Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2023 Jun 29;13:10532. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-37166-1

Small mammals reduce activity during high moon illumination under risk of predation by introduced predators

P Taylor 1,2, M Swan 1,, H Sitters 1, A Smith 1, J Di Stefano 1
PMCID: PMC10310734  PMID: 37386037

Abstract

Predation influences prey survival and drives evolution of anti-predator behaviour. Anti-predator strategies by prey are stimulated by direct encounters with predators, but also by exposure to indicators of risk such as moonlight illumination and vegetation cover. Many prey species will suffer increased risk on moonlit nights, but risk may be reduced by the presence of dense vegetation. Determining the role of vegetation in reducing perceived risk is important, especially given predictions of increased global wildfire, which consumes vegetation and increases predation. We used remote cameras in southeastern Australia to compare support for the predation risk and habitat-mediated predation risk hypotheses. We examined the influence of moonlight and understorey cover on seven 20–2500 g mammalian prey species and two introduced predators, red foxes and feral cats. Activity of all prey species reduced by 40–70% with increasing moonlight, while one species (bush rat) reduced activity in response to increasing moonlight more sharply in low compared to high understorey cover. Neither predator responded to moonlight. Our findings supported the predation risk hypothesis and provided limited support for the habitat-mediated predation risk hypothesis. For prey, perceived costs of increased predation risk on moonlit nights outweighed any benefits of a brighter foraging environment.

Subject terms: Ecology, Behavioural ecology, Biodiversity

Introduction

Predation is a key biological process that influences interactions among individuals, shapes species’ distributions and alters the structure of ecological communities1,2. For example, the consumption of prey by predators, and competition between different predator species, can result in downstream influence on other groups of animals and plants, and on the functional roles these species play1. Further, the importance of predation to ecosystem function is evidenced by the large effects of predator removal or reintroduction on other species and ecosystem processes35. The most obvious effects of predation are the direct influence that predators have on prey populations by consuming individuals, but indirect effects are also common. For example, the conservation of mammalian carnivores can increase carbon storage by palatable plants due to the downwards pressure predators place on herbivore populations6,7.

From the perspective of prey species, predator activity translates to a perception of risk that varies spatially and temporally, influencing both intraspecific and interspecific interactions2,8,9. For example, female song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) subject to predator calls but not to actual predation produced 40% fewer offspring per year compared to individuals that were exposed to calls of non-predatory species10. Further, in cases where predation has an influence on prey fitness, it can drive the evolution of prey behaviours that minimise detection by predators2,11. For instance, optimal foraging theory predicts that prey species trade-off the need to acquire food with the risk of predation associated with foraging12,13. To do this prey must be able to evaluate predation risk using either direct or indirect cues14. Direct cues include physical encounters, scats, urine, markings and calls, while indirect cues include abiotic factors such as habitat structure, weather (e.g. wind and rain), reproductive status (e.g. breeding or non-breeding) and moonlight2,1416.

Approximately sixty-nine percent of mammals are nocturnal17, thus changes in moonlight over the lunar cycle is expected to influence the behaviour of many species18. Increased moonlight can improve the hunting efficiency of both mammalian and non-mammalian predators19,20, resulting in increased predation risk and reduced activity for prey species2124. For example, the detectability of new holland mice (Pseudomys navaehollandiae) decreased with increasing moonlight and six times more survey effort was required to reach 95% confidence that the species was absent during full moon compared to new moon21. These observed behavioural patterns form the basis of the predation risk hypothesis, which predicts that increased moonlight will result in lowered activity in prey species and increased activity in predators2527.

Dense vegetation provides mammals with a variety of resources including shelter from predators20,28,29. For example, Allenby’s gerbils (Gerbillus andersoni allenbyi) exposed to owls and vipers harvested substantially more seed (a measure of perceived predation risk) from sheltered compared to open patches28. Potentially, reduced predation risk in sheltered environments can interact with increased predation risk associated with illumination, resulting in the suppressive influence of moonlight on prey activity diminishing with increasing shelter. This interaction is described by the habitat mediated predation risk hypothesis27, although tests of this hypothesis have rendered mixed results. For example, consistent with the habitat mediated predation risk hypothesis, Meriam’s kangaroo rat (Dipodomys merriami) removed fewer seeds from open habitat compared to sheltered habitat during a full moon30. In contrast moonlight had no effect on the use of dense or open microhabitats by the agile antechinus (Antechinus agilis)31.

In this study, we investigated the response of seven small–medium sized mammals ranging from 20 to 2500 g (prey species) and two introduced predators (feral cats, Felis catus and red foxes, Vulpes vulpes) to moonlight and vegetation cover in the woodlands of south–western Victoria, Australia. Wildfire is a natural process in this ecosystem, and prescribed fire is used as a management tool to reduce fuel loads and manage biodiversity. Infrequent wildfire and annual application of prescribed fire in our study area has resulted in a large range of vegetation states from open recently burnt sites to dense regenerating areas, making it an ideal location to investigate the responses of prey and predator species to moonlight and vegetation cover. Furthermore, there is increasing evidence that fire-induced vegetation change can increase predation on small mammals by introduced predators3234 thus determining the role of vegetation cover in mediating perceived predation risk is important. For example, increased predation in recently burnt areas with reduced vegetation cover could potentially be moderated by implementing patchy burns resulting in unburnt vegetation within the fire perimeter for prey species to shelter in.

We tested the following hypotheses (Fig. 1):

  1. Predation risk hypothesis Moonlight increases predation risk. We predict a negative relationship between moonlight and prey species activity and a positive relationship between moonlight and predator activity.

  2. Habitat mediated predation risk hypothesis Moonlight increases predation risk to a greater extent in open compared to sheltered habitats. Both the negative relationship between moonlight and prey activity and the positive relationship between moonlight and predator activity will be stronger in open compared to sheltered habitats.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Conceptual model outlining the expected relationships between both prey and predator activity and moonlight under the predation risk and habitat-mediated predation risk hypotheses. The symbols on the left and right of the x-axis represent low and high moon illumination respectively.

Results

We detected 11 native prey species, two introduced prey species and two introduced predator species in 17,699 camera trap nights across 154 sites (Table 1; Supplementary Table S1). Six native prey species (heath mouse, yellow-footed antechinus, bush rat, southern brown bandicoot, common ringtail possum and common brushtail possum) one introduced prey species (house mouse) and two introduced predators (feral cat and red fox) occurred at ≥ 10 sites and were subject to formal analysis (Table 1).

Table 1.

The number of sites at which each prey and predator species was detected.

Species Number of sites detected
2018/2019 2019/2020 Total
Prey species
 Bush rat (Rattus fuscipes) 26 14 29
 Common brushtail possum (Trichosurus vulpecula) 74 75 82
 Common ringtail possum (Pseudocheirus peregrinus) 29 33 48
 Heath mouse (Pseudomys shortridgei) 34 33 41
 House mouse (Mus musculus) 22 11 29
 Southern brown bandicoot (Isoodon obesulus) 10 12 14
 Yellow-footed antechinus (Antechinus flavipes) 37 30 51
Introduced predators
 Feral cat (Felis catus) 6 8 12
 Red fox (Vulpes vulpes) 39 42 69

The first two numeric columns represent the number sites occupied during the 2018/2019 and 2019/2020 sampling seasons while the last column represents the total number of uniquely occupied sites across both seasons. Only species recorded at ≥ 10 sites in total are included; species detected at fewer sites are shown in Supplementary Table S1.

Among prey species, our analysis demonstrated strong support for the predation risk hypothesis; increased moonlight resulted in reduced activity for every species (Fig. 2, Table 2). Declines in activity were substantial and ranged between 40% for the common brushtail possum and 70% for the house mouse (Fig. 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Predictions from a generalised linear mixed model representing the responses of prey and predator species to the moonlight index. Detectable negative relationships (blue) indicate support for the predation risk hypothesis. Null effects are shown in purple. If cloud cover or season were present, predictions were generated at the mean of cloud cover and for season 1. Shading reflects 95% confidence limits.

Table 2.

Models of the response of prey and predator species to the moonlight index (MI), understorey cover (UC), cloud cover (C) and season (S).

Species Model Estimate (95% CL) Supported hypothesis Change in AICc Akaike weight R2m (R2c)
Bush Rat (Rattus fuscipes) MI × UC + C + S 0.20 (0.10, 0.30) HMPR 0.00 1.00 0.21 (0.53)
MI + UC + C + S − 0.22 (− 0.29, − 0.16) PR 13.07 0.00 0.18 (0.51)
Null 305.03 0.00 0.00 (0.47)
Common Brushtail Possum (Trichosurus vulpecula) MI + C + S − 0.17 (− 0.22, − 0.12) PR 0.00 0.52 0.01 (0.31)
MI × UC + S + C − 0.04 (− 0.10, 0.01) 1.23 0.28 0.01 (0.31)
MI + UC + S + C 1.99 0.19 0.01 (0.31)
Null 63.87 0.00 0.00 (0.30)
Common Ringtail Possum (Pseudocheirus peregrinus) MI − 0.19 (− 0.36, − 0.01) PR 0.00 0.25 0.01 (0.23)
MI + S 1.44 0.12 0.01 (0.24)
MI + C 1.98 0.09 0.01 (0.23)
MI + UC 2.00 0.09 0.01 (0.23)
MI × UC 0.12 (− 0.05, 0.29) 2.02 0.09 0.01 (0.24)
Null 2.72 0.07 0.00 (0.23)
Heath Mouse (Pseudomys shortridgei) MI + UC + C − 0.22 (− 0.30, − 0.14) PR 0.00 0.32 0.03 (0.36)
MI + UC + C + S 0.47 0.25 0.04 (0.36)
MI × UC + C − 0.04 (− 0.13, 0.04) 1.07 0.18 0.03 (0.35)
MI × UC + C + S 1.53 0.15 0.03 (0.35)
Null 51.98 0.00 0.00 (0.35)
House Mouse (Mus musculus) MI + UC + S − 2.28 (− 2.91, − 1.64) PR 0.00 0.52 0.31 (0.70)
MI × UC + S − 0.04 (− 0.21, 0.14) 1.85 0.21 0.32 (070)
MI + UC + S + C 1.97 0.20 0.31 (0.70)
Null 217.24 0.00 0.00 (0.31)
Yellow-footed Antechinus (Antchinus flavipes) MI × UC + C − 0.10 (− 0.23, 0.03) 0.00 0.32 0.05 (0.33)
MI + UC + C − 0.30 (− 0.44, − 0.16) PR 0.18 0.30 0.06 (0.34)
MI × UC + C + S 1.68 0.14 0.05 (0.34)
MI + UC + C + S 1.89 0.13 0.06 (0.34)
Null 27.04 0.00 0.00 (0.32)
Southern Brown Bandicoot (Isoodon obesulus) MI + UC + S − 0.21 (− 0.41, − 0.02) PR 0.00 0.25 0.14 (0.37)
MI × UC + S 0.16 (− 0.15, 0.47) 0.94 0.16 0.17 (0.39)
MI + UC 1.32 0.13 0.12 (0.35)
MI + UC + S + C 1.57 0.11 0.14 (0.37)
Null 20.85 0.00 0.00 (0.34)
Feral cat (Felis catus) Null None 0.00 0.18 0.00
UC + C 0.73 0.13 0.07
UC 0.77 0.13 0.02
MI + C 0.12 (− 0.35, 0.59) 1.70 0.08 0.05
MI 1.81 0.07 0.00
MI × UC + C 0.20 (− 0.29, 0.69) 3.84 0.03 0.09
Red fox (Vulpes vulpes) UC + S None 0.00 0.37 0.02 (0.24)
UC + S + C 1.64 0.17 0.02 (0.24)
MI + UC + S − 0.03 (− 0.16, 0.10) 1.73 0.16 0.02 (0.24)
MI × UC + S − 0.02 (− 0.15, 0.11) 3.66 0.06 0.02 (0.24)
Null 7.74 0.01 0.00 (0.23)

Models were ranked using Akaike’s information criterion adjusted for small sample size (AICc) and those displayed include all models within 2 AICc units of the best and, if not already in this set, the best models containing the moonlight index with and without its interaction with understorey cover. The null model is also included for reference. Estimates and their 95% confidence limits have been included for the MI term in the most highly ranked model containing the moonlight index (a test of the predation risk hypothesis) and the MI by UC interaction term for the most highly ranked model containing the interaction between the moonlight index and understorey cover (a test of the habitat mediated predation risk hypothesis). Hypotheses that are supported by the data are indicated in the Supported hypothesis column; habitat mediated predation risk and predation risk are coded as HMPR and PR. Change in AICc represents the difference in AICc between the associated model and the best model, Akaike weights are the probability that the model is the best in the set of tested models, and marginal and conditional r-squared values (R2m and R2c) represent the variance explained by fixed factors and both fixed and random factors respectively. R2c associated with cat models do not exist as random effects were excluded (see methods for details).

In contrast, there was little support for the habitat-mediated predation risk hypothesis with bush rats the only species for which an interaction between the moonlight index and understorey cover was detected (Fig. 3, Table 2). When understorey cover was high (75th percentile), increased moonlight resulted in a 34% decline in bush rat activity, while the decline at low understorey (25th percentile) cover was 75% (Fig. 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Predictions from a generalised linear mixed model representing the responses of prey and predator species to the interaction between the moonlight index and understorey cover; species’ responses to the moonlight index are shown at low (25th percentile) and high (75th percentile) understorey cover. Detectible interaction effects (blue) indicate support for the habitat mediated predation risk hypothesis. Null effects (i.e., no detectible interaction) are shown in purple. If cloud cover or season were present, predictions were generated at the mean of cloud cover and for season 1. Shading reflects 95% confidence limits.

Neither of the predators, feral cats and red foxes, responded either positively or negatively to moonlight (Fig. 2, Table 2). Cats did not respond to any of our modelled predictors (the null model was the best), while for foxes we detected a negative response to understorey cover (estimate (95% CL) − 0.23 (− 0.45, − 0.01), p = 0.04).

For all modelled species, the estimate and lower and upper 95% confidence limits associated with each parameter in the top models are presented in Supplementary Tables S2S10.

Discussion

Prey species have evolved to respond to both direct and indirect cues of predation risk but can also learn appropriate responses to novel threats over shorter time periods14,35. In the absence of direct cues, prey species may change their behaviour in response to stimuli that indirectly indicates an increase in predation risk, such as moonlight and vegetation cover, helping to decrease interactions with predators27. We found that the predation risk hypothesis was most strongly supported by our results; the activity of all seven prey species was negatively correlated with the moonlight index. The response of only one prey species, the bush rat (Rattus fuscipes), supported the habitat mediated predation risk hypothesis while cats and foxes did not respond to the moonlight index. We discuss our results in relation to the key hypotheses and consider their implications for land management.

Predation risk hypothesis

There was strong support for the predation risk hypothesis, with all six native species and the introduced house mouse reducing their activity with increasing moonlight. Our results are consistent with a recent global meta-analysis showing that, on average, moonlight suppressed the activity of mammalian prey that use non-visual (e.g. olfactory) senses to detect predators27. Nevertheless, support for the hypothesis is not universal. For example, while Bolam’s mouse (Pseudomys bolamii), mallee ningaui (Ningaui. yvonneae) and verreaux mouse (Praomys verreauxii) all responded negatively to moonlight15,23, several critical weight range (35–5500 g)36 prey species in a fenced conservation reserve in southeastern Australia did not respond to moonlight37, presumably because introduced vertebrate predators were absent.

Considering variable support in the literature for the predation risk hypothesis, the strong negative response to moonlight shown by all prey species in our study suggests that predation risk could be substantial. Further, several lines of evidence point to much of this risk originating from introduced vertebrate predators. First, foxes were widespread in our study area (Table 1) and cats were also detected but at fewer sites. Although we do not have good evidence of fox and cat densities, our sites were among a matrix of pasture and plantations with an extensive track network—factors that may increase predation pressure by these species38,39. Second, native dingoes are absent from western Victoria40, and as they can supress cat and fox populations41,42, their absence is likely to intensify predation pressure by these two introduced species. Finally, as noted above, a fenced community of prey species did not respond to moonlight when introduced predators were excluded37 . Nevertheless, our prey species are also subject to predation by two nocturnal foragers (Barn Owl, Tyto alba and Southern Boobook, Ninox boobook) who likely influence prey behaviour to some extent. Determining the relative influence of native and introduced predators on anti-predator behaviour is a fertile area for future work; however, manipulative experiments are required to disentangle these effects.

Our findings suggest that native prey are actvating a strong anti-predator response strategy in the face of predation risk from by introduced species. This is consistent with recent evidence demonstrating that responses of prey to native and introduced predators are usually similar, and even if prey are initially naïve to novel predators they quickly learn apropriate anti-predator strategies over short time frames35,43,44. Our results are in contrast to the prey naivety hypothesis that predicts native prey will fail to recognise and respond appropriately to an evolutionary novel threat45,46. The strong negative response to moonlight by all prey in this study support the view that prey are susceptible to threats from all predators regardless of their origins, and that the strength of anti-predator responses are more likely to be driven by other factors, such as the relative mass of predators and prey43.

