Abstract
Primate sanctuaries provide a solution for the increasing number of primates being taken from their home countries to support the demands of the illegal pet trade. To help end the primate trade and raise awareness about the risks this trade poses to delicate ecosystems, sanctuaries are increasingly developing conservation education programs. Education and raising awareness must be one of the primary roles of primate sanctuaries. However, there are few evaluations of the impacts of conservation education programs for school children published in scientific literature. To address this gap, we conducted an evaluation of educator-led visits of school children at Fundació Mona, a primate sanctuary located in Catalunya, Spain. Questionnaires for an experimental and control group were conducted with 3,205 school children, ages 8 to 18 from 83 different schools, to evaluate changes in their attitudes and knowledge of primate welfare and conservation. We found that Fundació Mona’s program of environmental activities had a positive impact on children, both female and male students, in terms of attitudes and knowledge of primate welfare and conservation. Although female students gave better responses regarding welfare and conservation, all children showed gains in pro-conservation responses. This study demonstrates that environmental education activities focused on children can help shape a change in knowledge and attitudes toward primate welfare and conservation. Educator-led visits of school children to primate sanctuaries such as Fundació Mona can also serve to amplify biodiversity conservation messages among children and their families. We encourage primate sanctuaries to promote empirical studies of attitudes and knowledge of primate welfare and conservation and to conduct systematical evaluations to strengthen their educational activities.
Keywords: Conservation, Education, School children, Assessment, Primates, Sanctuary, Attitudes, Knowledge, Wildlife trade, Chimpanzees
Introduction
The primary goal of primate sanctuaries is to provide a safe and nurturing environment for primates who have been rescued from the illegal pet trade (Farmer, 2002; Ferrie et al., 2014), research laboratories (Lopresti-Goodman, Bezner & Ritter, 2015; Fultz, 2017), or other harmful situations (Feliu et al., 2022). Many sanctuaries are developing and carrying out conservation education programs to support efforts not only to end the illegal primate trade but also to raise awareness regarding the risks this trade poses to primates’ endemic ecosystems (Kuhar et al., 2012; Hansen et al., 2016) and promoting attitudes in society that favor primate conservation (Brent, 2001; Farmer, Buchanan-Smith & Jamart, 2006; Beck et al., 2007; André et al., 2008; Kuhar et al., 2012).
The main goal of most environmental education programs is to change participants’ attitudes to more sustainable and environmentally friendly behavior (Liefländer & Bogner, 2014; Jacobson, McDuff & Monroe, 2015; Esson & Moss, 2016). To achieve this, sanctuaries can adopt non-formal educational programs with school children fostering values of respect for animals and nature. For this reason, primate sanctuaries provide unique opportunities for children to learn about and connect with the primate world, and to develop an understanding of the importance of protecting them and preserving their habitats. Therefore, primate sanctuaries are key educational centers for environmental education programs and provide a platform for raising awareness of future generations and reinforcing environmental childhood education in the short, medium, and long term (André et al., 2008; Kuhar et al., 2012; Bowie et al., 2020).
Primates living at a sanctuary have names and usually tragic stories. Children engage emotionally with them, as they learn about their life histories. Their stories, though unfortunate, can promote a sense of connection with the rescued primates (Skibins & Powell, 2013) and as flagship species, the knowledge about their plight can contribute to the conservation of other taxa (Wich & Marshall, 2016) and increase positive attitudes toward conservation of the species and their habitat (Lukas & Ross, 2005). These types of positive experiences with nature and animals during childhood promote children’s commitment to protecting the environment when they become adults (Wells & Lekies, 2006; Chawla & Cushing, 2007). The knowledge and respect for an endangered species that children acquire when visiting a sanctuary can be also transmitted to adults (Rakotomamonjy et al., 2015). It is critical that young generations acquire respect for wildlife conservation as the future of many species will be in their hands (Schuttler et al., 2019).
Despite their important role in primate conservation and raising awareness, many primate sanctuaries tend to not have the necessary human or financial resources to run and assess conservation education programs for school children mainly due to the lack of funds. The educational and outreach value of sanctuaries has been poorly studied (Falk et al., 2007; Kuhar et al., 2010; Lukas et al., 2017). André et al. (2008) and Bowie et al. (2020) found four hundred Congolese children successfully acquired key knowledge about conservation after a visit to a primate sanctuary and that the information was retained after a second visit a year later. Kuhar et al. (2010) published data from the educational programs of other five sanctuaries in Africa showing stronger pro-conservation attitudes toward primates after visiting primate sanctuaries.
The zoo community, however, has a history of experience related to conducting and evaluating educational programs. Many studies have been carried out to assess the increase in knowledge and change in attitudes after visiting zoos (Ogden et al., 2004). Children after visiting zoos increase in learning outcomes (Randler, Kummer & Wilhelm, 2012; Jensen, 2014), in conservation-related knowledge and attitudes towards conservation (Moss, Jensen & Gusset, 2017b). Educational assessment programs have also been carried out at natural parks and biosphere reserves showing an increase in knowledge and positive attitudes toward the environment after completing the program (Kuhar et al., 2007; Rakotomamonjy et al., 2015). These findings are also supported by other studies done at sanctuaries (André et al., 2008; Kuhar et al., 2010; Kuhar et al., 2012; Grúňová et al., 2017; Bowie et al., 2020).
In order for environmental education programs in sanctuaries to evaluate their impacts, both within the framework of well-being and conservation, and improve their applicability in future initiatives, it is necessary to generate evidence from studies that provide solid and systematic data and have a significant presence at local and regional level in school children in the territory. The main goal of our study was to assess whether school children’s participation in an educator-led visit to the Fundació Mona Primate Sanctuary increased biodiversity conservation knowledge and fostered positive attitudes toward primate welfare and conservation. We also evaluated whether the grade level and gender of the participating children impacted the outcomes. To achieve our goals, we did two different studies. In Study 1, we used a questionnaire to assess children’s knowledge regarding primate species, their welfare and conservation after going through Environmental educational activities (EEA) at the sanctuary. We predicted that children going through EEA across grade levels of both genders will increase accuracy compared to the ones that did not participate in the EEA. We also predicted that gender and grade level of the participants will influence the responses obtained after going through the EEA. In Study 2 we used a questionnaire to assess children’s attitudes toward primate welfare and conservation. We predicted that children across grade levels of both genders will increase their pro-conservation responses after going through the EEA. We have included the variables of sex and grade since previous studies have found differences in the responses for these variables (Borchers et al., 2014).
Due to the shortage of educational studies at primate sanctuaries, the results of this study are important in encouraging other primate sanctuaries to develop and evaluate educational programs for school children. The results can also support the primate community, as program evaluation is important to developing best practices in environmental and conservation education (Jacobson, 1987; Bennett, 1989; Thomas, 2016).
Materials and Methods
We develop two studies with 3,205 school children between 8 and 18 years old to assess the knowledge, welfare and conservation of primate species and the attitudes toward primate welfare and conservation. The research, conducted with school children, was approved by the Board of Trustees of the Fundació Mona and the Bioethics Committee of the University of Barcelona.
Study site
The Fundació Mona (Mona) sanctuary is a primate rescue center located in the province of Catalunya, in the Northeast of Spain. It was founded in 2001 to provide a shelter for pet and entertainment primates smuggled into Spain from Africa. The primary goal of the Mona sanctuary during this time was to provide lifelong care for rescued chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and other primates, as well as to ensure a species-appropiate environment where they can live out the rest of their lives. The Mona sanctuary houses two groups of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in an open environment of 5,640 square meters. It also houses a group of Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvana).
Fundació Mona began its education program for schools three years after its first chimpanzee rescue. In 2004 the sanctuary created an education department to develop educational activities for children ages 3 through 18. Since 2004, over 18,778 school children have participated in educational activities at the Mona sanctuary. In 2014 the education department developed the MonaEduca program, a new educational project dedicated to raising awareness of the dangers faced by primates around the world.
The MonaEduca program
In addition to raising awareness, the MonaEduca program aims to provide education related to primate welfare and conservation and to transmit the core values of respect for wildlife and for nature. The program aims to empower children to become agents of change for sustainability and to foster a society that is more respectful towards the environment. MonaEduca educational materials are designed to increase knowledge about primate species. Specifically, they educate about conservation issues in the wild, the illegal pet trade and the entertainment industry, and the role of sanctuaries in providing conservation support, and in rescuing, rehabilitating, and housing primates that have become victims of human activities. The program is carried out through a single school visit to the sanctuary that lasts approximately four hours. The curriculum targets all ages within primary, secondary, and high schools (8 to 18 years old). Educators use age-specific content for each audience that is delivered to groups with a maximum of 25 participants. The activities are always carried out by a guide/educator who has significant training in primatology and conservation. During the MonaEduca Program all children participate in three different environmental education activities (EEA) (Supplementary Material 1).
1. An indoor, introductory, dynamic, and participatory talk, where school children learn the objectives and roles of the sanctuary, how to behave in the presence of the rescued primates and information related to taxonomy, distribution, natural history, behavior, welfare and conservation.
2. An outdoor, educator-led visit where children observe the primates and learn about the story of each of the rescued chimpanzees and macaques.
3. A learning-through-play activity adapted to different age groups. The concept is that the children will participate in playing the role of every player involved in a conservation scenario. For example, they can play the role of the director of a palm oil company, a conservationist or a man from the local community working for a palm oil company because this is his only source of income which is essential to feed his family. This provides the school children participating in the MonaEduca activities with the knowledge and experience to protect the interests of every person involved in the conservation scenario.
Study 1: knowledge assessment
Knowledge assessment methods
In this study we developed and administered a questionnaire to obtain information about children’s knowledge about primate species, their welfare and conservation to assess whether school children participating in the EEA learned about primate species and primate welfare and conservation through a questionnaire with eight closed-format questions related to natural history, behavior, primate welfare and conservation (Fig. 1). The content of each question was presented during the educator-led visit. In each of the questions, children had to choose one option from among two, three, or four possible answers. Questions 2, 5, 6 and 7 addressed general knowledge about chimpanzee species, and questions 1, 3, 4 and 8 captured the participants’ knowledge about chimpanzee welfare and conservation. A total of 1,549 school children participate in this study. In terms of educational levels, 40% were in primary school, 57% were in secondary school and 3% were in high school.
After arriving at the Mona’s educational area, children were randomly divided into two groups: the control (CO) group and the experimental (EX) group. The control group completed the questionnaire without participating in the EEA, and the experimental group completed the questionnaire after participating in the EEA at the sanctuary. This study had a between-subjects design, so each participant made up one of the two conditions (control or experimental). This ensured that participants’ responses were not affected by being exposed to the same question twice (Clayton, 2017; Bowie et al., 2020). The questionnaire included a preliminary section on demographic details including gender, age, grade, and school name (Table 1). All responses were treated anonymously. The teachers who accompanied the school children who participated in the survey were informed of the goals of the survey before the children answered the questions. Children answered the questionnaire independently via a google survey using tablets on site. Six children at a time were able to answer the questionnaire, as we had six tablets placed at the entrance of the educational room. To measure the internal consistency of the questionnaire we used a Kuder-Richardson Formula 20 (KR-20) (Kuder-Richardson 20 Formula, 2014). The KR-20 for the knowledge questionnaire was 0.7003.
Table 1. Descriptive information of the participants for Study 1.
Sample Size |
Mean Std.err |
Std dev | Mean age std.err |
M/F ratio | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Study 1: Knowledge | |||||
Control | 752 | 0.52 ± 0.01 | 0.18 | 11.63 ± 0.08 | 1.08 |
Experimental | 797 | 0.75 ± 0.01 | 0.15 | 11.87 ± 0.08 | 0.88 |
For the overall analysis of the questionnaire, a score (questionnaire score) was computed based on the proportion of correct responses. The number of correct responses divided by the total number of responses (correct and non-correct responses) was calculated. Additionally, and following the questionnaire score method, two global scores were extracted, one for the questions related to the knowledge of the species category (questions 2, 5, 6 and 7) and one for the questions related to the knowledge of conservation and welfare category (questions 1, 3, 4 and 8). In a further analysis, these two categories were compared globally, by gender and EEA.
To know the effects of the predictors (EEA, gender, and grade) in the questionnaire score, we used a linear model (Baayen, 2008). Standard linear Models were run in R version 1.2.533 (R Core Team, 2020) by lmer function (Bates et al., 2014). We generated various models and selected the model in which the predictors offered the most parsimonious combination through the corrected Akaike information criterion (AICc) (‘aictab’ function) (Burnham & Anderson, 2002). We checked whether our models fit the criteria of normal distribution and homogeneous residuals by visual explorations of histograms and qqplot of the residuals as well as residuals plotted against fitted values. We checked the significance of the predictors at the global level by contrasting the full model and the null model, excluding all predictors (Dobson & Barnett, 2008; R Core Team, 2020). Finally, we used a Chi-squared test to evaluate the effect of the EEA for each one of the items of the questionnaires. In this way we compared the total volume of correct responses of each of the questions between control and experimental groups. Descriptive statistics (mean, median, standard deviation, maximum, minimum) for each item question, (control and experimental groups), were also calculated. In addition, the demographic variable age in the sample of children from experimental group and control group were compared using a t-test.
Knowledge assessment results
A total of 1,549 responses were recorded during the data collection campaign. Of these, 752 (48.54%) corresponded to the control group and 797 (51.46%) corresponded to the experimental group. The largest volume of participants came from secondary schools, aged 12 to 16 years (n = 890), and from primary schools (n = 609), aged 8 to 12 years. High school students, ranging in age from 17 to 18 years (n = 50), participated less frequently (Table 1). In addition, the frequency of male students and female students was similar, 49% and 51% respectively.
Overall impact of the EEA on knowledge assessment
The best fit model (AIC = −1197.88) included the predictors (1) EEA, (2) grade, (3) and gender, (Table 2). The best fit model compared to the null model was significantly better at predicting the score of correct responses (χ2 = 203.08, df = 4, P < 0.001).
Table 2. Model selection for Study 1.
Model | EEA | GRADE | SEX | AICc | ΔAICc | Weight |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Full Model | X | X | X | −1197.88 | 0 | 1 |
Model 2 | X | −1180.12 | 17.76 | 0 | ||
Model 1 | X | X | −1178.60 | 19.28 | 0 | |
Null Model | −551.51 | 646.38 | 0 |
Notes.
ΔAICc, Akaike’s information criteria.
EEA, environmental education activities.
The generated model showed a significant effect of the predictor EEA in the proportion of correct responses (F = 788.520; df = 1; p < 0.010). In this sense, participants produced a significantly higher volume of correct responses in the experimental groups (Fig. 2). The model revealed significant differences in the grade predictor (F = 11.71; df = 2; p < 0.001). According to the post-hoc, primary school students (mean = 0.613; sd = 0.006) obtained a significantly lower number of correct responses than did secondary school students (mean = 0.654; sd = 0.005), (t = −4.738; df = 1; p < 0.000). On the other hand, gender was a non-significant predictor variable. (F = 0.7721; df = 1; p < 0.533) (See Table S1).
Impact of EEA on question responses
In general, the students chose more correct responses in the experimental group. Only in question 7 the experimental group showed a decrease in the percentage of correct responses (Fig. 3). Educational activities showed the greatest impact in questions 6 and 8. For question 6, the volume of correct responses in the experimental group was 50% higher than in the control group. (χ2 = 296,242; p < 0.010). This difference was 44% for questions 8 (χ2 = 300.456; p < 0.010) (Table 3).
Table 3. Value of the contrast test for each of the questions between the control (CO) and the experimental (EX) groups related to knowledge assessment.
Questions | N | M_CO ±SD | M_EX ±SD | χ 2x | P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. If you can have a chimpanzee at home since being a baby, do you think it could be a good pet? | 1,538 | 0,47 ± 0,50 | 0,86 ± 0,34 | 273,037 | 0.000 |
2. The chimpanzee is A solitary/social animal/ He lives in groups/on his own | 1,531 | 0,92 ± 0,27 | 0,97 ± 0,15 | 19,636 | 0.000 |
3. Do you think chimpanzees are good for TV commercials? | 1,538 | 0,80 ± 0,40 | 0,95 ± 0,20 | 86,724 | 0.000 |
4. Does training animals like chimpanzees to participate in movies or commercials hurt them? | 1,533 | 0,56 ± 0,50 | 0,75 ± 0,43 | 56,033 | 0.000 |
5. Chimpanzees are endangered/ vulnerable/not threatened | 1,529 | 0,45 ± 0,50 | 0,70 ± 0,46 | 99,393 | 0.000 |
6. How many years a chimpanzee can live in captivity? | 1,518 | 0,37 ± 0,48 | 0,87 ± 0,40 | 296,242 | 0.000 |
7. How much can an adult male chimpanzee weigh? | 1,527 | 0,31 ± 0,46 | 0,25 ± 0,43 | 8,599 | 0.003 |
8. A primate rescue center like the Mona sanctuary: (a) Rescues and socializes primates that come from circuses, TV commercials and the pet trade. (b) Heals the chimpanzees and then takes them back to their habitat. (c) Heals primates injured in the jungle. (d) All of these are correct |
1,530 | 0,31 ± 0,46 | 0,75 ± 0,43 | 300,456 | 0.000 |
Notes.
- CO
- Control
- EX
- Experimental
Impact of EEA on knowledge by categories
If we cluster the analysis of the questions according to the categories: knowledge of the species and knowledge of primate welfare and conservation, the LMM shows that all participants obtained more correct responses on the questions related to the knowledge of welfare and conservation than on those related to knowledge of species (Category; F = 135.3642; df = 1; p < 0.010). In terms of gender, female students scored more correct responses on items related to the knowledge of welfare and conservation compared to male students (Category × gender; F = 11.007; df = 1; p < 0.010) (Fig. 4). No significant gender differences were found for the cluster of questions related to knowledge of the species (See Table S2).
Study 1. Knowledge assessment discussion
Overall, in this study, children in the experimental group responded more correct responses than did those in the control group, which suggests that the content provided in the educator-led visits, as well as the experience of engaging in a visit of close proximity to primates, resulted in children increasing their knowledge about primate species, their welfare and conservation. Question number 7, “how much an adult male chimpanzee can weigh?”, was the only question that had less correct responses in the experimental group compared to the control group. There could be various explanations for this result. First, the response options were not very clear as the thresholds for chimpanzee weights were very similar, thus the question could have been difficult. Another explanation could be that when educators talk about the dimorphism of the species, they talk about the weight of males and females, and perhaps participants do not retain the information because either it is confusing, or they do not consider it relevant. Given these results, this question could be adapted for future questionnaires, because the most important fact for children to retain is that chimpanzees are heavy wild animals which does not make them good pets.
In terms of grade level, secondary school children chose more correct answers on the knowledge questionnaires than primary school children. These results agree with studies carried out by Borchers et al. (2013), in which differences were observed in terms of academic training in students in the fifth and sixth grades. Burnett et al. (2016) also found that children in higher grades had better scores. These results could be related to the fact that high school students have more learning experiences (Borchers et al., 2013) and may be more familiar with environmental education concepts and terms than participants in earlier grades. High school students likely gain more knowledge about educational action than younger age groups (Lawson, 1983). Another explanation could be related to the type of questionnaire used. For instance, unlike the attitude questionnaire (visual type), the knowledge questionnaire utilized written questions and answers. Thus, none of the questions and answers were adapted to grade level. All participants, regardless of grade level and background, received the same questionnaire. As educators did not help children when answering the questions, it is possible that students in lower grade levels had more difficulty understanding some of the terms and questions in the questionnaire than higher grade level students.
Questions included in this knowledge assessment fell into two categories: knowledge of the species (questions 2,5,6 and 7) and welfare and conservation of chimpanzees (questions 1,3,4 and 8). When we analyzed the questions according to the categories, we observed important differences. Participants obtained better scores in the “conservation category” than in the “knowledge of the species category”. One explanation could be related to the type of visit and the predisposition of the educators, who might be more focused on conservation than on general knowledge of the species. Similarly, it is possible that participants had more general learning experience related to conservation than related to chimpanzee-specific issues, and therefore performed better in the conservation category. Additionally, this study showed an interaction between gender and the welfare and conservation category. Female students scored better than male students in this category, although no significant gender differences were found for the cluster of questions related to knowledge of the species. This finding is consistent with other studies (Bogner & Wiseman, 2004; Wiseman, Wilson & Bogner, 2012), which suggest that female students show stronger pro-conservation attitudes than male students.
Study 2: Attitude assessment
Attitude assessment methods
In this study, we assessed participants’ conservation attitudes toward primate welfare and conservation. We used a questionnaire to assess participants’ choice for pro-conservation messages over non-conservation messages. The questionnaire consisted of eight questions, each with two possible responses. Photos rather than text were used to represent each answer: one pro-conservation option and one non-conservation option (Fig. 5).
This attitude assessment method was based on a questionnaire developed by Bowie et al. (2020) in order to evaluate conservation attitudes among students participating in the education program at a primate sanctuary in the Democratic Republic of Congo. Some of the survey questions referred to the choice between a bonobo as a pet and a bonobo in the wild. The novelty of the questionnaire was that questions asked participants to choose ideas to design publicity to attract more visitors to the sanctuary hoping to have more unconscious ideologies when answering the questions. In conventional attitude assessments, participants often respond with answers they think are correct instead of providing answers that accurately convey their actual beliefs (Falk et al., 2007). For our study, we eliminated 4 out of the 12 original questions to concentrate the participants attention on chimpanzees that have been poached, trafficked, and/or are living in inadequate situations in Europe. Specifically, questions focused on topics like the presence of soldiers in the streets, bonobos being sold as bushmeat and life conditions in African countries. We included a preliminary section on demographic details including gender, age, grade, and school name (Table 4). A total of 1656 schoolchildren participate in this study. In terms of educational levels, 48% were in primary school, 43% were in secondary school and 9% were in high school.
Table 4. Descriptive information of the participants for Study 2.
Sample Size |
Mean Std.err |
Std dev | Mean age std.err |
M/F ratio | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Study 2: Attitude | |||||
Control | 822 | 0.72 ± 0.01 | 0.18 | 11.61 ± 0.09 | 0.92 |
Experimental | 834 | 0.86 ± 0.01 | 0.15 | 11.59 ± 0.09 | 0.82 |
For the attitude questionnaire, we proceeded using the same methodology as in Bowie et al. (2020) as we have used a similar questionnaire as the one used by the author. We calculated the total number of pro-conservation messages divided by the total number of messages (pro-conservation and non-conservation). In all the cases, continuous scores from 0 to 1 were obtained for each questionnaire and participant. To know the effects of the predictors (EEA, gender, and grade) on the questionnaire score, we used the same models as those used in the knowledge study. In addition, the demographic variable age in the sample of children from experimental group and control group were compared using a t-test. As in the knowledge questionnaire, we measured the internal consistency of the attitude questionnaire with a Kuder-Richardson Formula 20 (KR-20) (Kuder-Richardson Formula, 2014). The KR-20 for the attitude questionnaire was 0.711.
Attitude assessment results
A total of 1,656 responses were recorded during the data collection. Of these, 822 (49.63%) corresponded to the control group and 834 (50.36%) corresponded to the experimental group. The largest volume of participants came from primary schools, aged 8 to 12 years (n = 798) and secondary schools, aged 12 to 16 years (n = 712). High school students aged 17 to 18 years (n = 146) participated less frequently (Table 4). Additionally, 53.51% of the responses were from female students (n = 886) and 46.49% were from male students (n = 770). There were no significant differences (t-test = 0.1731, df = 1654, p-value = 0.8626) in the age variable of the sampled children exposed to or not exposed to environmental education.
Overall impact of the EEA on school children’s attitudes
The best fit model (AIC = −1308.61) included the predictors (1) EEA, (2) and gender, (Table 5). The best fit model compared to the null model was significantly better at predicting the score of correct responses (χ2 = 191.93, df = −2, P < 0.001).
Table 5. Model selection for the attitude questionnaire.
Model | EEA | GRADE | SEX | AICc | ΔAICc | Weight |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | X | X | −1308.61 | 0 | 0.85 | |
Full Model | X | X | X | −1305.06 | 3.85 | 0.15 |
Model 2 | X | −1275.03 | 33.58 | 0 | ||
Null Model | −966.93 | 341.68 | 0 |
Notes.
- ΔAICc
- Akaike’s information criteria
- EEA
- environmental education
The model revealed a statistically significant effect of the EEA predictor (EEA; F = 347.2837; df = 1; p < 0.001) and gender predictor (gender; F = 34.2497; df = 1; p < 0.001) on the proportion of pro-conservation responses. Our results showed that pro-conservation attitudes were influenced by EEA and by gender. Participants produced a significantly greater proportion of pro-conservation responses in the experimental (Mean = 0.86, std. err = 0.006) group than in the control group (Mean = 0.72, std. err = 0.006). In terms of gender, female students (Mean = 0.815, std. err = 0.005) showed a greater proportion of pro-conservation responses than male students (Mean = 0.767, std. err = 0.006).
Impact of EEA on attitude questions responses
Figure 6 shows the percentages of choices of pro-conservation messages for each of the questions. Except for question 4, in which participants showed a similar percentage of responses between the control and the experimental group, participants had a significantly higher proportion of pro-conservation responses in the experimental group. Mona’s EEA produced a significant positive change in pro-conservation messages in seven of the eight questions surveyed. EEA activities showed the greatest impact in questions 6 and 7. For question 6, the proportion of pro-conservation messages increased by 33% in the experimental group. Responses to question 7 the pro-conservation messages were 38% higher in the experimental group than in the control groups (Table 6; Fig. 6).
Table 6. Value of the contrast test for each of the questions between control (CO) and experimental groups (EX) for the attitude questionnaire.
Question | N | M_Control (CO) (SD) |
M_Experimental(EX) (SD) |
X2 | P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Which group do you think chimpanzees belong to? | 1,637 | 0,89 ± 0,32 | 0,93 ± 0,25 | 10,910 | 0.001 |
2. Which of these photos do you prefer to see in a Mona sanctuary ad? | 1,642 | 0,70 ± 0,46 | 0,85 ± 0,36 | 53,736 | 0.000 |
3. Which group do you think chimpanzees belong to? | 1,644 | 0,62 ± 0,48 | 0,75 | 31,972 | 0.000 |
4. Which photo best shows the value of the forest? | 1,640 | 0,97 ± 0,16 | 0,98 ± 0,15 | 0,272 | 0.602 |
5. Which group do you think chimpanzees belong to? | 1,639 | 0,83 ± 0,38 | 0,89 | 15,059 | 0.000 |
6. Which of these photos do you prefer to see in a Mona sanctuary ad? | 1,644 | 0,54 ± 0,50 | 0,87 ± 0,33 | 208,268 | 0.000 |
7. Which of these two situations do you prefer to be in? | 1,627 | 0,40 ± 0,49 | 0,78 ± 0,41 | 239,383 | 0.000 |
8. How do you like to see this chimpanzee? | 1,644 | 0,83 ± 0,37 | 0,94 ± 0,24 | 45,418 | 0.000 |
Study 2. Attitude assessment discussion
Overall, participants in the experimental group were significantly more likely to choose the pro-conservation responses than in the control group. These findings suggest that the MonaEduca program seems to be well-oriented in promoting pro-conservation behavior and positive attitudes in children, thus we believe that it is fulfilling a very clear objective as a precursor to these changes. Although the literacy levels among our participants were similar, we believed that using pictures as choice options instead of written response options would facilitate the predisposition to answer the questionnaires (Bowie et al., 2020). On another level, providing electronic devices such as tablets to answer the questionnaires encourages children to participate.
One of the key messages of the MonaEduca program is that primates face threats from the international pet trade. International routes of primate trafficking indicate that Europe is one of the main markets for great apes (Stiles et al., 2013) thus the educator-led visit emphasizes that primates are not good pets. How primates are featured in social media can push people to have them as pets (Ross, Vreeman & Lonsdorf, 2011; Aldrich, 2018). Thus, for the MonaEduca it is very important to convey an anti-pet trade message to children, who are exposed to content on social networks that do not place value on primates or primate conservation. Thus question 8 asks “How would you like to see this chimpanzee, as a pet or in the wild?” This was the question that received more pro-conservation responses, as 93% of children in the experimental group answered that they would prefer to see a chimpanzee in the wild.
In this study, we found that female students were more likely to choose pro-conservation responses than male students. Females seem to show stronger moral attitudes than male students (Eagles & Demare, 1999; Bogner & Wiseman, 2006). As defined by Kellert (1982) a moral attitude refers to one’s concern for right and wrong when it comes to our relationship with animals. This result does not have a clear explanation. Some studies suggest that formal education’s social and cultural environments may predispose males to a utilitarian use of the planet and females to a more protective attitude towards nature and animal conservation due to gender socialization (Zelezny, Chua & Aldrich, 2000; Arnocky & Stroink, 2011; Xiao & McCright, 2012). These studies suggest that females usually report stronger ecocentric environmental attitudes than males (Zelezny, Chua & Aldrich, 2000), show more respect toward animals than men, and are significantly less anthropocentric and more compassionate (Kaliský & Kaliská, 2022). However, there might be other explanations related to specific aspects of the environmental activity, such as the activity itself or the gender of the educator. Shutts and colleagues (2010) show evidence that children’s learning is influenced by gender. The authors observed that human infants tend to retain information more effectively if the educator or informant is of the same gender. As such, according to Shutts and colleagues (2010), one possible explanation for females scoring more pro-conservation responses might be that most educators from the MonaEduca team were women (4 versus 1).
General Discussion
Our study is the first assessment of an environmental education program in Europe carried out at a primate sanctuary. In this study, we evaluated the MonaEduca program, which focuses on stimulating changes in attitudes and increasing knowledge of primate conservation among primary, secondary, and high school children aged 8 through 18. According to our hypothesis, the results of this study demonstrate that an educational program carried out in an informal setting such as a primate sanctuary has a positive effect on children’s attitudes toward primate welfare and conservation and can support short-term knowledge acquisition. As an outdoor activity, the visit to a primate sanctuary can have a major impact on children’s emotions as they have an up-close encounter with the animals as happening in zoos (Hacker & Miller, 2016). Learning firsthand the reasons why the animals are housed in the sanctuary, and their life history and behavior (Prokop, Tuncer & Kvasničák, 2007) supports the formation of an emotional bond that is related to the will to protect the animals and their natural environment (Zhang, Goodale & Chen, 2014). Our results are aligned with those of other studies carried out in animal reserves, national parks, and biosphere reserves (Kuhar et al., 2007; Borchers et al., 2013; Rakotomamonjy et al., 2015; Burnett et al., 2016; Grúňová et al., 2017). These results are also supported by studies carried out in primate sanctuaries such as the Lola Ya Bonobo sanctuary in the Republic of Congo (André et al., 2008) and other sanctuaries in Central Africa (Kuhar et al., 2012), as well as studies carried out at zoos (Lukas & Ross, 2005; Moss & Esson, 2010; Jensen, 2014; Moss, Jensen & Gusset, 2014; Chalmin-Pui & Perkins, 2017; Moss, Jensen & Gusset, 2017a; Moss, Jensen & Gusset, 2017b; Spooner et al., 2019; Collins et al., 2020).
Implications for the development of educational programs in animal sanctuaries
The results of this study suggest that the work carried out in education conservation programs with children in a sanctuary is an important aspect in improving knowledge and attitudes toward charismatic species such as primates. Overall, all children from the 83 schools that participated in this study showed improvement in their attitude and knowledge after completing the program. Conservation education programs are very rarely integrated into the national education curriculum. Thus, school children engage in this type of outdoor program at a sanctuary as an extracurricular activity. These programs carried out outside of school, aim to reinforce the curricular program but also to help children engage in new activities provided by the opportunity to visit a sanctuary near their school. These types of positive experiences with nature and animals during childhood promote children’s commitment to protecting the environment when they become adults (Wells & Lekies, 2006; Chawla & Cushing, 2007). The work carried out at primate sanctuaries like Mona can help to engage emotions in children when they are exposed to primates that are in a rehabilitation process. These emotions help to connect participants with the animals and improve children’s attitudes toward primate welfare and conservation (Clayton, Fraser & Saunders, 2009). Emotional connections made during environmental educational programs in young people are the main triggers of the measured outcomes (Stern, Powell & Hill, 2014). Given the unfortunate disconnect between children and wildlife, it can be important to allow children to experience nature as these experiences have been shown to promote biodiversity conservation (Schuttler et al., 2019). Additionally, the primary use of data and all knowledge generated by this study will be used directly to improve the current MonaEduca Activities. This will in turn allow Mona to positively impact children’s emotions and empathy towards primates.
Limitations and Future Research
The main limitation of this study was that the tool we used measured only the immediate impact of the educational activity. Although the short-term effects of the activity are encouraging, we cannot know how long participants will sustain their new knowledge. What we know is that attitude changes in children can be transmitted to adults (Rakotomamonjy et al., 2015). The emotion children show when engaging with the stories of rescued chimpanzees is likely transmitted to their families as many families decide to visit the sanctuary after a school visit from their child (Feliu, pers. comm., 2020). The schools participating in the study came from different urban and rural areas, with very different levels of family education and socioeconomic status. Personal experiences, outdoor activities, and families influence the attitudes of children toward nature (Eagles & Demare, 1999). Since we did not have access to these sources of information in our surveys, we cannot know how these variables influenced our results. Our survey instrument did not contain any personal questions that measured student involvement in other environmental activities outside school that influence participants’ environmental attitudes.
As there are few studies of conservation education with school children in sanctuaries, it is difficult to compare the results obtained from Mona’s educational program with the programs of other sanctuaries. In the absence of such studies, we encourage primate sanctuaries to promote empirical studies of attitudes and knowledge of primate welfare and conservation and conduct systematical evaluations to strengthen their educational activities. It is important to assess if these programs work, but also why and how they work (Stern, Powell & Hill, 2014). Despite these caveats, we believe that the information presented in this study is very valuable. It is one of the first studies that shows the critical importance of the educational role of primate sanctuaries.
Supplemental Information
Acknowledgments
Thanks to Emily Felt for the English proofreading, to Martí Masip for helping with the bibliography to the Collaboration Agreement between Fundació Mona and Instituto de Investigaciones Biológicas of the Universidad Veracruzana, and to the MonaEduca team for doing such a great job and for organizing the children for the data collection. Thanks to all the teachers and children that participated in the survey.
Funding Statement
The authors received no funding for this work.
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare there are no competing interests.
Author Contributions
Olga Feliu conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, and approved the final draft.
Arturo González-Zamora conceived and designed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, review, editing and supervision, and approved the final draft.
David Riba conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, and approved the final draft.
Teresa Sauquet conceived and designed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, and approved the final draft.
Sònia Sánchez-López conceived and designed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, review, editing and supervision, and approved the final draft.
Carmen Maté conceived and designed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, review, editing and supervision, and approved the final draft.
Human Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
Ethical approval was granted by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Barcelona.
Data Availability
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data is available in the Supplemental Files.
References
- Aldrich (2018).Aldrich BC. The use of primate actors in feature films 1990–2013. Anthrozoos. 2018;31(1):5–21. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2018.1406197. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- André et al. (2008).André C, Kamate C, Mbonzo P, Morel D, Hare B. The conservation value of Lola ya Bonobo Sanctuary. In: Furuichi TJ, editor. The bonobos. developments in primatology: progress and prospects. Springer; New York: 2008. pp. 303–322. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Arnocky & Stroink (2011).Arnocky S, Stroink ML. Gender differences in environmentalism: the mediating role of emotional empathy. Current Research in Social Psychology. 2011;16(9):1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Baayen (2008).Baayen RH. Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R. 2008;Vol. 505 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511801686. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bates et al. (2014).Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Package Lme4: linear mixed-effects models using eigen and S4. R package version. 67 2014
- Beck et al. (2007).Beck B, Walkup K, Rodrigues M, Unwin S, Stoinski T, Travis D. Best practice guidelines for the re-introduction of great apes. IUCN; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett (1989).Bennett DB. Four steps to evaluating environmental education learning experiences. The Journal of Environmental Education. 1989;20(2):14–21. [Google Scholar]
- Bogner & Wiseman (2004).Bogner FX, Wiseman M. Outdoor ecology education and pupils’ environmental perception in preservation and utilization. Science Education International. 2004;15(1):27–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bogner & Wiseman (2006).Bogner FX, Wiseman M. Adolescents’ attitudes towards nature and environment: quantifying the 2-MEV model. Environmentalist. 2006;26(4):247–254. doi: 10.1007/s10669-006-8660-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Borchers et al. (2013).Borchers C, Boesch C, Riedel J, Guilahoux H, Ouattara D, Randler C. Environmental education in Côte d’Ivoire/West Africa: extra-curricular primary school teaching shows positive impact on environmental knowledge and attitudes. International Journal of Science Education, Part B. 2013;4(3):240–259. doi: 10.1080/21548455.2013.803632. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Borchers et al. (2014).Borchers C, Boesch C, Riedel J, Guilahoux H, Ouattara D, Randler C. Environmental education in Côte d’Ivoire/West Africa: extra-curricular primary school teaching shows positive impact on environmental knowledge and attitudes. International Journal of Science Education, Part B: Communication and Public Engagement. 2014;4(3):240–259. doi: 10.1080/21548455.2013.803632. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie et al. (2020).Bowie A, Krupenye C, Mbonzo P, Minesi F, Hare B. Implicit measures help demonstrate the value of conservation education in the Democratic Republic of the Congo. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11:386. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brent (2001).Brent L. A brief history of captive chimpanzees in the United States. In: Brent L, editor; Brent L, editor. The care and management of captive chimpanzees. Special topics in primatology. Vol. 2. American Society of Primatologists; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Burnett et al. (2016).Burnett E, Sills E, Peterson MN, De Perno C. Impacts of the conservation education program in Serra Malagueta Natural Park, Cape Verde. Environmental Education Research. 2016;22(4):538–550. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2015.1015497. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Burnham & Anderson (2002).Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd Edition. Springer-Verlag; New York: 2002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chalmin-Pui & Perkins (2017).Chalmin-Pui LS, Perkins R. How do visitors relate to biodiversity conservation? An analysis of London Zoo’s ‘BUGS’ exhibit. Environmental Education Research. 2017;23(10):1462–1475. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2016.1259395. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chawla & Cushing (2007).Chawla L, Cushing DF. Education for strategic environmental behavior. Environmental Education Research. 2007;13(4):437–452. doi: 10.1080/13504620701581539. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton (2017).Clayton SPAC. Public support for biodiversity after a zoo visit: environmental concern, conservation knowledge and self-efficacy. Curator: The Museum Journal. 2017;60:87–100. doi: 10.1111/cura.12188. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton, Fraser & Saunders (2009).Clayton S, Fraser J, Saunders CD. Zoo experiences: conversations, connections, and concern for animals. Zoo Biology. 2009;28(5):377–397. doi: 10.1002/zoo.20186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins et al. (2020).Collins C, Corkery I, McKeown S, McSweeney L, Flannery K, Kennedy D, O’Riordan R. An educational intervention maximizes children’s learning during a zoo or aquarium visit. Journal of Environmental Education. 2020;51(5):361–380. doi: 10.1080/00958964.2020.1719022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dobson & Barnett (2008).Dobson AJ, Barnett AG. In: An introduction to generalized linear models. Third Edition. Dobson AJ, Barnett AG, editors. Chapman & Hall/CRC; Boca Raton: 2008. p. 307. [Google Scholar]
- Eagles & Demare (1999).Eagles PFJ, Demare R. Factors influencing children’s environmental attitudes. Journal of Environmental Education. 1999;30(4):33–37. doi: 10.1080/00958969909601882. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Esson & Moss (2016).Esson M, Moss A. The challenges of evaluating conservation education across cultures. International Zoo Yearbook. 2016;50(1):61–67. doi: 10.1111/izy.12113. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Falk et al. (2007).Falk JH, Reinhard EM, Vernon C, Bronnenkant K, Heimlich JE, Deans NL. Association of Zoos & Aquariums; Silver Spring: 2007. Why zoos & aquariums matter: assessing the impact of a visit to a zoo or aquarium. [Google Scholar]
- Farmer (2002).Farmer KH. Pan-African Sanctuary Alliance: status and range of activities for great ape conservation. American Journal of Primatology. 2002;58(3):117–132. doi: 10.1002/AJP.10054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farmer, Buchanan-Smith & Jamart (2006).Farmer KH, Buchanan-Smith HM, Jamart A. Behavioral adaptation of Pan troglodytes troglodytes. International Journal of Primatology. 2006;27(3):747. doi: 10.1007/s10764-006-9041-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Feliu et al. (2022).Feliu O, Masip M, Maté C, Sánchez-López S, Crailsheim D, Kalcher-Sommersguter E. Behavioural development of three former pet chimpanzees a decade after arrival at the MONA Sanctuary. Animals. 2022;12(2):138. doi: 10.3390/ani12020138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrie et al. (2014).Ferrie G, Farmer KH, Kuhar C, Grand A, Sherman J, Bettinger T. The social, economic, and environmental contributions of Pan African Sanctuary Alliance primate sanctuaries in Africa. AnimalsBiodiversity and Conservation. 2014;23 doi: 10.1007/s10531-013-0592-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fultz (2017).Fultz A. A guide for modern sanctuaries with examples from a captive Chimpanzee Sanctuary. Animal Studies Journal. 2017;6(2):9–29. [Google Scholar]
- Grúňová et al. (2017).Grúňová M, Brandlová K, Svitálek J, Hejcmanová P. Environmental education supports conservation action by increasing the immediate and long-term environmental knowledge of children in West Africa. Applied Environmental Education and Communication. 2017;16(1):3–16. doi: 10.1080/1533015X.2016.1273153. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hacker & Miller (2016).Hacker CE, Miller LJ. Zoo visitor perceptions, attitudes, and conservation intent after viewing African elephants at the San Diego Zoo Safari Park. Zoo Biology. 2016;35(4):355–361. doi: 10.1002/zoo.21303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen et al. (2016).Hansen BK, Ross SR, Hopper LM, Fultz A. Developing and evaluating tools to assess the impact of chimpanzee sanctuaries via a unique zoo-sanctuary partnership. PeerJ. 2016;4:e1739v1. doi: 10.7287/peerj.preprints.1739v1. PrePrints. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson, McDuff & Monroe (2015).Jacobson SK, McDuff MD, Monroe MC. Conservation education and outreach techniques. Oxford University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson (1987).Jacobson S. Conservation education programmes: evaluate and improve them. Environmental Conservation. 1987;14:201–206. doi: 10.1017/S0376892900016398. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen (2014).Jensen E. Evaluating children’s conservation biology learning at the zoo. Conservation Biology. 2014;28(4):1004–1011. doi: 10.1111/cobi.12263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaliský & Kaliská (2022).Kaliský J, Kaliská L. Man’s attitude towards animals within the context of gender, age, place of living, eating habits, and worldview in Slovakia. Studia Ecologiae et Bioethicae. 2022;19(4):39–53. doi: 10.21697/seb.2021.19.4.04. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kellert (1982).Kellert SR. Public attitudes toward critical wildlife and natural habitat issues. Washington: D. C.: US Department of the Interior, Fish and WIldlife Service1982
- Kuder-Richardson 20 Formula (2014).Kuder-Richardson 20 Formula . In: Encyclopedia of quality of life and well-being research. Michalos AC, editor. Springer Netherlands; 2014. pp. 3482–3482. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhar et al. (2012).Kuhar CW, Bettinger TL, Lehnhardt K, Cartwright B, Cress D. Education program evaluation at multiple primate sanctuaries in Equatorial Africa. International Journal of Primatology. 2012;33(1):208–217. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9557-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhar et al. (2007).Kuhar CW, Bettinger TL, Lehnhardt K, Townsend S, Cox D. Into the forest: the evolution of a conservation education program at Kalinzu Forest Reserve, Uganda. Applied Environmental Education and Communication. 2007;6(2):159–166. doi: 10.1080/15330150701598213. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhar et al. (2010).Kuhar CW, Bettinger TL, Lehnhardt K, Tracy O, Cox D. Evaluating for long-term impact of an environmental education program at the Kalinzu Forest Reserve, Uganda. American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72(5):407–413. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawson (1983).Lawson AE. Predicting science achievement: the role of developmental level, disembedding ability, mental capacity, prior knowledge, and beliefs. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. 1983;20(2):117–129. doi: 10.1002/tea.3660200204. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Liefländer & Bogner (2014).Liefländer AK, Bogner FX. The effects of children’s age and sex on acquiring pro-environmental attitudes through environmental education. Journal of Environmental Education. 2014;45(2):105–117. doi: 10.1080/00958964.2013.875511. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lopresti-Goodman, Bezner & Ritter (2015).Lopresti-Goodman SM, Bezner J, Ritter C. Psychological distress in Chimpanzees rescued from laboratories. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation. 2015;16(4):349–366. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2014.1003673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukas et al. (2017).Lukas KE, Leeds A, Slavin MA, Tinka J, Kendall CJ. Impact of teacher training in conservation education on student learning in primary schools adjacent to Kibale National Park, Uganda. Oryx. 2017;53(3):497–504. doi: 10.1017/S0030605317000965. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lukas & Ross (2005).Lukas KE, Ross SR. Zoo Visitor Knowledge and attitudes toward Gorillas and Chimpanzees. The Journal of Environmental Education. 2005;36(4):33–48. [Google Scholar]
- Moss & Esson (2010).Moss A, Esson M. Visitor interest in zoo animals and the implications for collection planning and zoo education programmes. Zoo Biology. 2010;29(6):715–731. doi: 10.1002/zoo.20316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss, Jensen & Gusset (2014).Moss A, Jensen E, Gusset M. WAZA magazine, Issue. 2014. A global evaluation of biodiversity literacy in zoo and aquarium visitors. [Google Scholar]
- Moss, Jensen & Gusset (2017a).Moss A, Jensen E, Gusset M. Impact of a global biodiversity education campaign on zoo and aquarium visitors. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment. 2017a;15(5):243–247. doi: 10.1002/fee.1493. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moss, Jensen & Gusset (2017b).Moss A, Jensen E, Gusset M. Probing the link between biodiversity-related knowledge and self-reported proconservation behavior in a global survey of zoo visitors. Conservation Letters. 2017b;10(1):33–40. doi: 10.1111/conl.12233. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ogden et al. (2004).Ogden J, Routman E, Vernon C, Wagner K, Winsten K, Falk J. Inspiring understanding, caring, and conservation action: do we or don’t we? Communiqué. 2004;13(43):10–11. [Google Scholar]
- Prokop, Tuncer & Kvasničák (2007).Prokop P, Tuncer G, Kvasničák R. Short-term effects of field programme on students’ knowledge and attitude toward biology: a Slovak experience. Journal of Science Education and Technology. 2007;16(3):247–255. doi: 10.1007/s10956-007-9044-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rakotomamonjy et al. (2015).Rakotomamonjy SN, Jones JPG, Razafimanahaka JH, Ramamonjisoa B, Williams SJ. The effects of environmental education on children’s and parents’ knowledge and attitudes towards lemurs in rural Madagascar. Animal Conservation. 2015;18(2):157–166. doi: 10.1111/acv.12153. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Randler, Kummer & Wilhelm (2012).Randler C, Kummer B, Wilhelm C. Adolescent learning in the zoo: embedding a non-formal learning environment to teach formal aspects of vertebrate biology. Journal of Science Education and Technology. 2012;21(3):384–391. doi: 10.1007/s10956-011-9331-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2020).R Core Team R: a language and environment for statistical computing. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/ 2020
- Ross, Vreeman & Lonsdorf (2011).Ross SR, Vreeman VM, Lonsdorf EV. Specific image characteristics influence attitudes about chimpanzee conservation and use as pets. PLOS ONE. 2011;6(7):e22050. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuttler et al. (2019).Schuttler SG, Stevenson K, Kays R, Dunn RR. Children’s attitudes towards animals are similar across suburban, exurban, and rural areas. PeerJ. 2019;7:e7328. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shutts, Banaji & Spelke (2010).Shutts K, Banaji MR, Spelke ES. Social categories guide young children’s preferences for novel objects. Developmental Science. 2010;13(4):599–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00913.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skibins & Powell (2013).Skibins JC, Powell RB. Conservation caring: Measuring the influence of zoo visitors’ connection to wildlife on pro-conservation behaviors. Zoo Biology. 2013;32(5):528–540. doi: 10.1002/zoo.21086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spooner et al. (2019).Spooner SL, Jensen EA, Tracey L, Marshall AR. Evaluating the impacts of theatre-based wildlife and conservation education at the zoo. Environmental Education Research. 2019;25(8):1231–1249. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2019.1569201. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stern, Powell & Hill (2014).Stern MJ, Powell RB, Hill D. Environmental education program evaluation in the new millennium: what do we measure and what have we learned? Environmental Education Research. 2014;20(5):581–611. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2013.838749. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stiles et al. (2013).Stiles D, Redmond I, Cress D, Nellemann C, Formo RK. Stolen apes –the illicit trade in Chimpanzees, Gorillas, Bonobos and Orangutans. A rapid response assessment. United Nations Environment Programme & GRID-Arendal 2013 [Google Scholar]
- Thomas (2016).Thomas S. Editorial: future perspectives in conservation education. International Zoo Yearbook. 2016;50:9–15. doi: 10.1111/izy.12134. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wells & Lekies (2006).Wells NM, Lekies KS. Nature and the life course: pathways from childhood nature experiences to adult environmentalism. 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Wich & Marshall (2016).Wich SA, Marshall AJ, editors. An introduction to primate conservation. Oxford University Press; 2016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wiseman, Wilson & Bogner (2012).Wiseman M, Wilson G, Bogner FX. Environmental values and authoritarianism. Psychology Research. 2012;2(1):25–31. [Google Scholar]
- Xiao & McCright (2012).Xiao C, McCright AM. Explaining gender differences in concern about environmental problems in the United States. Society & Natural Resources. 2012;25(11):1067–1084. doi: 10.1080/08941920.2011.651191. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zelezny, Chua & Aldrich (2000).Zelezny LC, Chua P-P, Aldrich C. Elaborating on gender differences in environmentalism. Journal of Social Issues. 2000;56(3):443–458. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00177. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, Goodale & Chen (2014).Zhang W, Goodale E, Chen J. How contact with nature affects children’s biophilia, biophobia and conservation attitude in China. Biological Conservation. 2014;177:109–116. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2014.06.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data is available in the Supplemental Files.