Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Jun 21;18(6):e0280779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280779

The association between the risk perceptions of COVID-19, trust in the government, political ideologies, and socio-demographic factors: A year-long cross-sectional study in South Korea

Yo Han Lee 1,, Hyun-Hee Heo 2,, Hyerim Noh 3, Deok Hyun Jang 4, Young-Geun Choi 5,*,, Won Mo Jang 6,7,*,, Jin Yong Lee 7,8
Editor: Natalie J Shook9
PMCID: PMC10284396  PMID: 37343005

Abstract

Risk perception research, targeting the general public, necessitates the study of the multi-faceted aspects of perceived risk through a holistic approach. This study aimed to investigate the association between the two dimensions of risk perception of COVID-19, i.e., risk as a feeling and analysis, trust in the current government, political ideologies, and socio-demographic factors in South Korea. This study used a year-long repeated cross-sectional design, in which a national sample (n = 23,018) participated in 23 consecutive telephone surveys from February 2020 to February 2021. Most factors differed in the magnitude and direction of their relationships with the two dimensions of risk perception. However, trust in the current government, alone, delineated an association in the same direction for both dimensions, i.e., those with a lower level of trust exhibited higher levels of cognitive and affective risk perception. Although these results did not change significantly during the one-year observation period, they are related to the political interpretation of risk. This study revealed that affective and cognitive risk perceptions addressed different dimensions of risk perception. These findings could help governments and health authorities better understand the nature and mechanisms of public risk perception when implementing countermeasures and policies in response to the COVID-19 pandemic and other public health emergencies.

Introduction

Tracking the trajectory of people’s risk perception and related factors is crucial for an improved understanding of health-related behavior. According to the decision theory of health behavior, people who perceive a greater risk of disease, illness, or injury are generally more motivated to practice preventive and protective behaviors [1, 2]. Thus, understanding risk perception in the context of a pandemic could play a crucial role in risk management, such as social distancing and vaccination compliance. Furthermore, risk perception of emerging infectious diseases may affect some particular behaviors and a set of behavioral patterns called the behavioral immune system [3, 4]. The behavioral immune system includes the detection of and response to potential pathogens using psychological and behavioral defense mechanisms. In addition, risk perception was associated with pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical preventive behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic [510].

Previous research has shown how humans perceive risk could be conceptualized into two main dimensions—cognitive and affective [1113]. The cognitive dimension (risk as an analysis), in which risk is analytically regarded, represents the logical, rational, and scientific consideration of risk management. It relates to how people come to know and understand risks. However, the risk is viewed emotionally within the affective dimension (risk as a feeling). It represents a quick, instinctive, and intuitive reaction to risks, and it refers to how individuals feel about risks. Affective risk perception can be understood as a type of heuristics (mental shortcut) process, which is more predictable than cognitive risk perception [1419]. Several studies have revealed that affective risk perception of infectious disease was related to health behaviors [4, 79, 2022]. Furthermore, longitudinal studies in China revealed that worry and anxiety decreased after the peak of the epidemic [23]. However, to the best of our knowledge, no research has investigated the changes in the two dimensions of risk perception and its associated factors concerning the pandemic and its progress.

Risk perceptions can be influenced by multiple factors, such as socio-demographic, psychological, and politico-contextual variables, across time [24]. In previous studies, trust in institutions was found to be heuristic, which was associated with risk perception [25, 26]. The higher the trust in the government, the lower the level of risk perception of emerging infectious diseases during an outbreak [2729]. Furthermore, political ideologies may be related to the risk perception of COVID-19 [3032]. However, this association varied in different contexts. For example, conservatives, more or less, were likely to perceive COVID-19 risks according to the political context. Risk perceptions, perceived susceptibility, and risk severity often relate to the media representation of a threat or risk [33, 34]. A systematic review of news coverage related to the H1N1 pandemic outbreak revealed that enormous volume of news, overemphasis on threat protection, and news coverage with a frightening tone and manner influenced the amplification of the perceived risk [35]. In addition, a meta-analysis of 47 studies suggested that concern regarding COVID-19 infection was associated with media exposure worldwide [36].

Concerning the socio-demographic factors, older people perceived a lower risk of contracting COVID-19, yet a higher risk of dying due to COVID-19 [37, 38]. The risk level was higher for women and those with higher economic status and education [3945]. However, in previous risk perception studies, age, gender, economic status, and education, as socio-demographic variables, had a weak or non-significant association with risk perception [15].

Until February 2021, the pandemic could be divided into five phases according to the upsurge of confirmed cases in South Korea [46]. Three cluster outbreaks related to religious facilities, large-scale downtown gatherings, nursing homes, and healthcare facilities occurred between January 2020 and February 2021. The five phases were as follows: before the first cluster outbreak (Phase 1), the first cluster outbreak started from the religious facilities in non-metropolitan areas (Phase 2), subsided intermediate periods (Phase 3), the second cluster outbreak started due to massive anti-government rallies in the metropolitan area (Phase 4), and the third cluster outbreak in which the coronavirus spread from nursing homes and healthcare facilities in the metropolitan area to an unspecified majority (Phase 5) (Fig 1, S2 Table in S1 File). The characteristics of the social events in each phase could be related to the perceived risk of COVID-19 and other associated factors. For example, in Phase 4, when the coronavirus outbreak was spread to metropolitan areas due to large-scale, anti-government protests led by far-right groups, people who trusted the government and the president (Democratic Party) and those with a liberal orientation may have experienced increased risk and threat perception compared to the conservatives.

Fig 1. Number of confirmed cases and cognitive and affective risk perceptions by phase.

Fig 1

Even though both dimensions of risk perception of COVID-19 may be related to trust in the government, political ideologies, and socio-demographic variables over time, relevant research is limited. This study investigated the association between these factors and risk perception (affective and cognitive), using a year-long, five-phase, longitudinal design to answer the following research questions (RQ).

  • RQ1: How did the associations between trust in the current government, political ideologies, socio-demographic factors, and risk perception vary depending on affective risk perception (ARP) and cognitive risk perception (CRP)?

  • RQ2: How did the associations between trust in the government, political ideologies, socio-demographic factors, and risk perception change across the five phases of the COVID-19 outbreak?

Methods

Study design and population

This study consisted of 23 independent and consecutive telephone surveys conducted over one year, from the first week of February 2020, when COVID-19 reportedly began in South Korea, to the third week of February 2021. Each survey was conducted by trained interviewers via computer-assisted telephone interviews (CATI; 85% of interviews on mobile phones and 15% of interviews on landlines) at approximately two-week intervals. For each survey, 1,000 people (approximately) over the age of 18, across the country, were randomly selected from a digit-dialing sample frame (S1 Table in S1 File). Stratified samples were extracted by age, gender, and region, and weights were assigned proportionally to the parameters for each stratification to ensure that the participants were representative of all South Korean adults. In total, 23,018 individuals participated in the survey and were included in the analysis. All the surveys were conducted by Gallup Korea, an affiliate of Gallup International. Such telephone survey-based consecutive cross-sectional studies have been considered during other critical infectious disease outbreaks to monitor community response, starting from the initial phase of the epidemic [27, 47]. Detailed information about the study population by phase and survey is presented in S3 and S4 Tables of S1 File.

Measures

Factors potentially associated with both the risk perception dimensions included socio-demographic factors, trust in the government, and political ideologies.

Demographics

Socio-demographic factors included gender, age, occupation, household economic status, and region of residence. Age (in years) was divided into 18–29, 30–39, 40–49, 50–59, and 60 and above [9, 48]. Occupations were classified as unemployed, farming/forestry/fishing, self-employed, blue-collar workers, white-collar workers, full-time homemaker and students. Self-reported household economic status was classified as lower/lower middle, middle, upper middle/upper. The residential area was divided into five regions (Metropolitan area, Seoul, Incheon, and Gyeonggi Province; Chungcheong, a middle region; Yeongnam, a south-eastern region; Honam, a south-western region; None of the above, Gangwon/Jeju Province) [48, 49].

Political characteristics

Trust in the current government was evaluated by asking, “Do you approve or disapprove of the way President Moon Jae-in is handling his job?” Respondents were asked to choose one of the following options–“approval,” “disapproval,” “neither/nor,” and “do not know.” Political ideologies were measured as “conservative”, “liberal”, “neutral”, and “no opinion”.

Risk perception

The ARP was rated on a four-point scale using the question, “How worried are you about contracting the COVID-19 infection?” The answers were rated as “very worried” (4), “a little worried” (3), “not very worried” (2), and “not worried at all” (1). The CRP was assessed on a four-point scale, using the question, “How likely are you to contract COVID-19?” The answers were rated as “very likely” (4), “somewhat likely” (3), “not very likely” (2), and “not at all likely” (1). Owing to the urgency of the outbreak, the validity of the questionnaires was not assessed. For a simple analysis and results, the scores for both risk perceptions were reclassified as dichotomous. Scores 1 and 2 were combined to indicate “not a perceived risk,” and scores 3 and 4 were combined to indicate “perceived risk.” Shentu and his colleagues [50] noted that dichotomization might reduce the estimation bias when the response was contaminated by measurement errors. However, if the measurement was accurate, dichotomization implied a loss of information that could lead to conservative results [51].

Analysis

The one year was divided into five phases, as defined by the Korean Disease Control and Prevention Agency and the South Korean government, based on the number of confirmed cases and specific social events (S2 Table in S1 File). In addition to the factors mentioned above, the number of confirmed cases in the country (log scale) was included as a potentially associated factor, as in the previous studies [10]. One can expect the risk perceptions in South Korea to vary according to the number of confirmed cases. Since almost all South Korean adults have access to daily updates and news via the internet and mass media, these numbers are regularly reported to the public [52]. This was considered in the previous studies [10], whereby, intuitively, risk perceptions varied according to the number of confirmed cases.

We reported the survey response rates over time. The relationship between each factor and risk perception was investigated by univariate analyses, using the chi-squared test (categorical variables) and two-sample t-test (numeric variables). The correlations between the number of confirmed cases and the two dimensions of risk perception were evaluated using Pearson’s correlation coefficient and a t-test for correlation. Multiple logistic regression models were used to evaluate the adjusted odds ratio (aOR) and the confidence interval (CI) for the associated effect of each factor on the two dimensions. Eight covariates were considered explanatory variables to control for each other’s effects. To observe the change in the effect of these factors by phase, an analysis was performed separately for each phase. The researchers calculated the p-values that tested the homogeneity of the aORs over each phase for each factor (p-value for trend), using likelihood ratio tests. Respondents with any missing values were lower than 2.9% of the study population and excluded from the analysis.

Ethics

This study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the Seoul Metropolitan Government-Seoul National University Boramae Medical Center (IRB No. 07-2021-38). The need for informed consent was waived by the IRB because the data were analyzed anonymously.

Results

Descriptive statistics and time trends of the ARP and the CRP

Table 1 presents that the overall proportion of the people who perceived affective and cognitive risk were 71.4% and 53.6%, respectively. Both risk perceptions were significantly different based on trust in the current government, political ideologies, and socio-demographic factors (p <0.05). The ARPs were higher than the CRPs in all the subgroups for all the associated factors. However, the ARP and the CRP levels did not maintain a proportional pattern across the subgroups. For example, those in their 60s and above had the highest ARP, yet the lowest CRP. In addition, women had higher ARP yet lower CRP than men.

Table 1. Overall levels of affective and cognitive risk perceptions.

Overall Risk Perception of COVID-19
Respondents Affective Cognitive
N (%) N (%) p-value N (%) p-value
Total 23,018 100% 16,434 71.4% 12,213 53.6%
Age (years)
 Mean ± SD 49.3 ± 16.7 49.9 ± 16.9 < 0.001 46.2 ± 15.8 < 0.001
 18–29 3,630 15.8% 2,466 67.9% < 0.001 2,280 62.8% < 0.001
 30–39 3,505 15.2% 2,496 71.2% 2,188 62.4%
 40–49 4,397 19.1% 3,003 68.3% 2,613 59.4%
 50–59 4,802 20.9% 3,381 70.4% 2,498 52.0%
 60+ 6,684 29.0% 4,997 74.8% 2,634 39.4%
Gender < 0.001 0.005
 Men 11,613 50.5% 7,797 67.1% 6,364 54.8%
 Women 11,405 49.5% 8,546 74.9% 5,849 51.3%
Job < 0.001 < 0.001
 Unemployed 2,722 11.8% 2,041 75.0% 1,222 44.9%
 Farming/Forestry/Fishing 682 3.0% 491 72.0% 244 35.8%
 Self-employed 3,339 14.5% 2,319 69.5% 1,797 53.8%
 Blue-collar 3,552 15.4% 2,448 68.9 1,902 53.5%
 White-collar 7,051 30.6% 4,869 69.1% 4,307 61.1%
 Homemaker and Student 5,579 24.2% 4,175 74.8% 2,741 49.1%
Self-reported Household Economic Status < 0.001 < 0.001
 Upper/Upper Middle 3,617 15.7% 2,398 66.3% 2,056 56.8%
 Middle 10,331 44.9% 7,248 70.2% 5,564 53.9%
 Lower Middle/Lower 9,070 39.4% 6,697 73.8% 4,593 50.6%
Residential Area 0.001 < 0.001
 Metropolitan 11,561 50.2% 8,250 71.4% 6,488 56.1%
 Chungcheong 2,384 10.4% 1,709 71.7% 1,246 52.3%
 Yeongnam 2,270 9.9% 1,539 67.8% 1,009 44.4%
 Honam 5,807 25.2% 4,168 71.8% 2,986 51.4%
 None of the above 996 4.3% 677 68.0% 484 48.6%
Trust in the Current Government < 0.001 < 0.001
 Approval 11,368 49.4% 7,415 65.2% 5,736 50.5%
 Disapproval 9,668 42.0% 7,507 77.6% 5,568 57.6%
 Neither/Nor 857 3.7% 616 71.9% 409 47.7%
 No Opinion 1,125 4.9% 805 71.6% 500 44.4%
Political Ideology < 0.001 < 0.001
 Conservative 5,822 25.3% 4,333 74.4% 3,142 54.0%
 Liberal 6,568 28.5% 4,761 64.4% 3,567 57.9%
 Neutral 6,670 29.0% 4,294 72.5% 3,861 54.3%
 No opinion 3,958 17.2% 2,955 74.7% 1,643 41.5%

Notes: p-values were calculated using the chi-square test (categorical variables) and the two-sample t-test (numeric variables).

The ARPs were higher than the CRPs in all five phases (Fig 1). In each phase, the ARPs rose and fell faster than the CRPs. Overall, there were subtle significant positive correlations between the number of confirmed cases and the ARP and the CRP (r = 0.1). However, the phase-wise correlations with the CRP were smaller than those with the ARP. Although the highest number of confirmed cases occurred in Phase 5; the level of risk perception did not increase.

Pooled analysis

Fig 2 presents the magnitude and direction of the associations between the eight factors and the two dimensions. Most of the factors differed in strength and direction of associations with risk perception. The ARP for individuals aged ≥ 30 years was significantly greater than the baseline group (18–29 years), although it was unclear whether the ARP increased with age. Conversely, the CRP was significantly lower for individuals aged ≥ 30 years than the baseline group (18–29 years) and it decreased with increasing age. Women had significantly higher ARP than men; however, there was no significant gender difference in the CRP levels. Although there was no significant region-wise difference in the ARP, non-metropolitan regions had a significantly lower CRP level than metropolitan regions. Similarly, the occupation had an insignificant effect on the ARP, except in the self-employed group; however, the CRP levels in the self-employed, blue-collar, and white-collar groups were significantly higher than in the unemployed group.

Fig 2. Associations between related factors and cognitive and affective risk perceptions.

Fig 2

Notes: Affective risk perception (ARP), cognitive risk perception (CRP), adjusted odds ratio (aOR). The reported aORs are the exponentials of the fitted coefficients of the logistic regression models; the midpoint of each CI is 1.

The lower income group had significantly increased ARP compared to the upper and upper middle income groups; however, there were no significant differences in the CRP levels. Trust in the current government was the only factor with the same direction of association between the two risk perceptions, with significantly low values in all the groups. Political ideologies had an insignificant association with the dimensions of risk perception. The number of confirmed cases showed a significant positive relationship with both the ARP and the CRP levels; however, indicating a slightly stronger relationship with the former.

Stratified analysis (over phase)

Tables 2 and 3 represent the changes in the associated effects when the analysis was stratified over time (phase) for the ARP and the CRP, respectively. The results of the pooled analysis (Fig 2) were reproduced without significant changes in all the phases; however, women’s ARP changed over time (p-value for trend = 0.0378). This may be because the ARP in women was lower in Phase 4 than in the other phases. The factors that showed a significant change in their effect on the ARP were trust in the current government (p for trend < 0.0001), no trust in the current government (p-value for trend = 0.0200), liberal political orientation (p-value for trend = 0.0033), and the number of confirmed cases (p-value for trend < 0.0001). Individuals who indicated that they trusted the current government, who were politically liberal, or had no opinion had a stronger ARP in Phase 4 than in the other phases. The effect of the number of confirmed cases on the ARP was significant during Phases 1 through 4; however, it was insignificant in Phase 5 (Table 2).

Table 2. Factors associated with affective risk perception of COVID-19 infection by phase.

Phase 1 Phase 2 Phase 3 Phase 4 Phase 5
Variables aOR (95% CI) aOR (95% CI) aOR (95% CI) aOR (95% CI) aOR (95% CI) p-value for trend
Age (years)
 18–29 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 30–39 1.20 (0.86 to 1.68) 1.53 (1.29 to 1.83) 1.15 (0.94 to 1.41) 1.11 (0.80 to 1.54) 1.34 (1.01 to 1.77) 0.2163
 40–49 0.97 (0.70 to 1.33) 1.18 (1.01 to 1.40) 1.13 (0.93 to 1.37) 0.92 (0.68 to 1.26) 1.11 (0.86 to 1.44) 0.6888
 50–59 0.83 (0.60 to 1.14) 1.36 (1.15 to 1.60) 1.16 (0.95 to 1.40) 0.88 (0.65 to 1.19) 1.12 (0.86 to 1.45) 0.0533
 60+ 1.11 (0.81 to 1.52) 1.45 (1.23 to 1.70) 1.29 (1.07 to 1.55) 0.92 (0.68 to 1.23) 1.14 (0.89 to 1.47) 0.0955
Gender
 Men 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Women 1.65 (1.33 to 2.04) 1.46 (1.31 to 1.63) 1.53 (1.35 to 1.74) 1.28 (1.05 to 1.56) 1.51 (1.27 to 1.79) 0.0378
Residential Area
 Metropolitan 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Chungcheong 1.00 (0.72 to 1.38) 1.00 (0.84 to 1.19) 0.91 (0.75 to 1.11) 1.00 (0.74 to 1.37) 0.97 (0.75 to 1.26) 0.9689
 Yeongnam 0.92 (0.73 to 1.17) 1.03 (0.91 to 1.17) 0.88 (0.76 to 1.01) 0.88 (0.71 to 1.10) 0.96 (0.79 to 1.16) 0.5072
 Honam 0.96 (0.69 to 1.34) 0.90 (0.76 to 1.07) 0.88 (0.72 to 1.07) 0.95 (0.69 to 1.31) 1.34 (1.00 to 1.78) 0.3898
 None of the above 0.78 (0.48 to 1.26) 0.79 (0.61 to 1.01) 0.91 (0.68 to 1.22) 0.76 (0.50 to 1.18) 0.87 (0.59 to 1.28) 0.8735
Job
 Unemployed 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Farming/Forestry/Fishing 1.29 (0.66 to 2.51) 0.93 (0.65 to 1.32) 0.67 (0.46 to 0.97) 1.13 (0.63 to 2.04) 1.25 (0.72 to 2.17) 0.2707
 Self-employed 0.91 (0.60 to 1.36) 0.85 (0.69 to 1.06) 0.89 (0.70 to 1.13) 0.78 (0.54 to 1.11) 0.93 (0.68 to 1.27) 0.9578
 Blue-collar 1.05 (0.72 to 1.55) 0.88 (0.71 to 1.08) 0.85 (0.67 to 1.07) 0.82 (0.58 to 1.16) 0.97 (0.71 to 1.31) 0.9149
 White-collar 1.30 (0.89 to 1.89) 0.95 (0.78 to 1.15) 0.84 (0.67 to 1.05) 1.05 (0.75 to 1.47) 0.85 (0.64 to 1.13) 0.399
 Homemaker and Student 1.20 (0.82 to 1.76) 0.92 (0.75 to 1.12) 0.93 (0.74 to 1.17) 1.11 (0.79 to 1.57) 1.13 (0.84 to 1.53) 0.5648
Self-reported Household Status
 Upper/Upper middle 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Middle 0.87 (0.66 to 1.14) 1.16 (1.01 to 1.34) 1.21 (1.03 to 1.42) 1.25 (0.97 to 1.62) 1.32 (1.07 to 1.63) 0.0023
 Lower middle/Lower 0.91 (0.68 to 1.23) 1.43 (1.22 to 1.67) 1.45 (1.22 to 1.72) 1.37 (1.05 to 1.79) 1.45 (1.15 to 1.82) 0.5139
Trust in the Current Government
 Disapproval 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Approval 0.36 (0.29 to 0.45) 0.46 (0.40 to 0.52) 0.78 (0.67 to 0.90) 0.88 (0.71 to 1.09) 0.63 (0.53 to 0.75) <0.0001
 Neither/Nor 0.41 (0.23 to 0.73) 0.60 (0.45 to 0.80) 0.81 (0.60 to 1.10) 0.80 (0.52 to 1.25) 1.10 (0.69 to 1.74) 0.02
 No Opinion 0.90 (0.54 to 1.51) 0.63 (0.48 to 0.84) 0.75 (0.56 to 0.99) 0.91 (0.58 to 1.42) 0.65 (0.46 to 0.93) 0.6946
Political Ideologies
 Conservative 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Liberal 1.08 (0.81 to 1.43) 0.67 (0.58 to 0.78) 0.90 (0.76 to 1.06) 1.34 (1.02 to 1.75) 0.94 (0.74 to 1.19) 0.0002
 Neutral 1.06 (0.82 to 1.38) 0.91 (0.79 to 1.05) 0.97 (0.82 to 1.15) 1.30 (1.01 to 1.66) 0.94 (0.76 to 1.17) 0.1787
 No Opinion 1.74 (1.25 to 2.44) 0.97 (0.81 to 1.16) 1.11 (0.91 to 1.36) 1.33 (1.00 to 1.78) 0.85 (0.65 to 1.10) 0.0033
Confirmed Cases (log scale) 2.47 (1.53 to 3.99) 1.47 (1.33 to 1.63) 4.06 (3.07 to 5.36) 2.76 (1.82 to 4.20) 0.90 (0.52 to 1.58) <0.0001