Predictions of increased predator activity with moonlight under the predation risk hypothesis were not realised; the moonlight index was not associated with the activity of foxes or cats. Previous studies from diverse locations have reported an increase in hunting success or activity with moonlight for both species19,47,48. However, one Australian study found that foxes ate fewer small mammals when the moon was full compared to other phases of the lunar cycle49. There may be several reasons why the predators in our study did not respond positively to moonlight. One possibility is that the negative association between prey species activity and moonlight acted as an effective anti-predator strategy and increasing their activity when moonlight was high rendered no benefit to predators. Red foxes and cats eat a wide variety of food items50,51, so it is possible that their foraging strategies shift when small mammal prey species are less active when the moon is bright. For example, Jaguars changed their habitat use during the full moon when armadillos avoided foraging above ground52. Alternatively, a lack of numerical response (more predators or more active predators) may be counterbalanced by a functional response, (i.e. hunting efficiency is greater without an increase in predator activity)34. In general, the optimal foraging decisions for medium-sized predators are likely to be less predictable than for small mammals. Medium-sized predators are at risk of predation themselves, so the optimal behavioural pattern involves complex trade-offs between foraging and predator avoidance53.

Habitat-mediated predation risk hypothesis

Although many studies have analysed how moonlight illumination affects small mammal behaviour, few have extended their analysis to investigate the interaction between moonlight and vegetation cover. We found limited support for the habitat mediated predation risk hypothesis, with only bush rats showing a smaller decline of activity with increased moonlight at high compared to low vegetation cover. This suggests that bush rats perceived an elevated predation risk in more open vegetation compared to closed vegetation when moonlight illumination was high. The overall scarcity of interactions between moonlight and vegetation cover suggests dense understorey vegetation does not substantially alter perceived predation risk at high moonlight for most of the prey species we studied.

Compared to the predation risk hypothesis there are fewer tests of the habitat mediated version, and existing results provide variable support. For example, in a meta-analysis of giving up density (GUD) studies, seed consumption increased on dark compared to moonlit nights, but this effect was similar in open and closed habitats27. Studies using different response variables have rendered similar results; for example, the number of captures of agile antechinus (A. agilis) in open and closed habitats was not influenced by increasing moonlight or experimentally modified light levels31. In contrast, desert rodents in China and the house mouse (Mus domesticus) in Australia shifted to foraging in denser patches of vegetation when moon illumination was high compared to when it was low, providing support for the habitat-mediated predation risk hypothesis54,55.

Four prey species (bush rat, yellow-footed antechinus, heath mouse and southern brown bandicoot) showed increased activity in dense compared to open understorey vegetation regardless of moonlight effects. This is a common response for many small mammals; sheltered habitats may reduce predation risk as they provide greater refuge from predators than open areas5658. One notable exception was the house mouse whose activity was substantially higher where the understorey was more open. This species is introduced and known to occupy open or disturbed environments, which may be a strategy to avoid competition with native small mammals in Australian systems59. Foxes also responded negatively to understorey cover, likely reflecting their attraction to recently burnt areas32,33.

Cloud cover

We expected cloud cover to have a positive influence on prey species activity levels as it may limit the amount of moonlight reaching the forest floor, reducing the suppressive influence of moon illumination. Cloud cover had a positive influence on the activity of three prey species, a negative influence on the activity of one prey species, and did not influence the activity of the remaining three prey species or the two predators. Many studies on the effect of moonlight on prey species have failed to incorporate cloud cover into their analyses24,30,60. Those that have exhibit variable outcomes similar to our observations. For example, in southeastern Australia, prey species increased their activity during periods of high cloud cover37. In contrast, cloud cover was found to have no significant effect on the response of giant kangaroo rats to moonlight61. One explanation for these mixed results is that weather station data do not include metrics of cloud thickness, a factor that likely influences moon illumination.

Furthermore, cloud is related to other weather factors that may affect mammal behaviour, independent of their effects on moonlight, such as atmospheric pressure, precipitation and wind. Both wind and precipitation may influence predation risk by interfering with the auditory and olfactory cues of either predator or prey. For example, high wind speed has been found to have a positive influence on capture rates of small mammals as the wind noise may mask their movement, decreasing predation risk15. Disentangling the interacting influences of weather, cloud cover and moon illumination is challenging and will require manipulative experiments to tease apart their effects.

Conclusion

The activity of all six native mammal prey species and the introduced house mouse was negatively associated with moonlight levels, lending strong support to the predation risk hypothesis. In addition, four native prey species (bush rat, yellow-footed antechinus, heath mouse and southern brown bandicoot) were more active at higher compared to lower vegetation cover. Predation, especially from introduced predators, has been a key driver of mammal extinction in Australia and other parts of the world62 and maintaining habitats containing dense vegetation is seen as a key conservation action to protect vulnerable native prey. This is especially pertinent in flammable ecosystems; for example in Australia there is evidence highlighting increased vulnerability of prey species to introduced cats and foxes immediately after fire, with patchy fires that leave unburnt vegetation as shelter suggested as a potential conservation strategy reviewed by34. It remains unknown, however, how moonlight interacts with fire and vegetation to influence the vulnerability of native species to predation. For instance, the vulnerability of native prey species to predation occurring in the days to weeks after prescribed fire could potentially be reduced if prescribed burns were conducted at an appropriate point in the lunar cycle; however further research is needed to test this hypothesis.

Methods

Study area

The study area (Fig. 4) has a temperate climate characterised by cool, wet winters and warm dry summers. Mean annual minimum and maximum temperatures are 8.3 °C and 20.0 °C respectively while mean annual rainfall ranges from 656 mm in the north to 741 mm in the south63. Mean annual minimum and maximum temperatures are 8.3 °C and 20.0 °C respectively while mean annual rainfall ranges from 656 mm in the north to 741 mm in the south63. There is little topographical variation (70–90 m a.s.l) and soils are deep, highly acidic, fine to medium-grained Aeolian quartz sand with little organic matter64.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Study area map with sites overlying the main vegetarian types in southeastern Australia (see inset at the top-right). The inset at the bottom-left shows the location of the cameras (crosses) and vegetation sampling plots (circles) along a 200 m transect. An enlargement of a vegetation sampling plot is also shown.

Large patches of native eucalypt woodland and forest (1000–10,000 ha) are surrounded by pasture and plantation forestry. The native vegetation is primarily composed of Heathy woodland, Grassy/heathy dry forest and Forby forest. Heathy woodland is characterised by shrubs, including silver banksia (Banksia marginata) and heath tea tree (Leptospermum myrsinoides), and the grass trees Xanthorrhoea australis and X. caespitosa. It has an overstorey of brown stringybark (Eucalyptus baxterii) but is treeless in low-lying areas with impeded drainage33. Grassy/heathy dry forest has an understorey of bracken (Pteridium esculentum), sedges, grass trees (X. australis) while brown stringybark dominates the overstorey. Forby forest has an open understorey dominated by bracken, sedges and tussock grasses. Black wattle (Acacia mearnsii) occurs through the sparse midstory while messmate (E. obliqua) and brown stringybark dominate the overstorey. Climatic and soil gradients result in taller and shrubbier woodlands in the south. The region is susceptible to wildfires and planned burns are applied regularly (usually during autumn) to reduce wildfire risk and promote biodiversity.

Study design

To ensure we sampled a range of vegetation densities, we stratified the study area by (a) vegetation type (described above) and (b) fire age class, a categorical representation of time since fire, defined as recently burnt (0–3 years), early successional (4–10 years), mid successional (11–34 years) and late successional (35+ years)65. Within these strata we randomly selected a total of 154 sites in loose clusters of 5 to increase sampling efficiency. Sites were a minimum of 1 km from each other and at least 50 m from roads and other edges created by vegetation types or age classes. At each site we established a 200 m randomly-orientated transect. Native vegetation and fire history maps used during the design phase were accessed from the Data Vic website (https://www.data.vic.gov.au/).

Camera surveys

Prey and predator activity were detected using motion-sensing cameras. Camera trapping took place principally during the austral spring, summer and autumn; November to June in 2018/2019 and repeated November to April in 2019/2020. Camera trapping was conducted with approval from the University of Melbourne Animal Ethics Committee (ID 1604071) and a scientific research permit issued by the Department of Environment, Land, Water and Planning (ID 10008227).

One ReconyxTM HC500 (infrared) and one ReconyxTM HC550 (white-flash) was placed at each site at the 50 m and 150 m (randomly allocated) mark of the transect (Fig. 4). The white-flash cameras were included to help identify small mammals where colour provides diagnostic information. Cameras were attached to trees 30 cm off the ground, facing a bait station 1.5 m to the south. The bait consisted of a mixture of rolled oats, peanut butter, golden syrup and pistachio essence and was placed into perforated polypipe canisters (10 × 5 cm) attached to wooden stakes and suspended 20 cm off the ground (Supplementary Fig. S1). Vegetation was cleared between the camera and bait as well as 50 cm behind and to the sides of the bait allowing for better species identifications and false trigger reduction. Cameras were set on high sensitivity and five images were taken in rapid succession for each activation with no delay between activations. Resolution was set to 3.1 MP and night mode was balanced. Cameras were deployed for 30 days to capture the duration of the lunar cycle. Of 616 deployments (154 sites × 2 camera × 2 seasons), 44 cameras failed but 26 of these were successfully redeployed for the full 30 days. Deployment time for the remaining 18 cameras ranged between 0 and 29 days. We excluded one site in season 2 as both cameras failed. At all other sites at least one camera operated for the full 30-day deployment period.

Five researchers identified camera images in both 2018/2019 and 2019/2020. Species were identified using a field guide66 and reference images from previous studies. Positively identified images were labelled with a species code using digiKam version 6.2.0 (www.digikam.org/) and we extracted and summarised the metadata in R version 4.0.2 (R Core Team, 2020) using camtrapR67. These data were converted into an activity index representing the number of hours per night each species was detected; we did not calculate activity for species occurring at fewer than 10 sites (Table S1). The activity index was calculated separately for each night of the 30-day deployment period to reflect changes in activity across the lunar cycle.

Vegetation surveys

Vegetation surveys were conducted at each site between October 2019 and March 2020 at the 20 m, 60 m, 100 m, 140 m and 180 m marks of each transect (Fig. 4). At each of these five locations, a 2 m pole was placed at 16 sampling points to quantify presence or absence of vegetation between 0 and 100 cm (a total of 80 sampling points per site). The number of presences out of 80 was used as an understorey cover score for statistical modelling. We used vegetation within 100 cm of the ground as preliminary analysis of vegetation data sampled between the ground and the canopy (unpublished data) showed that on average 72% of the vegetation resource was within this zone.

Moon illumination

Proportion of the moon disc illuminated, moonrise and moonset times and sunrise and sunset times for each day during the study period were calculated using the R package suncalc68. Moon illumination was calculated at Casterton (37.58 S, 141.4 E; centrally located in the study area) as the proportion of the moon disc illuminated, multiplied by the proportion of the night that the moon was above the horizon. The resulting index ranged from 0 to 1 with 0 representing a new moon or a night when the moon does not appear above the horizon, and 1 representing a full moon that is above the horizon for the entire night.

Cloud cover

Cloud cover data were acquired from three weather stations within 100 km of the study area; Mount Gambier Aero, Portland Airport and Hamilton (Cashmore Airport)63. Cloud cover was measured as the fraction of the total sky covered in cloud in eighths. At each station cloud cover values at 2100, 0000 and 0300 h each night were averaged, and then the values from the three stations were averaged to represent nightly cloud cover for the whole study area. Cloud cover ranged from 0 to 8 where 0 represents the absence of clouds and 8 represents a completely overcast night.

Statistical modelling

Models were built in R version 4.2.069 to determine the response of species activity to the moonlight index, understorey cover and cloud cover. We also included season to account for the surveys in 2018/2019 and 2019/2020. Although we used vegetation type and fire age class as strata in our design, we did not use these variables for modelling. We excluded vegetation type as heathy woodland dominated the study area thus the number of sites in other vegetation types was small (Fig. 4). We excluded fire age class (or time since fire) as the reason for selecting sites within different age classes was to ensure we sampled a broad range of understorey cover, which we then used as a predictor variable. Prior to analysis, the correlation between the moonlight index, understorey cover and cloud cover was checked using scatterplots and Pearson’s correlation coefficient; all correlations were ≤ |0.20| indicating that collinearity was acceptable for modelling70. Predictor variables were centred and standardised to allow for better interpretation of interactions between continuous variables and to make coefficients associated with different predictor variables comparable71. Data for modelling was derived from sites where species were present, and only species occurring at ≥ 10 sites were included.

