Abstract
(1) Background: In recent years, placebo and nocebo effects have been extensively documented in different medical conditions, including pain. The scientific literature has provided strong evidence of how the psychosocial context accompanying the treatment administration can influence the therapeutic outcome positively (placebo effects) or negatively (nocebo effects). (2) Methods: This state-of-the-art paper aims to provide an updated overview of placebo and nocebo effects on pain. (3) Results: The most common study designs, the psychological mechanisms, and neurobiological/genetic determinants of these phenomena are discussed, focusing on the differences between positive and negative context effects on pain in experimental settings on healthy volunteers and in clinical settings on chronic pain patients. Finally, the last section describes the implications for clinical and research practice to maximize the medical and scientific routine and correctly interpret the results of research studies on placebo and nocebo effects. (4) Conclusions: While studies on healthy participants seem consistent and provide a clear picture of how the brain reacts to the context, there are no unique results of the occurrence and magnitude of placebo and nocebo effects in chronic pain patients, mainly due to the heterogeneity of pain. This opens up the need for future studies on the topic.
Keywords: placebo effect, nocebo effect, expectation, conditioning, pain, contextual factor
1. Introduction
In recent years, placebo and nocebo effects have strongly influenced pain studies, which fostered the interest in this topic and encouraged debate among scholars, researchers, and clinicians worldwide [1,2,3].
From their earliest days, placebos have been identified as inert substances (e.g., sugar pills, saline injections) used in clinical trials to control the efficacy of new treatments [3]. Today, neuroscientists and clinicians recognize that placebos are more than inert substances, introducing the concept of “context surrounding a treatment” [4]. Accordingly, placebo and nocebo effects are now defined as, respectively, the positive or negative effects due to the administration of a treatment (be it real or simulated) in a therapeutic context [5]. The context that triggers these effects comprises symbols, rituals, and cues (e.g., provider’s words, patient’s expectations and previous experiences, physical aspects of the treatment) that accompany patients during their healthcare experiences [6,7]. In the field of pain, the administration of an inert treatment in a positive context can induce a reduction of pain (as reported by subjective pain reports) as well as a modulation of specific brain circuits involved in pain processing. On the contrary, when an inert treatment is administered in a negative context, participants/patients can experience pain exacerbation as well as increased activation of pain-related brain regions [8]. Similarly, it has been documented that administering treatments without a proper context (e.g., when patients are unaware that a medication/drug has been delivered) strongly reduced the efficacy of the medication [9].
Thus, from this perspective, analyzing how the therapeutic context can influence treatment efficacy represents an opportunity for both clinicians and researchers. This state-of-the-art paper aims to provide an updated overview of placebo and nocebo studies on pain, showing how treatments (active or inert) administered in positive or negative contexts trigger different outcomes. Thus, this paper will serve to help clinicians to be more aware of the use of context in their medical routine. Moreover, it will serve to help researchers to build upon the best evidence for designing future trials and implementing new studies to increase our knowledge on the biological determinants of placebo and nocebo effects on pain. The first section provides the reader with a solid background of the mechanisms and the neurobiological determinants of placebo and nocebo effects on pain. The second section describes the future implications for clinical practice to maximize the medical routine. Moreover, implications for research are discussed to help researchers design future trials and develop new innovative studies on pain.
This state-of-the-art paper has been prepared and developed following methodological guidelines for narrative reviews (Table 1) [10]. The articles included in this state-of-the-art overview needed to (1) be scientific works (experimental studies, systematic or narrative reviews (including meta-analyses), or RTCs) published in peer-reviewed journals; (2) be primarily focused on the analgesia/hyperalgesia manifestation of placebo/nocebo effects and/or on the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms involved; (3) provide significant data for a comprehensive, descriptive, and state-of-the-art overview; and (4) provide a detailed description of the methodological approaches used (only in the case of experimental articles). Additionally, the articles presented in Section 3 needed to focus on chronic pain conditions, specifically. Overall, 80 experimental studies and RCTs on placebo and nocebo effects on healthy volunteers and chronic pain patients have been reviewed. Study characteristics of these experimental studies are summarized in Table A1 (Appendix A), including the sample size, population involved, pain type or pain type induction, investigated outcome, objective measures, and level of significance reported by the authors. Furthermore, 31 reviews and 24 meta-analyses and systematic reviews have been included in order to provide a clear and broad overview of the literature concerning placebo/nocebo phenomena in healthy volunteers and chronic pain patients.
Table 1.
Typology | Details |
---|---|
Sources accessed |
|
Search terms |
|
Limits |
|
Studies included |
|
Steps for writing |
|
Abbreviations: CINAHL, Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature; EMBASE, Excerpta Medica database.
2. State of the Art
2.1. Experimental Approaches to Study Placebo and Nocebo Effects
Placebo and nocebo effects on pain have been extensively studied using experimental research designs [6,11,12,13,14,15]. Different approaches have been used to trigger pain amelioration or exacerbation: the two most common procedures are (1) the use of positive or negative expectations and (2) the use of conditioning approaches [11,12]. In the first case, inert treatments are administered along with verbal information that a real treatment is delivered: using this approach, participants or patients are made to believe that a treatment is administered and a positive or negative effect is expected [16,17,18,19]. In the second case, using conditioning protocols, a real treatment is administered for different trials and subsequently replaced by an inert treatment: using this approach, participants or patients experience a positive effect when the active treatment is administered, and they expect the same effect when the inert treatment is delivered unbeknownst to them [20,21,22]. Studies in healthy volunteers showed that conditioning protocols produce more robust [23,24] and long-lasting placebo effects that cannot be attributable to carryover effects of the active treatment. On the contrary, nocebos seem to result in a great worsening of pain even without a conditioning procedure [25]. Interestingly, the conditioned placebo effect seems to be transferable from one modality (analgesia conditioning) to another (motor performance) [26].
Besides expectation and conditioning studies, context effects have been extensively documented using the so-called “open-hidden” design, in which participants or patients receive a real analgesic drug in two different conditions: in the open condition, they are aware that the drug is administered (presence of the context), in the hidden one, they are unaware of receiving it (absence of the context) [27]. Studies consistently find pain relieving medication of established effectiveness to be significantly more effective when administered in an open fashion as compared to when individuals are unaware of receiving the medication [28]. Thus, the difference between the two conditions shows how exposure to a context influences the effectiveness of a treatment which is in fact proven to be active. Recently, another approach has been used is the open–label nondeceptive approach, whereby participants are informed that an inert treatment will be administered, and that this treatment can be effective [11,12,29,30]. These two approaches (open–hidden and open–label) offer the possibility to study placebo effects in clinical settings without the ethical controversies of deception: indeed, in the first case, a real drug is administered, and the effect of the context is studied without using an inert treatment. In the second case, the use of a placebo is fully disclosed.
2.2. Neurobiology
Over the last few decades, different studies and projects have been conducted, using different approaches ranging from pharmacology to neuroimaging [31,32,33], to describe the brain circuitry and neurotransmitter systems that trigger or block placebo and nocebo effects. The study of the neurobiological determinants of these phenomena is crucial for different reasons: (1) it provides solid knowledge of the objective effects of the context on our brain, (2) it demonstrates that placebo/nocebo and drugs share common biochemical pathways and activate the same receptor pathways, which suggests possible interference between the context and rituals that surround a treatment on the one hand and pharmacological agents on the other. Major studies on healthy participants exposed to experimental pain will be discussed in the next sections. Subsequently, a focus on patients with chronic pain will be presented.
2.2.1. Pharmacological Evidence
Pharmacological studies demonstrated that inert treatments activate the endogenous opioid and endocannabinoid systems (Figure 1A). In these studies, conducted on healthy volunteers, a conditioning protocol was induced, in which opioids (e.g., morphine) or cannabinoids (e.g., ketorolac) were administered and subsequently replaced by a placebo. After morphine administration, µ-opioid antagonists (e.g., naloxone) block placebo analgesia [20,34,35]. The same effect has been discovered using CB1-antagonist (e.g., rimonabant) after cannabinoid administration [36]. Interestingly, naloxone has also been seen to block open–label nondeceptive placebo analgesia, indicating that the same mechanisms may mediate nondeceptive and deceptive placebo analgesia [37]. Indirect confirmations of the involvement of the opioid system have been reported investigating the role of cholecystokinin (CCK), an anti-opioid peptide, and in particular, the role of CCK antagonists (e.g., proglumide) and CCK agonists (e.g., pentagastrin). Proglumide enhances placebo analgesic effects while pentagastrin disrupts them [38,39,40,41,42]. Furthermore, nocebo hyperalgesia seems to be modulated by the activation of the opioid system, as CCK antagonists can reverse it [38]. Scott et al. (2008) [43] found a deactivation of the μ-opioid receptor system during nocebo hyperalgesia (Figure 1B).
Beside opioid and cannabinoid systems, the dopamine system has been explored in this context [32,33]. Some studies indicate that dopamine may be involved in placebo analgesia influencing the activity of pain-related areas, such as the thalamus, insula, anterior cingulate cortex [44,45], and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex [46]. These data are controversial. Indeed, it is likely that dopamine may not be fundamental for placebo analgesia itself [47,48], but it may be more generally involved in placebo responsiveness [46,49]. In particular, dopamine may affect patients’ expectations and desire for improvement [47] and the recalled efficacy of a placebo [46].
Other neurotransmitters, e.g., oxytocin and vasopressin, may be involved in expectancy-induced analgesia [50,51]. Interestingly, the administration of vasopressin has been observed to be associated with increased placebo analgesia, but the effect was restricted to women [50]. The hypothesis behind the involvement of these neurotransmitters takes into account their role in social behavior [52,53], but the results are still preliminary, as other studies do not support the facilitating effect of oxytocin on placebo analgesia [50]. Finally, placebos and nocebos modulate the synthesis of prostaglandins, being important targets of analgesic drugs [54], and the plasma level of pro-inflammatory cytokine (IL-18) during pain experience [55]. It is crucial to consider that the mechanisms addressed above were studied in healthy volunteers exposed to experimental pain protocols. As will be discussed below, fewer studies investigated placebo and nocebo effects in patients with chronic pain, and it has been suggested that the knowledge derived from studies on healthy volunteers may not be entirely transferrable to chronic pain populations [56].
2.2.2. Neuroimaging Studies
Neuroimaging studies have provided crucial insights into how exposure to a context can positively change pain perception at different temporal phases and high and low levels of the central nervous system [57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65].
Temporal Aspects
Considering the temporal aspects, pain can be studied during the expectation phase (e.g., when pain is anticipated) and during the perception phase (e.g., when pain is experienced) (Figure 2). During the expectation phase, activation of the anterior cingulate cortex, precentral and lateral prefrontal cortex, and periaqueductal gray has been documented; during the perception phase, deactivation has been found in different brain regions such as the mid- and posterior cingulate cortex, superior temporal and precentral gyri, the anterior and posterior insula, the claustrum and putamen, and the thalamus and caudate body [66] (Figure 2A). As for nocebo effects, where hyperalgesia is expected, increased activity in different brain regions involved in nociceptive processing and emotion regulation (such as the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex and insula, primary somatosensory cortex, cerebellum, superior temporal gyrus, and operculum) has been documented [67,68,69,70]. During the perception phase, an enhanced activation has been found in regions such as the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, middle frontal gyrus, insula, claustrum, putamen, superior parietal lobule, amygdala, hippocampus, middle temporal gyrus, and periaqueductal gray [71,72] (Figure 2B). These findings concerning the temporal component of pain are confirmed by electroencephalographic (EEG) studies. Interestingly, placebos and nocebos can change EEG brain activity during both the expectation and perception phases [23,73,74]. For example, the expectation of receiving a nonpainful or painful stimulus respectively decreases or increases the amplitude of the contingent negative variation, i.e., an EEG slow negative wave that represents an objective measure of expectation of a specific incoming event (e.g., the expectation of analgesia or hyperalgesia) [23]. Considering the “perception phase”, placebo treatments produce decreased laser-evoked potentials, which represents an early measure of nociceptive processes, since it occurs 200–250 ms after painful stimulation [73]. The source of both these evoked potentials has been evaluated and the supplementary motor area, anterior cingulate cortex, middle cingulate cortex, and insula seem fundamental for contingent negative variation, and anterior cingulate cortex, operculum, and secondary sensorial cortex for laser-evoked potentials [75,76]. Moreover, placebo analgesia treatments significantly reduce the amplitude of the N1, P2, and P3 event-related potential components elicited by painful stimulation [77] (Figure 2C).
Central Nervous System
Placebos and nocebos can affect the activity and the connectivity of cortical, subcortical, and spinal areas (Figure 3).
High Central Nervous System Levels
Starting from the cortical and subcortical levels in placebo expectation studies where inert treatments were delivered along with a verbal suggestion of symptom amelioration, an increase in μ opioid neurotransmission has been observed in different brain areas, such as the pre- and subgenual rostral anterior cingulate cortex [78,79,80,81,82], dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [79,80,81], orbitofrontal cortex [80,82], anterior insular cortex [79,80,81,82], nucleus accumbens [79,81,82], amygdala [79,80,82], thalamus [79,80], and periaqueductal gray [79,82]. On the contrary, when pain exacerbation is expected, a subjective increase in pain ratings has been reported along with increased activity in different brain regions involved in pain processing and emotion regulation, such as the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and insula [70,71,83,84].
Similar results have been observed in open–hidden studies, where the open (placebo) condition, which maximizes the context effects, produced a behavioral analgesic effect along with deactivation of pain matrix areas, such as the mid and posterior cingulate cortex, insula, and thalamus, and activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and rostral anterior cingulate cortex [85]. On the contrary, in the hidden (nocebo) condition, which is a condition that significantly reduces the context effects, no changes in pain perception and no pain matrix deactivation were observed. Interestingly, expectations of drug interruption, e.g., expecting the analgesic effect to end, were followed by a blockage of drug analgesia and enhanced activity in the hippocampus [85].
Among all these areas, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and intraparietal sulcus seem to play a pivotal role in placebo responsiveness [31]. Studies supporting these conclusions are on healthy volunteers and patients with impairment in frontal regions. In Alzheimer’s patients who show compromised frontal lobes, the placebo analgesia negatively correlates with prefrontal activity impairment [86]. In healthy subjects, the prefrontal inactivation with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation results in a blockade of the placebo response [87], while active transcranial direct current stimulation, compared with sham transcranial direct current stimulation, boosts the placebo and blunts the nocebo effects [88]. Frontal activity seems to be crucial for placebo and nocebo responsiveness as researchers found a correlation between frontal activity and placebo effect magnitude; for example, placebo analgesia has been found to correlate with (1) fronto-parietal activity in regions associated with emotion regulation [63], (2) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex connectivity [89,90,91], and (3) opioid binding in the prefrontal cortex [45,65].
Low Central Nervous System Levels
Besides the study of high-level regions, recent studies have shown that placebo analgesia also involves nociception inhibition at the spinal level [92] and modulation of thalamocortical pathways related to nociception and pain [93,94]. At the same time, connectivity studies have documented changes in functional connectivity between precuneus-hippocampus and middle temporal gyrus-postcentral gyrus [95], and between the rostral anterior cingulate cortex and brain stem [63,90,96]. In particular, significant results suggest the involvement of the descending rostral anterior cingulate cortex-periaqueductal gray-rostral ventromedial medulla pain-modulating pathway, which in concert with other brainstem sites, such as the parabrachial nucleus, substantia nigra, and locus coeruleus, can influence the experience of pain by modulating activity at the level of the dorsal horn [97]. Interestingly, reductions in brain activity in areas that are not often considered, such as the habenula and the cerebellum, have been found [98]. Moreover, neural interactions between the prefrontal areas, brainstem, and spinal cord seem to regulate the nocebo effect. In particular, cognition interacts with the pain pathway through the rostral anterior cingulate cortex-periaqueductal gray-spinal axis, influencing nociceptive processing at the spinal level [99]. When nocebo hyperalgesia occurs, functional connectivity changes have been observed among hippocampus-operculum and other brain areas, including the anterior cingulate cortex, insula, primary motor cortex, and primary somatosensory cortex [71]. In addition, a recent study suggests a relevant role of the hippocampus and its functional connectivity with brain regions involved in the processing of sensory-discriminative aspects of pain, such as the periaqueductal gray and amygdala, in nocebo hyperalgesia [100].
Despite placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia interfering in pain perception and changing activity in different areas involved in nociceptive processing, it is still unclear if there is a strong correlation between the magnitude of the subjective placebo analgesia and objective changes in the latter areas. Given that the available literature suggests only a small subjective–objective correlation, it is likely that other mechanisms beyond the bottom-up nociceptive processing are involved in placebo analgesia [101]. Indeed, brain regions that are not associated with nociception but with self-regulation and high-level action selection, particularly the supplementary motor area, exhibit reduced activity during placebo analgesia. These effects may reflect a shift in motivation and decision making in the context of pain [31].
2.3. Genetics
Finally, a crucial and novel aspect of placebo and nocebo responsiveness is related to the role of genetic factors that can substantially contribute to these phenomena. The research in this field is in its early years, but it is plausible that placebo effects are determined by a complex network of genetic factors, individual medical experiences, and environmental factors [102]. The study of polymorphisms associated with placebo responsiveness has been focused on the systems involved in the placebo response, e.g., dopamine, opioid, and endocannabinoid systems [103,104,105]. For example, the polymorphism of the µ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) seems to be involved in the individual differences in placebo responsiveness [105,106]. Due to the high incidence of placebo effects in randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of treatments for mood diseases, an interplay with placebo-effect-related genes may also be present in the serotonergic system [103]. Several genes have been suggested to be involved in the serotoninergic system related to placebo remission [79,102]. Hall and colleagues coined the term “placebome” [103] to define the plausible genetic factors that influence the responsiveness to placebos [107]. The former created a placebome module consisting of 54 proteins and evaluated the proximity of the module to modules related to diseases or symptoms known to have a high or low-to-no placebo response by utilizing a seed connector algorithm. Results showed that the placebome was located proximate to the module for diseases or symptoms known to have a high placebo response and distal to conditions known to have a low-to-no placebo response [104]. It is worth noticing that, despite the role played by genetic factors in placebo responsiveness, results from a recent pilot twin study suggest that individual learning experiences are more important than genetic influences, at least in placebo analgesia induced through a conditioning paradigm [108].
2.4. Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Chronic Pain
The study of placebo and nocebo effects in chronic pain patients is extremely complicated. Patients with chronic pain are usually exposed to different long-lasting painful conditions, generally longer than three months, with different levels of pain experience [109]. Indeed, chronic pain is used as an “umbrella term” that incorporates a wide range of clinical conditions, ranging from fibromyalgia, migraine, musculoskeletal pain, or long-standing pain states with or without actual known causes [109]. Therefore, there are no consistent results for the occurrence and magnitude of placebo analgesia in chronic pain disorders [3]. Different studies report that placebo treatments successfully induce analgesia in chronic pain patients [90,110,111,112], and the effect seems to be stronger in women than in men [113]. RCTs point out that some of the common therapies for low back pain were no better than placebo [114] or only minimally better [115], suggesting that placebo responses can be large and clinically significant [116,117]. Other studies report mixed results. For example, in the meta-analysis of Morozov et al. (2022), placebo demonstrated a significant efficacy on subjective parameters (e.g., visual analogic scale and McGill pain questionnaire) [14]. Generally, a positive patient–clinician communication atmosphere seems a relevant aspect that triggers placebo analgesic effects; for instance, Kaptchuk et al. (2008) compared two placebo acupuncture treatments in patients with irritable bowel syndrome and showed that, while both treatments were superior to a natural history group, the positive therapeutic relationship further increased the efficacy of placebo acupuncture [111].
Overall, even if different studies have confirmed the occurrence of placebo analgesia in patients with chronic pain, it remains unclear if the mechanisms underlying these effects are different or similar to those observed in response to experimental pain protocols in healthy participants [13]. One crucial point is that, due to their personal medical experiences, both populations show completely different pain and treatment efficacy expectations [118,119]. These experiences would likely change the responsiveness to placebo or nocebo contexts. For example, the meta-analysis of Peerdman et al. (2016) indicates that expectations of patients may largely influence experimental and acute pain, whereas they have small effects on chronic pain [120]. Moreover, Muller et al. (2016) observed that, even if placebo analgesia was found to be large for both acute experimental and chronic pain, the two placebo responses were not related [118]. The main role of prior therapeutic experiences is supported by the results of Colloca et al. (2020) that showed a similar placebo analgesia magnitude in both healthy participants and chronic pain patients, which was directly linked to prior therapeutic experiences (conditioning procedure) [121].
Also, from a neurobiological point of view, there seem to be differences between patients and healthy controls in terms of placebo responsiveness, starting with the observation that naloxone appears not to block placebo analgesia in chronic pain states [110,122]. The results suggest that, in chronic pain patients, the opioid system may not be involved in placebo analgesia as in healthy subjects. From one perspective, it is surprising since pharmacological opioids are often used to treat chronic pain [123,124,125], but it is still true that the efficacy of opioids on chronic pain is debated, especially for long-term treatment [126]. A possible explanation for these results lays in the altered functioning of the opioid system as reported in chronic pain animal models [127] and human patients [128,129,130]. Different theories try to explain the persistence of pain in chronic conditions. For example, pain perception can be viewed as an inferential process in which top-down expectations and priors interact with bottom-up sensorial data. After administering a treatment, when bottom-up sensorial data changes, priors can be updated following bottom-up changes or maintained. In the case of chronic pain patients, there could be a bias in the interpretation of bottom-up information along with the use of immunization strategies that prevent the update of priors and expectations [131]. In line with this, chronic pain patients tend to explain ambiguous stimuli as pain- or condition-related without positively updating their previous expectations and cognitions [132,133,134,135]. An inability to update expectations based on outcomes (e.g., when the pain experience is less than anticipated) would result in a system that is poorly attuned to the external environment [135], and patients with chronic pain seem to lack this ability: studies show that patients are less capable of improving their performance on reward-dependent learning tasks [136,137,138,139] and showed an altered loss aversion in a monetary gambling task [140]. In line with this, it is suggested that the reward-related processes in the inability to update expectations are playing a role in the development of prolonged pain [141]. One hypothesis takes into account the possible absence of reward signaling related to endogenous opioid transmission [125], as supported by the studies on the altered opioid system in chronic pain patients [128,129,130].
Beyond the role of the opioid system, differences in the dopamine system, described both in animals and humans with chronic pain [142], may contribute to the development and maintenance of a chronic pain condition [143]. For example, a single-blinded-placebo trial in chronic pain patients showed that placebo responders had higher functional connectivity enriched by the dopamine transporter than nonresponders. This result suggests that those patients with the strongest dopamine-related neurotransmission might benefit the most from expectancy/placebo effects [125].
Differences in placebo responsiveness in chronic pain patients have also been related to other brain structures and function characteristics. In particular, (functional) Magnetic Resonance Imaging ((f)MRI) research demonstrated that subcortical limbic volume asymmetry, sensorimotor cortical thickness, and functional coupling of prefrontal regions, anterior cingulate cortex, and periaqueductal gray were predictive of placebo responses [90]. It is worth noting that these brain traits were present before administrating a placebo treatment, which provides evidence for a placebo responsiveness propensity and, as demonstrated using a machine learning algorithm, a biosignature to predict the placebo response at group level [90,144].
Despite these differences between healthy controls and chronic pain patients, close correspondence in mechanisms underlying placebo responses in these populations has also been found. For example, levels of activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal cortex, as well as the coupling of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and rostral anterior cingulate cortex with antinociceptive circuitry [89,90], are believed to be part of both placebo responses [90].
Overall, it remains debatable whether the mechanisms underlying placebo responses in patients really differ from the ones in healthy controls, as well as whether there are true differences in these mechanisms in response to either acute or chronic pain. However, it seems plausible that the results of placebo research in experimental settings on healthy volunteers may not be totally transferable to placebo responses in chronic pain populations.
3. Future Directions for Clinical Practice
As documented in the previous section, the mechanistic placebo literature suggests that inert interventions provided within a specific context can relieve pain [5]. Translation of these findings into clinical practice requires the acknowledgement that positive clinical outcomes in patients seeking care for different painful conditions (e.g., musculoskeletal pain) are related to many factors [131]. Generally, an intervention’s effectiveness for a given patient may be attributable to a combined effect of: (1) factors such as natural history and regression to the mean: the natural history of many musculoskeletal disorders is favorable, and patients tend to seek care when their symptoms are at their worst, resulting in regression to the mean with repeated assessment over time; (2) the specific effects of the intervention resulting in improved outcomes regardless of the context of administration; and (3) factors related to the context of the intervention such as whether the patient expects the intervention to be effective and the relationship between the patient and the provider [145,146]. Positive and negative contexts influence the effectiveness of all pain management interventions [147,148,149]. For example, contextual effects accounted for more than 75% of the improvements observed in RCTs of interventions for osteoarthritis [150] and following surgical interventions for pain [151]. In patients with painful conditions, individual interventions often fail to show added value when directly compared to other interventions with modest treatment effects at best [152,153]. Observing only small differences in effects across multiple interventions that are different based on their theoretical working mechanisms suggests a significant role for contextual factors that these interventions have in common [131]. For instance, consciously seeking to maximize the contextual effects in clinical practice offers an intriguing opportunity to enhance treatment effects by maximizing the specific mechanisms of interventions as well as the context surrounding intervention administration [6,7].
Previously highlighted factors known to influence placebo analgesia also influence clinical outcomes in patients with different chronic pain conditions. For example, recovery expectations [154,155,156] and the relationship between the patient and provider [157] are known influential factors for the clinical outcome of patients experiencing musculoskeletal pain. Expectations mediating placebo analgesia appear to be depending on social learning [21,22,158,159,160]. Specifically, expectations may be formed and manipulated through verbal instruction, observation, and conditioning [158,160]. Experimental studies suggest that providing a placebo intervention with the following instruction: “the agent you have just received is known to powerfully reduce pain in some patients” [110], having a participant watch someone else experience pain relief in response to a placebo [161], or undergoing a conditioning protocol [162] are all approaches to enhance expectations which can result in increased placebo analgesia. Similar approaches in the clinic, such as educating patients on the effectiveness of a chosen intervention, making patients aware of the provider’s own personal observations of success, the use of patient testimonials, or providing interventions to which a patient has previously had positive experiences, may all be ways to maximize the contextual benefits of interventions for pain through the maximizing of expectations [62,120].
Therapeutic alliance is characterized in psychotherapy as the bond including trust and attachment between the patient and provider and includes consideration of agreement on the goals of therapy and assignment of tasks [163]. The literature on placebos suggests that therapeutic alliance can be enhanced and placebo analgesia increased when a sham intervention is administered by a provider who is warm and friendly, practices active listening, expresses empathy, and expresses confidence in the intervention [111,164,165]. These clinical results support the findings from the literature on experimental placebos [166]. Consequently, outcomes of patients presenting with pain may be improved when a strong therapeutic alliance is established between the patient and the provider [1,2].
In summary, patients with chronic pain may experience improved outcomes in response to an intervention for a variety of reasons beyond the specific effect of the intervention [6,7]. Contextual effects are a component of all interventions for pain that clinicians should implement in their clinical practice (Table 2). The literature on mechanistic placebos provides insight into how these effects can be successfully utilized in clinical practice.
Table 2.
Area | Actions |
---|---|
Clinical Practice |
|
Research |
|
4. Future Directions for Research and Clinical Trials
High-quality RCTs are the gold standard for treatment effectiveness. The traditional interpretation of null findings in placebo RCTs is considering the experimental intervention as ineffective. Specific to pain as an outcome, this assumption neglects the potential analgesic response to a placebo [145,146]. Consequently, a studied intervention providing no greater pain relief than a placebo comparator may suggest two equally effective interventions, potentially with differing mechanisms behind their effectiveness [167]. Different factors need to be considered for designing and interpreting placebo-controlled studies on interventions for pain [145,146]. The blinding of both patients and providers is an important consideration in placebo-controlled trials given that participants are made aware during the consent process of a 50% chance of receiving a placebo [146]. Blinding may be compromised due to poorly designed placebos which are not credible. Furthermore, blinding may be lost in placebo-controlled medication studies due to sensations unique to the studied intervention [168] or side effects in the active arm [169]. Based on a literature review of sham-controlled trials concerning back pain interventions, it appeared that a higher percentage of participants in active trial arms correctly identified their intervention, e.g., active and not sham, while blinding was successful in the sham arms of the studies [170]. Importantly, larger treatment effect sizes were observed in response to both the studied intervention and sham intervention when participants believed they received the active intervention [170]. Therefore, blinding should be carefully considered in placebo-controlled trials of pain management interventions and care should be taken to design sham or placebo comparators which are effective in maintaining blinding. Furthermore, blinding success should be assessed and reported in such trials [145,146].
Moreover, expectations are a primary mechanism of placebo analgesia [147]. Discrepancies between participant expectations concerning the success of a provided intervention between the active and placebo arms of a study could influence the observed outcomes [171]. Consequently, when designing placebo comparators for interventions for pain, care should be taken to assess expectations and ensure that the expectations for each arm of the study are similar [160].
Then, the true effect size of contextual effects on clinical outcomes requires additional consideration beyond the traditional two-arm placebo RCT. First, attributing changes in outcomes in a placebo treatment arm of a study to the placebo effect is temping; however, such an approach can be misleading [145,146]. Changes in the placebo arm should be considered as the placebo response; however, accurately measuring the placebo effect requires a no-treatment control group to account for influences such as natural history and regression to the mean [8].
Participants in an RCT are aware through the consent process of having a 50% chance of receiving a placebo. Consequently, individuals volunteering to participate in an RCT may differ from those presenting for clinical care, where expectations for improvement tend to be high [172,173]. Placebo mechanism studies differ from placebo-controlled studies given that participants are provided a placebo but instructed that they are receiving an effective intervention [147,148]. This study design is more consistent with clinical care in which interventions are generally provided by enthusiastic practitioners who instruct the patient of the likely effectiveness of the chosen intervention [147,148]. Placebo responses are greater in placebo mechanism studies than in placebo control studies [147] and similar approaches may result in a more accurate representation of the magnitude of contextual effect sizes in clinical practice. Furthermore, placebo-controlled studies may underestimate the effect of interventions. A literature review of studies on antidepressants observed significantly greater responses to treatment in terms of depression in studies with active comparators as compared to placebo-controlled studies [174]. Participants in studies with an active comparator were twice as likely to respond and one and a half times as likely to experience remission compared to participants in a traditional placebo-controlled study on antidepressants [174]. Such findings may be attributable to the expectations of participants in the active arm of the placebo-controlled studies who are also aware of the possibility that their intervention is a placebo [171]. Collectively, these findings suggest RCTs may underestimate both the placebo and treatment effects due to differences in expectations from those observed in clinical care [171]. Carefully designed studies may be necessary to account for the true magnitude of the influence of these factors on outcomes and provide a more accurate indication of their role in the effectiveness of interventions, offering opportunity for future research (Table 2).
5. Conclusions and Limitations
In summary, while studies on healthy participants seem consistent and provide a clear picture of how the brain reacts to different contexts at biological, neurophysiological, and genetical levels, there are no consistent results for the occurrence and magnitude of placebo and nocebo effects in chronic pain patients, mainly due to the heterogeneity of painful conditions. Thus, while it is a common experience that the same therapy offered in different contexts may influence the patient’s outcome in care settings representing an opportunity for clinicians, future studies on placebo and nocebo effects on patients with chronic pain are urgently needed, calling researchers and trialists to action worldwide.
This state-of-the-art paper presents some limitations. First, given that this paper comprises a narrative overview of the current state of the art, the included studies and data were not selected by adopting a systematic review approach. However, recommendations for performing a narrative biomedical review have been followed [10]. Second, the paper is mainly focused on the neurobiological and clinical aspects of placebo and nocebo effects, without describing the psychological mechanisms and determinants of these phenomena in detail. Third, the paper is limited to the specific topic of pain, even if it is well documented that there is not one sole placebo/nocebo effect, and instead many effects are mediated by a variety of psychological and biological mechanisms.
Abbreviations
RCT | Randomized Controlled Trial |
fMRI | functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging |
PET | Positron Emission Tomography |
EEG | electroencephalography |
VAS | Visual Analogue Scale |
NRS | Numerical Rating Scale |
CI | Confidence Interval |
rTMS | repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation |
ROI | Regions of Interest |
M | Male |
F | Female |
Appendix A
Table A1.
Paper ID | Sample Size (M, F, Not Analysed) * |
Population Type | Pain Type/Pain Induction | Investigated Outcome | Outcome Measure | Level of Significance |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Amanzio and Benedetti, 1999 [20] | 229 (132, 97) | Healthy subjects | Experimental ischemic pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | Pain tolerance (min) | p < 0.05 |
Amanzio et al., 2001 [21] | 364 (278 patients; 86 healthy controls) |
Patients (thoracic surgery) and healthy controls | Postoperative pain; experimental ischemic arm pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Benedetti et al., 1995 [40] | 93 (52, 41) | Patients (thoracotomy for lung surgery) | Post-surgery pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.02 |
Benedetti et al., 1996 [39] | 340 (154, 186) | Healthy subjects | Experimental ischemic pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) |
NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Benedetti et al., 1997 [38] | 180 (119, 61) | Patients (video-assisted thoracoscopy) | Post-surgery pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Benedetti et al., 2006 [42] | 49 (23, 26) | Healthy subjects | Experimental ischemic pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Benedetti et al., 2006 [86] | 44 (28 patients (11, 17), 16 controls) | Patients (Alzheimer’s disease) and healthy subjects | Burning pain after venipuncture | Electrophysiological (EEG) | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Benedetti et al., 2010 [41] | 40 (20, 20) | Healthy subjects | Experimental ischemic pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | Tolerance time | p < 0.05 |
Benedetti et al., 2011 [36] | 82 (41, 41) | Healthy subjects | Experimental ischemic pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | Tolerance time | 95%CI |
Benedetti et al., 2014 [54] | 74 (30, 44) | Healthy subjects | Hypobaric hypoxia headache | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | NRS (0–10) | 95%CI |
Benedetti et al., 2022 [37] | 149 (82, 67) | Healthy subjects | Experimental ischemic pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | 0–10 rating scale | p < 0.05 |
Bingel et al. 2011 [85] | 22 (15, 7) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Bingel et al., 2022 [100] | 22 (15, 7) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging; functional connectivity (fMRI) |
VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Bush et al., 2021 [95] | 37 (12, 25) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging; functional connectivity (fMRI) |
VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Camerone et al., 2021 [16] | 166 (78, 88, 9) | Healthy subjects | Electrical stimuli | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Camerone et al., 2021 [17] | 77 (24, 24, 29) | Healthy subjects | Cold pressor test (CPT) | Behavioral | Numerical Pain Intensity (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Camerone et al., 2022 [18] | 51 (24, 27, 10) | Healthy subjects | Cold pressor test (CPT) | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Carlino et al., 2015 [73] | 34 (20, 14) | Healthy subjects | Laser stimulation | Electrophysiology (EEG) | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Carlino et al., 2016 [26] | 80 (34, 46) | Healthy subjects | Electrical stimuli | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Colloca et al., 2006 [24] | 30 (5, 25) | Healthy subjects | Electrical stimuli | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Colloca et al., 2008 [21] | 116 (0, 116) | Healthy subjects | Electrical stimuli | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Colloca et al., 2010 [25] | 46 (16, 30) | Healthy subjects | Electrical stimuli | Behavioral | VAS (0–10 | p < 0.05 |
Colloca et al., 2016 [50] | 109 (55, 54, 1) | Healthy subjects | Electrical stimuli | Behavioral | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Colloca et al., 2019 [105] | 160 (58, 102) | Healthy subjects | Electrical and heat stimuli | DNA genotyping; epistasis | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.001 |
Colloca et al., 2020 [121] | 763 (363 patients (85, 278); 400 healthy controls (162; 238) | Patients (chronic orofacial pain) and healthy subjects | Heat stimuli | Behavioral | VAS | p < 0.05 |
Disley et al., 2021 [30] | 104 (10, 65, 29) | Healthy subjects | Cold pressor test (CPT) | Behavioral | VAS (0–100) | p = 0.05 |
Eippert et al., 2009 [34] | 48 (48, -, 8) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p ≤ 0.05 |
Eippert et al., 2009 [92] | 15 (15, 0) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Ellerbrock et al., 2015 [35] | 40 (20, 20, 1) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging; functional connectivity (fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Fuentes et al., 2014 [164] | 117 | Patients (chronic low back pain) | - | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Hashmi et al., 2014 [91] | 42 | Patients (chronic knee osteoarthritis) | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | Gracely Sensory Scale (0–20) | p < 0.05 |
Jarcho et al., 2016 [46] | 15 (0, 15) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (PET; fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.005 |
Kaptchuk et al., 2008 [111] | 262 (63, 199) | Patients (irritable bowel syndrome) | - | Behavioral | Global improvement scale (range 1–7); adequate relief of symptoms; symptom severity | p < 0.01 |
Kelley et al., 2009 [165] | 189 | Patients (irritable bowel syndrome) | - | Behavioral | Combined outcome (IBS Symptom Severity Scale; IBS Quality of Like Scale; IBS Global Improvement Scale; IBS Adequate Relief) | p < 0.05 |
Kessner et al., 2013 [51] | 80 (80, 0) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Behavioral (Pharmacological) |
Visual Analogue Scale (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Klinger et al., 2017 [112] | 48 (12, 36) | Patients (chronic back pain) | Electrical stimuli | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | 95% CI |
Kong et al., 2006 [57] | 24 (13, 11) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | 0–20 Sensory Box Scale |
p < 0.0001 for ROI p = 0.05 |
Kong et al., 2008 [71] | 20 (5, 8, 7) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | Gracely Sensory and Affective Scales | p < 0.05 |
Koyama et al., 2005 [70] | 10 (8, 2) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS | p < 0.01 |
Krummenacher et al., 2010 [87] | 40 (40, 0) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | rTMS | VAS (0–10) | p ≤ 0.05 |
Kube et al., 2020 [29] | 117 (48, 53, 16) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Behavioral | Pain tolerance | p < 0.05 |
Lieberman et al., 2004 [58] | 52 (29 active drug; 23 placebo condition) | Patients (irritable bowel syndrome) | - | Neuroimaging (PET) | Symptom diary (4 weeks) | p < 0.005 |
Malfiet et al., 2019 [79] | 83 | Patients (chronic neck pain) | - | Behavioral | VAS (0–100) | p = 0.05 |
Martins et al., 2022 [125] | 56 | Patients (chronic knee osteoarthritis) | - | Neuroimaging; functional connectivity (fMRI) | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Morton et al., 2010 [74] | 67 (21, 35, 11) | Healthy subjects | Laser stimulation | Electrophysiological (EEG) | 0–10 scale | p = 0.05 |
Müller et al., 2016 [118] | 50 (27, 32, 1) | Patients (chronic pain) | Pressure-pain stimuli | Behavioral | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Olson et al., 2021 [113] | 280 (65, 215) | Patients (chronic orofacial pain) | Heat pain | Behavioral | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Peciña et al., 2015 [106] | 50 (21, 29) | Healthy subjects | 5% hypertonic saline | DNA genotyping; Neuroimaging (PET) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Petrovic et al., 2002 [60] | 9 | Healthy subjects | Heat stimuli | Neuroimaging (PET) | VAS (0–100) | p = 0.005 |
Petrovic et al., 2010 [59] | 24 (9, 15) | Healthy subjects | Heat stimuli | Neuroimaging (PET; fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Piedimonte et al., 2017 [23] | 34(16, 18, -) | Healthy subject | Electrical stimuli | Electrophysiological (EEG) | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Ploghaus et al., 1999 [67] | 12 (7, 5) | Healthy subjects | Heat stimuli | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Pollo et al., 2001 [81] | 38 | Patients (thoracotomized patients) | - | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.01 |
Porro et al., 2002 [69] | 30 (10, 16, 4) | Healthy subjects | Acid solution injection | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | 0–100 scale rating | p < 0.05 |
Price et al., 1999 [162] | 40 (16, 24) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Behavioral | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Price et al., 2007 [61] | 9 | Patients (irritable bowel syndrome) | Barostat balloon distension—pressure stimuli | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | 100-unit rating scale | p < 0.05 |
Prossin et al., 2022 [55] | 37 (12, 25) | Healthy subjects | Hypertonic saline injection | Neuroimaging (PET, MRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Rief et al., 2012 [168] | 144 (50, 904) | Healthy participants | Heat pain | Behavioral | Pain threshold change in °C | p < 0.05 |
Ruscheweyh et al., 2014 [98] | 60 (30 patients, 30 controls) | Patients (cerebellum infarction) and healthy subjects | Heat; pressure; pinprick pain | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Sawamoto et al., 2000 [83] | 10 (10, 0) | Healthy subjects | Laser thermal stimulation | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | 0–100 scale | p < 0.05 |
Schmid et al., 2015 [84] | 44 (22, 22) | Healthy subjects | Rectal distension | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Schwartz et al., 2022 [161] | 44 (18, 26) | Patients (chronic low back pain) | - | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Scott et al., 2007 [49] | 48 (30 Study1; 16 Study2; 18 Male controls) | Healthy subjects | 5% hypertonic saline injection | Neuroimaging (Study1—PET, fMRI Study2—fMRI) |
VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Scott et al., 2008 [43] | 20 (9, 11); 18 (18, 0) | Healthy subjects | Sustained muscle pain challenge | Neuroimaging (PET, MRI) | VAS (0–100) |
p < 0.0001 for ROI p = 0.05 |
Skyt et al., 2018 [47] | 19 (10, 9) | Patients (neuropathic pain) | Pinprick-evoked pain; wind-up-like pain | Behavioral | VAS (0–10; 0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Tétreault et al., 2016 [89] | 98 (17 Study1; 39 Study2; 42 Study3) | Patients (chronic knee osteoarthritis pain) | - | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS (0–10); Western Ontario and McMaster Universities Osteoarthritis Index | p < 0.05 |
Tinnermann et al., 2017 [99] | 57 (27, 22, 8) | Healthy subjects | Heat stimuli | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Tu et al., 2021 [88] | 81 (44, 37) | Healthy subjects | Heat stimuli | Neuroimaging (fMRI); tDCS | Gracely Sensory Scale (0–20) | p < 0.05 |
Vachon-Presseau et al., 2018 [90] | 129 (43 placebo group, 20 controls, 66 excluded) | Patients (chronic back pain) | Back pain intensity | Neuroimaging (MRI, fMRI) | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Vachon-Presseau et al., 2022 [144] | 181 (94 randomized to 3 arms, 87 excluded) | Patients (chronic low back pain) | Back pain intensity | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | Likert Scale (twice a day) | p < 0.05 |
Van der Meulen et al., 2017 [72] | 30 (13, 17) | Healthy subjects | Heat stimuli | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
Vase et al., 2003 [82] | 13 | Patients (irritable bowel syndrome) | Evoked rectal distension; heat pain | Behavioral | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Vase et al., 2005 [110] | 26 (0, 26) | Patients (irritable bowel syndrome) | Rectal distension | Behavioral (Pharmacological) | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Vecchio et al., 2021 [77] | 63 (31, 32) | Healthy subjects | Electrical stimuli | Electrophysiological (EEG) | 7 point Likert scale | p = 0.05 |
Wager et al., 2004 [64] | 47 | Healthy subjects | Shock pain; heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | 10 point scale | p < 0.05 |
Wager et al., 2007 [65] | 15 (15, 0) | Healthy subjects | Heat stimuli | Neuroimaging (PET) | VAS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Wager et al., 2011 [63] | 47 | Healthy subjects | Shock pain; heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI) | 10 point scale | p < 0.001 |
Wanigasekera et al., 2018 [96] | 16 | Patients (Post-traumatic neuropathic pain) | - | Neuroimaging (MRI) | NRS (0–10) | p = 0.05 |
Weimer et al., 2019 [108] | 39 (25 monozygotic; 14 dizygotic twin pairs) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Behavioral | NRS (0–10) | p < 0.05 |
Wrobel et al., 2014 [48] | 50 (28, 32, 12) | Healthy subjects | Heat pain | Neuroimaging (fMRI); Pharmacological | VAS (0–100) | p < 0.05 |
* If not differently specified.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, G.R. and E.C.; writing—original draft preparation, G.R., F.C., J.B., L.V. and E.C.; writing—review and editing, G.R., F.C., J.B., E.H., L.V. and E.C.; visualization, G.R., F.C., J.B., E.H., L.V. and E.C.; supervision, E.C.; project administration, E.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Institutional Review Board Statement
Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement
Being a state of the art review, this study does not contain original data.
Conflicts of Interest
GR leads educational programs on placebo, nocebo effects, and contextual factors in healthcare to under- and postgraduate students along with private CPD courses. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflicts of interest.
Funding Statement
This research received no external funding.
Footnotes
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
References
- 1.Hohenschurz-Schmidt D., Thomson O.P., Rossettini G., Miciak M., Newell D., Roberts L., Vase L., Draper-Rodi J. Avoiding nocebo and other undesirable effects in chiropractic, osteopathy and physiotherapy: An invitation to reflect. Musculoskelet. Sci. Pract. 2022;62:102677. doi: 10.1016/j.msksp.2022.102677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Palese A., Rossettini G., Colloca L., Testa M. The impact of contextual factors on nursing outcomes and the role of placebo/nocebo effects: A discussion paper. Pain Rep. 2019;4:e716. doi: 10.1097/PR9.0000000000000716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Colloca L. Placebo effects in pain. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2020;153:167–185. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2020.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ongaro G., Kaptchuk T.J. Symptom perception, placebo effects, and the Bayesian brain. Pain. 2019;160:1–4. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Benedetti F., Frisaldi E., Shaibani A. Thirty Years of Neuroscientific Investigation of Placebo and Nocebo: The Interesting, the Good, and the Bad. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 2022;62:323–340. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-052120-104536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Carlino E., Frisaldi E., Benedetti F. Pain and the context. Nat. Rev. Rheumatol. 2014;10:348–355. doi: 10.1038/nrrheum.2014.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Carlino E., Benedetti F. Different contexts, different pains, different experiences. Neuroscience. 2016;338:19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Benedetti F. Placebo and the new physiology of the doctor-patient relationship. Physiol. Rev. 2013;93:1207–1246. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00043.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Colloca L., Lopiano L., Lanotte M., Benedetti F. Overt versus covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:679–684. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00908-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gasparyan A.Y., Ayvazyan L., Blackmore H., Kitas G.D. Writing a narrative biomedical review: Considerations for authors, peer reviewers, and editors. Rheumatol. Int. 2011;31:1409–1417. doi: 10.1007/s00296-011-1999-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Benedetti F., Piedimonte A. The neurobiological underpinnings of placebo and nocebo effects. Semin. Arthritis Rheum. 2019;49:S18–S21. doi: 10.1016/j.semarthrit.2019.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Carlino E., Piedimonte A., Benedetti F. Chapter 48—Nature of the placebo and nocebo effect in relation to functional neurologic disorders. In: Hallett M., Stone J., Carson A., editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Volume 139. Elsevier; Amsterdam, The Netherlands: 2016. pp. 597–606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Klinger R., Stuhlreyer J., Schwartz M., Schmitz J., Colloca L. Clinical Use of Placebo Effects in Patients with Pain Disorders. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2018;139:107–128. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2018.07.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Morozov A., Bazarkin A., Babaevskaya D., Taratkin M., Kozlov V., Suvorov A., Spivak L., McFarland J., Russo G.I., Enikeev D. A systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo effect in clinical trials on chronic prostatitis/chronic pelvic pain syndrome. Prostate. 2022;82:633–656. doi: 10.1002/pros.24311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Colagiuri B., Schenk L.A., Kessler M.D., Dorsey S.G., Colloca L. The placebo effect: From concepts to genes. Neuroscience. 2015;307:171–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Camerone E.M., Piedimonte A., Testa M., Wiech K., Vase L., Zamfira D.A., Benedetti F., Carlino E. The Effect of Temporal Information on Placebo Analgesia and Nocebo Hyperalgesia. Psychosom. Med. 2021;83:43–50. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Camerone E.M., Wiech K., Benedetti F., Carlino E., Job M., Scafoglieri A., Testa M. ‘External timing’ of placebo analgesia in an experimental model of sustained pain. Eur. J. Pain. 2021;25:1303–1315. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Camerone E.M., Battista S., Benedetti F., Carlino E., Sansone L.G., Buzzatti L., Scafoglieri A., Testa M. The Temporal Modulation of Nocebo Hyperalgesia in a Model of Sustained Pain. Front. Psychiatry. 2022;13:807138. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.807138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rosenkjær S., Lunde S.J., Kirsch I., Vase L. Expectations: How and when do they contribute to placebo analgesia? Front. Psychiatry. 2022;13:817179. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.817179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Amanzio M., Benedetti F. Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: Expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:484–494. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00484.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Colloca L., Sigaudo M., Benedetti F. The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects. PAIN®. 2008;136:211–218. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Colloca L., Miller F.G. How placebo responses are formed: A learning perspective. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2011;366:1859–1869. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Piedimonte A., Guerra G., Vighetti S., Carlino E. Measuring expectation of pain: Contingent negative variation in placebo and nocebo effects. Eur. J. Pain. 2017;21:874–885. doi: 10.1002/ejp.990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Colloca L., Benedetti F. How prior experience shapes placebo analgesia. Pain. 2006;124:126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Colloca L., Petrovic P., Wager T.D., Ingvar M., Benedetti F. How the number of learning trials affects placebo and nocebo responses. Pain. 2010;151:430–439. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Carlino E., Guerra G., Piedimonte A. Placebo effects: From pain to motor performance. Neurosci. Lett. 2016;632:224–230. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.08.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Benedetti F., Carlino E., Pollo A. Hidden administration of drugs. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 2011;90:651–661. doi: 10.1038/clpt.2011.206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Amanzio M., Pollo A., Maggi G., Benedetti F. Response variability to analgesics: A role for non-specific activation of endogenous opioids. Pain. 2001;90:205–215. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00486-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kube T., Rief W., Vivell M.B., Schäfer N.L., Vermillion T., Körfer K., Glombiewski J.A. Deceptive and Nondeceptive Placebos to Reduce Pain: An Experimental Study in Healthy Individuals. Clin. J. Pain. 2020;36:68–79. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Disley N., Kola-Palmer S., Retzler C. A comparison of open-label and deceptive placebo analgesia in a healthy sample. J. Psychosom. Res. 2021;140:110298. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2020.110298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zunhammer M., Spisák T., Wager T.D., Bingel U. Meta-analysis of neural systems underlying placebo analgesia from individual participant fMRI data. Nat. Commun. 2021;12:1391. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-21179-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Frisaldi E., Shaibani A., Benedetti F. Understanding the mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects. Swiss Med. Wkly. 2020;150:w20340. doi: 10.4414/smw.2020.20340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ortega Á., Salazar J., Galban N., Rojas M., Ariza D., Chávez-Castillo M., Nava M., Riaño-Garzón M.E., Díaz-Camargo E.A., Medina-Ortiz O., et al. Psycho-Neuro-Endocrine-Immunological Basis of the Placebo Effect: Potential Applications beyond Pain Therapy. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2022;23:4196. doi: 10.3390/ijms23084196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Eippert F., Bingel U., Schoell E.D., Yacubian J., Klinger R., Lorenz J., Büchel C. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron. 2009;63:533–543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ellerbrock I., Wiehler A., Arndt M., May A. Nocebo context modulates long-term habituation to heat pain and influences functional connectivity of the operculum. Pain. 2015;156:2222–2233. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Benedetti F., Amanzio M., Rosato R., Blanchard C. Nonopioid placebo analgesia is mediated by CB1 cannabinoid receptors. Nat. Med. 2011;17:1228–1230. doi: 10.1038/nm.2435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Benedetti F., Shaibani A., Arduino C., Thoen W. Open-label nondeceptive placebo analgesia is blocked by the opioid antagonist naloxone. Pain. 2022;164:984–990. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Benedetti F., Amanzio M., Casadio C., Oliaro A., Maggi G. Blockade of nocebo hyperalgesia by the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide. Pain. 1997;71:135–140. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)03346-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Benedetti F. The opposite effects of the opiate antagonist naloxone and the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide on placebo analgesia. Pain. 1996;64:535–543. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00179-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Benedetti F., Amanzio M., Maggi G. Potentiation of placebo analgesia by proglumide. Lancet. 1995;346:1231. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(95)92938-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Benedetti F., Amanzio M., Thoen W. Disruption of opioid-induced placebo responses by activation of cholecystokinin type-2 receptors. Psychopharmacology. 2011;213:791–797. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2037-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Benedetti F., Amanzio M., Vighetti S., Asteggiano G. The biochemical and neuroendocrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effect. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:12014–12022. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2947-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Scott D.J., Stohler C.S., Egnatuk C.M., Wang H., Koeppe R.A., Zubieta J.K. Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2008;65:220–231. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Irizarry K.J., Licinio J. An explanation for the placebo effect? Science. 2005;307:1411–1412. doi: 10.1126/science.307.5714.1411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Peciña M., Zubieta J.K. Molecular mechanisms of placebo responses in humans. Mol. Psychiatry. 2015;20:416–423. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Jarcho J.M., Feier N.A., Labus J.S., Naliboff B., Smith S.R., Hong J.Y., Colloca L., Tillisch K., Mandelkern M.A., Mayer E.A., et al. Placebo analgesia: Self-report measures and preliminary evidence of cortical dopamine release associated with placebo response. NeuroImage Clin. 2016;10:107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Skyt I., Moslemi K., Baastrup C., Grosen K., Benedetti F., Petersen G.L., Price D.D., Hall K.T., Kaptchuk T.J., Svensson P., et al. Dopaminergic tone does not influence pain levels during placebo interventions in patients with chronic neuropathic pain. Pain. 2018;159:261–272. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Wrobel N., Wiech K., Forkmann K., Ritter C., Bingel U. Haloperidol blocks dorsal striatum activity but not analgesia in a placebo paradigm. Cortex. 2014;57:60–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Scott D.J., Stohler C.S., Egnatuk C.M., Wang H., Koeppe R.A., Zubieta J.K. Individual differences in reward responding explain placebo-induced expectations and effects. Neuron. 2007;55:325–336. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Colloca L., Pine D.S., Ernst M., Miller F.G., Grillon C. Vasopressin Boosts Placebo Analgesic Effects in Women: A Randomized Trial. Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;79:794–802. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kessner S., Sprenger C., Wrobel N., Wiech K., Bingel U. Effect of oxytocin on placebo analgesia: A randomized study. JAMA. 2013;310:1733–1735. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.277446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Feng C., Hackett P.D., DeMarco A.C., Chen X., Stair S., Haroon E., Ditzen B., Pagnoni G., Rilling J.K. Oxytocin and vasopressin effects on the neural response to social cooperation are modulated by sex in humans. Brain Imaging Behav. 2015;9:754–764. doi: 10.1007/s11682-014-9333-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Thompson R., Gupta S., Miller K., Mills S., Orr S. The effects of vasopressin on human facial responses related to social communication. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:35–48. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00133-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Benedetti F., Durando J., Vighetti S. Nocebo and placebo modulation of hypobaric hypoxia headache involves the cyclooxygenase-prostaglandins pathway. Pain. 2014;155:921–928. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Prossin A., Koch A., Campbell P., Laumet G., Stohler C.S., Dantzer R., Zubieta J.K. Effects of placebo administration on immune mechanisms and relationships with central endogenous opioid neurotransmission. Mol. Psychiatry. 2022;27:831–839. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01365-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Skyt I., Lunde S.J., Baastrup C., Svensson P., Jensen T.S., Vase L. Neurotransmitter systems involved in placebo and nocebo effects in healthy participants and patients with chronic pain: A systematic review. Pain. 2020;161:11–23. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Kong J., Gollub R.L., Rosman I.S., Webb J.M., Vangel M.G., Kirsch I., Kaptchuk T.J. Brain activity associated with expectancy-enhanced placebo analgesia as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:381–388. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3556-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lieberman M.D., Jarcho J.M., Berman S., Naliboff B.D., Suyenobu B.Y., Mandelkern M., Mayer E.A. The neural correlates of placebo effects: A disruption account. NeuroImage. 2004;22:447–455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Petrovic P., Kalso E., Petersson K.M., Andersson J., Fransson P., Ingvar M. A prefrontal non-opioid mechanism in placebo analgesia. Pain. 2010;150:59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Petrovic P., Kalso E., Petersson K.M., Ingvar M. Placebo and opioid analgesia—Imaging a shared neuronal network. Science. 2002;295:1737–1740. doi: 10.1126/science.1067176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Price D.D., Craggs J., Verne G.N., Perlstein W.M., Robinson M.E. Placebo analgesia is accompanied by large reductions in pain-related brain activity in irritable bowel syndrome patients. Pain. 2007;127:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Tracey I. Getting the pain you expect: Mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans. Nat. Med. 2010;16:1277–1283. doi: 10.1038/nm.2229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wager T.D., Atlas L.Y., Leotti L.A., Rilling J.K. Predicting individual differences in placebo analgesia: Contributions of brain activity during anticipation and pain experience. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:439–452. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3420-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Wager T.D., Rilling J.K., Smith E.E., Sokolik A., Casey K.L., Davidson R.J., Kosslyn S.M., Rose R.M., Cohen J.D. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science. 2004;303:1162–1167. doi: 10.1126/science.1093065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Wager T.D., Scott D.J., Zubieta J.K. Placebo effects on human mu-opioid activity during pain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:11056–11061. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702413104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Palermo S., Benedetti F., Costa T., Amanzio M. Pain anticipation: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:1648–1661. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Ploghaus A., Tracey I., Gati J.S., Clare S., Menon R.S., Matthews P.M., Rawlins J.N. Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science. 1999;284:1979–1981. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5422.1979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Koyama T., Tanaka Y.Z., Mikami A. Nociceptive neurons in the macaque anterior cingulate activate during anticipation of pain. NeuroReport. 1998;9:2663–2667. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199808030-00044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Porro C.A., Baraldi P., Pagnoni G., Serafini M., Facchin P., Maieron M., Nichelli P. Does anticipation of pain affect cortical nociceptive systems? J. Neurosci. 2002;22:3206–3214. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-08-03206.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Koyama T., McHaffie J.G., Laurienti P.J., Coghill R.C. The subjective experience of pain: Where expectations be come reality. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:12950–12955. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408576102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Kong J., Gollub R.L., Polich G., Kirsch I., Laviolette P., Vangel M., Rosen B., Kaptchuk T.J. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study on the neural mechanisms of hyperalgesic nocebo effect. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:13354–13362. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2944-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Van der Meulen M., Kamping S., Anton F. The role of cognitive reappraisal in placebo analgesia: An fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017;12:1128–1137. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Carlino E., Torta D.M., Piedimonte A., Frisaldi E., Vighetti S., Benedetti F. Role of explicit verbal information in conditioned analgesia. Eur. J. Pain. 2015;19:546–553. doi: 10.1002/ejp.579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Morton D.L., Brown C.A., Watson A., El-Deredy W., Jones A.K. Cognitive changes as a result of a single exposure to placebo. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1958–1964. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Nagai Y., Critchley H.D., Featherstone E., Fenwick P.B., Trimble M.R., Dolan R.J. Brain activity relating to the contingent negative variation: An fMRI investigation. NeuroImage. 2004;21:1232–1241. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Garcia-Larrea L., Frot M., Valeriani M. Brain generators of laser-evoked potentials: From dipoles to functional significance. Neurophysiol. Clin. 2003;33:279–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2003.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Vecchio A., De Pascalis V. ERP Indicators of Self-Pain and Other Pain Reductions due to Placebo Analgesia Responding: The Moderating Role of the Fight-Flight-Freeze System. Brain Sci. 2021;11:1192. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11091192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Linde K., Atmann O., Meissner K., Schneider A., Meister R., Kriston L., Werner C. How often do general practitioners use placebos and non-specific interventions? Systematic review and meta-analysis of surveys. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0202211. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Malfliet A., Lluch Girbés E., Pecos-Martin D., Gallego-Izquierdo T., Valera-Calero A. The Influence of Treatment Expectations on Clinical Outcomes and Cortisol Levels in Patients With Chronic Neck Pain: An Experimental Study. Pain Pract. 2019;19:370–381. doi: 10.1111/papr.12749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Pavlov I., Thompson W. The Work of the Digestive Glands. C. Griffin; London, UK: 1902. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Pollo A., Amanzio M., Arslanian A., Casadio C., Maggi G., Benedetti F. Response expectancies in placebo analgesia and their clinical relevance. Pain. 2001;93:77–84. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00296-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Vase L., Robinson M.E., Verne G.N., Price D.D. The contributions of suggestion, desire, and expectation to placebo effects in irritable bowel syndrome patients. An empirical investigation. Pain. 2003;105:17–25. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00073-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Sawamoto N., Honda M., Okada T., Hanakawa T., Kanda M., Fukuyama H., Konishi J., Shibasaki H. Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:7438–7445. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07438.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Schmid J., Bingel U., Ritter C., Benson S., Schedlowski M., Gramsch C., Forsting M., Elsenbruch S. Neural underpinnings of nocebo hyperalgesia in visceral pain: A fMRI study in healthy volunteers. NeuroImage. 2015;120:114–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Bingel U., Wanigasekera V., Wiech K., Ni Mhuircheartaigh R., Lee M.C., Ploner M., Tracey I. The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: Imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil. Sci. Transl. Med. 2011;3:70ra14. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3001244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Benedetti F., Arduino C., Costa S., Vighetti S., Tarenzi L., Rainero I., Asteggiano G. Loss of expectation-related mechanisms in Alzheimer’s disease makes analgesic therapies less effective. Pain. 2006;121:133–144. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Krummenacher P., Candia V., Folkers G., Schedlowski M., Schönbächler G. Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia. Pain. 2010;148:368–374. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Tu Y., Wilson G., Camprodon J., Dougherty D.D., Vangel M., Benedetti F., Kaptchuk T.J., Gollub R.L., Kong J. Manipulating placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia by changing brain excitability. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118 doi: 10.1073/pnas.2101273118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Tétreault P., Mansour A., Vachon-Presseau E., Schnitzer T.J., Apkarian A.V., Baliki M.N. Brain Connectivity Predicts Placebo Response across Chronic Pain Clinical Trials. PLoS Biol. 2016;14:e1002570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Vachon-Presseau E., Berger S.E., Abdullah T.B., Huang L., Cecchi G.A., Griffith J.W., Schnitzer T.J., Apkarian A.V. Brain and psychological determinants of placebo pill response in chronic pain patients. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:3397. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05859-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Hashmi J.A., Kong J., Spaeth R., Khan S., Kaptchuk T.J., Gollub R.L. Functional network architecture predicts psychologically mediated analgesia related to treatment in chronic knee pain patients. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:3924–3936. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3155-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Eippert F., Finsterbusch J., Bingel U., Büchel C. Direct evidence for spinal cord involvement in placebo analgesia. Science. 2009;326:404. doi: 10.1126/science.1180142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Duerden E.G., Albanese M.C. Localization of pain-related brain activation: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging data. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013;34:109–149. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Segerdahl A.R., Mezue M., Okell T.W., Farrar J.T., Tracey I. The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:499–500. doi: 10.1038/nn.3969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Bush N., Robinson M., Bryan M., Staud R., Boissoneault J. Task-dependent functional connectivity of pain-related brain regions is related to magnitude of placebo analgesia. J. Pain. 2021;22:603. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2021.03.102. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wanigasekera V., Wartolowska K., Huggins J.P., Duff E.P., Vennart W., Whitlock M., Massat N., Pauer L., Rogers P., Hoggart B., et al. Disambiguating pharmacological mechanisms from placebo in neuropathic pain using functional neuroimaging. Br. J. Anaesth. 2018;120:299–307. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2017.11.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Crawford L.S., Mills E.P., Hanson T., Macey P.M., Glarin R., Macefield V.G., Keay K.A., Henderson L.A. Brainstem Mechanisms of Pain Modulation: A within-Subjects 7T fMRI Study of Placebo Analgesic and Nocebo Hyperalgesic Responses. J. Neurosci. 2021;41:9794–9806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0806-21.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Ruscheweyh R., Kühnel M., Filippopulos F., Blum B., Eggert T., Straube A. Altered experimental pain perception after cerebellar infarction. Pain. 2014;155:1303–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Tinnermann A., Geuter S., Sprenger C., Finsterbusch J., Büchel C. Interactions between brain and spinal cord mediate value effects in nocebo hyperalgesia. Science. 2017;358:105–108. doi: 10.1126/science.aan1221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Bingel U., Wiech K., Ritter C., Wanigasekera V., Ní Mhuircheartaigh R., Lee M.C., Ploner M., Tracey I. Hippocampus mediates nocebo impairment of opioid analgesia through changes in functional connectivity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2022;56:3967–3978. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Zunhammer M., Bingel U., Wager T.D. Placebo Effects on the Neurologic Pain Signature: A Meta-analysis of Individual Participant Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data. JAMA Neurol. 2018;75:1321–1330. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Cai L., He L. Placebo effects and the molecular biological components involved. Gen. Psychiatry. 2019;32:e100089. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2019-100089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Hall K.T., Loscalzo J., Kaptchuk T.J. Genetics and the placebo effect: The placebome. Trends Mol. Med. 2015;21:285–294. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2015.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Hall K.T., Loscalzo J., Kaptchuk T. Pharmacogenomics and the Placebo Response. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 2018;9:633–635. doi: 10.1021/acschemneuro.8b00078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Colloca L., Wang Y., Martinez P.E., Chang Y.C., Ryan K.A., Hodgkinson C., Goldman D., Dorsey S.G. OPRM1 rs1799971, COMT rs4680, and FAAH rs324420 genes interact with placebo procedures to induce hypoalgesia. Pain. 2019;160:1824–1834. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Peciña M., Love T., Stohler C.S., Goldman D., Zubieta J.K. Effects of the Mu opioid receptor polymorphism (OPRM1 A118G) on pain regulation, placebo effects and associated personality trait measures. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:957–965. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Wang R.S., Hall K.T., Giulianini F., Passow D., Kaptchuk T.J., Loscalzo J. Network analysis of the genomic basis of the placebo effect. JCI Insight. 2017;2:e93911. doi: 10.1172/jci.insight.93911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Weimer K., Hahn E., Mönnikes N., Herr A.K., Stengel A., Enck P. Are Individual Learning Experiences More Important Than Heritable Tendencies? A Pilot Twin Study on Placebo Analgesia. Front. Psychiatry. 2019;10:679. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Raffaeli W., Tenti M., Corraro A., Malafoglia V., Ilari S., Balzani E., Bonci A. Chronic Pain: What Does It Mean? A Review on the Use of the Term Chronic Pain in Clinical Practice. J. Pain Res. 2021;14:827–835. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S303186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Vase L., Robinson M.E., Verne N.G., Price D.D. Increased placebo analgesia over time in irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) patients is associated with desire and expectation but not endogenous opioid mechanisms. Pain. 2005;115:338–347. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Kaptchuk T.J., Kelley J.M., Conboy L.A., Davis R.B., Kerr C.E., Jacobson E.E., Kirsch I., Schyner R.N., Nam B.H., Nguyen L.T., et al. Components of placebo effect: Randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ. 2008;336:999–1003. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39524.439618.25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Klinger R., Kothe R., Schmitz J., Kamping S., Flor H. Placebo effects of a sham opioid solution: A randomized controlled study in patients with chronic low back pain. Pain. 2017;158:1893–1902. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Olson E.M., Akintola T., Phillips J., Blasini M., Haycock N.R., Martinez P.E., Greenspan J.D., Dorsey S.G., Wang Y., Colloca L. Effects of sex on placebo effects in chronic pain participants: A cross-sectional study. Pain. 2021;162:531–542. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Machado G.C., Maher C.G., Ferreira P.H., Pinheiro M.B., Lin C.W., Day R.O., McLachlan A.J., Ferreira M.L. Efficacy and safety of paracetamol for spinal pain and osteoarthritis: Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised placebo controlled trials. BMJ. 2015;350:h1225. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h1225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Roelofs P.D., Deyo R.A., Koes B.W., Scholten R.J., van Tulder M.W. Non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs for low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2008:CD000396. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD000396.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Chaparro L.E., Furlan A.D., Deshpande A., Mailis-Gagnon A., Atlas S., Turk D.C. Opioids compared with placebo or other treatments for chronic low back pain: An update of the Cochrane Review. Spine. 2014;39:556–563. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0000000000000249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Henschke N., Kuijpers T., Rubinstein S.M., van Middelkoop M., Ostelo R., Verhagen A., Koes B.W., van Tulder M.W. Injection therapy and denervation procedures for chronic low-back pain: A systematic review. Eur. Spine J. 2010;19:1425–1449. doi: 10.1007/s00586-010-1411-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Müller M., Kamping S., Benrath J., Skowronek H., Schmitz J., Klinger R., Flor H. Treatment history and placebo responses to experimental and clinical pain in chronic pain patients. Eur. J. Pain. 2016;20:1530–1541. doi: 10.1002/ejp.877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Constantino M.J., Arnkoff D.B., Glass C.R., Ametrano R.M., Smith J.Z. Expectations. J. Clin. Psychol. 2011;67:184–192. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Peerdeman K.J., van Laarhoven A.I.M., Keij S.M., Vase L., Rovers M.M., Peters M.L., Evers A.W.M. Relieving patients’ pain with expectation interventions: A meta-analysis. Pain. 2016;157:1179–1191. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Colloca L., Akintola T., Haycock N.R., Blasini M., Thomas S., Phillips J., Corsi N., Schenk L.A., Wang Y. Prior Therapeutic Experiences, Not Expectation Ratings, Predict Placebo Effects: An Experimental Study in Chronic Pain and Healthy Participants. Psychother. Psychosom. 2020;89:371–378. doi: 10.1159/000507400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Kupers R., Maeyaert J., Boly M., Faymonville M.E., Laureys S. Naloxone-insensitive epidural placebo analgesia in a chronic pain patient. Anesthesiology. 2007;106:1239–1242. doi: 10.1097/01.anes.0000265418.68005.8a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.De Leon-Casasola O.A. Opioids for chronic pain: New evidence, new strategies, safe prescribing. Am. J. Med. 2013;126:S3–S11. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2012.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Meske D.S., Lawal O.D., Elder H., Langberg V., Paillard F., Katz N. Efficacy of opioids versus placebo in chronic pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis of enriched enrollment randomized withdrawal trials. J. Pain Res. 2018;11:923–934. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S160255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Martins D., Veronese M., Turkheimer F.E., Howard M.A., Williams S.C.R., Dipasquale O. A candidate neuroimaging biomarker for detection of neurotransmission-related functional alterations and prediction of pharmacological analgesic response in chronic pain. Brain Commun. 2022;4:fcab302. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcab302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Ballantyne J.C., Shin N.S. Efficacy of opioids for chronic pain: A review of the evidence. Clin. J. Pain. 2008;24:469–478. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e31816b2f26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Thompson S.J., Pitcher M.H., Stone L.S., Tarum F., Niu G., Chen X., Kiesewetter D.O., Schweinhardt P., Bushnell M.C. Chronic neuropathic pain reduces opioid receptor availability with associated anhedonia in rat. Pain. 2018;159:1856–1866. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.DaSilva A.F., Zubieta J.K., DosSantos M.F. Positron emission tomography imaging of endogenous mu-opioid mechanisms during pain and migraine. Pain Rep. 2019;4:e769. doi: 10.1097/PR9.0000000000000769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Maarrawi J., Peyron R., Mertens P., Costes N., Magnin M., Sindou M., Laurent B., Garcia-Larrea L. Differential brain opioid receptor availability in central and peripheral neuropathic pain. Pain. 2007;127:183–194. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Harris R.E., Clauw D.J., Scott D.J., McLean S.A., Gracely R.H., Zubieta J.K. Decreased central mu-opioid receptor availability in fibromyalgia. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:10000–10006. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2849-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Rossettini G., Colombi A., Carlino E., Manoni M., Mirandola M., Polli A., Camerone E.M., Testa M. Unraveling Negative Expectations and Nocebo-Related Effects in Musculoskeletal Pain. Front. Psychol. 2022;13:789377. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.789377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Crombez G., Van Ryckeghem D.M.L., Eccleston C., Van Damme S. Attentional bias to pain-related information: A meta-analysis. Pain. 2013;154:497–510. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Pincus T., Morley S. Cognitive-processing bias in chronic pain: A review and integration. Psychol. Bull. 2001;127:599–617. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Schoth D.E., Liossi C. Biased interpretation of ambiguous information in patients with chronic pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis of current studies. Health Psychol. 2016;35:944–956. doi: 10.1037/hea0000342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Rizvi S.J., Gandhi W., Salomons T. Reward processing as a common diathesis for chronic pain and depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021;127:749–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Apkarian A.V., Sosa Y., Krauss B.R., Thomas P.S., Fredrickson B.E., Levy R.E., Harden R.N., Chialvo D.R. Chronic pain patients are impaired on an emotional decision-making task. Pain. 2004;108:129–136. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Becker S., Kleinböhl D., Baus D., Hölzl R. Operant learning of perceptual sensitization and habituation is impaired in fibromyalgia patients with and without irritable bowel syndrome. Pain. 2011;152:1408–1417. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.02.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Verdejo-García A., López-Torrecillas F., Calandre E.P., Delgado-Rodríguez A., Bechara A. Executive function and decision-making in women with fibromyalgia. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2009;24:113–122. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acp014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Walteros C., Sánchez-Navarro J.P., Muñoz M.A., Martínez-Selva J.M., Chialvo D., Montoya P. Altered associative learning and emotional decision making in fibromyalgia. J. Psychosom. Res. 2011;70:294–301. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Berger S.E., Baria A.T., Baliki M.N., Mansour A., Herrmann K.M., Torbey S., Huang L., Parks E.L., Schnizter T.J., Apkarian A.V. Risky monetary behavior in chronic back pain is associated with altered modular connectivity of the nucleus accumbens. BMC Res. Notes. 2014;7:739. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-7-739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Finan P.H., Smith M.T. The comorbidity of insomnia, chronic pain, and depression: Dopamine as a putative mechanism. Sleep Med. Rev. 2013;17:173–183. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Taylor A.M.W., Becker S., Schweinhardt P., Cahill C. Mesolimbic dopamine signaling in acute and chronic pain: Implications for motivation, analgesia, and addiction. Pain. 2016;157:1194–1198. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Borsook D., Erpelding N., Becerra L. Losses and gains: Chronic pain and altered brain morphology. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2013;13:1221–1234. doi: 10.1586/14737175.2013.846218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Vachon-Presseau E., Abdullah T.B., Berger S.E., Huang L., Griffith J.W., Schnitzer T.J., Apkarian A.V. Validating a biosignature-predicting placebo pill response in chronic pain in the settings of a randomized controlled trial. Pain. 2022;163:910–922. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Carlino E., Vase L. Can knowledge of Placebo and Nocebo Mechanisms Help Improve Randomized Clinical Trials? Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2018;138:329–357. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2018.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Benedetti F., Carlino E., Piedimonte A. Increasing uncertainty in CNS clinical trials: The role of placebo, nocebo, and Hawthorne effects. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15:736–747. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00066-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Vase L., Petersen G.L., Riley J.L., 3rd, Price D.D. Factors contributing to large analgesic effects in placebo mechanism studies conducted between 2002 and 2007. Pain. 2009;145:36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Petersen G.L., Finnerup N.B., Colloca L., Amanzio M., Price D.D., Jensen T.S., Vase L. The magnitude of nocebo effects in pain: A meta-analysis. Pain. 2014;155:1426–1434. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Tsutsumi Y., Tsujimoto Y., Tajika A., Omae K., Fujii T., Onishi A., Kataoka Y., Katsura M., Noma H., Sahker E., et al. Proportion attributable to contextual effects in general medicine: A meta-epidemiological study based on Cochrane reviews. BMJ Evid. Based Med. 2023;28:40–47. doi: 10.1136/bmjebm-2021-111861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Zou K., Wong J., Abdullah N., Chen X., Smith T., Doherty M., Zhang W. Examination of overall treatment effect and the proportion attributable to contextual effect in osteoarthritis: Meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 2016;75:1964–1970. doi: 10.1136/annrheumdis-2015-208387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Jonas W.B., Crawford C., Colloca L., Kaptchuk T.J., Moseley B., Miller F.G., Kriston L., Linde K., Meissner K. To what extent are surgery and invasive procedures effective beyond a placebo response? A systematic review with meta-analysis of randomised, sham controlled trials. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e009655. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Karlsson M., Bergenheim A., Larsson M.E.H., Nordeman L., van Tulder M., Bernhardsson S. Effects of exercise therapy in patients with acute low back pain: A systematic review of systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 2020;9:182. doi: 10.1186/s13643-020-01412-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Artus M., van der Windt D.A., Jordan K.P., Hay E.M. Low back pain symptoms show a similar pattern of improvement following a wide range of primary care treatments: A systematic review of randomized clinical trials. Rheumatology. 2010;49:2346–2356. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/keq245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Hayden J.A., Wilson M.N., Riley R.D., Iles R., Pincus T., Ogilvie R. Individual recovery expectations and prognosis of outcomes in non-specific low back pain: Prognostic factor review. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2019:CD011284. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011284.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Mohamed Mohamed W.J., Joseph L., Canby G., Paungmali A., Sitilertpisan P., Pirunsan U. Are patient expectations associated with treatment outcomes in individuals with chronic low back pain? A systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Int. J. Clin. Pract. 2020;74:e13680. doi: 10.1111/ijcp.13680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Wassinger C.A., Edwards D.C., Bourassa M., Reagan D., Weyant E.C., Walden R.R. The Role of Patient Recovery Expectations in the Outcomes of Physical Therapist Intervention: A Systematic Review. Phys. Ther. 2022;102:pzac008. doi: 10.1093/ptj/pzac008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Kinney M., Seider J., Beaty A.F., Coughlin K., Dyal M., Clewley D. The impact of therapeutic alliance in physical therapy for chronic musculoskeletal pain: A systematic review of the literature. Physiother. Theory Pract. 2020;36:886–898. doi: 10.1080/09593985.2018.1516015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Bajcar E.A., Bąbel P. How Does Observational Learning Produce Placebo Effects? A Model Integrating Research Findings. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:2041. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Colloca L. Placebo, nocebo, and learning mechanisms. Handb. Exp. Pharm. 2014;225:17–35. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Peerdeman K.J., van Laarhoven A.I., Peters M.L., Evers A.W. An Integrative Review of the Influence of Expectancies on Pain. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1270. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Schwartz M., Fischer L.M., Bläute C., Stork J., Colloca L., Zöllner C., Klinger R. Observing treatment outcomes in other patients can elicit augmented placebo effects on pain treatment: A double-blinded randomized clinical trial with patients with chronic low back pain. Pain. 2022;163:1313–1323. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Price D.D., Milling L.S., Kirsch I., Duff A., Montgomery G.H., Nicholls S.S. An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm. Pain. 1999;83:147–156. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00081-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Bordin E.S. The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. 1979;16:252–260. doi: 10.1037/h0085885. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Fuentes J., Armijo-Olivo S., Funabashi M., Miciak M., Dick B., Warren S., Rashiq S., Magee D.J., Gross D.P. Enhanced therapeutic alliance modulates pain intensity and muscle pain sensitivity in patients with chronic low back pain: An experimental controlled study. Phys. Ther. 2014;94:477–489. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20130118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Kelley J.M., Lembo A.J., Ablon J.S., Villanueva J.J., Conboy L.A., Levy R., Marci C.D., Kerr C.E., Kirsch I., Jacobson E.E., et al. Patient and practitioner influences on the placebo effect in irritable bowel syndrome. Psychosom. Med. 2009;71:789–797. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181acee12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Di Blasi Z., Harkness E., Ernst E., Georgiou A., Kleijnen J. Influence of context effects on health outcomes: A systematic review. Lancet. 2001;357:757–762. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)04169-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Wartolowska K.A., Hohenschurz-Schmidt D., Vase L., Aronson J.K. The importance of using placebo controls in nonpharmacological randomised trials. Pain. 2022;164:921–925. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Rief W., Glombiewski J.A. The hidden effects of blinded, placebo-controlled randomized trials: An experimental investigation. Pain. 2012;153:2473–2477. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Shah E., Triantafyllou K., Hana A.A., Pimentel M. Adverse events appear to unblind clinical trials in irritable bowel syndrome. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 2014;26:482–488. doi: 10.1111/nmo.12289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Freed B., Williams B., Situ X., Landsman V., Kim J., Moroz A., Bang H., Park J.J. Blinding, sham, and treatment effects in randomized controlled trials for back pain in 2000–2019: A review and meta-analytic approach. Clin. Trials. 2021;18:361–370. doi: 10.1177/1740774520984870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Frisaldi E., Shaibani A., Benedetti F. Why We should Assess Patients’ Expectations in Clinical Trials. Pain Ther. 2017;6:107–110. doi: 10.1007/s40122-017-0071-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Zeppieri G., Jr., George S.Z. Patient-defined desired outcome, success criteria, and expectation in outpatient physical therapy: A longitudinal assessment. Health Qual. Life Outcomes. 2017;15:29. doi: 10.1186/s12955-017-0604-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Zeppieri G., Jr., Lentz T.A., Atchison J.W., Indelicato P.A., Moser M.W., Vincent K.R., George S.Z. Preliminary results of patient-defined success criteria for individuals with musculoskeletal pain in outpatient physical therapy settings. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2012;93:434–440. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Rutherford B.R., Sneed J.R., Roose S.P. Does study design influence outcome? The effects of placebo control and treatment duration in antidepressant trials. Psychother. Psychosom. 2009;78:172–181. doi: 10.1159/000209348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Being a state of the art review, this study does not contain original data.