Notes: Adjusted odds ratio (aOR), confidence interval (CI). The reported aORs are the exponentials of the fitted coefficients of the logistic regression models; the midpoint of each CI is 1. The p-values for testing the existence of a trend (p-value for trend) were calculated from the likelihood ratio test.

Table 3. Factors associated with cognitive risk perception of COVID-19 infection by phase.

Phase 1 Phase 2 Phase 3 Phase 4 Phase 5
Variables aOR (95% CI) aOR (95% CI) aOR (95% CI) aOR (95% CI) aOR (95% CI) p-value for trend
Age (years)
 18–29 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 30–39 0.78 (0.57 to 1.08) 0.87 (0.74 to 1.03) 1.00 (0.82 to 1.21) 1.00 (0.76 to 1.32) 0.80 (0.62 to 1.04) 0.4083
 40–49 0.67 (0.49 to 0.92) 0.73 (0.63 to 0.85) 0.95 (0.79 to 1.14) 0.94 (0.72 to 1.22) 0.70 (0.55 to 0.90) 0.0355
 50–59 0.40 (0.29 to 0.55) 0.58 (0.49 to 0.67) 0.63 (0.52 to 0.75) 0.82 (0.63 to 1.06) 0.54 (0.43 to 0.69) 0.0001
 60+ 0.36 (0.26 to 0.49) 0.39 (0.33 to 0.45) 0.48 (0.41 to 0.57) 0.50 (0.39 to 0.64) 0.30 (0.24 to 0.38) <0.0001
Gender
 Men 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Women 0.97 (0.79 to 1.20) 0.98 (0.89 to 1.09) 1.06 (0.94 to 1.19) 0.93 (0.79 to 1.09) 0.90 (0.77 to 1.05) 0.5238
Residential Area
 Metropolitan 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Chungcheong 0.78 (0.57 to 1.08) 0.97 (0.83 to 1.13) 0.81 (0.67 to 0.97) 0.93 (0.72 to 1.20) 0.89 (0.71 to 1.13) 0.4507
 Yeongnam 0.75 (0.59 to 0.95) 0.97 (0.87 to 1.09) 0.75 (0.66 to 0.86) 0.92 (0.76 to 1.11) 0.82 (0.70 to 0.97) 0.0138
 Honam 0.87 (0.62 to 1.23) 0.73 (0.62 to 0.86) 0.72 (0.59 to 0.87) 0.67 (0.51 to 0.88) 0.81 (0.64 to 1.03) 0.0009
 None of the above 1.26 (0.78 to 2.03) 0.83 (0.66 to 1.05) 0.77 (0.59 to 1.01) 0.76 (0.52 to 1.11) 0.84 (0.60 to 1.19) 0.6943
Job
 Unemployed 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Farming / Forestry / Fishing 1.10 (0.56 to 2.16) 0.74 (0.53 to 1.03) 0.72 (0.50 to 1.04) 1.04 (0.64 to 1.69) 0.88 (0.57 to 1.36) 0.6857
 Self-Employed 1.21 (0.80 to 1.84) 1.31 (1.08 to 1.59) 1.08 (0.87 to 1.35) 1.07 (0.79 to 1.45) 1.34 (1.02 to 1.75) 0.5473
 Blue-collar 0.95 (0.64 to 1.41) 1.23 (1.01 to 1.49) 1.23 (1.00 to 1.52) 1.13 (0.84 to 1.52) 1.17 (0.90 to 1.51) 0.9337
 White-collar 1.37 (0.94 to 2.00) 1.34 (1.12 to 1.61) 1.39 (1.13 to 1.70) 1.28 (0.96 to 1.70) 1.51 (1.17 to 1.94) 0.8283
 Homemaker and Student 1.21 (0.82 to 1.78) 1.01 (0.84 to 1.21) 1.03 (0.84 to 1.26) 1.08 (0.81 to 1.43) 1.21 (0.94 to 1.55) 0.7262
Self-reported Household Status
 Upper/Upper middle 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Middle 0.99 (0.76 to 1.30) 0.92 (0.80 to 1.05) 1.04 (0.89 to 1.21) 1.07 (0.86 to 1.33) 1.09 (0.90 to 1.32) 0.0237
 Lower middle/Lower 0.88 (0.66 to 1.18) 1.04 (0.90 to 1.21) 1.16 (0.99 to 1.37) 1.03 (0.81 to 1.30) 1.30 (1.05 to 1.59) 0.2846
Trust in the Current Government
 Disapproval 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Approval 0.50 (0.40 to 0.63) 0.61 (0.55 to 0.69) 0.77 (0.68 to 0.88) 0.85 (0.71 to 1.02) 0.79 (0.67 to 0.93) < 0.0001
 Neither/Nor 0.67 (0.37 to 1.20) 0.50 (0.38 to 0.65) 0.66 (0.50 to 0.87) 1.13 (0.77 to 1.66) 1.15 (0.79 to 1.68) 0.0007
 No opinion 0.53 (0.33 to 0.85) 0.52 (0.40 to 0.67) 0.86 (0.66 to 1.11) 0.72 (0.50 to 1.04) 0.52 (0.38 to 0.72) 0.0153
Political Ideologies
 Conservative 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 Liberal 0.90 (0.68 to 1.19) 0.88 (0.77 to 1.01) 1.11 (0.94 to 1.30) 1.34 (1.07 to 1.69) 1.02 (0.83 to 1.25) 0.0198
 Neutral 1.04 (0.81 to 1.35) 0.99 (0.87 to 1.13) 1.09 (0.93 to 1.26) 1.20 (0.97 to 1.48) 1.13 (0.94 to 1.36) 0.5934
 No opinion 0.79 (0.58 to 1.09) 0.77 (0.66 to 0.90) 0.75 (0.63 to 0.90) 0.84 (0.66 to 1.07) 0.67 (0.54 to 0.84) 0.7028
Confirmed Cases (log scale) 1.95 (1.22 to 3.14) 1.14 (1.04 to 1.25) 2.36 (1.81 to 3.08) 1.37 (0.98 to 1.92) 0.97 (0.59 to 1.58) < 0.0001

Note: Adjusted odds ratio (aOR), confidence interval (CI). The reported aORs are the exponentials of the fitted coefficients of the logistic regression models; the midpoint of each CI is 1. The p-values for testing the existence of a trend (p-value for trend) were calculated from the likelihood ratio test.

The association of these factors with the CRP by phase was largely reproduced in the pooled analysis results. However, there were statistically significant changes in the effects of age, residential area, trust in the current government, and political ideologies. For example, aOR value for the impact of trust in the current government on the CRP was significantly below 1 in Phases 1–3 and 5; however, it was the highest in Phase 4 (aOR = 0.85, 95% CI, 0.71, 1.02). Similarly, the politically liberal group had the highest aOR in Phase 4 (aOR = 1.34, 95% CI, 1.07, 1.69). The number of confirmed cases had a significant effect on the CRP in Phases 1–3; however, it had little effect in Phases 4 and 5 (Table 3).

Discussion

This study investigated how the two dimensions of risk perceptions—affective and cognitive—are related to trust in the current government, political ideologies, and socio-demographic factors and how they evolved with the progress of the coronavirus pandemic. This study revealed that the ARP and the CRP address different dimensions of risk perception by delineating their distinct associations with their respective factors. The results of the associations between the CRP and its related factors strongly indicate that the risk perception is based on the condition of the environment of the perceiver. For example, outdoor activities decrease with increasing age; however, there is no difference in the biological vulnerability of men and women to COVID-19. The number of confirmed cases is concentrated in the metropolitan area, and the likelihood of infection was higher when working in an indoor office environment. Hence, these findings indicate that the CRP is based on rational thinking to a certain extent and the general public continues to think rationally about the likelihood of infection even during an active pandemic [53].

Trust in the current government was the only factor that presented an association in the same direction for both the ARP and the CRP. People who did not trust the government were more concerned about contracting COVID-19 and considered themselves more likely to contract the disease than those who trusted the current government or had no clear political stance. Although trust in the current government is not related to an individual’s actual likelihood of getting infected, it has shown a significant association with the CRP. Hence, the lack of trust in the current government interferes with risk judgement with logical reasoning and may be related to emotional responses such as fear, anxiety, or anger. Considering that people with less trust in the current government have a higher rate of vaccine hesitancy [9], this factor is related more to an emotional response than to rational thinking. The relationship between political ideologies and risk perception requires further exploration and interpretation within the South Korean political context. Previous studies have shown that people with a conservative political orientation generally have a lower risk perception; however, this study showed the opposite result for the ARP. In addition, the findings contrast with the results of a study in which Conservative Party supporters, when it was the ruling party, indicated lower risk perception levels during the Middle East respiratory syndrome coronavirus (MERS-CoV) outbreak [27]. Thus, the political support for the ruling party, rather than an absolute political ideology, decisively correlates with the risk perception levels.

Although the overall magnitude or association direction of the related factors did not change substantially for each phase, minor changes were observed in Phase 4. This is probably related to the fact that Phase 4 was triggered by and spread through mass gatherings led by far-right groups who strongly opposed the ruling democratic government. Understandably, the ARP and the CRP levels of those who trust the current government or have the same political affiliation as the ruling party in 2020 increased in Phase 4. This indicates that political ideologies and trust in the current government are strongly associated with instinctive anxiety among the public.

Both the ARP and the CRP were not significantly associated with the number of confirmed cases in Phase 5. In fact, the CRP showed this pattern even in Phase 4. The number of confirmed cases increased; however, the actual risk perception did not increase. This suggests that the government’s strong social distancing policy may not have been as impactful as the increase in the number of confirmed cases. This is in line with reports stating that social distancing is less effective in deterring people’s movement as the COVID-19 pandemic becomes a prolonged event [54].

Regarding other important associated factors, gender plays a vital role in shaping risk perceptions. It is usually understood that women’s risk perception level is higher than that of men; a view supported by the results obtained in this study. In addition, it was found that women have an increased ARP [39]. Furthermore, the study found that older people have a lower risk perception, particularly the CRP, which, too, is in line with previous studies [55, 56]. Previously, older age has been associated with less distress after the 9/11 attacks, with reduced fear of future attacks, and a steep decline in post-traumatic stress over time [57]. The present findings suggest that older adults have less risk perception about the COVID-19 crisis [37].

This study had several limitations. A major caveat is that this study did not include education level due to the limited information of the representative national survey for this study. Previous studies found associations between low levels of education and higher perceived severity and lower perceived probability [45]. Education level must be included in future research as it may affect the ability to acquire, comprehend, and communicate objective knowledge, which could predict reduced risk perception [15]. In addition, the factors that were important in other studies, such as direct experience, socio-cultural factors, psychological factors, trust in science, and media exposure, were not included in this analysis because they were beyond the scope of this study. However, future research must assess how media exposure or its use is related to personal risk perception levels and the mechanism of its relationships. When people socially experience risk, media functions as an “amplification station” for the social experience of risk by intensifying or weakening risk perception through its risk portrayal [58]. Indeed, fear and anger mediate the associations between social media exposure and MERS-CoV risk perception in South Korea [59]. Furthermore, although researchers assessed two major dimensions of risk perception during public health emergencies, each measure relied on a single item; thus, researchers could not verify their validity and reliability. This limitation was shared by the other variables in this study. Finally, this study did not include a disease severity measure, which may add to the perceived threat of COVID-19. However, as the fatality rate of COVID-19 in South Korea was relatively low compared to other epidemics, such as MERS-CoV, it may not be significantly associated with the major findings.

Notwithstanding its limitations, this study provides insights into how different risk perceptions were associated with trust in the current government, political ideologies, and socio-demographic factors during the COVID-19 outbreak in South Korea. Our findings confirmed the empirical distinction between affective and cognitive risk perceptions concerning these factors. However, trust in the current government showed a correlation in the same direction for both dimensions—those with a lower level of trust in the current government, exhibited a higher level of risk perception. Although these results did not change significantly during the one-year observation period, they were associated with significant political events. Our results suggest that trust in the current government may play a role in shaping the risk perceptions of a pandemic, with potentially significant socio-demographic factors for public health outcomes. Risk perceptions are influenced by the changes in the underlying risk along with political interpretations of the risk. Therefore, it is necessary to design health risk communication messages, tailored for each target population group, to address the difference in the risk perception of COVID-19 according to socio-demographic backgrounds and political opinions. Indeed, a better understanding of risk perception and the socio-demographic and political factors linked with the perceived risk could help governments and health authorities implement countermeasures and policies in response to future public health emergencies.

Supporting information

S1 File

(DOCX)

Data Availability

Data from this study cannot be publicly shared, because we have used third-party data from Gallup Korea, and are not entitled to share the data. Gallup Korea Daily Opinion survey is a telephone research program that has been operated weekly by Gallup Korea since January 2012. It examines basic state-run indicators, including presidential job performance evaluation and political party support, and Koreans’ thoughts on major political issues, economy, society, life and culture. Results of basic analysis will be released every Friday at 10 a.m. on Gallup Korea’s website (www.gallup.co.kr). Gallup Korea plans and pays for itself, and anyone interested can use the results of the survey for free. However, the use of raw data from the Gallup Korea is allowed only for researchers conducting a joint study with a Gallup Korea researcher. Detailed data approval procedures are carried out in accordance with Gallup Korea’s internal guidelines. More information about sharing the data can be obtained by contacting press@gallup.co.kr.

Funding Statement

This research received funding from Catholic University of Korea (K2225791), National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2022S1A5B5A16057001), and National Research Foundation of Korea (2020R1G1A1A01006229). However, any funder did not play any role in the study design, data collection, analysis, interpretation, and publication decision.

References

  • 1.Carter WB. Psychology and decision making: Modelling health behavior with multiattribute utility theory. J Dent Educ. 1992;56: 800–807. doi: 10.1002/j.0022-0337.1992.56.12.tb02707.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K. Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice. John Wiley & Sons; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schaller M. The behavioural immune system and the psychology of human sociality. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366: 3418–3426. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Shook NJ, Sevi B, Lee J, Oosterhoff B, Fitzgerald HN. Disease avoidance in the time of COVID-19: The behavioral immune system is associated with concern and preventative health behaviors. PLOS ONE. 2020;15: e0238015. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Heydari ST, Zarei L, Sadati AK, Moradi N, Akbari M, Mehralian G, et al. The effect of risk communication on preventive and protective Behaviours during the COVID-19 outbreak: Mediating role of risk perception. BMC Public Health. 2021;21: 54. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-10125-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bruine de Bruin WB, Bennett D. Relationships between initial COVID-19 risk perceptions and protective health behaviors: A national survey. Am J Prev Med. 2020;59: 157–167. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2020.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jang WM, Cho S, Jang DH, Kim UN, Jung H, Lee JY, et al. Preventive behavioral responses to the 2015 Middle East respiratory syndrome coronavirus outbreak in Korea. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16: 2161. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16122161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Jang WM, Jang DH, Lee JY. Social distancing and transmission-reducing practices during the 2019 coronavirus disease and 2015 Middle East respiratory syndrome coronavirus outbreaks in Korea. J Korean Med Sci. 2020;35: e220. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2020.35.e220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Park HK, Ham JH, Jang DH, Lee JY, Jang WM. Political ideologies, government trust, and COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in South Korea: A cross-sectional survey. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18: 10655. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182010655 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Schneider CR, Dryhurst S, Kerr J, Freeman ALJ, Recchia G, Spiegelhalter D, et al. COVID-19 risk perception: A longitudinal analysis of its predictors and associations with health protective behaviours in the United Kingdom. J Risk Res. 2021;24: 294–313. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2021.1890637 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004;24: 311–322. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lerner JS, Li Y, Valdesolo P, Kassam KS. Emotion and decision making. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66: 799–823. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Loewenstein GF, Weber EU, Hsee CK, Welch N. Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull. 2001;127: 267–286. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Keller C, Siegrist M, Gutscher H. The role of the affect and availability heuristics in risk communication. Risk Anal. 2006;26: 631–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2006.00773.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Siegrist M, Árvai J. Risk perception: Reflections on 40 years of research. Risk Anal. 2020;40: 2191–2206. doi: 10.1111/risa.13599 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Conner M, Rhodes RE, Morris B, McEachan R, Lawton R. Changing exercise through targeting affective or cognitive attitudes. Psychol Health. 2011;26: 133–149. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2011.531570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lawton R, Conner M, McEachan R. Desire or reason: Predicting health behaviors from affective and cognitive attitudes. Health Psychol. 2009;28: 56–65. doi: 10.1037/a0013424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Rolison JJ, Hanoch Y. Knowledge and risk perceptions of the Ebola virus in the United States. Prev Med Rep. 2015;2: 262–264. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2015.04.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cameron LD, Leventhal H. The self-regulation of health and illness behaviour. Psychology Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Shook NJ, Fitzgerald HN, Oosterhoff B, MacFarland E, Sevi B. Is disgust proneness prospectively associated with influenza vaccine hesitancy and uptake? J Behav Med. 2022: 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10865-022-00324-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Savadori L, Lauriola M. Risk perception and protective behaviors during the rise of the COVID-19 outbreak in Italy. Front Psychol. 2020;11: 577331. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.577331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Savadori L, Lauriola M. Risk perceptions and COVID-19 protective behaviors: A two-wave longitudinal study of epidemic and post-epidemic periods. Soc Sci Med. 2022;301: 114949. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114949 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wang Z, Luo S, Xu J, Wang Y, Yun H, Zhao Z, et al. Well-being reduces COVID-19 anxiety: A three-wave longitudinal study in China. J Happiness Stud. 2021;22: 3593–3610. doi: 10.1007/s10902-021-00385-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ferrer RA, Klein WM. Risk perceptions and health behavior. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015;5: 85–89. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Siegrist M. Trust and risk perception: A critical review of the literature. Risk Anal. 2021;41: 480–490. doi: 10.1111/risa.13325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Siegrist M. The influence of trust and perceptions of risks and benefits on the acceptance of gene technology. Risk Anal. 2000;20: 195–203. doi: 10.1111/0272-4332.202020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jang WM, Kim UN, Jang DH, Jung H, Cho S, Eun SJ, et al. Influence of trust on two different risk perceptions as an affective and cognitive dimension during Middle East respiratory syndrome coronavirus (MERS-CoV) outbreak in South Korea: Serial cross-sectional surveys. BMJ Open. 2020;10: e033026. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-033026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dryhurst S, Schneider CR, Kerr J, Freeman ALJ, Recchia G, van der Bles AM, et al. Risk perceptions of COVID-19 around the world. J Risk Res. 2020;23: 994–1006. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2020.1758193 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Albertson B, Gadarian SK. Anxious politics: Democratic citizenship in a threatening world. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Weil AM, Wolfe CR. Individual differences in risk perception and misperception of COVID-19 in the context of political ideology. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2022;36: 19–31. doi: 10.1002/acp.3894 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kyung E, Thomas M, Krishna A. How political identity influences COVID-19 risk perception: A model of identity-based risk perception. J Assoc Con Res. 2022;7: 316–324. doi: 10.1086/719672 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ju Y, You M. It’s politics, isn’t it? Investig Direct Indirect Influences Pol Orient Risk Percept COVID. 2021;19. Risk Analysis.
  • 33.Hove T, Paek H-J, Yun M, Jwa B. How newspapers represent environmental risk: The case of carcinogenic hazards in South Korea. J Risk Res. 2015;18: 1320–1336. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2014.923025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Slovic P. Perception of risk. Science. 1987;236: 280–285. doi: 10.1126/science.3563507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Klemm C, Das E, Hartmann T. Swine flu and hype: A systematic review of media dramatization of the H1N1 influenza pandemic. J Risk Res. 2016;19: 1–20. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2014.923029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Chu TH, Yeo TED, Su Y. Effects of exposure to COVID-19 news and information: A meta-analysis of media use and uncertainty-related responses during the pandemic. Journalism Mass Commun Q. 2022;99: 89–112. doi: 10.1177/10776990211068857 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bruine de Bruin W. Age differences in COVID-19 risk perceptions and mental health: Evidence from a national U.S. Survey conducted in March 2020. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2021;76: e24–e29. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaa074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rosi A, van Vugt FT, Lecce S, Ceccato I, Vallarino M, Rapisarda F, et al. Risk perception in a real-world situation (COVID-19): How it changes from 18 to 87 years old. Front Psychol. 2021;12: 646558. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646558 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rana IA, Bhatti SS, Aslam AB, Jamshed A, Ahmad J, Shah AA. COVID-19 risk perception and coping mechanisms: Does gender make a difference? Int J Disaster Risk Reduct. 2021;55: 102096. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lewis A, Duch R. Gender differences in perceived risk of COVID-19. Soc Sci Q. 2021;102: 2124–2133. doi: 10.1111/ssqu.13079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kim SH, Park MS, Kim TG. Comparison of safety and health risk perceptions toward COVID-19 pandemic based on gender in Korean University students’ work while studying. Saf Health Work. 2022;13: 336–342. doi: 10.1016/j.shaw.2022.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Qiao S, Li Z, Liang C, Li X, Rudisill C. Three dimensions of COVID-19 risk perceptions and their socioeconomic correlates in the United States: A social media analysis. Risk Anal. 2022. doi: 10.1111/risa.13993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Reed-Thryselius S, Fuss L, Rausch D. The relationships between socioeconomic status, COVID-19 risk perceptions, and the adoption of protective measures in a mid-western city in the United States. J Community Health. 2022;47: 464–474. doi: 10.1007/s10900-022-01070-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Pförtner TK, Hower KI. Educational inequalities in risk perception, perceived effectiveness, trust and preventive behaviour in the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany. Public Health. 2022;206: 83–86. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2022.02.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rattay P, Michalski N, Domanska OM, Kaltwasser A, De Bock F, Wieler LH, et al. Differences in risk perception, knowledge and protective behaviour regarding COVID-19 by education level among women and men in Germany. Results from the COVID-19 Snapshot Monitoring (COSMO) study. PLOS ONE. 2021;16: e0251694. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Jang J, Kim Y, Yeom H, Hwang I, Park K, Park Y, et al. Coronavirus Disease-19 (COVID-19) one-year outbreak major cluster infection report as of January 19, 2021, in the Republic of Korea. Public Health Weekly Report. 2021;14: 482–495. Available from: http://www.kdca.go.kr/filepath/boardDownload.es?bid=0034&list_no=712537&seq=1. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lau JTF, Yang X, Tsui H, Kim JH. Monitoring community responses to the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong: from day 10 to day 62. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57: 864–870. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.11.864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kang W B S. Regionalism and Party System Change at the Sub-national Level: The. Korean National Assembly election. J Int Area Stud. 2016;93–111: 2018;25. doi: 10.23071/jias.2018.25.1.93 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Moon W. Decomposition of regional voting in South Korea: Ideological conflicts and regional interests. Party Pol. 2005;11: 579–599. doi: 10.1177/1354068805054981 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Shentu Y, Xie M. A note on dichotomization of continuous response variable in the presence of contamination and model misspecification. Stat Med. 2010;29: 2200–2214. doi: 10.1002/sim.3966 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.MacCallum RC, Zhang S, Preacher KJ, Rucker DD. On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychol Methods. 2002;7: 19–40. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Silver L. Smartphone ownership is growing rapidly around the world, but not always equally; 2019 February 5. [Cited 2022 September 23]. https://www.pewresearch.org/global/2019/02/05/smartphone-ownership-is-growing-rapidly-around-the-world-but-not-always-equally/. Pew Research Center.
  • 53.Brun W. Cognitive components in risk perception: Natural versus manmade risks. J Behav Decis Mak. 1992;5: 117–132. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wellenius GA, Vispute S, Espinosa V, Fabrikant A, Tsai TC, Hennessy J, et al. Impacts of social distancing policies on mobility and COVID-19 case growth in the US. Nat Commun. 2021;12: 3118. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-23404-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Carstensen LL, Pasupathi M, Mayr U, Nesselroade JR. Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;79: 644–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Neubauer AB, Smyth JM, Sliwinski MJ. Age differences in proactive coping with minor hassles in daily life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2019;74: 7–16. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gby061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Scott SB, Poulin MJ, Silver RC. A lifespan perspective on terrorism: Age differences in trajectories of response to 9/11. Dev Psychol. 2013;49: 986–998. doi: 10.1037/a0028916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kasperson RE, Renn O, Slovic P, Brown HS, Emel J, Goble R, et al. The social amplification of risk: A conceptual framework. Risk Anal. 1988;8: 177–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.1988.tb01168.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Oh SH, Lee SY, Han C. The effects of social media use on preventive behaviors during infectious disease outbreaks: The mediating role of self-relevant emotions and public risk perception. Health Commun. 2021;36: 972–981. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2020.1724639 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Natalie J Shook

25 Jul 2022

PONE-D-22-09993Patterns and trends in factors associated with affective and cognitive risk perceptions of COVID-19PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Jang,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Although the reviewers and I found your research question interesting, a number of concerns were raised. The reviewers have provided detailed comments, and I will not reiterate their points. However, an overarching issue is the lack of detail and explanation. With regard to the introduction, a more extensive review of the differences between affective and cognitive risk perception is necessary. What do these constructs differentially predict? Why is it important to examine ARP and CRP separately? Why might this matter in the context of COVID-19? A clear rationale for the study is lacking.  For the treatment of variables (e.g., dichotomizing) and analyses, a more detailed description of the analytic strategy should be provided. Also, please provide references supporting your analytic approach.  In the results section, it is unclear at times whether descriptions of the data are based on statistical comparisons or observation of the data. Be sure to report all statistical analyses.    In the limitations, all of your variables of interest (CRP, ARP, trust in government, political orientation) are measured with single items. This is a shortcoming that needs more attention. This study is also correlational. so causal claims cannot be made. In a few places, causal language is used to discuss the results. This is inappropriate and should be corrected.           

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 08 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Natalie J. Shook

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified (1) whether consent was informed and (2) what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

If you are reporting a retrospective study of medical records or archived samples, please ensure that you have discussed whether all data were fully anonymized before you accessed them and/or whether the IRB or ethics committee waived the requirement for informed consent. If patients provided informed written consent to have data from their medical records used in research, please include this information.

3. Thank you for stating the following in the Competing Interests section: 

Jang DH is affiliated with Gallup Korea (https://www.gallup.co.kr/), but did not receive any funding from them for this work.

Please confirm that this does not alter your adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials, by including the following statement: "This does not alter our adherence to  PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.” (as detailed online in our guide for authors http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests).  If there are restrictions on sharing of data and/or materials, please state these. Please note that we cannot proceed with consideration of your article until this information has been declared. 

Please include your updated Competing Interests statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

"Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Dear authors,

Authors have that they have analyze the trends and patterns in associations of two risk perceptions dimensions for COVID-19 —cognitive risk perception and affective risk perception— and their associated factors with a year-long five

phase longitudinal design. I have few suggestions to offer. The following are the recommendations that needs attention for this manuscript.

Title:

"Patterns and trends in factors associated with affective and cognitive risk perceptions of COVID-19", what is meant by pattern and trends, I mean to say, how these two are different from each other. Please explain both in context.

Abstract:

Be specific in your writing, try to reduce more text in abstract, and use numbers to claim your contributions.

Introduction:

The Introduction part is badly written. There is no logical flow and almost same things are mentioned repeatedly in haphazard manner. I would suggest the authors improve the introduction section of the article. I will recommend to re-write the introcudtion part and the content should a follow a logical flow.

Methods:

I am just curious to know if you also take into considertaion about the accessibility of the participants to the daily updates, news, internet. If I missed somewhere please highlight that or else at least discuss that in limitation.

My major concern with this study is the sample size and the methods used (lack of spatial analysis for spatial variation). South Korea is a big country with over 51 million people, therefore a sample size of less 23000 for spatial analysis is not reasonable. More importantly, the authors should provide information on the number of participants in each province everytime, like the the survey was conducted 23 times over a period of one year.

Results

A lot of over interpretation of the results is the major limitation.

Conclusion:

The conclusion is vague and didn't provide any clear and useful information . It needs to be rewritten

Reviewer #2: This study examined the associated predictors of risk perceptions during COVID-19. A multi-faceted approach was used to examine risk taking and a large sample was utilized. The study has some potential for publication but it’s not ready in the current form. More elaboration in the intro and clarification regarding the methodology is needed. I present my detailed comments below in the order of appearance in the text.

Abstract. “associated factors” is a vague term. There should be at least some reasoning for why these factors are associated. I comment more on this in the introduction.

p5 l78. “10 Asian, American, and European” what is the distribution of these 10 countries according to continents.

p5 l85. What is the justification for choosing a factor as “key”? You should elaborate more on why you choose specific factors and why you expected them to be associated. If there is a theoretical basis of these choices it should also be explained. Currently, the study is atheoretical and there is no solid background for why these factors are studied.

p5 l88. Do you expect significant differences between the five-phases? If yes, how? If not, what may this exploration show us in the end?

p5 l89. You’ve presented the details for the five phases in Table S2. I think these phases are an essential part of the study and possibly unfamiliar to non-Korean readers. Adding the necessary details in text would be helpful.

p5 l92. “related factors” as said this is vague term and I don’t see any reasoning for why only the factors that are present in the parentheses are chosen. These should have been introduced with justifications earlier.

p6 l101. This info on current standing of the literature should be presented earlier in the introduction.

p6 l115. So each survey actually has different samples, which makes over 23,000 people in total. From the abstract my first impression was that 23,000 people participated in 23 waves of the study. This should be clearly explained in the abstract.

Further, this is the first time I’ve encountered a design like this. For me to evaluate the following versions of the manuscript better I’d appreciate if the authors can include some example papers. Including these examples in text may also be helpful for the reader since this may not be common for many readers as well.

p7 l119. Please add the info that the descriptive information can be found at Table S3.

p7 l121. Instead of “potentially associated factors” it can be better to divide this as “demographics” and “political characteristics”. Then list them under a “measures” title along with the measured of “Risk perception”.

p7 l125. Why was age divided this way?

p7 l131. Why were these demographics chosen? For example, why is education not included?

p8 l142. Why were these scores combined and not used as a continuous measure?

p8 l145. This essential info should be presented in the introduction.

p8 l148. Are these confirmed cases based on the location of the participants or the whole country?

p9 l164. Are these percentages of people who perceived risk?

p15 l183. Add the indication that these are pooled analysis in the first place you start explaining the analysis.

p15 l 185. If the results are not statistically significant i don’t think they should be mentioned. They may raise more confusion.

p15 l188. I do not have expertise on this type of analysis and a question. You report that women’s ARP change over time according to the p-value of the test of homogeneity of aORs. How do you define that this is due to Phase 4 being lower than other phases? Did you test for differences between phases or is this just an interpretation based on the aORs?

Also for your confidence intervals you mid-point is not 0. This is also something I’m not used to. I'm not sure if this is a typo or a way of reporting that I'm used to. If it is a typo it should be fixed, if not it would be great to add in notes for what the mid point of the intervals are.

p19 l223. The other way around may also be present. Strong emotional responses may be provoking distrust towards the government. Did you test this possible other direction?

p21 l259. How the listed limitation actually limited should be explained. For example not including education level is listed as a limitation, but you did not explain how did this actually limited your study.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Dr Junaid Ahmad

Reviewer #2: Yes: Barış Sevi

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jun 21;18(6):e0280779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280779.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


7 Oct 2022

Response to peer reviewer comments

Dear Natalie J. Shook,

Thank you for giving us the opportunity to submit a revised draft of our manuscript titled “The Association among Risk Perceptions of COVID-19, Trust in Government, Political Ideology, and Socio-Demographic Factors: A Year Consecutive Cross-Sectional Study in South Korea” [PONE-D-22-09993] to the PLOS ONE. We appreciate the time and effort that you and the reviewers have dedicated to providing valuable feedback on the manuscript. We are also grateful to the reviewers for their insightful comments on the paper. We have been able to incorporate changes in response to a majority of the suggestions provided by the reviewers.

Below is a point-by-point response to the reviewers’ comments and concerns.

Comments from Editorial Corrections

� Comment 1: With regard to the introduction, a more extensive review of the differences between affective and cognitive risk perception is necessary. What do these constructs differentially predict? Why is it important to examine ARP and CRP separately? Why might this matter in the context of COVID-19? A clear rationale for the study is lacking.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have removed vague expressions and rewritten the whole logical flow of the introduction section. (p4-7, 57-128)

� Comment 2: For the treatment of variables (e.g., dichotomizing) and analyses, a more detailed description of the analytic strategy should be provided. Also, please provide references supporting your analytic approach.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for bringing this important point. Due to the urgency of the outbreak, the validity of questionnaires on risk perception and government trust had not been assessed. Thus, we chose dichotomization for simple analysis and results. Shentu and his colleagues noted that dichotomization may reduce bias in estimation when the response is contaminated by measurement errors. However, if the measurement is accurate, the dichotomization implies loss of information that can lead to conservative results [MacCallum et al.]. We added this paragraph into the methods section(p9, 170-173).

Furthermore, we have updated the weakness of variables treatment in the limitation paragraph as follows:

“Third, although the researchers assessed two major dimensions of risk perception during public health emergencies, each measure relied on a single item, and thus the researchers could not verify their validity and reliability. It was also the same limitation to other variables of this study.” (p22-23, 284-287).

� Comment 3: In the results section, it is unclear at times whether descriptions of the data are based on statistical comparisons or observation of the data. Be sure to report all statistical analyses.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have updated the vague expressions in the results section according to your comments. (p10, 198-214)

� Comment 4: In the limitations, all of your variables of interest (CRP, ARP, trust in government, political orientation) are measured with single items. This is a shortcoming that needs more attention. This study is also correlational. so causal claims cannot be made. In a few places, causal language is used to discuss the results. This is inappropriate and should be corrected.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have removed the causality expression, and added the limitation of single item.

“Third, although the researchers assessed two major dimensions of risk perception during public health emergencies, each measure relied on a single item, and thus the researchers could not verify their validity and reliability. It was also the same limitation to other variables of this study.” (p22-23, 284-287).

Comments from Reviewer 1

Title & Abstract

� 1. Do the title and abstract cover the main aspect of the work? "Patterns and trends in factors associated with affective and cognitive risk perceptions of COVID-19", what is meant by pattern and trends, I mean to say, how these two are different from each other. Please explain both in context.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have updated the title and the abstract section. (p3, 38-53)

Updated title:

The Association among Risk Perceptions of COVID-19, Trust in Government, Political Ideology, and Socio-Demographic Factors: A Year Consecutive Cross-Sectional Study in South Korea.

� 2. Be specific in your writing, try to reduce more text in abstract, and use numbers to claim your contributions.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have removed vague expressions and updated the abstract section. (p3, 38-53)

Introduction

� 3. The Introduction part is badly written. There is no logical flow and almost same things are mentioned repeatedly in haphazard manner. I would suggest the authors improve the introduction section of the article. I will recommend to re- write the introcudtion part and the content should a follow a logical flow.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have removed vague expressions and rewritten the whole logical flow of the introduction section. (p4-7, 57-128)

Methods

� 4. I am just curious to know if you also take into considertaion about the accessibility of the participants to the daily updates, news, internet. If I missed somewhere please highlight that or else at least discuss that in limitation.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In The accessibility of the participants to the daily updates, news, and internet was not included in the analysis. However, it is reasonable to assume that and almost all South Korean adults has the access to the daily updates and news via internet and mass media; for example, more than 95% of South Korean adults owned smartphone as of 2019. We addressed this point as follows:

“the number of confirmed cases in the whole country (log scaled) was also included as a potentially associated factor as in previous studies [10]. One can expect that risk perceptions in South Korea may tend to vary according to the number of confirmed cases. Because it is regularly reported to the public and almost all South Korean adults has the access to the daily updates and news via internet and mass media [50].” (p9, 178-182)

Furthermore, a review of the literature on the relationship between risk perception and media exposure was added to the introduction. Media exposure-related variables are beyond the scope of this study and are therefore mentioned as limitations in the discussion section.

“Risk perceptions often relate to how media represent a threat of risk [33, 34]. Perceived susceptibility and severity of risk tend to be affected by the way media frame the issue of threat. A systematic review of news coverage related to the H1N1 pandemic outbreak reveals that the excessive media coverage, overemphasis of threat over protection, and news coverage with frightening tone and manner influence amplifying the perceived risk [35]. A meta-analysis of 47 studies also suggests that concern for COVID-19 infection associated with media exposure worldwide [36].” (p5, 88-95)

“Second, factors that were found to be important in other studies, such as direct experience, socio-cultural factors, psychological factors, trust in science, and media exposure were not included in the analysis since they are beyond the scope of this study. However, future research is needed to assess how media exposure or media use is related to personal-level risk perception and the mechanism of its relationships. According to the social amplification of risk framework, media functions as an “amplification station” for the social experience of risk by intensifying or attenuating risk perception through its portrayal of risk [56]. Indeed, social media exposure is linked to the two emotions, fear and anger, and these emotions mediate the associations between social media exposure and risk perception towards MERS-CoV in South Korea [57].” (p22, 275-284)

� 5. My major concern with this study is the sample size and the methods used (lack of spatial analysis for spatial variation). South Korea is a big country with over 51 million people, therefore a sample size of less 23000 for spatial analysis is not reasonable.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

We agree with the point that spatial variation could be better captured via spatial analysis methods. To address the spatial variation with the limited sample size, we had included a categorized version of the participants’ residential area into five regions: (Yeongnam, a south-eastern region; Honam, a southwestern region; Capital Metro, a Seoul metropolitan area (Seoul, Incheon, and Gyeonggi Province), Chungcheong, and Gangwon/Jeju). This categorization is common in other studies on cognition and decision-making about political issues in Korea.

Kang, W.; Bae, J.S. Regionalism and party system change at the sub-national level: The 2016 Korean National Assembly Elec-tion. J. Inter. Area Stud. 2018, 25, 93–112.

Moon, W. Decomposition of Regional Voting in South Korea. Party Politi. 2005, 11, 579–599.

Park HK, Ham JH, Jang DH, Lee JY, Jang WM. Political Ideologies, Government Trust, and COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy in South Korea: A Cross-Sectional Survey. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021 Oct 12;18(20):10655. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182010655.

Kim JH, Jang DH, Jang WM. Association Between Self-Rated Political Orientation and Attitude Toward the Cash Transfer Policy During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Nationwide Cross-Sectional Survey Conducted in South Korea. Front Public Health. 2022 May 17;10:887201. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.887201

� 6. More importantly, the authors should provide information on the number of participants in each province everytime, like the the survey was conducted 23 times over a period of one year.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your constructive feedback. We updated Table S3 in the supplementary material to report the number of participants in each residential area and other demographic information for each of all 23 surveys.

Results

� 7. A lot of over interpretation of the results is the major limitation.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your comments. We have removed the expression of over interpretation.

Conclusion

� 8. The conclusion is vague and didn't provide any clear and useful information. It needs to be rewritten

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we revised the conclusion as follows.

“Notwithstanding its limitations, the present study provided insights into how two different risk perceptions associate with trust in government, political ideology, and sociodemographic factors during the COVID-19 outbreak in South Korea. Our findings confirmed the empirical distinction between affective and cognitive risk perceptions in relation with those factors. However, trust in the government showed a correlation in the same direction for both dimensions of risk perception; those with the lower level of trust in the government exhibited the higher level of risk perception. Although these results did not change significantly during the one-year observation period, they were associated with political events. Our results suggest that trust in government may play a role in shaping risk perceptions of a pandemic, with potentially significant socio-demographic factors for public health outcomes. Risk perceptions are influenced not only by changes in fundamental underlying risk, but also by political-related interpretations of the risk. Therefore, it is required to design health risk communication messages tailoring for each target population in a situation where the risk perception of COVID-19 differs according to various socio-demographic backgrounds and political opinions on the risk. Indeed, a better understanding of not only risk perception but also the sociodemographic and political-related factors that linked with perceived risk of the pandemic could help government and health authorities implement countermeasures and policies in response to any future public health emergencies.” (p23, 291-308)

Comments from Reviewer 2

Abstract

� 1. “associated factors” is a vague term. There should be at least some reasoning for why these factors are associated. I comment more on this in the introduction.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have updated the abstract according to your comments.

“This study aimed to investigate the association among two dimensions of risk perceptions for COVID-19 (risk as feeling and analysis), trust in the government, political ideology, and sociodemographic factors.” (p3, 39-41)

� 2. From the abstract my first impression was that 23,000 people participated in 23 waves of the study. This should be clearly explained in the abstract.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have updated the abstract according to your comments.

“This study used a year-long repeated cross-sectional design, in which a South Korean national sample (n=23,018) participated in 23 consecutive telephone surveys from February 2020 to February 2021.” (p3, 41-44)

Introduction

� 3. “10 Asian, American, and European” what is the distribution of these 10 countries according to continents (p5, 78).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have removed vague and redundant sentences and rewritten the whole logical flow of the introduction section. (p4-7, 57-128)

� 4. What is the justification for choosing a factor as “key”? (p5, 85).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have removed vague expressions like “key factors” and rewritten the whole logical flow of the introduction section. (p4-7, 57-128)

� 5. Do you expect significant differences between the five-phases? If yes, how? If not, what may this exploration show us in the end (p5, 88)?

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we explained characteristics of each phase and its expected difference in introduction and discussed the related findings in the end.

“The five phases can be described as following before the first cluster outbreak (phase 1), the first cluster outbreak started from religious facilities of non-metropolitan area (phase 2), subsided intermediate period (phase 3), the second cluster outbreak started from massive anti-government rallies in the metropolitan area (phase 4), the third cluster outbreak, which coronavirus spread again from nursing homes and healthcare facilities in the metropolitan area to an unspecified majority (phase 5) (Fig 1, Table S2). The characteristics of social events in each phase may be related to the perceived risk of COVID-19 and potentially associated factors. For example, in phase 4, when the coronavirus outbreak was spread to metropolitan area due to large-scale anti-government protests led by far-right groups, people who support presidential job (Democratic Party) and those with a liberal orientation may increase risk perception of threat compared to the conservatives.” (p6, 105-116)

“This is probably related to the fact that Phase 4 was triggered by and spread through mass gatherings led by far-right groups who strongly opposed the government which democratic party president led. It is understandable that the ARP and CRP levels of those who trust in the government or have the same political affiliation as the ruling party in 2020 have increased in Phase 4.” (p21, 248-252)

� 6. You’ve presented the details for the five phases in Table S2. I think these phases are an essential part of the study and possibly unfamiliar to non-Korean readers. Adding the necessary details in text would be helpful (p5, 89).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we explained the details in the introduction section. (p6, 105-116)

� 7. “related factors” as said this is vague term and I don’t see any reasoning for why only the factors that are present in the parentheses are chosen. These should have been introduced with justifications earlier (p5, 92).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we explained the details in the introduction section. (p5, 81-101)

� 8. This info on current standing of the literature should be presented earlier in the introduction (p6, 101).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we have rewritten the whole logical flow of the introduction section. (p4-7, 57-128)

Methods

� 9. I’d appreciate if the authors can include some example papers. Including these examples in text may also be helpful for the reader since this may not be common for many readers as well (p6, 115).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you very much for detailed comments. Thank you for your valuable comments. In response, we revised the abstract and methods as follows.

“This study used a year-long repeated cross-sectional design, in which a South Korean national sample (n=23,018) participated in 23 consecutive telephone surveys from February 2020 to February 2021.” (Abstract; p3, 41-44)

“This study consisted of 23 independent and consecutive telephone surveys conducted over a one-year period from the first week of February 2020, when COVID-19 reportedly began in South Korea, to the third week of February 2021.” (Methods; p7, 132-134)

In addition, we cited example papers in which the design is telephone survey-based consecutive cross-sectional study in other urgent infectious diseases outbreak (Methods; p7, 142-145).

� 10. Please add the info that the descriptive information can be found at Table S3 (p7, 119).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you. We added the information in methods (p7, 144-145).

� 11. Instead of “potentially associated factors” it can be better to divide this as “demographics” and “political characteristics”. Then list them under a “measures” title along with the measured of “Risk perception” (p7, 121).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for the constructive suggestion. We renamed the paragraphs as suggested; see “Measures” subsection on page 7-8.

� 12. Why was age divided this way? (p7, 125)

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

We used this categorized age in the analysis to encourage interpretability and simplicity of results, while we allow nonlinear relationship between age and risk perception. And this categorization is frequently and practically used in some survey studies. We have cited the references.

Kang, W.; Bae, J.S. Regionalism and party system change at the sub-national level: The 2016 Korean National Assembly Elec-tion. J. Inter. Area Stud. 2018, 25, 93–112.

Jang WM, Jang DH, Lee JY. Social Distancing and Transmission-reducing Practices during the 2019 Coronavirus Disease and 2015 Middle East Respiratory Syndrome Coronavirus Outbreaks in Korea. J Korean Med Sci. 2020 Jun;35(23):e220. https://doi.org/10.3346/jkms.2020.35.e220

Jang WM, Kim UN, Jang DH, Jung H, Cho S, Eun SJ, Lee JY. Influence of trust on two different risk perceptions as an affective and cognitive dimension during Middle East respiratory syndrome coronavirus (MERS-CoV) outbreak in South Korea: serial cross-sectional surveys. BMJ Open. 2020 Mar 4;10(3):e033026. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-033026.

� 13. Why were these demographics chosen? For example, why is education not included? (p7, 131)

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

We appreciate raising the point. Due to the length limitations of telephone-based survey questionnaire, some questions related to demographics, such as education, was omitted. We further clarified this point in the Discussion section as follows.

“This study has several limitations. First, the major caveat concerns that this study does not include education level due to the limited information of representative national survey for this study. Previous studies found associations between low education level and higher perceived severity, and between low education level and lower perceived probability [45]. Education level should be included in future research as it may affect the ability to acquire, comprehend, and communicate objective knowledge on coronavirus which could predict reduced risk perception [15]. Second, factors that were found to be important in other studies, such as direct experience, socio-cultural factors, psychological factors, trust in science, and media exposure were not included in the analysis since they are beyond the scope of this study. However, future research is needed to assess how media exposure or media use is related to personal-level risk perception and the mechanism of its relationships. According to the social amplification of risk framework, media functions as an “amplification station” for the social experience of risk by intensifying or attenuating risk perception through its portrayal of risk [56]. Indeed, social media exposure is linked to the two emotions, fear and anger, and these emotions mediate the associations between social media exposure and risk perception towards MERS-CoV in South Korea [57].” (Discussion; p22, 269-284)

� 14. Why were these scores combined and not used as a continuous measure? (p8, 142)

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for bringing this important point. Due to the urgency of the outbreak, the validity of questionnaires on risk perception and government trust had not been assessed. Thus, we chose dichotomization for simple analysis and results. Shentu and his colleagues noted that dichotomization may reduce bias in estimation when the response is contaminated by measurement errors. However, if the measurement is accurate, the dichotomization implies loss of information that can lead to conservative results [MacCallum et al.]. We added this paragraph into the main body on page 9, lines 170-173.

� 15. This essential info should be presented in the introduction (p8, 145).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for the suggestion. We added more information in the Introduction section as follows. Thank you.

“Until February 2021, a year pandemic period can be divided into five phases according to the upsurge of confirmed cases of COVID-19 in South Korea [46]. Three times the cluster outbreak had occurred related to religious facilities, large-scale downtown gatherings, nursing homes, and healthcare facilities from January 2020 to February 2021. The five phases can be described as following before the first cluster outbreak (phase 1), the first cluster outbreak started from religious facilities of non-metropolitan area (phase 2), subsided intermediate period (phase 3), the second cluster outbreak started from massive anti-government rallies in the metropolitan area (phase 4), the third cluster outbreak, which coronavirus spread again from nursing homes and healthcare facilities in the metropolitan area to an unspecified majority (phase 5) (Fig 1, Table S2). The characteristics of social events in each phase may be related to the perceived risk of COVID-19 and potentially associated factors. For example, in phase 4, when the coronavirus outbreak was spread to metropolitan area due to large-scale anti-government protests led by far-right groups, people who support presidential job (Democratic Party) and those with a liberal orientation may increase risk perception of threat compared to the conservatives.” (Introduction; p6, 102-116)

� 16. Are these confirmed cases based on the location of the participants or the whole country? (p8, 148)

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

It is based on the whole country. We clarified this on page 9, lines 178. Thank you.

Results

� 17. Are these percentages of people who perceived risk? (p9, 164)

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Yes, these are. We clarified this on page 10, lines 199-200. Thank you.

� 18. Add the indication that these are pooled analysis in the first place you start explaining the analysis (p15, 183).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

We appreciate the suggestion. We included titles for each paragraph in the Result section in the revised manuscript.

� 19. If the results are not statistically significant i don’t think they should be mentioned. They may raise more confusion (p15, 185).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for bringing this important point. To avoid confusion, we excluded those redundant statistically insignificant results. The revised paragraph is on page 16, lines 198-214.

� 20. You report that women’s ARP change over time according to the p-value of the test of homogeneity of aORs. How do you define that this is due to Phase 4 being lower than other phases? (p15, 188)

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

We deeply thank you for this clarifying question. It is the interpretation based on aOR; we clarified this point on page 16, lines 186-189.

� 21. Also for your confidence intervals you mid-point is not 0. This is also something I’m not used to. I'm not sure if this is a typo or a way of reporting that I'm used to. If it is a typo it should be fixed, if not it would be great to add in notes for what the mid point of the intervals are (p15, 188).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for raising an important point. In the multivariable logistic regression model, the aOR is defined by $\\exp(\\beta)$ where $\\beta$ is a fitted coefficient corresponding to each factor. Thus, the mid-point becomes 1; we clarified that the midpoint 1 in the note of each table (lines 181-182, 198-199, 203-204)

Discussion

� 22. The other way around may also be present. Strong emotional responses may be provoking distrust towards the government. Did you test this possible other direction? (p19 223)

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

As this study explores the relationship between risk perception and trust in government, the causal relationship was not tested. Therefore, the sentence that may cause confusion in the reader has been modified as follows.

“Although trust in the government is not related to an individual’s actual likelihood of getting infected, it has been shown to have a significant association with CRP. This means that lack of trust in the government interfere with a risk judgement using logic reasoning and may be related to emotional responses such as fear, anxiety, or anger.” (p20, 232-235)

� 23. How the listed limitation actually limited should be explained. For example not including education level is listed as a limitation, but you did not explain how did this actually limited your study (p21, 259).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for the suggestion. We have updated more information in the discussion section as follows. Thank you.

“This study has several limitations. First, the major caveat concerns that this study does not include education level due to the limited information of representative national survey for this study. Previous studies found associations between low education level and higher perceived severity, and between low education level and lower perceived probability [45]. Education level should be included in future research as it may affect the ability to acquire, comprehend, and communicate objective knowledge on coronavirus which could predict reduced risk perception [15].” (p22, 269-275)

Journal Requirements

2. participant informed consent

- Response to the comment:

We have updated the ethics section as follows. Thank you.

This study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the Seoul Metropolitan Government-Seoul National University Boramae Medical Center (IRB No. 07-2021-38). The need for informed consent was waived by the IRB due to the fact that the data were analyzed anonymously.

3. Competing Interests section

- Response to the comment:

We have updated the competing interests section as follows. Thank you.

Jang DH is affiliated with Gallup Korea (https://www.gallup.co.kr/), but did not receive any funding from them for this. This does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.

4. Data Availability statement

- Response to the comment:

We have been confirmed that out data availability statement was acceptable under PLOS guidelines according to email from Adam Thompson on September 6.

Attachment

Submitted filename: 221007 Response_to_peer_reviewer.docx

Decision Letter 1

Natalie J Shook

1 Dec 2022

PONE-D-22-09993R1The Association among Risk Perceptions of COVID-19, Trust in Government, Political Ideology, and Socio-Demographic Factors: A Year Consecutive Cross-Sectional Study in South Korea.PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Jang,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please see Reviewer 2's comments.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 15 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Natalie J. Shook

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: I Don't Know

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for revising the manuscript. I think the comments are addressed very well. Thank you for giving us the opportunity to review the revised draft of your manuscript titled “The Association among Risk Perceptions of COVID-19, Trust in Government, PoliticalIdeology, and Socio-Demographic Factors: A Year Consecutive Cross-Sectional Study

in South Korea”. I don't have any more commments.

Reviewer #2: I thank the authors for addressing my comments. The manuscript looks much improved. I have a few more comments taht I believe will help the manuscript improve even more.

P8L147. Not sure "Factors that were potentially associated with both dimensions of risk perception included sociodemographic factors, trust in the government, and political ideology." this sentence belong under the subtitle demographics. Maybe it should have come right after the Measures title.

P8L157. Since you are asking about the current president and not asking about general attitudes towards government it would be more appropriate to label this variable as "Trust in current government". This point may also be discussed in the discussion section.

Results. The analyses plan explains what analyses are conducted. However mentioning the name of the type of tests you conducted before reporting the results would be helpful for readers who are not custom to your analyses. For example you report a p value at p10l203, but it's not clear what test is used. You can also add this info at the tables, I think it would be very helpful for the readers.

Table 1. The label "Presidential job approval rating" is not consistent with how you labeled this variable in your measures.

Table 3&4. At table 2 you indicated the reference variables, this was not done at Tables 3 and 4. Is there a reason for this difference? If not I think it helps to include that indication.

Overall comment. Labels of gender (men, women) and sex (male, female) are used interchangeably. Please be consistent on the use of this variable.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Junaid Ahmad

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jun 21;18(6):e0280779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280779.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


14 Dec 2022

Response to peer reviewer comments

Dear Natalie J. Shook,

Thank you for giving us the opportunity to submit a 2nd revised draft of our manuscript titled “The Association among Risk Perceptions of COVID-19, Trust in Government, Political Ideology, and Socio-Demographic Factors: A Year Consecutive Cross-Sectional Study in South Korea” [PONE-D-22-09993R1] to the PLOS ONE. We appreciate the time and effort that you and the reviewers have dedicated to providing valuable feedback on the manuscript. We are also grateful to the reviewers for their insightful comments on the paper. We have been able to incorporate changes in response to a majority of the suggestions provided by the reviewers.

Below is a point-by-point response to the reviewers’ comments and concerns.

Comments from Reviewer 2

Methods

� 1. Not sure "Factors that were potentially associated with both dimensions of risk perception included sociodemographic factors, trust in the government, and political ideology." this sentence belong under the subtitle demographics. Maybe it should have come right after the Measures title (P8L147).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for your thorough review. Edited as suggested.

� 2. Since you are asking about the current president and not asking about general attitudes towards government it would be more appropriate to label this variable as "Trust in current government". This point may also be discussed in the discussion section (P8L157).

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

We are grateful for the precise feedback. We updated P8L157 and the others accordingly.

Results

� 3. The analyses plan explains what analyses are conducted. However mentioning the name of the type of tests you conducted before reporting the results would be helpful for readers who are not custom to your analyses. For example you report a p value at p10l203, but it's not clear what test is used. You can also add this info at the tables, I think it would be very helpful for the readers.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you very much for important point. To convey accurate information to readers, we added the type of statistical test we used in each table. In addition, we further specified the type of tests and analysis for Table 1 and Figure 1 in the "Analysis" paragraph in the Method section, like the following:

"... We reported survey response rates over time. The relationships between each factor and risk perception were investigated by univariate analyses with the chi-squared test (categorical variables) and the two-sample t-test (numeric variables). The correlations between the number of confirmed cases and the two dimensions of risk perception were evaluated using Pearson’s correlation coefficient and the t-test for correlation. ...."

� 4. Table 1. The label "Presidential job approval rating" is not consistent with how you labeled this variable in your measures.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

We have updated all sentences from "presidential job approval rating" to "trust in the current government."

� 5. Table 3&4. At table 2 you indicated the reference variables, this was not done at Tables 3 and 4. Is there a reason for this difference? If not I think it helps to include that indication.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

Thank you for important point. We added indication "1.00 (reference)" into the reference groups in Tables 3&4.

� 6. Overall comment. Labels of gender (men, women) and sex (male, female) are used interchangeably. Please be consistent on the use of this variable.

- Response to the reviewer’s comment:

We are grateful for careful review. The updated version replaced sex (male, female) by the labels of gender (men, women) for consistency.

Lastly, We have added information about the support for YGC’s work (P23l374).

Attachment

Submitted filename: 221214 Response_to_peer_reviewer.docx

Decision Letter 2

Natalie J Shook

10 Jan 2023

The association between the risk perceptions of COVID-19, trust in government, political ideologies, and socio-demographic factors: A year-long cross-sectional study in South Korea.

PONE-D-22-09993R2

Dear Dr. Jang,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Natalie J. Shook

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Natalie J Shook

14 Mar 2023

PONE-D-22-09993R2

The association between the risk perceptions of COVID-19, trust in the government, political ideologies, and socio-demographic factors: A year-long cross-sectional study in South Korea.

Dear Dr. Jang:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Natalie J. Shook

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: 221007 Response_to_peer_reviewer.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: 221214 Response_to_peer_reviewer.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Data from this study cannot be publicly shared, because we have used third-party data from Gallup Korea, and are not entitled to share the data. Gallup Korea Daily Opinion survey is a telephone research program that has been operated weekly by Gallup Korea since January 2012. It examines basic state-run indicators, including presidential job performance evaluation and political party support, and Koreans’ thoughts on major political issues, economy, society, life and culture. Results of basic analysis will be released every Friday at 10 a.m. on Gallup Korea’s website (www.gallup.co.kr). Gallup Korea plans and pays for itself, and anyone interested can use the results of the survey for free. However, the use of raw data from the Gallup Korea is allowed only for researchers conducting a joint study with a Gallup Korea researcher. Detailed data approval procedures are carried out in accordance with Gallup Korea’s internal guidelines. More information about sharing the data can be obtained by contacting press@gallup.co.kr.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES