Abstract
Recent evidence shows that a nap and acute exercise synergistically enhanced memory. Additionally, human-based cross-sectional studies and animal experiments suggest that physical exercise may mitigate the cognitive impairments of poor sleep quality and sleep restriction, respectively. We evaluated whether acute exercise may offset sleep restriction’s impairment of long-term declarative memory compared to average sleep alone. A total of 92 (82% females) healthy young adults (24.6 ± 4.2 years) were randomly allocated to one of four evening groups: sleep restriction only (S5, 5–6 h/night), average sleep only (S8, 8–9 h/night), high-intensity interval training (HIIT) before restricted sleep (HIITS5), or HIIT before average sleep (HIITS8). Groups either followed a 15-min remote HIIT video or rest period in the evening (7:00 p.m.) prior to encoding 80 face-name pairs. Participants completed an immediate retrieval task in the evening. The next morning a delayed retrieval task was given after their subjectively documented sleep opportunities. Long-term declarative memory performance was assessed with the discriminability index (dʹ) during the recall tasks. While our results showed that the dʹ of S8 (0.58 ± 1.37) was not significantly different from those of HIITS5 (−0.03 ± 1.64, p = 0.176) and HIITS8 (−0.20 ± 1.28, p = 0.092), there was a difference in dʹ compared to S5 (−0.35 ± 1.64, p = 0.038) at the delayed retrieval. These results suggest that the acute evening HIIT partially reduced the detrimental effects of sleep restriction on long-term declarative memory.
Keywords: sleep restriction, declarative memory, discriminability index, acute exercise, high-intensity interval training
Graphical abstract
Graphical Abstract.
Statement of Significance.
A combination of a nap and acute moderate-intensity exercise has been shown to enhance long-term memory in young adults. Furthermore, exercise training in sleep-deprived animals has neuroprotective effects on hippocampus-dependent memory, while cross-sectional data suggest that exercise may mitigate the cognitive impairments of poor sleep quality in humans. Here, we show for the first time that performing acute bout of high-intensity exercise before a night-time sleep restriction dampened the detrimental effect of sleep restriction on long-term declarative memory. This novel finding extends knowledge on exercise–sleep interaction on declarative memory and has implications for using acute exercise as a countermeasure to memory deficits of short-term sleep loss. Future studies should investigate the moderators of the exercise–sleep interaction on long-term declarative memory in sleep loss.
Introduction
Despite the importance of adequate sleep duration for maintaining optimal brain and cognitive functioning [1, 2], the prevalence of insufficient sleep is increasing, with over a third of adults aged 18–64 years old sleeping less than the recommended 7 to 9 hours per night [3, 4]. A good night’s sleep is essential for memory processes, namely encoding, consolidation, and retrieval [2, 5]. In particular, post-learning nocturnal non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep rich in slow-wave sleep (SWS) promotes hippocampus-dependent declarative memory consolidation [2, 5, 6]. In contrast, sleep deprivation potentially impairs the hippocampus-dependent declarative memory consolidation [6–8]. Consequently, studies have shown that short sleep duration (<7 h per night) is associated with adverse health outcomes [9, 10] and impaired cognitive functioning including poor attention, decreased concentration, and impaired memory [1, 6, 11, 12].
Pharmacological interventions for improving sleep and cognition have limited effectiveness [13, 14]. Cognitive behavioral therapies, although effective for treating insomnia, can be expensive and difficult to access [15, 16]. Alternatively, accumulating evidence suggests that acute cardiovascular or aerobic exercise is a promising, non-pharmacological approach for improving brain health and memory functions [17]. In addition to improving sleep [18], exercise also enhances memory [19–22] by inducing several physiological processes that modify key aspects of sleep essential for memory processing [23–25]. The beneficial effects of exercise on memory are modulated by factors such as the frequency, intensity, duration, and timing of exercise relative to the stages of memory formation [20, 21], as well as persons’ fitness levels [26]. For example, the meta-analysis by Roig et al. [21] revealed that acute exercise performed before encoding of memory trace led to moderate and moderate-to-large effect enhancements on short-term and long-term memories, respectively [21]. Furthermore, acute high-intensity exercise occurring before the encoding or early memory consolidation phase induced greater effects on long-term memory in young adults [19, 27]. This effect may result from the positive relationship between exercise intensity and brain-derived neurotrophic factors (BDNF) [28–31], as well as the activation of molecular pathways (e.g. long-term potentiation) that subserve long-term memory [32, 33]. These studies suggest that acute exercise improves memory in a time-dependent fashion by priming the molecular processes involved in the encoding and consolidation of newly acquired information [17, 32].
Although both physical exercise and sleep can individually enhance memory [2, 17], emerging evidence suggests that they may also interact synergistically to improve memory in adults [17, 34, 35]. Notably, the work by Mograss et al [35]. provided the first empirical evidence that exercise (i.e. 40-min moderate-intensity cycling) and sleep (i.e. a 60-min daytime NREM nap) operated synergistically to improve memory in 115 young adults. Additionally, cross-sectional interactive associations between physical activity and sleep on executive functions [36, 37] or memory [34] in adults indicate that physical exercise may mitigate the cognitive deficits of poor sleep quality. In animal studies [38, 39], but not in humans [40], exercise training resulted in neuroprotective effects on hippocampal-dependent short-term and long-term memory, possibly by preventing decreases in BDNF [38, 39]. To the best of our knowledge, however, no studies have investigated the extent to which acute exercise may compensate for the detrimental effects of sleep restriction on declarative memory.
In addition to inducing greater energy expenditure [41] and gains in cardiorespiratory fitness [42, 43], high-intensity interval training (HIIT), characterized by very intense exercise bouts interspersed with short rest intervals, is a time-efficient alternative to continuous moderate-intensity training, which requires longer exercise duration (e.g. 30–60 min) [44, 45]. Given that lack of time is a major barrier to exercise [46, 47], a low-volume HIIT paradigm may have greater uptake in the general population [46, 47]. HIIT can be performed in the home environment [45, 48], particularly HIIT programs employing body-weight resistance aerobic exercises [49, 50]. Moreover, HIIT may augment the memory effects of sleep [1, 2, 5] or provide memory enhancement that is superior or comparable to moderate-intensity exercise [31, 51–54]. While daytime moderate-to-vigorous intensity exercise is often recommended for improving sleep [55–57], an early evening high-intensity exercise may also promote phase advance of circadian sleep propensity, enhance physiological recovery and subsequent sleep [58]. In contrast to popular belief, meta-analyses have shown that acute evening exercise of low-to-moderate [59, 60] or high intensity [60, 61] occurring 2–4 h before bedtime do not disrupt subsequent nocturnal NREM sleep. In this context, HIIT in the early evening period may be particularly convenient and practical for some individuals to incorporate exercise habits into their daily routines. This timing of HIIT may be ideal for the evening- and/or neither-type chronotypes [62–65], as well as the morning types at risk of potential sleep disturbance following late-evening HIIT [62].
The objective of this study was to estimate the extent to which a remote, evening HIIT intervention performed in the evening prior to encoding (learning) would compensate for the negative effect of sleep restriction on long-term declarative memory (assessed with face-name association tasks) compared with an average sleep opportunity alone in healthy young adults. It was hypothesized that the acute evening HIIT would decrease the long-term declarative memory impairment after a few hours of sleep restriction. Based on prior research [35], we also hypothesized that acute evening HIIT would enhance long-term declarative memory recall following an average nocturnal sleep compared with an average sleep alone.
Methods
Study design
We conducted a home-based, remote study with a between-subject design comprising of four groups in a 2 (restricted sleep, 5–6 h/night or average sleep, 8–9 h/night) × 2 (HIIT or no exercise) design (Figure 1). To comply with the restrictions related to the COVID-19 pandemic, participants completed all the study procedures remotely through video conferencing (i.e. zoom) and/or remote access to research computers via TeamViewer software (v15.34.4, GmbH, Germany). Depending on the group, participants completed three or four online meetings (lasting 20–45 min) on three separate days for orientation and practice sessions involving the HIIT intervention (exercise groups only), and the memory task. The memory task (Face-Name Tasks) consisted of three phases: encoding, immediate retrieval, and delayed recall after either an average or a restricted night-time sleep opportunity in the participants’ home environment. This study was approved by the institutional ethics review board at Concordia University. All participants signed an informed consent form and provided verbal consent recorded via video conferencing.
Figure 1.
Study protocol. HIIT: high-intensity interval training; HRmax: maximal heart rate; RPE: rate of perceived exertion. S5: sleep restriction (5–6 h/night sleep opportunity); S8: average sleep (8–9 h/night sleep opportunity); HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction; HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity.
Participants
One hundred and eight (87 females and 21 males) healthy participants were initially recruited for this study from August, 2020 to August, 2022. Participants were young adults aged 18–35 years old with good sleep quality and reported having light to moderate physical activity levels based on metabolic equivalent (MET)-minutes per week [66]. We excluded volunteers who had a past or current history of sleep, psychiatric, neurological, or other medical conditions, and/or were shift workers or had traveled through more than one-time zone within the last month prior to the study. In addition, participants who were obese, pregnant, or taking medications with a known effect on sleep and cognition, as well as having uncorrected visual problems were excluded from the study. We excluded participants with obesity (i.e. BMI ≥30 kg/m2) since we could not objectively assess sleep disorders such as obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) through baseline polysomnography—the gold standard diagnostic test [67]. OSA increases progressively with increasing BMI and obesity is associated with OSA [68] and impaired cognitive function [69, 70]. However, participants with high risk of sleep apnea were excluded using the STOP-Bang questionnaire (SBQ: cutoff ≥5) [71]. Those participants without access to a functional computer compatible with the memory tasks software (PsychoPy v3.0.7, Open Science Tools Ltd.) and/or internet connectivity were excluded.
The final analysis was conducted on a total of 92 participants. The exclusion of 16 participants from the initial sample (N = 108) was due to non-adherence to the sleep protocol (n = 7), obesity (n =2), missing recall data (n = 5), and the other two either took a nap or exercised on the study day (Figure 2).
Figure 2.
Flow diagram showing participant recruitment. BMI: body mass index; HIIT: high-intensity interval training; TIB: time in bed; TST: total sleep time; SD: standard deviation. Groups: S8: average sleep; S5: sleep restriction; HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity and HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction.
Recruitment and screening
Participants were recruited through local flyers and advertisements on the Concordia university campus and in the community, as well as via social media platforms including Facebook and Instagram. Persons who initially expressed interest in participating in the study were asked to complete an online consent form prior to the screening questionnaires. Detailed descriptions of the screening questionnaires are published elsewhere [35]. Briefly, participants’ subjective sleep quality and daytime sleepiness were assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) [72] and the Epworth Sleepiness Scale [73], respectively. Participants were screened for sleep disorders using the Insomnia Severity Index [74] and the STOP-Bang Questionnaire (SBQ: for assessing the risk of OSA) [71] and the Ullanlinna Narcolepsy Scale [75]. Participants’ mood or psychological disorders were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) [76] and the Beck Anxiety Inventory [77]. We used the Morning-Eveningness Questionnaire to assess participants’ chronotype [78] and the short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire to estimate participants’ levels of physical activity in MET-minutes per week. The participants’ eligibility to participate in the study was determined by the scores of their completed questionnaires against the standard cutoff scores for each questionnaire (Supplementary Table S1). Where necessary, follow-up telephone calls or interviews were made to verify participants’ information. Eligible participants were then asked to fill out a sleep diary to ensure a consistent sleep schedule for at least 4 days prior to the experimental evening including their sleep on the experimental night.
Orientation and familiarization
Three to seven days prior to the scheduled experimental evening, participants underwent a virtual orientation with a practice session. Prior to this online meeting, participants received instructions and web-links to download the software (PsychoPy3 v.3.0.7, Open Science Tools Ltd) for the memory tasks (Face-Name task), the pre-recorded HIIT instructional video, and instructions for the software installations and the HIIT session. During the orientation, participants were given detailed information about the various components of the study protocol including the software to be used, HIIT intervention, Borg’s scale for monitoring exercise intensity, memory tasks and the sleep restriction protocol. The PsychoPy (v.3.0.7) software needed for the Face-Name memory tasks and remote access control software (TeamViewer) was installed on participants’ computers. After the software installations, the encoding and retrieval practice tasks were run to test the specific keys for the encoding (left and right arrow keys) and the retrieval (“u,” “i,” “o,” and “p” keys) tasks. In addition, participants were introduced to the HIIT video and were also asked to perform one set of 20 seconds each of the HIIT exercises to familiarize themselves with the HIIT protocol and to ensure they could correctly and safely perform them. This phase was carried out by trained kinesiologists and physiotherapists. Participants were instructed to arrange for a spacious place and wear comfortable clothes and shoes for the exercise. We also asked participants to refrain from strenuous exercise from the day of orientation to the experimental day and avoid napping, caffeine or alcohol intake and cigarette smoking in the 24 h preceding the experimental evening. As a safety measure, participants were instructed to stop the exercise if they experienced any cardiorespiratory or musculoskeletal signs and symptoms such as chest pain, cyanosis, difficulty breathing, dizziness or lightheadedness, intense muscle cramps or intense back or join pains. In the event of such adverse effects, they were asked to report by completing the HIIT symptom checklist (Supplementary Material S1).
Most importantly, participants were informed about their expected exercise intensity estimated based on each participant’s age-predicted maximal heart rate (HRmax) using the formula: (220 – age) × (0.85–0.95) [79, 80], corresponding to 85–95% of HRmax [42, 81]. This intensity range was equivalent to the rating of perceived exertion (RPE) scores of 15–19 (i.e. “very hard” to “very, very hard”) on the Borg’s scale (i.e. 6–20 scale) [82] for 18–35 year range. The participants were instructed to achieve their individualized target exercise intensity (i.e. RPE of 15–19 corresponding to 85–95% of HRmax) based on increases in heart rate, breathing rate, sweating, and muscle fatigue during the exercise intervention [83]. Although the preferred approach is to conduct a laboratory-based cardiorespiratory fitness test (i.e. maximal oxygen consumption test, VO2max) to directly determine exercise capacity, or alternatively, conduct a field test of cardiorespiratory fitness, as well as objectively monitor exercise intensity (using a heart rate monitor or accelerometer), these could not be performed because of the COVID-19 pandemic-related lockdowns and movement restrictions. We also did not have access to remote activity monitoring methods (e.g. Verisense system, Shimmer Research Ltd [84]) to objectively assess participants’ compliance with the exercise protocol.
Sample size and randomization
Based on a prior study [35] that found an interaction between acute moderate-intensity exercise and a NREM nap on declarative memory ( = 0.053, power = 71.1%), using G*power (v.3.1.9.6), a priori sample size calculation showed that 116 participants were needed for this study at the same study power. Participants were randomized using computer-generated random numbers by a research assistant who did not participate in other aspects of the study. The randomization was completed using the Microsoft Excel’s “RANDBETWEEN” and “CHOOSE” functions. These functions were combined as: (CHOOSE(RANDBETWEEN(1,4),”S5,””S8,””HIITS5,””HIITS8”)) to randomly and equally assign (i.e. 1:1:1:1 ratio) participants into one of the four groups: (1) S5: sleep restriction (5–6 h sleep opportunity), (2) S8: average sleep only (8-9 hour sleep opportunity), (3) HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction, and (4) HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity. The group allocation was stratified by sex (CHOOSE(RANDBETWEEN(1,2),”male,””female”)). Participants’ allocation was concealed until 24 h before experimental evening procedures.
Experimental procedures
On the experimental day, participants were asked to rest, be fully hydrated, have a bottle of water, and eat no less than 2 h before the exercise session. At 6:45 pm on the experimental evening, participants in the HIIT groups met remotely with research assistants to prepare for the HIIT session at 7:00 pm. After the exercise session, participants were at liberty to take a 5–10-min shower and have a light meal. Participants attended a second online meeting that evening to undertake the memory tasks (i.e. encoding and immediate recall) at either 10:00 pm for the average sleep or 11:00 pm for sleep restriction groups. Afterwards, the average sleep groups went to bed at 11:00 pm until 7:30 am the next morning, while the sleep restriction groups went to bed from 12:30 am to 6:00 am. All participants were phoned at their respective bedtimes and wake times to ensure adherence to the sleep opportunities. Finally, the participants attended their last zoom meeting and completed the delayed recall tasks an hour after waking (i.e. 7:00 am and 8:30 am, respectively for the restricted and average sleep groups, Figure 1). Upon completion, all participants received a participation compensation.
The HIIT and no-exercise interventions
Participants in the exercise groups (HIITS5 and HIITS8) completed a 15-min HIIT intervention during which the work phase of the HIIT was expected to be performed at an average RPE corresponding to 85–95% of their age-predicted HRmax. The choice of the 15-min HIIT protocol was in accordance with the current physical activity guidelines, which recommend that adults should accumulate at least 150 min of moderate-intensity physical activity or 75 min of vigorous-intensity exercise per week [85]. Participants exercised alongside the pre-recorded video. The HIIT intervention consisted of a 2-min warm-up, a 6-min work phase of high-intensity intervals at an average RPE corresponding to 85–95% of participants’ age-predicted HRmax, interspersed with 6 min of passive rest periods in a 1:1 work phase to rest ratio fashion and a cool-down session (Figure S1). Participants started the HIIT session with the warm-up consisting of four exercises (i.e. spot running, jumping jacks, high knees, and squats) at low-to-moderate intensity (RPE of 11–13) lasting 30 s each. This was followed by the actual HIIT workouts comprising six intervals including side jump skating, squat jump, caterpillar walk with contralateral shoulder tap, reverse lunge with hop, burpees, and mountain climbers, designed in a Tabata style (Figure S1). Participants performed 3 repetitions of each interval lasting 20 seconds (i.e. 6 min: 6 intervals × 3 sets × 20 s) and each repetition was followed by 20 s of passive rest (i.e. 6 min: 6 rest intervals × 3 × 20 s). The participants ended the HIIT session with a cool-down comprising of stretching and breathing exercises. Immediately after the HIIT session, participants gave their overall RPE during the work phase and resting phase using the Borg’s scale [82] and also completed a 4-item questionnaire for their feedback on the HIIT intervention (Supplementary Material S1). Although not monitored remotely via zoom, the non-exercise groups (S5 and S8) were asked to observe a sedentary procedure (i.e. sitting) for 15 min at home.
Assessment of outcomes
Long-term declarative memory.
We used a modified version of the face-name association task to assess long-term declarative memory recall [86]. The memory tasks consisted of three stages: encoding (learning), immediate, and delayed retrieval sessions. Each stage of the memory task was preceded by a short practice task. For each task, the full set of instructions were read to participants before they started the task. During the encoding session, participants were presented with 80 face-name pairs (50% females) only once to memorize for immediate and delayed retrievals. The photographs and associated names were presented on their computer screen for 5 s using the PsychoPy software (v 3.0.7). The participants were required to to select either “yes” or “no” with the “left” or “right” arrow keys of their computers’ keyboard, respectively in response to the question: “does the name match the face?.” On average, this stage took approximately 10 min. At the end of the encoding task, participants were asked to rate their sleepiness during the task by using the Karolinska sleepiness scale (KSS)—a 9-item questionnaire rating from 1 to 9 corresponding to “extremely alert” to “very sleepy,” respectively. Participants were allowed up to a 5-min break before the immediate retrieval task.
During the immediate and delayed retrievals, participants were presented with a set of 120 faces on their computer screen, where 40 of them were new faces added to the encoded stimuli as “foils.” The participants were required to select the correct face-name pairs as presented during encoding session by using the “u,” “i,” and “o” keys of their computers’ keyboard for the first, second and the third name options respectively, or selected “new” with the “p” key, if the face was not familiar (i.e. not previously seen during encoding). Whereas the immediate retrieval of the face-name tasks was done immediately (~5 to 10 min) after encoding, the delayed retrieval retest was conducted after the overnight average or restricted sleep opportunity. The retrieval tasks took approximately 20 min.
The primary outcome was long-term memory performance assessed with the discriminability index (dʹ) at the delayed retrieval and the difference in dʹ (i.e. delayed − immediate retrievals). The dʹ indicates a participant’s ability to discriminate between old and new items during the retrievals [87]. The dʹ was calculated using the formula: dʹ = ZHT − ZFA [87, 88]; where the ZHT and ZFA are the z scores of the hit (HT) and the false alarm (FA) rates, respectively. The HT is the probability that the participant classifies an old item as old, whereas the FA is the probability of classifying a new item as old [87]. The higher the value of dʹ, the higher the sensitivity to discriminate stimuli [87]. Secondary outcomes included the self-reported sleep variables, as well as the motivation, performance, and sleepiness ratings at both the immediate and delayed retrievals. Participants rated their motivation, perceived task difficulty and performance with subjective scales (i.e. 0, low to 10, high), as well as rating of their sleepiness (using KSS) at the end of the retrieval tasks.
Baseline and experimental night-time sleep quality.
Sleep quality was assessed 4 days prior to and the night following the experimental procedures with the consensus sleep diary [89]. Baseline sleep quality was assessed as the average of the 4 days preceding the experimental night. The sleep variables from the online sleep diary assessed were: (1) time in bed – total time spent in bed, (2) total sleep time (TST) is the amount of sleep duration (hours) from sleep onset to time out of bed, (3) sleep onset latency (SOL), the amount of time (minutes) participants took to fall asleep, (4) sleep efficiency (SE) defined as the percent of the time asleep out of the amount of time spent in bed, (5) wake after sleep onset (WASO) is the estimated amount of time (minutes) awake during sleep after initial sleep onset [89], and (6) awakenings, calculated as the estimated total number of nocturnal awakenings.
Statistical analysis
Data were analyzed with SPSS (version 28). Data were checked for a normality of distribution with the Shapiro–Wilks test. We conducted a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with interaction analyses between the exercise (HIIT or no exercise) and sleep (average or restricted sleep) conditions on the memory performance outcome (dʹ) at the immediate and delayed retrievals and their difference score (delayed − immediate retrievals). Where significant main effects of exercise and/or sleep as well as their interaction effects occurred, post hoc analyses with Bonferroni correction were performed to determine the differences in memory performance between the groups. An analysis of covariance was conducted to assess the effects of RPE during the HIIT on the differences in dʹ between the HIIT groups at the immediate and delayed retrievals, as well as the difference in dʹ (delayed – immediate retrievals). We used one-way ANOVA (TIB, TST, and SE) or Kruskal–Wallis test (SOL, awakenings WASO) to compare the sleep variables at baseline and the experimental night between the groups. Additionally, one-way ANOVA was used to compare the KSS, motivation, performance difficulty and perceived performance of the memory tasks scores between the groups. For all tests, statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05.
Results
Participants’ characteristics
The final analysis was conducted on a total of 92 participants (82% female) with an average age of 24.6 (SD, 4.2) years and a normal BMI (M = 22.2, SD = 2.9 kg/m2). On average, participants were moderately physically active (M = 1627.32, SD = 776.61 MET-minute) with an intermediate chronotype (M = 54.91, SD = 8.82). About 52%, 41%, and 7% of the participants were students, workers, and unemployed, respectively. There were no significant differences between the groups in the demographic, psychological, chronotype, and activity characteristics, except for PSQI score F(3, 88) = 4.223, p < 0.008; Table 1), which was lower in the S5 (M = 2.38, SD = 1.21) compared to the S8 (M = 3.57, SD = 1.61, p = 0.009). For the exercise groups (HIITS5 and HIITS8), the mean expected RPE corresponding to 85–95% of participants’ age-predicted HRmax was from 16.3 (SD = 0.52) to 18.7 (SD = 0.5) while the observed RPE during the HIIT session was 17.5 (SD = 1.0). There were no significant differences in the RPE ratings at baseline (p = 0.760), as well as during the exercise (p = 0.330) and the resting phases (p = 0.992) of the HIIT session between the HIITS5 and HIITS8.
Table 1.
Participants’ characteristics
Variable | S8 (N = 23) |
S5 (N = 24) |
HIIT S8 (N = 21) |
HIIT S5 (N = 24) |
Cutoff scores |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex, F (M) | 18 (5) | 19 (5) | 18 (3) | 20 (4) | N/A |
Age (years) | 25.7 (4.2) | 25.5 (3.9) | 23.5 (4.7) | 23.6 (3.9) | 18–35 |
BMI (kg/m2) | 21.5 (3.5) | 22.6 (2.8) | 22.0 (2.7) | 22.4 (2.8) | >30 |
PSQI* | 3.57 (1.61) | 2.38 (1.21) | 2.62 (1.36) | 2.58 (1.25) | >5 |
ESS | 3.65 (2.25) | 4.63 (3.42) | 4.14 (3.48) | 3.92 (2.57) | >10 |
MEQ | 54.39 (8.67) | 54.75 (10.39) | 56.38 (8.19) | 54.29 (8.20) | N/A |
BDI | 2.96 (3.69) | 2.58 (3.56) | 2.52 (4.26) | 2.88 (3.41) | >20 |
BAI | 2.78 (2.24) | 3.38 (4.67) | 3.33 (2.63) | 3.25 (3.43) | >26 |
ISI | 2.22 (3.09) | 2.00 (2.72) | 2.38 (3.46) | 2.08 (2.36) | >14 |
SBQ | 0.52 (0.67) | 0.54 (0.66) | 0.29 (0.56) | 0.58 (0.72) | ≥ 5 |
UNS | 4.70 (3.02) | 5.67 (3.93) | 4.62 (2.04) | 5.17 (1.81) | >14 |
EHI | 18.43 (4.86) | 13.38 (11.89) | 16.10 (9.34) | 14.46 (8.63) | N/A |
IPAQ (MET-min/wk) | 1376.97 (779.37) | 1819.19 (693.34) | 1570.95 (692.91) | 1724.69 (890.58) | >3000 |
Occupation | |||||
Students | 9 | 12 | 11 | 16 | N/A |
Workers | 11 | 11 | 8 | 8 | N/A |
Unemployed | 3 | 1 | 2 | 0 | N/A |
N = 92. Sex (females (males)) and occupation presented as numbers. Other data presented as Means (SD).
Groups; S8: average sleep opportunity; S5: sleep restriction; HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity; and HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction. HIIT: high-intensity interval training; F: females (N = 75, 81.5%); M: males (N = 17, 18.5%); PSQI: Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; ESS: Epworth Sleepiness Scale; MEQ: Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory; ISI: Insomnia Severity Index; SBQ: Obstructive Sleep Apnea Questionnaire; UNS: Ullanlinna Narcolepsy Scale; EHI: Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; MEG: Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire; IPAQ: International Physical Activity Questionnaire; BMI: body mass index; MET-min/wk: Metabolic Equivalent-Minutes per week; N/A: not applicable.
*PSQI: significantly different between S8 vs. S5, p = 0.009.
Memory performance accuracy
Table 2 shows 2 × 2 ANOVAs with interaction analyses of exercise (HIIT or no-HIIT) and sleep (average or restricted sleep) conditions on the discriminability index (dʹ) at the immediate and delayed retrievals, and their difference (delayed – immediate retrievals). There were no statistically significant main effects of the exercise [F(1, 88) = 0.015, p = 0.903, = 0.001] and sleep [F(1, 88) = 0.602, p = 0.440, = 0.007] conditions and their interaction [F(1, 88) = 3.267, p = 0.074, = 0.036] on the dʹ at the immediate retrieval (Table 2 and Figure 3).
Table 2.
2 × 2 ANOVA with interaction analysis between the HIIT and sleep conditions on memory performance
Variables | Source | SS | df | MS | F | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Immediate retrieval dʹ | HIIT/no-HIIT | 0.036 | 1 | 0.036 | 0.015 | 0.903 | 0.001 |
Average/restricted sleep | 1.430 | 1 | 1.430 | 0.602 | 0.440 | 0.007 | |
Interaction | 7.760 | 1 | 7.760 | 3.267 | 0.074 | 0.036 | |
Error | 209.028 | 88 | 2.375 | ||||
Total | 218.390 | 92 | |||||
Delayed retrieval dʹ | HIIT/no-HIIT | 1.193 | 1 | 1.193 | 0.532 | 0.468 | 0.006 |
Average/restricted sleep | 3.294 | 1 | 3.294 | 1.468 | 0.229 | 0.016 | |
Interaction | 6.745 | 1 | 6.745 | 3.006 | 0.050* | 0.033 | |
Error | 197.459 | 88 | 2.244 | ||||
Total | 208.770 | 92 | |||||
Difference in dʹ (delayed – immediate retrieval) | HIIT/no-HIIT | 2.814 | 1 | 2.814 | 1.631 | 0.205 | 0.018 |
Average/restricted sleep | 1.784 | 1 | 1.784 | 1.035 | 0.312 | 0.012 | |
Interaction | 0.223 | 1 | 0.223 | 0.129 | 0.720 | 0.001 | |
Error | 151.773 | 88 | 1.725 | ||||
Total | 156.665 | 92 |
N = 92; *p ≤ 0.05.
Groups: S8: average sleep opportunity (n = 23); S5: sleep restriction (n = 24); HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity (n = 21); HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction (n = 24).
dʹ: discriminability index : partial eta-squared; SS: Type III sum of squares; df: degree of freedom; MS: mean square.
Figure 3.
Comparisons of the immediate retrieval discriminability index (dʹ) between the non-exercise and high-intensity exercise groups. Groups: S5: sleep restriction; S8: average sleep opportunity; HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction; HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity; d: Cohen’s d. Means ± SEM.
There was a significant interaction between the effects of the exercise and sleep on the dʹ at the delayed retrieval [F(1, 88) = 3.006, p = 0.050, = 0.033] without significant main effects of the exercise [F(1, 88) = 0.532, p = 0.468, = 0.006] and sleep [F(1, 88) = 1.468, p = 0.229, = 0.016] conditions (Table 2 and Figure 4). Post hoc comparisons with Bonferroni correction showed that the dʹ of S8 (M = 0.58, SD = 1.37) was not statistically different from that of HIITS5 (M = −0.03, SD = 1.64, p = 0.176, d = 0.404) and HIITS8 (M = −0.20,SD = 1.28, p = 0.092, d = 0.581); but was significantly higher than that of S5 (M = −0.35, SD = 1.64, p = 0.038, d = 0.611). The dʹ of HIITS5 (M = −0.03, SD = 1.64) was not significantly different from those of S5 (M = −0.35, SD = 1.64, p = 0.469, d = 0.192) and HIITS8 (M = −0.20, SD = 1.28, p = 0.716, d = 0.116), as did the difference between S5 (M = −0.35, SD = 1.64) and HIITS8 (M = −0.20, SD = 1.28, p = 0.735, d = 0.102) at the delayed retrieval (Figure 4).
Figure 4.
Comparisons of the delayed retrieval discriminability index (dʹ) between the non-exercise and high-intensity exercise groups. Groups: S5: sleep restriction; S8: average sleep opportunity; HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction; HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity. Means ± SEM; d: Cohen’s d. Means ± SEM. *Significant, p = 0.038.
In addition, the exercise [F(1, 88) = 1.631, p = 0.205, = 0.018] and sleep [F(1, 88) = 1.035, p = 0.312, = 0.012] conditions and their interaction [F(1, 88) = 0.129, p = 0.720, = 0.001] showed no significant main effects on the difference in the dʹ (delayed − immediate retrievals) (Table 2 and Figure 5).
Figure 5.
Comparisons of the difference in discriminability index (delayed – immediate retrieval) between the non-exercise and high-intensity exercise groups. Groups: S5: sleep restriction; S8: average sleep opportunity; HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction; HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity; d: Cohen’s d. Means ± SEM.
An analysis of covariance revealed no significant effects of RPE during the HIIT on the differences in dʹ between HIITS5 and HIITS8 at the immediate (p = 0.212, = 0.037) and delayed (p = 0.184, = 0.042) retrievals, as well as the difference in dʹ (p = 0. 673, = 0.004) (Table S2).
Performance and sleepiness ratings
A one-way ANOVA showed that the ratings of performance (p = 0.771), performance difficulty (p = 0.148), motivation (p = 0.059), and KSS score (p = 0.116) during the immediate retrieval task were not significantly different between the groups (Figure S2A–D). During the delayed retrieval, while perceived performance and performance difficulty ratings were not significantly different between the groups (p = 0.465 and p = 0.823, respectively), motivation (p = 0.030) and KSS (p < 0.001) scores showed group differences (Figure S3A–D). Follow-up analyses showed that the HIITS5 (p = 0.073) and HIITS8 (p = 0.612) had a similar motivation to the S8 whose motivation level at the delayed retrieval was significantly higher than that of S5 (p = 0.041) (Figure S3A). The KSS score was significantly higher in the S5 relative to the S8 (p < 0.001) and HIITS8 (p < 0.001), while the sleepiness score was not significantly different between the HIITS5 and S8 (p = 0.595), as well as between HIITS5 and HIITS8 (p = 0.144) (Figure S3D).
Sleep quality
Participants’ self-reported sleep of the four nights preceding the experimental evening were comparable between the groups (p > 0.05, Table 3). As expected, TIB F(3,89) = 190.41, p < 0.001) and TST F(3,89) = 405.59, p < 0.001) showed significant group differences in the night following the experimental procedures (Table 4). The sleep restriction groups slept significantly shorter than the average sleep conditions: TIB (S5 and S8, HITS5 and HITS8, S8 and HITS5, S5 and HIITS8, p < 0.001) and TST (S5 and S8; HITS5 and HITS8; S8 and HITS5, S5 and HIITS8, p < 0.001). Only the SOL showed a significant group difference (p = 0.027) where the S5 slept faster compared to S8 (p = 0.026, d = −0.883) and HIITS8 (p = 0.036, d = 0.751) (Table 4).
Table 3.
Sleep diary variables four days before the evening experimental procedures
Variable | S8 (N = 23) |
S5 (N = 24) |
HIITS8 (N = 21) |
HIITS5 (N = 24) |
P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
TIB (h)* | 9.26 (0.73) | 9.2 (0.99) | 8.90 (0.52) | 9.14 (0.99) | 0.677 |
TST (h)* | 7.96 (0.78) | 8.08 (0.68) | 7.99 (0.53) | 8.09 (0.65) | 0.865 |
SOL (min)† | 15.32 (8.60) | 11.10 (6.96) | 14.23 (7.03) | 13.43 (6.46) | 0.145 |
AWK (number)† | 0.86 (0.73) | 0.98 (1.16) | 0.63 (0.62) | 0.50 (0.56) | 0.210 |
WASO (min)† | 6.98 (6.70) | 6.44 (9.90) | 4.43 (6.06) | 3.08 (3.35) | 0.126 |
SE (%)* | 87.17 (5.66) | 88.60 (6.30) | 89.82 (3.57) | 89.09 (5.76) | 0.245 |
N = 92. Data are presented as Mean (SD).
Groups; S8: average sleep opportunity; S5: sleep restriction; HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep opportunity; and HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction.
HIIT: high-intensity interval training; TIB: time in bed; TST: total sleep time is the amount of sleep duration between the time in bed and time out of bed; AWK: number of awakenings after sleep onset; SOL: sleep onset latency; SE: sleep efficiency is the percent of the time asleep out of the amount of time spent in bed; WASO: wake after sleep onset.
*One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA).
†Kruskal–Wallis test.
Table 4.
Sleep diary variables of the night following the experimental procedures
Variable | S8 (N = 23) |
S5 (N = 24) |
HIITS8 (N = 21) |
HIITS5 (N = 24) |
P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
TIB (h)*,‡ | 8.74 (0.40) | 6.0 (0.66) | 8.74 (0.40) | 5.9 (0.67) | <0.001* |
TST (h)*,§ | 8.09 (0.30) | 5.4 (0.54) | 7.96 (0.39) | 5.20 (0.19) | <0.001* |
SOL (min)†,‖ | 14.22 (7.00) | 8.50 (5.92) | 15.62 (12.37) | 12.29 (7.03) | 0.027* |
AWK (number)† | 1.09 (1.41) | 0.96 (1.12) | 0.76 (1.00) | 0.33 (0.70) | 0.065 |
WASO (min)† | 5.04 (7.02) | 3.25 (4.98) | 4.29 (7.91) | 2.02 (5.26) | 0.078 |
SE (%)* | 92.58 (3.86) | 89.93 (5.90) | 91.08 (5.38) | 88.35 (7.75) | 0.102 |
N = 92; *p < 0.05. Data are presented as Mean (SD).
Groups; S8: average sleep opportunity; S5: sleep restriction; HIITS8: HIIT plus average sleep; and HIITS5: HIIT plus sleep restriction.
HIIT: high-intensity interval training; TIB: time in bed; TST: total sleep time is the amount of sleep duration between the time in bed and time out of bed; AWK: number of awakenings after sleep onset; SOL: sleep onset latency; SE: sleep efficiency is the percent of the time asleep out of the amount of time spent in bed; WASO: wake after sleep onset.
*One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA).
†Kruskal–Wallis test.
‡TIB: Significantly different between S5 & S8; HITS5 & HITS8; S8 & HITS5 (all, p < 0.001).
§TST: Significantly different between S5 & S8; HITS5 & HITS8; S8 & HITS5 (all, p < 0.001).
‖SOL: Significantly different between S8 & S5 (p = 0.026); and between S5 & HITS8 (p = 0.031).
Discussion
To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to evaluate the extent to which acute exercise may compensate the long-term declarative memory impairment of sleep restriction in adults. As hypothesized, whereas acute sleep restriction impaired long-term declarative memory (S5 vs. S8), acute evening HIIT attenuated the memory impairment caused by sleep restriction on long-term memory performance relative to the average sleep condition (HIITS5 vs. S8). We found no statistically substantial difference in the discriminability index between the average sleep alone (S8) and the exercise conditions (HIITS5 and HIITS8), except the sleep restriction alone condition (S5) during the delayed retrieval. However, the discriminability index of the HIITS5 was not statistically higher than that of the S5. In addition, the difference in the discriminability index between the delayed and immediate retrievals was comparable between the conditions. Contrary to our hypothesis, the acute evening HIIT intervention did not increase memory performance after an average nocturnal sleep in the HIITS8.
The results of this study show that while sleep restriction impaired long-term declarative memory, a short bout of acute exercise at a high intensity appeared to have a neuroprotective effect by partially diminishing the negative impact of sleep restriction on long-term declarative memory performance at the delayed retrieval. This novel finding adds important insights to the growing body of literature on the exercise-sleep interaction on cognition [34–37]. Our findings partly support cross-sectional studies that suggest that physical exercise may mitigate the cognitive deficits of poor sleep quality [34, 36, 37].
Although a few human experimental studies have evaluated the interactions between exercise, total sleep deprivation, and cognition, they have mainly focused on the protective effects of exercise on executive functions and fatigue as opposed to memory [40, 90]. For example, Slutsky et al [90]. assessed the effects of a 15-min low-intensity cycling (40% of heart rate reserve) versus a seated control condition on cognitive performance including working memory in 24 active young adults and found no group difference in cognitive performance. Moreover, they administered the exercise intervention after the 24-hour sustained wakefulness and did not also assess the mediating effects of the acute low-intensity exercise on long-term memory [90].
Conversely, the potential of exercise to protect against sleep restriction-induced memory impairments has been previously investigated in a series of animal studies using regular or chronic exercise training protocols and sleep deprivation protocols mainly targeting REM sleep [38, 39, 91]. These animal studies demonstrated neuroprotective effects of a regular pre-sleep deprivation exercise training on short-term and long-term memory [38, 39]. Their findings provide rich insights into how acute exercise may mitigate the negative impact of sleep restriction on memory. For example, Zagaar et al [39]. observed that 4 weeks of regular treadmill exercise prior to a 24-h sleep deprivation prevented impairments of spatial learning, short-term memory (radial arm water maze) and early phase long-term potentiation in the CA1 neurons of rats’ hippocampus. Specifically, they found that regular exercise prevented the sleep deprivation-induced down-regulation of important signaling molecules such as BDNF and the phosphorylated calcium–calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (caMKII) critical for the induction and expression of long-term potentiation, a cellular correlate of learning and memory [92, 93]. In another study by Zagaar et al [38]., 4 weeks of exercise training prior to a 24-h sleep deprivation also preserved long-term memory and long-term potentiation by preventing decreases in levels of BDNF and phosphorylated cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) response element-binding protein (P-CREB) typical of sleep deprivation. In humans, studies testing the effects of acute exercise on circulating BDNF indicated a dose–response relationship with high-intensity exercises leading to greater increases in BDNF than low-intensity exercises [26, 29, 30, 51, 52], although levels comparable to those induced by moderate-intensity protocols have also been reported [54].
Contrary to our hypothesis, the acute evening HIIT intervention did not improve memory performance in HIITS8 over and beyond that of S8 at the delayed recall. This was an unexpected finding given previous evidence for a positive synergistic effects of acute daytime exercise and a nap on enhancing long-term memory [35]. Notably, Mograss et al [35]. found that a single session of a 40-min moderate-intensity (60% HRmax) cycling before a 60-min NREM nap operated synergistically to enhance long-term recognition memory better than the effects of exercise or nap alone in a sample of 115 young adults [35]. This discrepancy in findings may be attributed to methodological differences between this study and that of Mograss et al [35]. While this study used a remote, evening protocol of HIIT before night-time sleep in a home environment and tested association memory, their study used a laboratory-based daytime protocol of moderate-intensity cycling before a short nap and assessed recognition memory in a relatively larger sample of young adults but with fitness levels similar to those of this study. More importantly, their recognition stimuli were different, including neutral pictures of scenes, faces, objects and animals taken from the International Affective Pictures System [35]. It has been shown that the relationship between acute exercise and memory may vary based on the exercise intensity, temporality of exercise and the memory type evaluated [94]. This relationship may also be moderated by fitness levels [21, 95]; however, studies have reported no influence of objective physical activity levels on the effects of acute exercise on long-term memory [96, 97].
Previous research has shown that high-intensity exercise occurring prior to memory encoding improved long-term memory [98–100] by enhancing encoding of the memory stimuli and priming specific neural networks for the memory formation [27, 32]. Although our memory performance results did not show a statistically significant group difference at the immediate retrieval (evaluating encoding), the HIITS8 appeared to have a lower discriminability index relative to the S8, suggesting a possible poorer encoding. It is possible that our moderately and less active participants may not have fully recovered from the brain’s hemodynamic and neural activity changes due to the novel HIIT at the time of encoding (i.e. 2.8 h post-exercise). This could be explained by the transient hypofrontality hypothesis [101], which is dependent on exercise intensity [102] and fitness level [103], possibly affecting recovery after exercise. Thus, allowing recovery from the psychophysiological responses to the exercise, as well as recovering from fatigue and reduced vigilance known to decline cognitive performance [103] may maximize the cognitive effects of high-intensity exercise [26, 104].
In the present study, sleep-dependent memory consolidation indicated by the change scores in the discriminability index showed no significant group differences. Our sleep restriction protocol did not target the entire first half of the night during which SWS is predominant [1, 6, 7], probably resulting in a non-significant difference in the change scores across the conditions. Despite finding no evidence for a disturbed sleep the night following the exercise, we may have missed any post-exercise night-time sleep disruption particularly in the HIITS8 due to lack of objective sleep measurements. Acute high-intensity exercise in the evening has been shown to cause sympathetic hyperactivity [105], elevate core body temperature (CBT), and/or interfere with nocturnal CBT decline [106, 107]. These can potentially impair sleep by delaying sleep onset [108] and causing arousals during night-time sleep [57]. A previous meta-analysis of acute high-intensity exercises performed 2–4 h before bedtime decreased REM sleep by 2.3% without disruptive changes in other objective sleep variables [61].
Another possible explanation for our results is that this study was underpowered (N = 92, power = ~53% for the interaction effect), and we would have required a relatively larger sample size (N = 116) to observe a similar combined effect of exercise and sleep on memory in the exercise conditions (HIITS8 and HIITS5) as previously reported [35]. Additionally, the motivation scores of the exercise conditions (HIITS8 HIITS5) were not significantly different from that of the S8.
This study has limitations with implications for future studies. In addition to being underpowered and not using objective sleep measurements, the present study did not directly estimate exercise capacity (e.g. VO2max measurement) or use a field test of cardiorespiratory fitness to determine the exercise intensity and to confirm that the groups had comparable physical fitness levels. We also did not objectively monitor exercise intensity during the HIIT with wearable tools (e.g. heart rate monitors or accelerometers) nor used remote activity monitoring tools (e.g. Verisense system, Shimmer Research Ltd [84]) due to the lockdowns and movement restrictions imposed locally during the COVID-19 pandemic. Nonetheless, our participants’ average RPE (~18) during the HIIT was within the RPE range (i.e. 15–19) corresponding to 85–95% of their age-predicted HRmax. Further analysis showed that RPE was not a significant covariate for the difference in memory performance between the HIIT groups (HIITS5 and HIITS8). Whether the participants’ RPE scores precisely matched their expected exercise intensity range could not be objectively confirmed in this study. Nonetheless, RPE is a valid measure of exercise intensity and correlates with heart rate [82, 83, 109]. Future remote studies should use activity monitors or remote monitoring methods (e.g. Verisense system, Shimmer Research Ltd) [84] to assess adherence to exercise intensity and the overall fidelity of the exercise protocol. Furthermore, future studies may employ controlled, laboratory-based moderate- and/or high-intensity exercise protocols, directly measuring cardiorespiratory fitness to determine exercise intensity and further evaluate the mediating effects of acute and/or chronic exercises on declarative memory in sleep restriction. Another limitation of this study was that we did not standardize the memory task times across all four groups. Finally, we recruited only healthy young adults with no sleep or psychological disorders and the majority were females (82%), limiting the generalizability of our results. Future studies may be conducted in the populations at risk of sleep loss due to work, sleep disorders or lifestyle while ensuring equivalent distribution of both sexes.
In conclusion, this study demonstrates that acute sleep restriction negatively impacted long-term declarative memory performance. However, a short bout of acute evening HIIT appeared to partially decrease the detrimental effects of the sleep restriction. The acute evening HIIT intervention before an average night-time sleep did not enhance long-term declarative memory over and above that of the average sleep-alone condition. Future studies are needed to understand how best to maximize the effects of acute exercise in offsetting the deleterious effects of sleep loss on long-term declarative memory.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank our sleep lab volunteers and interns (Luca Delli Colli, Felicia Vacirca, Erika Ross, Chris Dimopoulos and Marlon Ibuna Quilatan) for their technical support. The authors would also like to thank the kinesiologists (Samantha St. Jules and Eva Peyrusqué) who created the HIIT video for the evening HIIT intervention. In addition, we wish to thank our study coordinator (Madeline Dickson) and our participants for their participation in this study.
Contributor Information
Emmanuel Frimpong, Sleep, Cognition and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Department of Health, Kinesiology, and Applied Physiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; PERFORM Center, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Centre de Recherche de l’Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, QC, Canada.
Melodee Mograss, Sleep, Cognition and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Department of Health, Kinesiology, and Applied Physiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; PERFORM Center, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Centre de Recherche de l’Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, QC, Canada.
Tehila Zvionow, Sleep, Cognition and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Department of Health, Kinesiology, and Applied Physiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; PERFORM Center, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada.
Arsenio Paez, Sleep, Cognition and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Department of Health, Kinesiology, and Applied Physiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; PERFORM Center, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada.
Mylene Aubertin-Leheudre, Centre de Recherche de l’Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, QC, Canada; Département des Sciences de l’activité physique, GRAPA, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada.
Louis Bherer, Centre de Recherche de l’Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, QC, Canada; Department of Medicine and Centre de recherche de l’Institut de cardiologie de Montréal, Université de Montréal, QC, Canada.
Véronique Pepin, Department of Health, Kinesiology, and Applied Physiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; PERFORM Center, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Centre de recherche, CIUSSS du Nord-de l’Île-de-Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada.
Edwin M Robertson, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK.
Thien Thanh Dang-Vu, Sleep, Cognition and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Department of Health, Kinesiology, and Applied Physiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; PERFORM Center, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Centre de Recherche de l’Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, QC, Canada.
Funding
This research was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research grant (MM, TDV; Award number: PJT 166167). TDV is also supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (grants MOP 142191, PJT 153115, PJT 156125, and PJT 166167), the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, the Canada Foundation for Innovation and the Fonds de Recherche du Québec—Santé. MA-L is supported by the Fonds de Recherche du Québec—Santé (FRQS).
Disclosure Statement
Financial Disclosure: None.
Nonfinancial Disclosure: None.
Author Contributions
MM and TTDV participated in the study conceptualization and design, obtained funding, supervised data collection, and edited the manuscript. EF contributed to the study conceptualization and design, data collection, final statistical analysis, results interpretation, drafting and editing the manuscript. TZ contributed to, data collection, and preliminary statistical analysis. AP data collection, results interpretation, and edited the manuscript. MA-L, LB, EMR, and VP study design, results interpretation, and edited the manuscript. All authors read and approved the submitted version of the manuscript.
Data Availability Statement
The data underlying this article will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author.
References
- 1. Diekelmann S. Sleep for cognitive enhancement. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8(46):1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Rasch B, Born J.. About sleep’s role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013;93(2):681–766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Chaput JP, Wong SL, Michaud I.. Duration and quality of sleep among Canadians aged 18 to 79. Health Rep. 2017;28(9):28–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Hirshkowitz M, Whiton K, Albert SM, et al. National Sleep Foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary. Sleep Health. 2015;1(1):40–43. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Ackermann S, Rasch B.. Differential effects of Non-REM and REM sleep on memory consolidation? Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2014;14(2):430. doi: 10.1007/s11910-013-0430-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Prince TM, Abel T.. The impact of sleep loss on hippocampal function. Learn Mem. 2013;20(10):558–569. doi: 10.1101/lm.031674.113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Cousins JN, Fernández G.. The impact of sleep deprivation on declarative memory. In: Van Dongen HPA, Whitney P, Hinson JM, Honn KA, Chee MWL (eds.) Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 246. Elsevier; 2019:27–53. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.01.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Gais S, Lucas B, Born J.. Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learn Mem. 2006;13(3):259–262. doi: 10.1101/lm.132106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Jean-Louis G, Williams NJ, Sarpong D, et al. Associations between inadequate sleep and obesity in the US adult population: analysis of the national health interview survey (1977–2009). BMC Public Health. 2014;14:290. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-290 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. St-Onge MP, Grandner Michael A, Brown D, et al. Sleep duration and quality: impact on lifestyle behaviors and cardiometabolic health: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2016;134(18):e367–e386. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0000000000000444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Chai Y, Fang Z, Yang FN, et al. Two nights of recovery sleep restores hippocampal connectivity but not episodic memory after total sleep deprivation. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):8774. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-65086-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Killgore WDS. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:105–129. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00007-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. McLellan TM, Caldwell JA, Lieberman HR.. A review of caffeine’s effects on cognitive, physical and occupational performance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:294–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Kripke DF. Chronic hypnotic use: deadly risks, doubtful benefit: review article. Sleep Med Rev. 2000;4(1):5–20. doi: 10.1053/smrv.1999.0076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Irwin MR, Cole JC, Nicassio PM.. Comparative meta-analysis of behavioral interventions for insomnia and their efficacy in middle-aged adults and in older adults 55+ years of age. Health Psychol. 2006;25(1):3–14. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Youngstedt SD. Effects of exercise on sleep. Clin Sports Med. 2005;24:355–65, xi. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2004.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Roig M, Cristini J, Parwanta Z, et al. Exercising the sleepying brain: exercise, sleep, and sleep loss on memory. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2022;50(1):38–48. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Kredlow MA, Capozzoli MC, Hearon BA, Calkins AW, Otto MW.. The effects of physical activity on sleep: a meta-analytic review. J Behav Med. 2015;38(3):427–449. doi: 10.1007/s10865-015-9617-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Loprinzi PD, Blough J, Crawford L, Ryu S, Zou L, Li H.. The temporal effects of acute exercise on episodic memory function: systematic review with meta-analysis. Brain Sciences. 2019;9(4):8787. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9040087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Roig M, Thomas R, Mang CS, et al. Time-dependent effects of cardiovascular exercise on memory. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2016;44(2):81–88. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Roig M, Nordbrandt S, Geertsen SS, Nielsen JB.. The effects of cardiovascular exercise on human memory: a review with meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(8):1645–1666. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Verburgh L, Königs M, Scherder EJA, Oosterlaan J.. Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2014;48(12):973–979. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091441 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Aritake-Okada S, Tanabe K, Mochizuki Y, et al. Diurnal repeated exercise promotes slow-wave activity and fast-sigma power during sleep with increase in body temperature: a human crossover trial. J Appl Physiol. 2019;127(1):168–177. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00765.2018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Chennaoui M, Arnal PJ, Sauvet F, Léger D.. Sleep and exercise: a reciprocal issue? Sleep Med Rev. 2015;20:59–72. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Park I, Díaz J, Matsumoto S, et al. Exercise improves the quality of slow-wave sleep by increasing slow-wave stability. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):4410. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83817-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL.. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012;1453:87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Loprinzi PD. Intensity-specific effects of acute exercise on human memory function: considerations for the timing of exercise and the type of memory. Health Promot Perspect. 2018;8(4):255–262. doi: 10.15171/hpp.2018.36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Afzalpour ME, Chadorneshin HT, Foadoddini M, Eivari HA.. Comparing interval and continuous exercise training regimens on neurotrophic factors in rat brain. Physiol Behav. 2015;147:78–83. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Knaepen K, Goekint M, Heyman EM, Meeusen R.. Neuroplasticity—exercise-induced response of peripheral brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Sports Med. 2010;40(9):765–801. doi: 10.2165/11534530-000000000-00000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Marquez CMS, Vanaudenaerde B, Troosters T, Wenderoth N.. High-intensity interval training evokes larger serum BDNF levels compared with intense continuous exercise. J Appl Physiol. 2015;119(12):1363–1373. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00126.2015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Okamoto M, Mizuuchi D, Omura K, et al. High-intensity intermittent training enhances spatial memory and hippocampal neurogenesis associated with BDNF signaling in rats. Cereb Cortex. 2021;31(9):4386–4397. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Loprinzi PD, Ponce P, Frith E.. Hypothesized mechanisms through which acute exercise influences episodic memory. Physiol Int. 2018;105(4):285–297. doi: 10.1556/2060.105.2018.4.28 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Loprinzi PD, Edwards MK, Frith E.. Potential avenues for exercise to activate episodic memory-related pathways: a narrative review. Eur J Neurosci. 2017;46(5):2067–2077. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13644 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Kuhn T, Heisz J.. Cardiorespiratory fitness may protect memory for poorer sleepers. Front Psychol. 2022;13:793875. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.793875 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Mograss M, Crosetta M, Abi-Jaoude J, et al. Exercising before a nap benefits memory better than napping or exercising alone. Sleep. 2020;43(9):1–9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Lambiase MJ, Gabriel KP, Kuller LH, Matthews KA.. Sleep and executive function in older women: the moderating effect of physical activity. J Gerontol A Bio Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(9):1170–1176. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Wilckens KA, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME.. Physical activity and cognition: a mediating role of efficient sleep. Behav Sleep Med. 2018;16(6):569–586. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2016.1253013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Zagaar M, Dao A, Levine A, Alhaider I, Alkadhi K.. Regular exercise prevents sleep deprivation associated impairment of long-term memory and synaptic plasticity in the CA1 area of the hippocampus. Sleep. 2013;36(5):751–761. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2642 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Zagaar M, Alhaider I, Dao A, et al. The beneficial effects of regular exercise on cognition in REM sleep deprivation: behavioral, electrophysiological and molecular evidence. Neurobiol Dis. 2012;45(3):1153–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.12.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Sauvet F, Arnal PJ, Tardo-Dino PE, et al. Beneficial effects of exercise training on cognitive performances during total sleep deprivation in healthy subjects. Sleep Med. 2020;65:26–35. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Swain DP, Franklin BA.. Comparison of cardioprotective benefits of vigorous versus moderate intensity aerobic exercise. Am J Cardiol. 2006;97(1):141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2005.07.130 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Gibala MJ, Gillen JB, Percival ME.. Physiological and health-related adaptations to low-volume interval training: influences of nutrition and sex. Sports Med. 2014;44(2):127–137. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0259-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Gibala MJ, Jones AM.. Physiological and performance adaptations to high-intensity interval training. Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser. 2013;76:51–60. doi: 10.1159/000350256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Eather N, Babic M, Riley N, et al. Integrating high-intensity interval training into the workplace: the Work-HIIT pilot RCT. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2020;30(12):2445–2455. doi: 10.1111/sms.13811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Gibala MJ, McGee SL.. Metabolic adaptations to short-term high-intensity interval training: a little pain for a lot of gain? Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2008;36(2):586–563. doi: 10.1097/jes.0b013e318168ec1f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Godin G, Desharnais R, Valois P, Lepage L, Jobin J, Bradet R.. Differences in perceived barriers to exercise between high and low intenders: observations among different populations. Am J Health Promot. 1994;8(4):279–285. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-8.4.279 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Cerin E, Leslie E, Sugiyama T, Owen N.. Perceived barriers to leisure-time physical activity in adults: an ecological perspective. J Phys Act Health. 2010;7(4):451–459. doi: 10.1123/jpah.7.4.451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Shepherd SO, Wilson OJ, Taylor AS, et al. Low-volume high-intensity interval training in a gym setting improves cardio-metabolic and psychological health. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0139056e0139056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Eather N, Riley N, Miller A, et al. Efficacy and feasibility of HIIT training for university students: the Uni-HIIT RCT. J Sci Med Sport. 2019;22(5):596–601. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.11.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Frimpong E, Dafkin C, Donaldson J, Millen AME, Meiring RM.. The effect of home-based low-volume, high-intensity interval training on cardiorespiratory fitness, body composition and cardiometabolic health in women of normal body mass and those with overweight or obesity: protocol for a randomized controlled trial. BMC Sports Sci Med Rehab. 2019;11(1):39. doi: 10.1186/s13102-019-0152-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Etnier JL, Wideman L, Labban JD, et al. The effects of acute exercise on memory and Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF). . J Sport Exercise Psycol. 2016;38(4):331–340. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Kovacevic A, Fenesi B, Paolucci E, Heisz JJ.. The effects of aerobic exercise intensity on memory in older adults. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2020;45(6):591–600. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2019-0495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Labban JD, Etnier JL.. The effect of acute exercise on encoding and consolidation of long-term memory . J Sport Exercise Psycol. 2018;40(6):336–342. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2018-0072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Marin Bosch B, Bringard A, Logrieco MG, et al. A single session of moderate intensity exercise influences memory, endocannabinoids and brain derived neurotrophic factor levels in men. Sci Rep. 2021;11:14371. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-93813-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Sleep Foundation. How can exercise affect sleep? Sleep Foundation. https://www.sleepfoundation.org/physical-activity/exercise-and-sleep. Published February 25, 2013. Accessed January 31, 2023. [Google Scholar]
- 56. Morin CM, Hauri PJ, Espie CA, Spielman AJ, Buysse DJ, Bootzin RR.. Nonpharmacologic treatment of chronic insomnia. Sleep. 1999;22(8):1134–1156. doi: 10.1093/sleep/22.8.1134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Stepanski EJ, Wyatt JK.. Use of sleep hygiene in the treatment of insomnia. Sleep Med Rev. 2003;7(3):215–225. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Buxton OM, Lee CW, L’Hermite-Balériaux M, Turek FW, Van Cauter E.. Exercise elicits phase shifts and acute alterations of melatonin that vary with circadian phase. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2003;284(3):R714–R724. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00355.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Stutz J, Eiholzer R, Spengler CM.. Effects of evening exercise on sleep in healthy participants: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2019;49(2):269–287. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-1015-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Yue T, Liu X, Gao Q, Wang Y.. Different intensities of evening exercise on sleep in healthy adults: a systematic review and network meta-analysis . NSS. 2022;14:2157–2177. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S388863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Frimpong E, Mograss M, Zvionow T, Dang-Vu TT.. The effects of evening high-intensity exercise on sleep in healthy adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;60:101535. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2021.101535 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Vitale JA, Bonato M, Galasso L, et al. Sleep quality and high intensity interval training at two different times of day: a crossover study on the influence of the chronotype in male collegiate soccer players. Chronobiol Int. 2017;34(2): 260–268. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2016.1256301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Vitale JA, Banfi G, Galbiati A, Ferini-Strambi L, La Torre A.. Effect of a night game on actigraphy-based sleep quality and perceived recovery in top-level volleyball athletes. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2019;14(2):265–269. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2018-0194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Thomas C, Jones H, Whitworth-Turner C, Louis J.. High-intensity exercise in the evening does not disrupt sleep in endurance runners. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2020;120(2):359–368. doi: 10.1007/s00421-019-04280-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Vlahoyiannis A, Aphamis G, Eddin DA, Giannaki CD.; The effect of evening cycling at different intensities on sleep in healthy young adults with intermediate chronobiological phenotype: a randomized, cross-over trial. J Sport Sci. 2020;39:1–8. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2020.1812194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjöström M, et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003;35(8):1381–1395. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000078924.61453.FB [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Kapur VK, Auckley DH, Chowdhuri S, et al. Clinical practice guideline for diagnostic testing for adult obstructive sleep apnea: an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Clinical Practice Guideline. J Clin Sleep Med. 2017;13(03):479–504. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.6506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Carter R, Watenpaugh DE.. Obesity and obstructive sleep apnea: or is it OSA and obesity? Pathophysiology. 2008;15(2):71–77. doi: 10.1016/j.pathophys.2008.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Nguyen JCD, Killcross AS, Jenkins TA.. Obesity and cognitive decline: role of inflammation and vascular changes. Front Neurosci. 2014;8:375. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Shen YC, Kung SC, Chang ET, Hong YL, Wang LY.. The impact of obesity in cognitive and memory dysfunction in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Int J Obes. 2019;43(2):355–361. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0138-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Chung F, Subramanyam R, Liao P, Sasaki E, Shapiro C, Sun Y.. High STOP-Bang score indicates a high probability of obstructive sleep apnoea. Br J Anaesth. 2012;108(5):768–775. doi: 10.1093/bja/aes022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ.. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1991;14(6):540–545. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Morin CM, Belleville G, Bélanger L, Ivers H.. The Insomnia Severity Index: psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep. 2011;34(5):601–608. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Hublin C, Kaprio J, Partinen M, Koskenvuo M, Heikkilä K. The Ullanlinna Narcolepsy Scale: validation of a measure of symptoms in the narcoleptic syndrome. J Sleep Res. 1994;3(1):52–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1994.tb00104.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK.. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. 1996;67:588–597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA.. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(6):893–897. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.6.893 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Horne JA, Ostberg O.. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4(2):97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Fox SM, Naughton JP, Haskell WL.. Physical activity and the prevention of coronary heart disease. Ann Clin Res. 1971;3(6):404–432. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Gellish RL, Goslin BR, Olson RE, McDonald A, Russi GD, Moudgil VK.. Longitudinal modeling of the relationship between age and maximal heart rate. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(5):822–829. doi: 10.1097/mss.0b013e31803349c6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Weston KS, Wisløff U, Coombes JS.. High-intensity interval training in patients with lifestyle-induced cardiometabolic disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2014;48(16):1227–1234. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-092576 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Borg GA. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1982;14(5):377–381. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Scherr J, Wolfarth B, Christle JW, Pressler A, Wagenpfeil S, Halle M.. Associations between Borg’s rating of perceived exertion and physiological measures of exercise intensity. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2013;113(1):147–155. doi: 10.1007/s00421-012-2421-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. McDevitt B, Moore L, Akhtar N, Connolly J, Doherty R, Scott W.. Validity of a novel research-grade physical activity and sleep monitor for continuous remote patient monitoring. Sensors. 2021;21(6):2034. doi: 10.3390/s21062034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Bull FC, Al-Ansari SS, Biddle S, et al. World Health Organization 2020 guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour. Br J Sports Med. 2020;54(24):1451–1462. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2020-102955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Mander BA, Rao V, Lu B, et al. Impaired prefrontal sleep spindle regulation of hippocampal-dependent learning in older adults. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(12):3301–3309. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J.. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1988. 1101;117(1):34–50. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.117.1.34 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Sivakumaran MH, Mackenzie AK, Callan IR, Ainge JA, O’Connor AR.. The discrimination ratio derived from novel object recognition tasks as a measure of recognition memory sensitivity, not bias. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):11579. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-30030-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Carney CE, Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. The consensus sleep diary: standardizing prospective sleep self-monitoring. Sleep. 2012;35(2):287–302. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1642 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Slutsky AB, Diekfuss JA, Janssen JA, et al. The effects of low-intensity cycling on cognitive performance following sleep deprivation. Physiol Behav. 2017;180:25–30. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.07.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Mohammadipoor-ghasemabad L, Sangtarash MH, Esmaeili-Mahani S, Sheibani V, Sasan HA.. Abnormal hippocampal miR-1b expression is ameliorated by regular treadmill exercise in the sleep-deprived female rats. Iran J Basic Med Sci. 2019;22(5):485–490. doi: 10.22038/ijbms.2019.31988.7734 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Alhaider IA, Aleisa AM, Tran TT, Alzoubi KH, Alkadhi KA.. Chronic caffeine treatment prevents sleep deprivation-induced impairment of cognitive function and synaptic plasticity. Sleep. 2010;33(4):437–444. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.4.437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Kim EY, Mahmoud GS, Grover LM.. REM sleep deprivation inhibits LTP in vivo in area CA1 of rat hippocampus. Neurosci Lett. 2005;388(3):163–167. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Loprinzi PD, Frith E, Edwards MK, Sng E, Ashpole N.. The effects of exercise on memory function among young to middle-aged adults: systematic review and recommendations for future research. Am J Health Promot. 2018;32(3):691–704. doi: 10.1177/0890117117737409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Pontifex MB, McGowan AL, Chandler MC, et al. A primer on investigating the after effects of acute bouts of physical activity on cognition. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2019;40:1–22. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.08.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Pontifex MB, Parks AC, O’Neil PC, et al. Poorer aerobic fitness relates to reduced integrity of multiple memory systems. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014;14(3):1132–1141. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0265-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Pontifex MB, Gwizdala KL, Parks AC, Pfeiffer KA, Fenn KM.. The association between physical activity during the day and long-term memory stability. Sci Rep. 2016;6(1):38148. doi: 10.1038/srep38148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Etnier J, Labban JD, Piepmeier A, Davis ME, Henning DA.. Effects of an acute bout of exercise on memory in 6th grade children. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2016;26(3):250–258. doi: 10.1123/pes.2013-0141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Frith E, Sng E, Loprinzi PD.. Randomized controlled trial evaluating the temporal effects of high-intensity exercise on learning, short-term and long-term memory, and prospective memory. Eur J Neurosci. 2017;46(10):2557–2564. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Winter B, Breitenstein C, Mooren FC, et al. High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;87(4):597–609. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Dietrich A. Transient hypofrontality as a mechanism for the psychological effects of exercise. Psychiatry Res. 2006;145(1):79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Loprinzi PD, Day S, Deming R.. Acute exercise intensity and memory function: evaluation of the transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Medicina. 2019;55(8):445445. doi: 10.3390/medicina55080445 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Tomporowski PD. Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol. 2003;112(3):297–324. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(02)00134-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Park J. Effect of arousal and retention delay on memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol Rep. 2005;97(2):339–355. doi: 10.2466/pr0.97.2.339-355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Uchida S, Shioda K, Morita Y, Kubota C, Ganeko M, Takeda N.. Exercise effects on sleep physiology. Front Neurol. 2012;3:1–5. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Murphy PJ, Campbell SS.. Nighttime drop in body temperature: a physiological trigger for sleep onset? Sleep. 1997;20(7):505–511. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.7.505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107. Sawka MN, Wenger CB, Pandolf KB.. Thermoregulatory responses to acute exercise-heat stress and heat acclimation. In: Terjung R, Baldwin KM, Edgerton VR, Wagner PD (eds.) Comprehensive Physiology. American Cancer Society; 2011:157–185. doi: 10.1002/cphy.cp040109 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Oda S, Shirakawa K.. Sleep onset is disrupted following pre-sleep exercise that causes large physiological excitement at bedtime. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2014;114(9):1789–1799. doi: 10.1007/s00421-014-2873-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Zinoubi B, Zbidi S, Vandewalle H, Chamari K, Driss T.. Relationships between rating of perceived exertion, heart rate and blood lactate during continuous and alternated-intensity cycling exercises. Biol Sport. 2018;35(1):29–37. doi: 10.5114/biolsport.2018.70749 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data underlying this article will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author.