Social Foundations of Thoughts and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Rasmussen M, Krolner R, Klepp KI et al. (2006) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part I: Quantitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Bauer KW, Larson NI, Nelson MC et al. (2009) Socio-environmental, personal and behavioural predictors of fast-food intake among adolescents. Public Health Nutr 12, 1767–1774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Wouters EJ, Larsen JK, Kremers SP et al. (2010) Peer influence on snacking behavior in adolescence. Appetite 55, 11–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Cutler GJ, Flood A, Hannan P et al. (2011) Multiple sociodemographic and socioenvironmental characteristics are correlated with major patterns of dietary intake in adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 230–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Pearson N, Biddle SJ & Gorely T (2009) Family correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr 12, 267–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. van der Horst K, Timperio A, Crawford D et al. (2008) The school food environment associations with adolescent soft drink and snack consumption. Am J Prev Med 35, 217–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Laska MN, Hearst MO, Forsyth A et al. (2010) Neighbourhood food environments: are they associated with adolescent dietary intake, food purchases and weight status? Public Health Nutr 13, 1757–1763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Stephens LD, McNaughton SA, Crawford D et al. (2011) Correlates of dietary resilience among socioeconomically disadvantaged adolescents. Eur J Clin Nutr 65, 1219–1232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. MacFarlane A, Crawford D, Ball K et al. (2007) Adolescent home food environments and socio-economic position. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 16, 748–756. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Marks GC, Webb K, Rutishauser IHE et al. (2001) Monitoring Food Habits in the Australian Population Using Short Questions. Canberra: Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Australian Bureau of Statistics (1998) National Nutrition Survey Users’ Guide. Catalogue no. 4801.0. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. The Cancer Council of Victoria (2009) Dietary Questionnaire for Epidemiological Studies (DQES v2) User Information Guide 2009. Carlton: Cancer Epidemiology Centre, Nutritional Assessment Office, Cancer Council Victoria. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Di Noia J & Contento IR (2009) Use of a brief food frequency questionnaire for estimating daily number of servings of fruits and vegetables in a minority adolescent population. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 1785–1789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Kvaavik E, Batty GD, Ursin G et al. (2010) Influence of individual and combined health behaviors on total and cause-specific mortality in men and women: the United Kingdom health and lifestyle survey. Arch Intern Med 170, 711–718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Prentice AM & Jebb SA (2003) Fast foods, energy density and obesity: a possible mechanistic link. Obes Rev 4, 187–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Food Standards Australia New Zealand (2007) AUSNUT (Australian Food and Nutrient Database) 2007. AUSNUT 2007 Microsoft Excel files. http://www.foodstandards.gov.au/consumerinformation/ausnut2007/ausnut2007microsofte4060.cfm (accessed August 2012).
  • 37. O'Rourke N, Hatcher L & Stepanski E (2005) A Step-by-Step Approach to Using SAS for Univariate and Multivariate Statistics. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Vereecken CA, Inchley J, Subramanian SV et al. (2005) The relative influence of individual and contextual socio-economic status on consumption of fruit and soft drinks among adolescents in Europe. Eur J Public Health 15, 224–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Bauer KW, Larson NI, Nelson MC et al. (2009) Fast food intake among adolescents: secular and longitudinal trends from 1999 to 2004. Prev Med 48, 284–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Martin K, Rosenberg M, Miller M et al. (2009) Child and Adolescent Physical Activity and Nutrition Survey 2008: Key Findings. Perth: Western Australian Government. [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Rangan AM, Kwan JS, Louie JC et al. (2011) Changes in core food intake among Australian children between 1995 and 2007. Eur J Clin Nutr 65, 1201–1210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Boutelle KN, Fulkerson JA, Neumark-Sztainer D et al. (2007) Fast food for family meals: relationships with parent and adolescent food intake, home food availability and weight status. Public Health Nutr 10, 16–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Cusatis DC & Shannon BM (1996) Influences on adolescent eating behavior. J Adolesc Health 18, 27–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Haerens L, Craeynest M, Deforche B et al. (2008) The contribution of psychosocial and home environmental factors in explaining eating behaviours in adolescents. Eur J Clin Nutr 62, 51–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Denney-Wilson E, Crawford D, Dobbins T et al. (2009) Influences on consumption of soft drinks and fast foods in adolescents. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 18, 447–452. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Bauer KW, Neumark-Sztainer D, Fulkerson JA et al. (2011) Familial correlates of adolescent girls’ physical activity, television use, dietary intake, weight, and body composition. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 8, 25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Krebs-Smith SM, Heimendinger J, Patterson BH et al. (1995) Psychosocial factors associated with fruit and vegetable consumption. Am J Health Promot 10, 98–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Lien N, Jacobs DR Jr & Klepp KI (2002) Exploring predictors of eating behaviour among adolescents by gender and socio-economic status. Public Health Nutr 5, 671–681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Lake AA, Rugg-Gunn AJ, Hyland RM et al. (2004) Longitudinal dietary change from adolescence to adulthood: perceptions, attributions and evidence. Appetite 42, 255–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Vereecken CA, Covents M, Matthys C et al. (2005) Young Adolescents’ Nutrition Assessment on Computer (YANA-C). Eur J Clin Nutr 59, 658–667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Hardy LL, King L, Espinel P et al. (2011) Methods of the NSW Schools Physical Activity and Nutrition Survey 2010 (SPANS 2010). J Sci Med Sport 14, 390–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Von Post-Skagegard M, Samuelson G, Karlstrom B et al. (2002) Changes in food habits in healthy Swedish adolescents during the transition from adolescence to adulthood. Eur J Clin Nutr 56, 532–538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Cleland V, Worsley A & Crawford D (2004) What are grade 5 and 6 children buying from school canteens and what do parents and teachers think about it? Nutr Diet 61, 145–150. [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Kremers SP, Brug J, de Vries H et al. (2003) Parenting style and adolescent fruit consumption. Appetite 41, 43–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Neumark-Sztainer D, Wall M, Perry C et al. (2003) Correlates of fruit and vegetable intake among adolescents. Findings from Project EAT. Prev Med 37, 198–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Sallis JF, Grossman RM, Pinksi RB et al. (1987) The development of scales to measure social support for diet and exercise behaviours. Prev Med 16, 825–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Fulkerson JA, Neumark-Sztainer D & Story M (2006) Adolescent and parent views of family meals. J Am Diet Assoc 106, 526–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Campbell KJ, Crawford DA, Salmon J et al. (2007) Associations between the home food environment and obesity-promoting eating behaviors in adolescence. Obesity (Silver Spring) 15, 719–730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Public Health Nutrition are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES