Abstract
Objective
The present study aimed to (i) assess the appetitive drives evoked by the visual cues of ultra-processed food and drink products and (ii) investigate whether text warnings reduce appetitive drives and consumers’ reported intentions to eat or drink ultra-processed products.
Design
In Study I, a well-established psychometric tool was applied to estimate the appetitive drives associated with ultra-processed products using sixty-four image representations. Sixteen product types with four exemplars of a given product were included. Pictures from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) served as controls. The two exemplars of each product type rated as more appetitive were selected for investigation in the second study. Study II assessed the impact of textual warnings on the appetitive drive towards these thirty-two exemplars. Each participant was exposed to two picture exemplars of the same product type preceded by a text warning or a control text. After viewing each displayed picture, the participants reported their emotional reactions and their intention to consume the product.
Setting
Controlled classroom experiments
Subjects
Undergraduate students (Study I: n 215, 135 women; Study II: n 98, 52 women).
Results
In Study I, the pictures of ultra-processed products prompted an appetitive motivation associated with the products’ nutritional content. In Study II, text warnings were effective in reducing the intention to consume and the appetitive drive evoked by ultra-processed products.
Conclusions
This research provides initial evidence favouring the use of text warnings as a public policy tool to curb the powerful influence of highly appetitive ultra-processed food cues.
Keywords: Ultra-processed food, Unhealthy diet, Warning labels, Emotion, Self-Assessment Manikin
Obesity is a major public health concern( 1 ) and a risk factor in the development of non-communicable chronic diseases( 2 ). Curbing or reversing the rising obesity trend is a public policy priority worldwide. This trend has been fuelled partly by inappropriate dietary patterns triggered by environmental factors, such as food-associated external cues. Vision plays an important role in the detection and evaluation of food cues in extra-personal space( 3 ). Given their relevance to survival, food cues, such as visual cues, activate the brain areas underlying attentional processing, appetitive behaviour and the reward centre, and elicit sensory–visceromotor responses linked with food-seeking engagement and consumption( 4 – 7 ). A growing body of evidence shows that visual food cues influence food choices, consumers’ intention to eat and taste perception( 8 – 12 ). For instance, exposure to food cues through advertising on television triggers automatic consumption predispositions, indicating that visual food cues are used strategically by advertisers( 13 – 15 ).
Stimuli-driven consumer predispositions following sensory food-cue exposure are closely related to emotion( 16 – 19 ). Studies grounded in affective neuroscience have shown that pictures of food are emotion-laden, evoking differential subjective, physiological and behavioural responses relative to non-food stimuli( 20 – 22 ). Based on subjective affective ratings, a standard method has been used to directly assess emotional responses to pictures( 23 ). This method assumes that the diversity of emotions can be determined by two main factors, valence (unpleasant v. pleasant) and arousal (the intensity of activation). This experimental approach is based on the International Affective Picture System (IAPS)( 24 ), a catalogue with hundreds of pictures designed to evoke a broad range of human emotional experience, and the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM)( 23 ), a normative instrument used to evaluate the valence and emotional arousal associated with each picture. The following two essential properties of the standardization method apply to the IAPS: (i) the emotional responses evoked by each picture are determined based on the average ratings of a group of participants; and (ii) the rating sessions must include pictures eliciting emotions ranging from an extremely negative to an extremely positive valence and from low to high levels of arousal. These properties will provide valid measures of evocativeness per picture that can be extrapolated to other individuals and groups, thereby facilitating between-group comparisons( 20 , 25 ). The valence and arousal ratings obtained with this method correlate with physiological responses to emotional visual cues and are, to an extent, considered an index of motivational tendencies that drive appetitive (‘approach-like’) or defensive (‘avoidance-like’) responses to stimuli( 26 , 27 ). This standard method has been applied to investigate appetitive (approach) motivational tendencies elicited by home-cooked food pictures, revealing greater emotional responses to pictures depicting foods with high amounts of sugar and fat( 20 , 28 ).
Recently, the Pan American Health Organization recognized that Latin America is immersed in an obesogenic environment and that market deregulation policies have contributed to this situation( 29 ). Along with lower prices and effective distribution channels, packaging and promotion-related marketing activities are also major factors. The sensory attractive cues of industrialized, ultra-processed food products are combined with aggressive advertising to induce a greater appetitive drive and overconsumption. It is therefore unsurprising that aggressive marketing campaigns for ultra-processed food and drink products are commonly identified as major contributors to unhealthy food intake and global epidemics of non-communicable diseases( 30 , 31 ).
The development of ultra-processed, ready-to-eat or ready-to-heat food and drink products involves several processing techniques. The products comprise several ingredients (typically more than five), including substances, such as additives and food industry ingredients, not used in culinary preparations( 32 ). These ultra-processed products are high in sugar, sodium, saturated and/or trans-fats, are hyper-palatable and have high energy densities( 33 ). Studies show that the consumption of these products is increasing in high- and middle-income countries and is linked to health problems, such as obesity, non-communicable diseases and dental caries, among others( 34 – 37 ).
Consumers’ ability to absorb and use nutrition information is limited and varies widely based on numeracy and literacy skills. For instance, in a study on consumers’ understanding of food labels, the authors found that only 32 % could compute the number of carbohydrates in a 20-ounce bottle of soda based on the nutrition label information. Additionally, education was positively associated with nutrition label comprehension( 38 ). In other words, whereas appetitive cues are successfully generated through salient and emotionally laden cues on products, packages and advertisements, information about nutritional content or hazards fails to persuasively communicate the risks associated with consuming ultra-processed products. The hidden, abstract and difficult-to-comprehend nutrition table on the back of packages exemplifies this failure in communication.
The growing concern about the harms associated with ultra-processed products and the lack of use or the misuse of nutrition information has intensified discussions in several countries about the labelling and warning statement requirements for packages and advertisements linked to these products. One proposal involves displaying a nutrition labelling system on the front rather than the back of food product packaging. Front-of-pack nutrition label schemes are designed to provide simplified nutritional content through colour coding (e.g. traffic-light nutrition labels), the presentation of nutrient-specific content in the context of an individual’s overall diet such as Guideline Daily Amounts, or the use of a single indicator of nutritional quality based on specific nutritional criteria (e.g. health logos)( 39 – 41 ). Various front-of-pack formats are being considered or are currently in use worldwide( 41 ).
Regarding text warning proposals, in the USA, San Francisco passed a law requiring advertising for sugar-sweetened beverages that displays text warnings to inform consumers about the presence of added sugar and its negative health effects, thus promoting informed consumer choice( 42 ). California’s Proposition 65 also requires businesses to provide text warnings related to products that contain non-secure levels of chemicals listed as carcinogens, as is the case with some food and beverage additives( 43 ). In Chile, products that exceed sugar, energy, fat and salt limits must carry a warning label of that fact, such as the text ‘High in sugar’( 44 ). In Brazil, the National Agency for Sanitary Surveillance proposed a norm whereby media advertising of unhealthy food products and beverages (high in sugar, sodium, saturated and/or trans-fats) would be followed by text warnings specifying risks related to the consumption of those products( 45 ). This norm, however, focused only on media advertising and did not consider product packaging.
Because the development of public health policies must be based on scientific evidence, the first aim of the present research was to assess the appetitive drive evoked by the visual cues of ultra-processed food products. An important concern with ultra-processed products is that they are designed to prompt extremely strong emotional reactions, elicit food cravings and be habit-forming( 29 ). Scientific evidence of ultra-processed products as emotionally evocative would support obesity-related policies by reinforcing beliefs about ultra-processed products as addictive( 46 ). In Study I, we addressed this issue by using the standardization method applied to the IAPS to measure, through affective normative ratings, the appetitive drive evoked by pictures of ultra-processed foods( 24 ).
The standardization method used in the present study also enabled us to select ultra-processed product pictures with the greatest affective reactions to subsequently test in Study II. In Study II, an experiment was conducted to assess the influence of textual warnings on the appetitive drive and consumption intentions associated with the ultra-processed products selected from Study I. Therefore, the second aim of the research was to test whether text warnings can counteract the powerful influence of highly appetitive ultra-processed product cues. In sum, we aimed to inform public health regulatory efforts with scientific evidence of the efficacy of text health warnings when paired with attractive visual cues of ultra-processed products.
Study I: Emotional normative ratings of ultra-processed product pictures
Methods
Participants
Two hundred and fifteen university student participants (135 women; mean age 21·3 (sd 3·4) years) were recruited. The participants were naïve to the purpose of the study. The Research Ethics Committee of Fluminense Federal University (Brazil) approved the experiments and all participants provided informed consent before any experimental procedure was conducted.
Evaluative report: hedonic valence and arousal
To establish the appetitive drive evoked by ultra-processed product pictures, we applied the SAM( 23 ), a picture-oriented instrument used to assess valence and arousal following exposure to emotional-laden pictures. Ratings were obtained for each picture with visual analogue scales ranging from 1 to 9. The scale for the hedonic valence dimension was composed of five pictorial drawings of manikins interleaved by blank spaces, with the expressions of the manikins ranging from ‘smiling/happy’ (score=9) to ‘frowning/unhappy’ (score=1). For the arousal dimension, the expressions of the manikins ranged from an ‘excited wide-eyed’ figure (score=9) to a ‘relaxed-sleepy’ figure (score=1). Lang et al. developed the SAM scale to create a set of standard pictures (the IAPS) calibrated for affective responses( 24 ). The methodology used in the IAPS generates standard picture sets that evoke similar emotional responses across individuals and groups.
Stimuli
Ultra-processed products
The ultra-processed product pictures included sixty-four pictures of highly industrialized products, mostly selected from the Internet, that are easily found on the Brazilian market.
Pictures were preselected so that an equal number of pictures represented the four following health risk categories: excessive salt, sugar, trans-fat and saturated fat. Each health risk category was represented by four different product types with four exemplars of a given product. ‘Sugar’ product types included carbonated soft drinks, chocolate bars, chocolate discs and gums; ‘salt’ product types included corn chips, potato chips, tortilla chips and instant noodles; ‘saturated fat’ product types included sausages, cooked pork salamis, nuggets and hot dogs; and ‘trans-fat’ product types included margarines, filled cookies, wafer cookies and ice creams (see Fig. 1 for examples). The pictures did not include the extrinsic proprieties of the products, such as the product’s name, brand or package.
A set of four different pictures of the same ultra-processed product type was used to select two exemplars per product type that evoked the greatest pleasant feelings and that would then be tested in Study II. This procedure was applied to minimize the likelihood that appetitive drive differences between ultra-processed product types would be mostly influenced by aesthetic variability( 8 ), because aesthetics is the only aspect that varies across pictures of the same product.
To confirm that all selected products could be classified as ultra-processed products( 29 ), we estimated the exemplars’ ingredients and nutritional compositions based on the NOVA system, which considers the extent and purpose of the industrial food processing( 32 ). One researcher randomly selected samples of the products available on the market. The data extracted from the nutritional tables displayed on the packages included information about the ingredients and nutrition facts. For each product type, we computed the nutrient profile based on the Food Standards Agency (FSA) score( 47 ), considering the nutrient content for 100 g. The FSA score is calculated by awarding each nutrient/food component positive points (0–10) for energy (kJ), total sugar (g), saturated fatty acids (g) and sodium (mg), and negative points (0–5) for fruits, vegetables and nuts (%), fibre (g) and protein (g). The final score varies from −15 (very high nutritional quality) to +40 (very low nutritional quality). To test the accuracy of these scores, a second researcher estimated the FSA scores for a randomly selected sub-sample of the products. The FSA scores obtained by the two researchers were highly correlated (r=0·97, P<0·001).
International Affective Picture System
Seventy-two pictures from diverse emotional categories, balanced in terms of both hedonic valence and arousal, were selected from the IAPS( 24 ) and served as a control background for the target pictures (ultra-processed products). The IAPS emotional categories were neutral (objects, people, animals and plants), pleasant (from low to high arousal: happy families, babies and puppies, nature scenes, sports and erotica) and unpleasant (from low to high arousal: pollution, loss, disgusting, disease, violence and mutilated bodies). The use of different IAPS pictures as backgrounds, varying in valence and arousal, served as the affective bases of comparison during the evaluation of the target pictures, thus anchoring the emotional rating scales( 20 , 24 , 25 ). This standard procedure provides valid between-group ratings, as previously noted( 20 , 25 , 26 , 48 ).
Apparatus
The ratings sessions were conducted in a dimly lit room with desks placed in rows in front of a slide projection screen. Each session had a maximum of forty participants. The desks were arranged in such a manner that the screen was perfectly visible to every participant. The stimuli were displayed with a projector connected to a microcomputer running the software E-prime 2.0 Professional (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, USA), which controlled the timings of stimulus delivery.
Experimental design
Each rating session presented sixteen of the sixty-four target ultra-processed food pictures, in addition to the seventy-two non-target background pictures from the IAPS (the same background pictures were shown in every session). The sixteen target pictures comprised all health risk categories, with one exemplar of each product type within a category. To complete the assessment of the sixty-four ultra-processed product pictures, four different sets of sixteen pictures were prepared. Each set was presented in a different rating session to different participants. To prevent fatigue, we included only sixteen target pictures (instead of sixty-four) in each session.
Procedure
Upon arrival, the participants were asked to be seated and to read and sign the consent form if they were willing to participate in the study. Each participant then received a copy of the instructions and a rating booklet. Before the start of the experimental session, a didactic video explained the incoming task, and training was performed using nine extra pictures from the IAPS from diverse emotional categories. The session included eighty-eight trials (sixteen target pictures plus seventy-two non-target pictures). Each trial began with a preparation slide (‘Get ready to rate the next picture’) that lasted 3s. Then, the picture was presented for 6 s to be appraised. During the next 7s, the participants were asked to estimate how they felt about the picture by rating it along the dimensions of hedonic valence and arousal using the paper-and-pencil version of the SAM scales( 23 ). The sequential order of the pictures’ presentation was pseudo-randomized, with the constraint that the emotional category (neutral, pleasant or unpleasant) could not be repeated more than three times consecutively. We did not provide the participants with any information about the sources of distinction between the control (IAPS) and experimental (ultra-processed products) pictures.
Data analysis
Valence and arousal ratings attributed to each emotion-laden picture were averaged across the participants. The affective space was represented in a Cartesian coordinate system. For better visualization, the ratings in the valence dimension were converted to numbers ranging from −4 (extremely unpleasant) to +4 (extremely pleasant), with 0 being neutral. In this way, valence becomes 0 at the midpoint of the transition from negative to positive ratings. This analysis was conducted to assess the consistency of a picture’s distribution in the bidimensional model( 23 , 26 , 27 ). According to this model, a set of pictures varying in valence and arousal ratings, if represented in a Cartesian coordinate system, is disposed in vectors that point in two directions, forming a ‘boomerang’ shape. The upper arm of the boomerang indexes appetitive (approach-like) motivation, while the lower arm indexes defensive (avoidance-like) motivation.
Spearman correlations were calculated to verify the similarity between the SAM ratings of the IAPS pictures in the present study and the SAM ratings from the original study with North American participants. This analysis tested whether the standardization method was effective for generating valid between-group affective ratings, as predicted.
Arousal and valence ratings were combined and transformed into a vector whose magnitude indicated the appetitive drive (approach-like motivation) towards each ultra-processed food picture. We jointly analysed valence and arousal, as previously described( 48 , 49 ).
Because the data exhibited a normal distribution (Shapiro–Wilk test), we calculated a Pearson correlation between the index of nutritional quality (the FSA score) and the appetitive drive evoked by the visualization of each type of ultra-processed product (sixteen product types). For this correlation, the appetitive drive was computed by each product’s type as the mean across the two most pleasant pictures of the same product.
The threshold for significance was set at P<0·05.
Results
The mean valence and arousal ratings and the appetitive drive of the sixty-four pictures are presented in Table 1. Four exemplars are used to represent each product type. The product types correspond to the four health risk categories, i.e. salt, sugar, trans-fat and saturated fat. Each product type at least had a high content of the component (salt, sugar, trans-fat and saturated fat) that represents the category to which it belongs. The average mean valence and arousal ratings for the sixty-four pictures were 6·9 (sd=0·47, minimum=5·9, maximum=8·1) for valence and 5·1 for arousal (sd=0·66, minimum=4·0, maximum=6·9).
Table 1.
Valence | Arousal | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Health risk category | Product type | Picture | Mean | sd | Mean | sd | Appetitive drive† |
Salt | Potato chips | 1 | 7·2 | 1·51 | 6·1 | 1·35 | 6·5 |
2 | 7·1 | 1·50 | 4·6 | 2·74 | 5·1 | ||
3 | 6·7 | 1·40 | 4·1 | 2·04 | 4·5 | ||
4 | 6·3 | 1·88 | 4·0 | 2·38 | 4·2 | ||
Corn chips | 1 | 6·6 | 1·56 | 4·7 | 2·29 | 4·9 | |
2 | 6·6 | 1·47 | 5·8 | 1·45 | 5·9 | ||
3 | 6·4 | 1·45 | 4·5 | 2·56 | 4·7 | ||
4 | 5·9 | 1·63 | 4·0 | 2·16 | 4·0 | ||
Tortilla chips | 1 | 7·1 | 1·45 | 5·1 | 2·51 | 5·5 | |
2 | 7·0 | 2·19 | 5·8 | 2·28 | 6·1 | ||
3 | 6·7 | 1·46 | 4·8 | 2·51 | 5·1 | ||
4 | 6·3 | 1·68 | 4·6 | 2·15 | 4·7 | ||
Instant noodles | 1 | 7·8 | 1·61 | 6·9 | 2·41 | 7·4 | |
2 | 7·1 | 1·41 | 5·8 | 2·15 | 6·1 | ||
3 | 6·6 | 1·26 | 5·0 | 2·26 | 5·2 | ||
4 | 6·5 | 2·10 | 4·8 | 2·61 | 5·0 | ||
Sugar | Chocolate bar | 1 | 7·9 | 1·20 | 5·9 | 2·65 | 6·6 |
2 | 7·6 | 1·86 | 6·0 | 2·56 | 6·6 | ||
3 | 7·6 | 1·59 | 5·5 | 2·79 | 6·1 | ||
4 | 7·2 | 1·48 | 5·3 | 2·36 | 5·7 | ||
Chocolate discs | 1 | 7·8 | 1·23 | 5·7 | 2·33 | 6·4 | |
2 | 7·4 | 1·24 | 5·0 | 2·36 | 5·6 | ||
3 | 7·3 | 1·38 | 5·4 | 2·42 | 5·8 | ||
4 | 6·7 | 1·84 | 5·0 | 2·59 | 5·2 | ||
Gums | 1 | 7·5 | 1·25 | 5·5 | 2·06 | 6·0 | |
2 | 7·1 | 1·85 | 5·2 | 2·12 | 5·6 | ||
3 | 6·8 | 1·48 | 4·8 | 2·26 | 5·1 | ||
4 | 6·4 | 1·99 | 4·3 | 2·37 | 4·5 | ||
Carbonated soft drink | 1 | 6·9 | 1·67 | 4·8 | 2·48 | 5·2 | |
2 | 6·7 | 1·48 | 4·8 | 2·33 | 5·1 | ||
3 | 6·3 | 1·69 | 4·6 | 2·08 | 4·7 | ||
4 | 6·1 | 1·56 | 4·6 | 2·76 | 4·6 | ||
Saturated fat | Sausage | 1 | 6·9 | 1·90 | 5·3 | 2·17 | 5·6 |
2 | 6·8 | 1·48 | 5·1 | 2·41 | 5·4 | ||
3 | 6·7 | 1·81 | 5·5 | 1·99 | 5·7 | ||
4 | 6·6 | 1·24 | 4·7 | 2·47 | 5·0 | ||
Cooked pork salami | 1 | 7·3 | 1·48 | 6·3 | 1·92 | 6·6 | |
2 | 7·3 | 1·46 | 5·2 | 2·07 | 5·7 | ||
3 | 6·7 | 1·87 | 5·1 | 2·31 | 5·3 | ||
4 | 6·4 | 1·44 | 4·6 | 2·15 | 4·7 | ||
Nuggets | 1 | 7·6 | 1·58 | 6·7 | 1·98 | 7·1 | |
2 | 7·2 | 1·48 | 4·8 | 2·04 | 5·3 | ||
3 | 6·9 | 1·76 | 4·8 | 2·37 | 5·2 | ||
4 | 6·5 | 1·65 | 5·4 | 2·50 | 5·5 | ||
Hot dog | 1 | 7·5 | 1·47 | 6·4 | 1·60 | 6·9 | |
2 | 7·3 | 1·29 | 5·7 | 1·68 | 6·2 | ||
3 | 6·7 | 1·70 | 5·0 | 2·22 | 5·2 | ||
4 | 6·6 | 1·35 | 5·6 | 2·11 | 5·7 | ||
Trans-fat | Margarine | 1 | 7·5 | 1·30 | 5·8 | 2·39 | 6·3 |
2 | 7·2 | 1·21 | 4·9 | 2·25 | 5·4 | ||
3 | 6·6 | 1·31 | 4·3 | 2·20 | 4·6 | ||
4 | 6·4 | 1·12 | 4·5 | 2·21 | 4·7 | ||
Ice cream | 1 | 8·1 | 1·10 | 6·6 | 2·16 | 7·3 | |
2 | 7·6 | 1·52 | 5·1 | 2·33 | 5·8 | ||
3 | 7·2 | 1·73 | 5·2 | 2·37 | 5·6 | ||
4 | 7·1 | 1·44 | 4·9 | 2·60 | 5·3 | ||
Filled cookies | 1 | 7·3 | 1·10 | 5·7 | 2·38 | 6·1 | |
2 | 7·1 | 1·45 | 4·8 | 2·46 | 5·3 | ||
3 | 7·0 | 1·55 | 4·9 | 2·54 | 5·4 | ||
4 | 7·0 | 1·81 | 4·5 | 2·29 | 4·9 | ||
Wafer cookies | 1 | 6·9 | 1·55 | 4·8 | 2·30 | 5·2 | |
2 | 6·6 | 1·72 | 4·5 | 2·08 | 4·8 | ||
3 | 6·6 | 1·20 | 4·0 | 2·03 | 4·4 | ||
4 | 6·3 | 1·71 | 4·3 | 2·21 | 4·4 |
The pictures selected for Study II are indicated in bold font.
Vector magnitude.
The two most aesthetically pleasing pictures of the same product type (based on the highest mean valence) were selected for Study II. Therefore, thirty-two pictures were selected from the initial sixty-four. The thirty-two pictures of ultra-processed products selected are presented in Fig. 1.
The ratings of valence and arousal for the emotion-laden pictures were plotted in a Cartesian plan as shown in Fig. 2. Each point in the Cartesian coordinates represents the mean valence on the y-axis and mean arousal on the x-axis for each IAPS or ultra-processed product picture. The correlation between the ratings of the IAPS pictures we collected and that reported for North Americans approached significance in both the hedonic valence (ρ=0·97, P<0·001) and emotional arousal (ρ=0·93, P<0·001) dimensions. As expected, the ratings were displayed in the Cartesian plan in vectors that point in two directions, representing a ‘boomerang’ shape( 23 ). Appetitive (approach-like) motivation is represented in the upper arm of the boomerang, while the lower arm shows defensive (avoidance-like) motivation.
Critically, the thirty-two selected pictures of ultra-processed products are displayed in the upper arm of the boomerang, in the appetitive space. Some pictures were classified as extremely arousing and pleasant and are positioned at the end of the upper appetitive arm of the space. The combination of the arousal and valence ratings into a vector revealed the appetitive drive evoked by each ultra-processed product picture. The greater the arousal and pleasant ratings values, the greater the appetitive drive.
All selected products fit into the ultra-processed product categories( 29 , 32 ). They comprised more than five ingredients, including additives and food industry ingredients. They were of low nutritional quality, far exceeding the suggested FSA score limit of ≥4 points for foods and ≥1 for drinks (see Table 2) relative to regulatory actions on advertising to children( 47 ). Notably, we observed a significant positive correlation between the appetitive drive evoked by the products and their nutritional quality indexed by the FSA score (r=0·52, P<0·05). The worse the ultra-processed products’ nutritional content was (i.e. the higher the number of kilojoules and amounts of sugar, saturated fat and salt, and the lack of fibre), the stronger the evoked appetitive drive responses were (Fig. 3).
Table 2.
Product type | Ingredients | Energy (kJ)† | Sugar (g)† | Saturated fat (g)† | Trans fat (g)† | Sodium (mg)† | FSA score |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Health risk category: Salt | |||||||
Potato chips | Dried potatoes, vegetable oils (sunflower and corn oils), rice flour, wheat starch, maltodextrin, salt, emulsifier mono- and diglycerides of fatty acids and citric acid acidulant | 2192·00 | 52·00§ | 4·80 | 0·00 | 540·00 | 22 |
Corn chips | Corn flour fortified with iron and folic acid, vegetable oils from sunflower (70 %) and palm (30 %). Prepared for ham flavoured savoury snack food (salt, potassium chloride, maltodextrin, hydrolysed wheat protein, cheese, coconut vegetable oil, glutamate flavour enhancer, monosodium. flavouring, antifoaming silicon dioxide and citric acid acidulant) and salt | 1907·90 | 2·80 | 2·80 | 0·00 | 688·00 | 11 |
Tortilla chips | Corn, palm oil and mixture for flavoured cheese chips (maltodextrin, salt, whey butter, wheat flour, cheese, sugar, soyabean oil and cottonseed oil, spices; flavour enhancers: monosodium glutamate, disodium inosinate and disodium guanylate, flavourings; acidity regulators: disodium phosphate, citric acid and lactic acid, artificial twilight yellow dye and caramel dye) | 2092·00 | 4·80 | 11·20 | 0·00 | 600·00 | 18 |
Instant noodles | Pasta: wheat flour enriched with iron and folic acid, vegetable fat, salt, vitamins B3 (niacin), B6 (pyridoxine), B2 (riboflavin) and B1 (thiamin). Acidity regulators: potassium carbonate and sodium carbonate. Sodium tripolyphosphate stabilizers: tetrasodium pyrophosphate and sodium monosodium phosphate. Synthetic food colouring identical to natural β-carotene. Powdered seasoning: mix for broth, salt, powdered potatoes, onion powder, carrot powder, powdered arracacha, rice flour, tomato powder, powdered yeast extract, garlic powder, soya sauce powder, crushed parsley. Flavour enhancers: monosodium glutamate, disodium inosinate and disodium guanylate. Antifoaming silicon dioxide. Food colouring: caramel IV and II, natural turmeric food colouring and flavourings. Contains gluten. It may contain traces of crustaceans, eggs, fish, celery, mustard and sesame and their derivatives. Contains soya and milk. Foods treated by an irrigation process | 1870·49 | 60·00§ | 8·71 | 0·00 | 1720·00 | 29 |
Health risk category: Sugar | |||||||
Chocolate bar | Sugar, whole milk powder, cocoa butter, chocolate biscuit (wheat flour enriched with iron and folic acid, sugar, hydrogenated vegetable fat, cocoa, inverted sugar, salt, chemical baking soda, soya lecithin emulsifier and flavouring), whey powder vegetable fat, cocoa butter, malt extract. Emulsifier: soya lecithin and polyglycerol polyricinoleate and flavouring. Additional information: produced in equipment that processes almond, peanut, hazelnut, Brazil nuts and cashew nuts. Contains: milk, wheat, soya. Contains gluten | 2209·15 | 56·00 | 16·80 | 0·00 | 192·00 | 28 |
Chocolate discs | Milk chocolate (sugar, chocolate, skimmed milk, cocoa butter, lactose, milk fat, soya lecithin, salt, artificial flavours), sugar, <2 % colouring (includes blue 1 lake, red 40, yellow 6, yellow 5, blue 1, yellow 5 lake, red 40 lake, yellow 6 lake, blue 2 lake, blue 2), dextrin, corn syrup, corn starch. Allergy information: contains milk and soya. May contain peanuts | 2050·16 | 56·67 | 20·00 | 0·00 | 76·67 | 22 |
Gums | Sugar, corn starch and glucose syrup. Contains acidulant citric acid, artificial blue colouring indigotine, tartrazine yellow, red eritruzin and red bordeaux. Artificial aromas of raspberry, strawberry, grape and natural aromas of mandarin and lime | 1548·08 | 92·00§ | 0·00 | 0·00 | 50·00 | 14 |
Carbonated soft drink‡ | Carbonated water, sugar, kola nut extract, caffeine, caramel colouring IV. Acidulant INS 338 and natural aroma | 177·46 | 10·69§ | Does not contain significant amounts | Does not contain significant amounts | 5·17 | 2 |
Health risk category: Saturated fat | |||||||
Sausage | Pork, porcine gill, water, salt, animal protein, sugar, spices. Flavour enhancer: monosodium glutamate (INS 621). Antioxidant: ascorbic acid (INS300). Stabilizer: sodium tripolyphosphate (INS 451i). Natural colouring. Preservatives: sodium nitrite (INS 250) and sodium nitrate (INS 251). Gluten-free | 906·53 | 10·00§ | 10·00 | 0·00 | 1100·00 | 22 |
Cooked pork salami | Beef, pork, bacon, water, salt. Humectant: sodium lactate, natural condiments. Stabilizer: sodium tripolyphosphate, glucose, monosodium glutamate and antioxidant sodium erythorbate | 1004·16 | 0·00§ | 8·00 | 0·00 | 1260·00 | 19 |
Nuggets | Chicken breast, breadcrumbs flour, chicken, cheese, hydrogenated vegetable fat, water, wheat flour enriched with iron and folic acid, rice flour, soya protein isolate, salt, starch, powdered milk, vinegar, hydrolysed vegetable protein, egg powder, sodium polyphosphate stabilizer (INS 452i), antioxidant sodium erythorbate (INS 316), monosodium glutamate flavour enhancer (INS 621), guar gum (INS 412) and natural white pepper flavouring. Contains gluten | 1084·62 | 20·00§ | 6·00 | 0·31 | 583·85 | 16 |
Hot dog | Mechanically separated chicken meat, pork fat, pork, water, turkey meat, isolated soya protein, manioc starch, salt, maltodextrin, natural condiments, black pepper, acid regulator sodium lactate (INS 325). Stabilizers: tripolyphosphate (INS 451i), sodium polyphosphate (INS 452i) and sodium acid pyrophosphate (INS 450i). Natural smoke flavouring, natural white pepper flavouring, natural and identical to natural Jamaican and black pepper flavouring, monosodium glutamate flavour enhancer (INS 621), sodium erythorbate antioxidant (INS 316), preservative sodium nitrite (INS 250) and urucum colouring (160b) | 1221·73 | 3·00 | 8·40 | 0·00 | 1150·00 | 21 |
Health risk category: Trans-fat | |||||||
Margarine | Liquid and hydrogenated vegetable oils, water, salt (1·0 %), reconstituted whey, vitamin A (1500 IU/100 g), stabilizers: Mono and diglycerides of fatty acids and soya lecithin, conservative sodium benzoate, flavourings, antioxidants: EDTA-calcium disodium, BHT and citric acid, acidulant lactic acid and colouring identical to natural β-carotene. Gluten-free. Allergic: Contains derivatives of soya and milk | 2677·76 | 0·00 | 19·00 | 19·00 | 400·00 | 22 |
Ice cream | Cream, condensed milk, water, sugar, egg yolk, flavouring and guar gum and carrageenan | 969·29 | 18·33 | 9·00 | 0·67 | 46·67 | 14 |
Filled cookies | Wheat flour enriched with iron and folic acid, sugar, hydrogenated vegetable fat, inverted sugar, calcium carbonate, cocoa, salt. Colouring: caramel III, carmine and synthetic β-carotene. Chemical ferments: sodium bicarbonate, monocalcium phosphate and ammonium bicarbonate. Flavouring. Emulsifiers: soya lecithin and diacetyl tartaric acid ester of mono- and diglycerides and acidulant citric acid. Allergic: contains wheat and soya derivatives. May contain barley, hazelnuts, milk, oats and rye. Contains gluten | 1994·37 | 33·33 | 7·67 | 1·00 | 230·00 | 21 |
Wafer cookies | Sugar, wheat flour enriched with iron and folic acid, vegetable fat, corn starch, cocoa, salt. Emulsifiers: soya lecithin and sodium stearoyl 2-lactyl lactate. Flavouring and chemical baking soda bicarbonate. Contains gluten | 2008·32 | 36·67 | 12·33 | 0·67 | 173·33 | 21 |
FSA, Food Standards Agency.
Per 100 g.
Per 100 ml.
Carbohydrate information is provided because sugar information was unavailable.
Discussion
The results of the first study provide two contributions. First, the pictures of ultra-processed products prompted an appetitive/attraction motivation and are positioned in the upper arm of the boomerang-shaped arousal–valence affective space. Furthermore, the higher the amount of saturated fat, salt and sugar (the FSA score), the stronger the evoked appetitive drive. This finding converges with the evidence that these components are addictive substances that prompt an uncontrolled approach-like behaviour in high amounts( 50 ). In fact, emotional reactivity assessed through arousal and valence ratings has been linked to food craving ratings and addictive behaviours to other substances( 28 , 51 ). Therefore, higher amounts of sugar, fat and salt would likely negatively influence the public’s ability to cope with ultra-processed food and drink formulations. An open question is whether text warnings can help curb the powerful influence of these highly appetitive ultra-processed food cues. Study II addresses this issue.
Study II: Text warning effects on appetitive drive and intention to consume
Methods
Participants
Ninety-eight (fifty-two women, mean age 20·2 (sd 2·19) years) university student participants were recruited. The participants were naïve to the purpose of the experiments. The Research Ethics Committee of Fluminense Federal University (Brazil) approved the experiments and all participants provided informed consent before any experimental procedure was conducted.
Evaluative report: hedonic valence and arousal
The same instrument (SAM) used in Study I was used in Study II. The participants separately rated valence and arousal using the visual analogue scales described above. Ratings were obtained for each picture of ultra-processed food and drink products.
Intention to consume
The participants were also asked to rate their perceived intention to consume the ultra-processed food or drink product depicted by the picture on a 9-point Likert-type scale (0=none, 8=maximum).
Stimuli
Ultra-processed product pictures
The thirty-two pictures selected from Study I were used (see Fig. 1). Each product type (e.g. corn chips, chocolate bar, carbonated soft drink, wafer cookies) was represented by a pair of pictures displaying different images of the same product type. Therefore, each product type (n 16) was represented by two exemplars (e.g. wafer cookie 1 and wafer cookie 2), totalling thirty-two pictures. One exemplar of each pair was randomly allocated to one of two experimental sets. Each experimental set containing sixteen pictures of ultra-processed product pictures was preceded by either a warning or a control text.
The IAPS pictures were excluded from Study II, as the aim was to compare ultra-processed product picture ratings after exposure to a text warning and a control text rather than obtaining standardized affective ratings.
Text warnings and controls
The text warnings and the ‘neutral’ controls are described in Table 3. Each text warning addressed a health risk category of products, namely the high content of a specific ‘component’: sugar (carbonated soft drink, chocolate bar, chocolate discs, gum), sodium (corn chips, potato chips, tortilla chips, instant noodles), saturated fat (sausage, cooked pork salami, nuggets, hot dog) and trans-fat (margarine, filled cookies, wafer cookies, ice cream). The text warnings were inspired by the norm from the Brazilian National Agency for Sanitary Surveillance that focuses on media advertising. The control texts matched the warning texts relative to the nutritional contents. However, the health risks associated with overconsumption were substituted with information about a product’s conservation and expiration date. The control and warning texts were centrally presented in white font on a green background (RGB:0.128.0) and occupied approximately 75 % of the screen dimensions.
Table 3.
Text warnings | Controls |
---|---|
‘This product contains excessive sugar and, if consumed in large amounts, increases the risk of obesity and dental cavities.’ (For: carbonated soft drink, chocolate bar, chocolate discs, gums) | ‘This product contains “component” and must be kept under refrigeration and be consumed before the expiration date.’ (For: ice cream, margarine, carbonated soft drinks, cooked pork salami) |
‘This product contains excessive sodium and, if consumed in large amounts, increases the risk of high blood pressure and heart disease.’ (For: corn chips, potato chips, tortilla chips, instant noodles) | ‘This product contains “component” and must be kept in a cool, dry place and be consumed before the expiration date.’ (For: chocolate bar, chocolate discs, gums, corn chips, potato chips, tortilla chips, filled cookies, wafer cookies) |
‘This product contains excessive saturated fat and, if consumed in large amounts, increases the risk of diabetes and heart disease.’ (For: sausage, cooked pork salami, nugget, hot dog) | ‘This product contains “component” and must be prepared by the expiration date and immediately consumed following preparation.’ (For: sausage, nuggets, hot dog, instant noodles) |
‘This product contains excessive trans-fat and, if consumed in large amounts, increases the risk of heart disease.’ (For: margarine, filled cookies, wafer cookies, ice cream) |
“Component” refers to sugar, sodium, saturated fat or trans-fat.
Apparatus
The apparatus employed was the same as in Study I, except that each rating session included thirty-two ultra-processed product pictures. Each set of the sixteen types of ultra-processed product pictures was preceded by a warning or control text.
Procedure
Before the start of the experimental session, a didactic video explained the incoming task, and four practice trials were performed. The instructions focused on the rating task regarding the ultra-processed product pictures. Therefore, no emphasis was given to the preceding texts. The experimental session included thirty-two trials divided into two blocks without an interval. The first block presented one set of the sixteen pictures of ultra-processed product types, and each picture presentation was preceded by a control text. The second block presented the other set of the sixteen pictures of ultra-processed product types, and each picture presentation was preceded by corresponding warning text. In each block, an experimental trial began with a ‘beep’ sound presented on a black screen for 500 ms. Then, text (control or warning) was presented for 6s. Immediately after the text offset, an ultra-processed product’s picture was presented and the participants attentively observed it for 5s. During the 15s following the picture offset, the participants should have rated the picture along hedonic valence and emotional arousal (the SAM dimensions) and the intention to consume (Fig. 4).
Each set of the sixteen pictures of ultra-processed product types was preceded by a warning or a control text. The combination of the two sets of pictures and the two conditions (warning or control) was counterbalanced between the participants. The set of sixteen ultra-processed product pictures presented in block 1 (control) for a group of participants in one session was presented to another group in block 2 (warning) in another session, and vice versa. For instance, wafer cookie 1 was presented in the warning condition for one group of participants and in the control condition for another group of participants. Therefore, each picture (from the thirty-two pictures) was presented under both control and warning conditions for different groups of participants, guaranteeing that any warning effects would not be associated with a specific pool of pictures. Additionally, the group of participants presented with wafer cookie 1 in the warning condition, was presented with wafer cookie 2 in the control condition. The other group of participants was presented with the pictures in the inverse order (i.e. wafer cookie 1 was presented in the control case and wafer cookie 2 was presented in the warning case). Therefore, for each condition (warning or control), half of the pictures (n 16) received ratings from one group of participants and the other half (n 16) received ratings from the other group of participants. For analysis, the mean ratings of the thirty-two pictures were considered, thus encompassing the ratings from both groups of participants and guaranteeing that any warning effects would not be associated with a specific group of participants. Additionally, the groups did not differ in terms of hunger (t (96)=−0·30, P=0·76), eating habits (t (96)=−0·53, P=0·60), BMI (t (96)=−0·75, P=0·45) or nutritional knowledge (t (96)=0·10, P=0·92). The self-reported measures are described in detail in the online supplementary material, Supplemental File 1. The average self-reported BMI of the participants in Study II was 23·3 kg/m2 (sd=3·85, minimum=16·55, maximum=38·59).
Data analysis
Valence and arousal ratings attributed to each ultra-processed food picture (from the thirty-two pictures) were averaged across the participants by condition (warning or control). Each point in the Cartesian coordinates represents the mean valence on the y-axis and the mean arousal on the x-axis for each picture during the warning and control conditions. Then, we applied a joint analysis of valence and arousal, as in Study I, to obtain the vector of appetitive drive for each picture.
We averaged the ratings for each picture on the intention to consume scale and the appetitive drive scores for the warning and control conditions. Paired t tests were used to compare the mean ratings of the thirty-two ultra-processed product pictures obtained for the warning condition and the mean ratings of the same thirty-two ultra-processed product pictures obtained for the control condition. Separate analyses were conducted for appetitive drive measures and the intention to consume. The effect sizes were computed in terms of Cohen’s d.
The threshold for significance was set at P<0·05.
Results
The paired t test revealed that text warnings significantly reduced the appetitive drive (t (31)=2·25, P<0·05; Cohen’s d=0·33) and the intention to consume (t (31)=3·80, P<0·001; Cohen’s d=0·45) evoked by the ultra-processed product pictures compared with the control condition, in which these same ultra-processed pictures were preceded by control texts. These results are detailed in Table 4.
Table 4.
Control | Warning | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | sd | Mean | sd | |
Appetitive drive | 5·0 | 0·94 | 4·7* | 0·88 |
Intention to consume | 3·6 | 0·88 | 3·2* | 0·90 |
Mean value was significantly different from that of the control: P<0·05.
The mean valence and mean arousal ratings for each ultra-processed food and drink product in Cartesian coordinates are depicted in Fig. 5. The mean ratings for each of the thirty-two pictures of ultra-processed products were distributed predominantly in the positive-valence domain when they were preceded by a control text. In the case of a warning text, the mean ratings of these same pictures moved towards a more central, ‘less emotional’ area of the bidimensional space (Fig. 5). The data presented in the online supplementary material, Supplemental File 2, show the vector magnitudes of the appetitive drive and the mean values of valence, arousal and intention to consume for the warning and control conditions for each picture of an ultra-processed product.
Discussion
Although the ultra-processed product pictures greatly impacted the participants, the text warnings were effective in reducing the appetitive drive and the intention to consume the corresponding products. Previous studies have shown that texts can change the emotional impact of emotion-laden pictures by activating cognitive reappraisal strategies( 52 – 54 ). A study using a brain electrical activity index of food pictures’ emotional impact demonstrated that thinking about the long-term consequences of eating high-energy foods modulated their emotional impact and the reported desire to consume them( 55 ). Considered together, the studies suggest that the use of text warnings may be effective for communicating the long-term consequences of ultra-processed products’ overconsumption, reducing appetitive drive and consumption predispositions towards them, possibly by activating cognitive regulation mechanisms.
General discussion
The main goal of the two studies was to assess the appetitive drives evoked by the visual cues of ultra-processed products and test whether text warnings would affect the appetitive drive and the reported intentions to consume these products. Study I estimated the appetitive drive elicited by ultra-processed product pictures using normative affective ratings. Ultra-processed product images prompted very strong emotional reactions that were associated with their nutrient profile. The results of Study II indicated that text warnings about the hazards of ultra-processed products’ overconsumption were effective in reducing the intention to consume and the appetitive drive evoked by pictures of these products.
Although the use of text warnings for ultra-processed products would address the problem of increasing obesity and poor health, studies that scientifically test the efficacy of this type of health warning have been overlooked. Recent evidence has shown that text warnings affect healthiness perception, purchase intention and preferences( 56 – 58 ). However, these articles focused exclusively on sugar-sweetened beverages. Furthermore, they did not assess the extent to which the emotional appetitive drive elicited by the product or packaging cues can counteract or moderate the impact of text warnings. Here, we showed that text warnings can superpose the appetitive drive elicited by ultra-processed product-related cues.
Determining the emotional reactivity elicited by visual cues of ultra-processed products is an important issue because food-evoked emotions affect consumers’ food choices( 17 , 18 ) and, consequently, their diet and health. Products with more addictive components( 50 ) evoked a stronger emotional appetitive drive. This result is useful because recognizing ultra-processed products as addictive would support obesity-related policies( 46 ). This result also supports the argument that characteristics of visual food cues, such as their nutritional content, can evoke differential automatic appetitive motivational tendencies( 59 ).
The present study had some limitations. Given that the original IAPS ratings were collected in university students, the study was conducted in this population to enable a more direct comparison between the ratings obtained here and those from the original IAPS study. However, the use of more diverse populations and a wider range of product types would be interesting in the future. Here, we also focused on the intrinsic sensory properties of the products. Further research is recommended to assess the influence of extrinsic properties, such as the brand or package( 17 ). Finally, we cannot exclude a desirability response bias, in that the reported arousal, valence and intention to consume during the text warning condition may have been lower because it was the desirable answer. However, for each picture, the mean ratings during the warning condition were obtained from one group of participants, and the ratings during the control condition were obtained from another. This procedure has been described to diminish desirability tendencies( 60 ). Measures of psychophysiological reactivity to emotional visual stimuli (e.g. heart rate, startle reflex, cerebral measures, skin conductance) could be applied in the future to provide a more objective and unbiased approach to track individuals’ appetitive motivational responses.
Despite these limitations, the study has a number of strengths. We provided timely new data on the emotional impact of ultra-processed product pictures through a gold-standard psychometric instrument to measure emotional reactions to visual stimuli( 23 ). We contributed controlled experimental evidence regarding text warnings’ efficacy by answering the call for new scientific evidence on this topic( 61 ). Finally, the study also collected interdisciplinary scientific knowledge from specialists in neuroscience, experimental psychology, public health nutrition and marketing.
Conclusion
In conclusion, visual cues of ultra-processed food products evoked strong emotional responses and text warnings of the corresponding health risks minimized their appetitive drive and the intention to consume them. Overall, the data reported here implicate text warnings as a potential effective strategy for public health policies that target chronic disease prevention and health promotion. The combination of these findings with new experimental and observational studies would amass evidence to support the use of text warnings in public health.
Acknowledgements
Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank Júlia de Souza Vale for her help with the data collection. This article is related to the PhD doctoral thesis of L.K. Financial support: This work was supported by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES), Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPQ), Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio de Janeiro (FAPERJ) and Fundação do Câncer. CAPES, CNPQ, FAPERJ and Fundação do Câncer had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: There were no conflicts of interests. Authorship: I.A.D., L.K., F.B., E.V., F.S.G., E.B.A., R.L.A., L.O. and M.G.P. conceptualized and designed the experiments. I.A.D., L.K., F.B., J.R.A. and R.L.A. performed the experiments. I.A.D. and L.K. drafted the manuscript. I.A.D., J.M.O., F.B., E.V., M.G.P., L.O., F.S.G., S.G., M.C.F.-S. and E.B.A. contributed for analysis and materials tools. I.A.D., F.B., L.K. and E.V. analysed the data. All authors reviewed draft versions of the manuscript for salient intellectual content and provided suggestions and critical feedback. All authors have made a significant contribution to this manuscript and approved the final version. Ethics of human subject participation: The study was approved by the local research ethics committee (approval number: UFF/CMM/HUAP nº 352/11; CAAE:0369.0.258.258-11).
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980017003263.
References
- 1. Lim SS, Vos T, Flaxman AD et al. (2012) A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 380, 2224–2260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. United Nations General Assembly, Sixty-sixth Session, Agenda item 117 (2012) Political Declaration of the High-level Meeting of the General Assembly on the Prevention and Control of Non-communicable Diseases. http://www.who.int/nmh/events/un_ncd_summit2011/political_declaration_en.pdf (accessed October 2015).
- 3. Spence C, Okajima K, Cheok AD et al. (2016) Eating with our eyes: from visual hunger to digital satiation. Brain Cogn 110, 53–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Rothemund Y, Preuschhof C, Bohner G et al. (2007) Differential activation of the dorsal striatum by high-calorie visual food stimuli in obese individuals. Neuroimage 37, 410–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Killgore WDS, Young AD, Femia LA et al. (2003) Cortical and limbic activation during viewing of high- versus low-calorie foods. Neuroimage 19, 1381–1394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Pursey KM, Stanwell P, Callister RJ et al. (2014) Neural responses to visual food cues according to weight status: a systematic review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Front Nutr 1, 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Telang F et al. (2004) Exposure to appetitive food stimuli markedly activates the human brain. Neuroimage 21, 1790–1797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Michel C, Velasco C, Gatti E et al. (2014) A taste of Kandinsky: assessing the influence of the artistic visual presentation of food on the dining experience. Flavour 3, 7. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Spence C (2015) Multisensory flavor perception. Cell 161, 24–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Jansen A (1998) A learning model of binge eating: cue reactivity and cue exposure. Behav Res Ther 36, 257–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Cohen D & Farley TA (2008) Eating as an automatic behavior. Prev Chronic Dis 5, A23. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Boswell RG & Kober H (2016) Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review. Obes Rev 17, 159–177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Harris JL, Bargh JA & Brownell KD (2009) Priming effect of television food advertising on eating behavior. Health Psychol 28, 404–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Boyland EJ, Harrold JA, Kirkham TC et al. (2011) Food commercials increase preference for energy-dense foods, particularly in children who watch more television. Pediatrics 128, e93–e100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Boyland EJ & Halford JCG (2013) Television advertising and branding. Effects on eating behaviour and food preferences in children. Appetite 62, 236–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Braesicke K, Parkinson JA, Reekie Y et al. (2005) Autonomic arousal in an appetitive context in primates: a behavioural and neural analysis. Eur J Neurosci 21, 1733–1740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Gutjar S, Dalenberg JR, de Graaf C et al. (2015) What reported food-evoked emotions may add: a model to predict consumer food choice. Food Qual Prefer 45, 140–148. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Dalenberg JR, Gutjar S, Ter Horst GJ et al. (2014) Evoked emotions predict food choice. PLoS One 9, e115388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Gutjar S, de Graaf C, Kooijman V et al. (2015) The role of emotions in food choice and liking. Food Res Int 76, 216–223. [Google Scholar]
- 20. Miccoli L, Delgado R, Guerra P et al. (2016) Affective pictures and the Open Library of Affective Foods (OLAF): tools to investigate emotions toward food in adults. PLoS One 11, e0158991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Soussignan R, Schaal B, Rigaud D et al. (2011) Hedonic reactivity to visual and olfactory cues: rapid facial electromyographic reactions are altered in anorexia nervosa. Biol Psychol 86, 265–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Drobes DJ, Miller EJ, Hillman CH et al. (2001) Food deprivation and emotional reactions to food cues: implications for eating disorders. Biol Psychol 57, 153–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Bradley M & Lang PJ (1994) Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 25, 49–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Lang PJ, Bradley MM & Cuthbert BN (2008) International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.
- 25. Paes J, De Oliveira L, Pereira MG et al. (2016) The perception of aversiveness of surgical procedure pictures is modulated by personal/occupational relevance. PLoS One 11, e0160582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Bradley MM, Codispoti M, Sabatinelli D et al. (2001) Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion 1, 276–298. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Greenwald MK, Cook EW III & Lang PJ (1989) Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. J Psychophysiol 3, 51–64. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Miccoli L, Delgado R, Rodriguez-Ruiz S et al. (2014) Meet OLAF, a good friend of the IAPS! The Open Library of Affective Foods: a tool to investigate the emotional impact of food in adolescents. PLoS One 9, e114515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Pan American Health Organization (2015) Ultra-Processed Food and Drink Products in Latin America: Trends, Impact on Obesity, Policy Implications. Washington, DC: PAHO. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Chandon P & Wansink B (2012) Does food marketing need to make us fat? A review and solutions. Nutr Rev 70, 571–593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Moodie R, Stuckler D, Monteiro C et al. (2013) Profits and pandemics: prevention of harmful effects of tobacco, alcohol, and ultra-processed food and drink industries. Lancet 381, 670–679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Monteiro C, Levy R, Claro R et al. (2010) A new classification of foods based on the extent and purpose of their processing. Cad Saude Publica 26, 2039–2049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Monteiro CA, Levy RB, Claro RM et al. (2011) Increasing consumption of ultra-processed foods and likely impact on human health: evidence from Brazil. Public Health Nutr 14, 5–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Martins APB, Levy RB, Claro RM et al. (2013) Increased contribution of ultra-processed food products in the Brazilian diet (1987–2009). Rev Saude Publica 47, 656–665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Monteiro CA, Moubarac JC, Cannon G et al. (2013) Ultra-processed products are becoming dominant in the global food system. Obes Rev 14, 21–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Moubarac JC, Paula A, Martins B et al. (2012) Consumption of ultra-processed foods and likely impact on human health. Evidence from Canada. Public Health Nutr 16, 2240–2248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Steele EM, Baraldi LG, Louzada MLDC et al. (2016) Ultra-processed foods and added sugars in the US diet: evidence from a nationally representative cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 6, e009892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Rothman RL, Housam R, Weiss H et al. (2006) Patient understanding of food labels: the role of literacy and numeracy. Am J Prev Med 31, 391–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Hersey JC, Wohlgenant KC, Arsenault JE et al. (2013) Effects of front-of-package and shelf nutrition labeling systems on consumers. Nutr Rev 71, 1–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Cecchini M & Warin L (2016) Impact of food labelling systems on food choices and eating behaviours: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized studies. Obes Rev 17, 201–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. European Food Information Council (2016) Global update on nutrition labelling. http://www.eufic.org/images/uploads/files/ExecutiveSummary.pdf (accessed November 2017).
- 42. San Francisco City & County of San Francisco (2015) Ordinance amending the Health Code to require advertisements for sugar-sweetened beverages to include a warning about the harmful health effects of consuming such beverages; and authorizing the Director of Health to impose. http://www.sfbos.org/ftp/uploadedfiles/bdsupvrs/ordinances15/o0100-15.pdf (accessed November 2017).
- 43. Office of Environmental Health Hazard Assessment, California Environmental Protection Agency (1986) Proposition 65 in Plain Language. https://oehha.ca.gov/media/downloads/proposition-65/general-info/p65plain.pdf (accessed January 2017).
- 44. Ramirez R, Sternsdorff N & Pastor C (2016) Chile’s Law on Food Labelling and Advertising: A Replicable Model for Latin America? http://www.desarrollando-ideas.com/wp-content/uploads/sites/5/2016/05/160504_DI_report_food_chile_ENG.pdf (accessed November 2017).
- 45. Agência Nacional de Vigilância Sanitária (2010) Resolução ANVISA no 24/2010. Dispõe sobre os critérios para a divulgação de produtos alimentícios. http://portal.anvisa.gov.br/wps/wcm/connect/34565380474597549fd4df3fbc4c6735/RDC24_10_Publicidade+de+alimentos.pdf?MOD=AJPERES (accessed October 2015).
- 46. Moran A, Musicus A, Soo J et al. (2016) Believing that certain foods are addictive is associated with support for obesity-related public policies. Prev Med 90, 39–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. UK Department of Health (2011) Nutrient Profiling Technical Guidance. https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/216094/dh_123492.pdf (accessed November 2017).
- 48. Nascimento BEM, Oliveira L, Vieira a S et al. (2008) Avoidance of smoking: the impact of warning labels in Brazil. Tob Control 17, 405–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Volchan E, David IA, Tavares G et al. (2013) Implicit motivational impact of pictorial health warning on cigarette packs. PLoS One 8, e72117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Ifland JR, Preuss HG, Marcus MT et al. (2009) Refined food addiction: a classic substance use disorder. Med Hypotheses 72, 518–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Khazaal Y, Zullino D & Billieux J (2012) The Geneva Smoking Pictures: development and preliminary validation. Eur Addict Res 18, 103–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Macnamara A, Foti D & Hajcak G (2009) Tell me about it: neural activity elicited by emotional pictures and preceding descriptions. Emotion 9, 531–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Mocaiber I, Pereira MG, Erthal FS et al. (2010) Fact or fiction? An event-related potential study of implicit emotion regulation. Neurosci Lett 476, 84–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Mocaiber I, Sanchez TA, Pereira MG et al. (2011) Antecedent descriptions change brain reactivity to emotional stimuli: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of an extrinsic and incidental reappraisal strategy. Neuroscience 193, 241–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Meule A, Kübler A & Blechert J (2013) Time course of electrocortical food-cue responses during cognitive regulation of craving. Front Psychol 4, 669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Roberto CA, Wong D, Musicus A et al. (2016) The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage health warning labels on parents’ choices. Pediatrics 137, e20153185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. VanEpps EM & Roberto CA (2016) The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage warnings: a randomized trial of adolescents’ choices and beliefs. Am J Prev Med 51, 664–672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Bollard T, Maubach N, Walker N et al. (2016) Effects of plain packaging, warning labels, and taxes on young people’s predicted sugar-sweetened beverage preferences: an experimental study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 13, 95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Bailey RL (2016) Modern foraging: presence of food and energy density influence motivational processing of food advertisements. Appetite 107, 568–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB & Podsakoff NP (2012) Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annu Rev Psychol 63, 539–569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Schillinger D & Jacobson MF (2016) Science and public health on trial: warning notices on advertisements for sugary drinks. JAMA 316, 1545–1546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980017003263.