We used a Generalised Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) with a binomial family to model species activity as a proportion; the number of hours each species was detected per night divided by the number of hours in the night (defined as the number of complete hours after sunset and before dawn). The response variable was constructed in this way to account for variation in night length during the study period (November to June in season 1 and November to April in season 2). We checked for overdispersion using the performance package72; the dispersion ratio was acceptable (< 1.40) in all cases with the exception of house mouse models where the maximum value was 2.0. We modelled this species using a beta-binomial family to resolve this problem73. Camera nested within site (site/camera) was used as the random factor to account for two sources of non-independence; the use of two cameras per site and the repeated sampling of each site in two seasons. In cat models we excluded the random part of the model as the variance associated with it was zero. We did not use models that accounted for imperfect detection, however previous work in study area suggests that our camera deployment period would result in very high detection probability for our suite of species74.

We used the package glmmTMB75 to build 17 models per species to test predictions associated with the predation risk and habitat mediated predation risk hypotheses (Fig. 1). The core model testing the predation risk hypothesis included the moonlight index as the sole predictor, and the core model testing the habitat mediated predation risk hypothesis included the moonlight index, understorey cover and their interaction. For each of these base models we built additional models that included the additive effects of (1) cloud cover, (2) season and (3) both cloud and season (eight models in total). We created an additional four models where the moonlight index was replaced with understorey cover, and another four that included only the additive effects of moonlight and understory cover (i.e. without an interaction term). We also included a null model as a point of reference. A full list of models and their purpose can be found in Supplementary Table S11.

Models were ranked using the small sample size correction of Akaike’s Information Criterion (AICc) computed in the package MuMIn76 with lower AICc values representing higher ranked, more parsimonious models. Akaike weights, the probability that a model is the best in the set, were also calculated. For each species we used the highest-ranked model including the moonlight index (but without the interaction between moonlight and understorey vegetation) to test the predation risk hypothesis and the highest-ranked model including the interaction between the moonlight index and understorey vegetation to test the habitat-mediated predation risk hypothesis. Predictions from these two models were represented graphically using the packages ggplot277 and cowplot78. For some species, several models received similar support, so we tabulated all models within 2 AICc units of the best and calculated and marginal and conditional r-squared values (R2m and R2c, respectively) using the package performance72. R2m represents the variance explained by the fixed effects only while R2c represents the variance explained by both fixed and random effects.

Supplementary Information

Acknowledgements

We thank Erin Thomas, Lily Wheeler and Rachel Nalliah for assistance with site establishment, all aspects of the field work and mammal identification, and Taylor Reid, Saumya Wanniarachchi and Amanda Lo Cascio for help with habitat assessments. Funding was provided by the Australian Research Council, the Holsworth Research Endowment, the Victorian Department of Environment, Land, Water and Planning, the South Australian Department for Environment and Water, Parks Victoria and SA Water.

Author contributions

P.T., M.S., H.S., J.D. and A.S. conceived the ideas and designed methodology; P.T. and A.S. collected the data; P.T. and J.D. analysed the data; P.T., M.S. and J.D. led the writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed critically to the drafts and gave final approval for publication.

Data availability

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available in the Figshare repository, https://figshare.com/articles/dataset/Moonlight_and_predation_risk/21861846.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-023-37166-1.

References

  • 1.Ripple WJ, et al. Status and ecological effects of the world’s largest carnivores. Science. 2014;343:1241484. doi: 10.1126/science.1241484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gaynor KM, Brown JS, Middleton AD, Power ME, Brashares JS. Landscapes of fear: Spatial patterns of risk perception and response. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2019;34:355–368. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2019.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Beschta RL, Ripple WJ. Riparian vegetation recovery in Yellowstone: The first two decades after wolf reintroduction. Biol. Conserv. 2016;198:93–103. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2016.03.031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ripple WJ, Beschta RL. Trophic cascades in Yellowstone: The first 15years after wolf reintroduction. Biol. Conserv. 2012;145:205–213. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2011.11.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Doherty TS, Glen AS, Nimmo DG, Ritchie EG, Dickman CR. Invasive predators and global biodiversity loss. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2016;113:11261–11265. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602480113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wilmers CC, Estes JA, Edwards M, Laidre KL, Konar B. Do trophic cascades affect the storage and flux of atmospheric carbon? An analysis of sea otters and kelp forests. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2012;10:409–415. doi: 10.1890/110176. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wilmers CC, Schmitz OJ. Effects of gray wolf-induced trophic cascades on ecosystem carbon cycling. Ecosphere. 2016;7:10. doi: 10.1002/ecs2.1501. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.LaManna JA, Martin TE. Costs of fear: Behavioural and life-history responses to risk and their demographic consequences vary across species. Ecol. Lett. 2016;19:403–413. doi: 10.1111/ele.12573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Preisser EL, Bolnick DI, Benard MF. Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator-prey interactions. Ecology. 2005;86:501–509. doi: 10.1890/04-0719. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Zanette LY, White AF, Allen MC, Clinchy M. Perceived predation risk reduces the number of offspring songbirds produce per year. Science. 2011;334:1398–1401. doi: 10.1126/science.1210908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stokes MK, Slade NA, Blair SM. Influences of weather and moonlight on activity patterns of small mammals: A biogeographical perspective. Can. J. Zool. 2001;79:966–972. doi: 10.1139/z01-059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Brown JS. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1988;22:37–47. doi: 10.1007/bf00395696. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Charnov EL. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor. Popul. Biol. 1976;9:129–136. doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nersesian CL, Banks PB, McArthur C. Behavioural responses to indirect and direct predator cues by a mammalian herbivore, the common brushtail possum. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011;66:47–55. doi: 10.1007/s00265-011-1250-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Senior KL, Ramsauer J, McCarthy MA, Kelly LT. The influence of weather and moon phase on small mammal activity. Aust. Mammal. 2021;43:160–167. doi: 10.1071/am19056. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Orrock JL, Danielson BJ. Rodents balancing a variety of risks: invasive fire ants and indirect and direct indicators of predation risk. Oecologia. 2004;140:662–667. doi: 10.1007/s00442-004-1613-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bennie JJ, Duffy JP, Inger R, Gaston KJ. Biogeography of time partitioning in mammals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2014;111:13727–13732. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216063110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Palmer MS, Fieberg J, Swanson A, Kosmala M, Packer C. A 'dynamic' landscape of fear: Prey responses to spatiotemporal variations in predation risk across the lunar cycle. Ecol. Lett. 2017;20:1364–1373. doi: 10.1111/ele.12832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Algar D, Angus GJ, Williams MR, Mellican AE. Influence of bait type, weather and prey abundance on bait uptake by feral cats (Felis catus) on Peron Peninsula, Western Australia. Conserv. Sci. West. Aust. 2007;6:109–149. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kotler BP, Brown JS, Hasson O. Factors affecting gerbil foraging behaviour and rates of owl predation. Ecology. 1991;72:2249–2260. doi: 10.2307/1941575. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Burns PA, et al. Accounting for detectability and abundance in survey design for a declining species. Divers. Distrib. 2019;25:1655–1665. doi: 10.1111/ddi.12966. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Orwings DH, Lockard RB. Different nocturnal activity patterns of Peromyscus californicus and Permyscus eremicus in lunar lighting. Psychon. Sci. 1971;22:63–64. doi: 10.3758/BF03335941. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.van Hensbergen HJ, Martin SC. Climatic factors affecting trapping sucess of some South African small mammals. S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 1993;23:87–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Read JL, Ward MJ, Moseby KE. Factors that influence trap success of sandhill dunnarts (Sminthopsis psammophila) and other small mammals in Triodia dunefields of South Australia. Aust. Mammal. 2015;37:212–218. doi: 10.1071/am14020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gigliotti LC, Diefenbach DR. Risky behavior and its effect on survival: Snowshoe hare behavior under varying moonlight conditions. J. Zool. 2018;305:27–34. doi: 10.1111/jzo.12532. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lang AB, Kalko EK, Romer H, Bockholdt C, Dechmann DK. Activity levels of bats and katydids in relation to the lunar cycle. Oecologia. 2006;146:659–666. doi: 10.1007/s00442-005-0131-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Prugh LR, Golden CD. Does moonlight increase predation risk? Meta-analysis reveals divergent responses of nocturnal mammals to lunar cycles. J. Anim. Ecol. 2014;83:504–514. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Makin DF, Kotler BP. How do Allenby’s gerbils titrate risk and reward in response to different predators? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2020;74:6. doi: 10.1007/s00265-019-2785-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Loggins AA, Shrader AM, Monadjem A, McCleery RA. Shrub cover homogenizes small mammals’ activity and perceived predation risk. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:16857. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-53071-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bowers MA. Seed removal experiments on desert rodents: The microhabitat by moonlight effect. J. Mammal. 1988;69:201–204. doi: 10.2307/1381778. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sutherland DR, Predavec M. The effects of moonlight on microhabitat use by Antechinus agilis (Marsupialia: Dasyuridae) Aust. J. Zool. 1999;47:1–17. doi: 10.1071/ZO98037. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hradsky BA, Mildwaters C, Ritchie EG, Christie F, Di Stefano J. Responses of invasive predators and native prey to a prescribed forest fire. J. Mammal. 2017;98:835–847. doi: 10.1093/jmammal/gyx010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Nalliah R, Sitters H, Smith A, Di Stefano J. Untangling the influences of fire, habitat and introduced predators on the endangered heath mouse. Anim. Conserv. 2022;25:208–220. doi: 10.1111/acv.12731. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hradsky BA. Conserving Australia’s threatened native mammals in predator-invaded, fire-prone landscapes. Wildl. Res. 2020;47:1–15. doi: 10.1071/wr19027. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Bytheway JP, Banks PB. Overcoming prey naivete: Free-living marsupials develop recognition and effective behavioral responses to alien predators in Australia. Glob. Change Biol. 2019;25:1685–1695. doi: 10.1111/gcb.14607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Burbidge AA, McKenzie NL. Patterns in the modern decline of western Australia’s vertebrate fauna: Causes and conservation implications. Biol. Conserv. 1989;50:143–198. doi: 10.1016/0006-3207(89)90009-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Linley GD, Pauligk Y, Marneweck C, Ritchie EG. Moon phase and nocturnal activity of native Australian mammals. Aust. Mammal. 2021;43:190–195. doi: 10.1071/am19070. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Doherty TS, Dickman CR, Nimmo DG, Ritchie EG. Multiple threats, or multiplying the threats? Interactions between invasive predators and other ecological disturbances. Biol. Conserv. 2015;190:60–68. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2015.05.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hradsky BA, et al. Human-modified habitats facilitate forest-dwelling populations of an invasive predator, Vulpes vulpes. Sci Rep. 2017;7:12291. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12464-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cairns KM, Shannon LM, Koler-Matznick J, Ballard JWO, Boyko AR. Elucidating biogeographical patterns in Australian native canids using genome wide SNPs. PLoS One. 2018;13:25. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0198754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Wooster EIF, et al. Predator protection dampens the landscape of fear. Oikos. 2022;2022:9. doi: 10.1111/oik.09059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Brook LA, Johnson CN, Ritchie EG. Effects of predator control on behaviour of an apex predator and indirect consequences for mesopredator suppression. J. Appl. Ecol. 2012;49:1278–1286. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2664.2012.02207.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Wallach AD, et al. Savviness of prey to introduced predators. Conserv. Biol. 2022 doi: 10.1111/cobi.14012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Banks PB, Carthey AJR, Bytheway JP. Australian native mammals recognize and respond to alien predators: a meta-analysis. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B-Biol. Sci. 2018;285:20180857. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.0857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sih A, et al. Predator–prey naïveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator invasions. Oikos. 2010;119:610–621. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.18039.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Paolucci EM, MacIsaac HJ, Ricciardi A. Origin matters: alien consumers inflict greater damage on prey populations than do native consumers. Divers. Distrib. 2013;19:988–995. doi: 10.1111/ddi.12073. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mori E, Menchetti M. Living with roommates in a shared den: Spatial and temporal segregation among semifossorial mammals. Behav. Processes. 2019;164:48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Mori E, Paniccia C, Munkhtsog B, Cicero M, Augugliaro C. Temporal overlap among small- and medium-sized mammals in a grassland and a forest-alpine meadow of Central Asia. Mam. Biol. 2021;101:153–162. doi: 10.1007/s42991-020-00085-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Molsher RL, Gifford EJ, McIlroy JC. Temporal, spatial and individual variation in the diet of red foxes (Vulpes vulpes) in central New South Wales. Wildl. Res. 2000;27:593–601. doi: 10.1071/wr99015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Doherty TS, et al. A continental-scale analysis of feral cat diet in Australia. J. Biogeogr. 2015;42:964–975. doi: 10.1111/jbi.12469. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Davis NE, et al. Interspecific and geographic variation in the diets of sympatric carnivores: Dingoes/wild dogs and red foxes in south-eastern Australia. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0120975. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Harmsen BJ, Foster RJ, Silver SC, Ostro LET, Doncaster CP. Jaguar and puma activity patterns in relation to their main prey. Mamm. Biol. 2011;76:320–324. doi: 10.1016/j.mambio.2010.08.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kronfeld-Schor N, et al. Chronobiology by moonlight. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B-Biol. Sci. 2013;280:20123088. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.3088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Roschlau C, Scheibler E. Foraging behaviour of a desert rodent community: Habitat or moon - Which is more influential? Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 2016;28:394–413. doi: 10.1080/03949370.2015.1076524. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Dickman CR. Predation and habitat shift in the house mouse, Mus domesticus. Ecology. 1992;73:313–322. doi: 10.2307/1938742. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Spencer RJ, Cavanough VC, Baxter GS, Kennedy MS. Adult free zones in small mammal populations: Response of Australian native rodents to reduced cover. Austral Ecol. 2005;30:868–876. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-9993.2005.01530.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Doherty TS, Davis RA, van Etten EJB. A game of cat-and-mouse: Microhabitat influences rodent foraging in recently burnt but not long unburnt shrublands. J. Mammal. 2015;96:324–331. doi: 10.1093/jmammal/gyv034. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Fordyce A, Hradsky BA, Ritchie EG, Di Stefano J. Fire affects microhabitat selection, movement patterns, and body condition of an Australian rodent (Rattus fuscipes) J. Mammal. 2016;97:102–111. doi: 10.1093/jmammal/gyv159. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Fox BJ, Gullick G. Interspecific competition between mice: A reciprocal field manipulation experiment. Aust. J. Ecol. 1989;14:357–366. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-9993.1989.tb01444.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Griffin PC, Griffin SC, Waroquiers C, Mills LS. Mortality by moonlight: predation risk and the snowshoe hare. Behav. Ecol. 2005;16:938–944. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ari074. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Prugh L, Brashares J. Basking in the moonlight? Effect of illumination on capture success of the endangered giant kangaroo rat. J. Mammal. 2010;91:1205–1212. doi: 10.1644/10-mamm-a-011.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Woinarski JCZ, Burbidge AA, Harrison PL. Ongoing unraveling of a continental fauna: Decline and extinction of Australian mammals since European settlement. PNAS. 2015;112:4531–4540. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417301112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Bureau of Meteorology. Climate Data Online. http://www.bom.gov.au/climate/data/.http://www.bom.gov.au/climate/data/ (2022).
  • 64.Duff TJ, Bell TL, York A. Managing multiple species or communities? Considering variation in plant species abundances in response to fire interval, frequency and time since fire in a heathy Eucalyptus woodland. For. Ecol. Manag. 2013;289:393–403. doi: 10.1016/j.foreco.2012.10.032. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Di Stefano J, et al. Defining vegetation age class distributions for multispecies conservation in fire-prone landscapes. Biol. Conserv. 2013;166:111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2013.06.022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Menkhorst P, Knight FA. A field guide to the mammals of Australia. Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Niedballa J, Sollmann R, Courtiol A, Wilting A, Jansen P. camtrapR: An R package for efficient camera trap data management. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2016;7:1457–1462. doi: 10.1111/2041-210x.12600. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Thieurmel, B., Elmarhraoui, A. suncalc: Compute sun position, sunlight phases, moon position and lunar phase. R package version 0.5.0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=suncalc (2019).
  • 69.R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2022).
  • 70.Dormann CF, et al. Collinearity: A review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography. 2013;36:27–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0587.2012.07348.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Schielzeth H. Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2010;1:103–113. doi: 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2010.00012.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lüdecke D, Ben-Shachar MS, Patil I, Waggoner P, Makowski D. performance: An R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. J. Open Source Softw. 2021;6:3139. doi: 10.21105/joss.03139. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Harrison XA. A comparison of observation-level random effect and Beta-Binomial models for modelling overdispersion in Binomial data in ecology & evolution. PeerJ. 2015;3:e1114. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Delaney L, Di Stefano J, Sitters H. Mammal responses to spatial pattern in fire history depend on landscape context. Landsc. Ecol. 2021;36:897–914. doi: 10.1007/s10980-020-01186-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Brooks ME, et al. glmmTMB Balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 2017;9:378–400. doi: 10.32614/rj-2017-066. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Barton, K. MuMIn: Multi-model inference. R package version 1.43.17. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn (2020).
  • 77.Wickham H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer-Verlag; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Wilke, C. O. cowplot: Streamlined Plot Theme and Plot Annotations for ‘ggplot2’. R package version 1.1.1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=cowplot (2020).

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available in the Figshare repository, https://figshare.com/articles/dataset/Moonlight_and_predation_risk/21861846.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES