Skip to main content
Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI logoLink to Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI
. 2023 Jun 30;13(13):2167. doi: 10.3390/ani13132167

Review of Rat (Rattus norvegicus), Mouse (Mus musculus), Guinea pig (Cavia porcellus), and Rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) Indicators for Welfare Assessment

Shari Cohen 1,2,*,, Cindy Ho 1,
Editor: Vera Baumans
PMCID: PMC10339950  PMID: 37443965

Abstract

Simple Summary

The monitoring and assessment of animals is important for their health and welfare. The appropriate selection of multiple, validated, and feasible welfare assessment indicators is required to effectively identify compromises or improvements in animal welfare. Animal welfare indicators available can be animal based or resource based and collated together to form various assessment tools. The literature contains a wide variety of indicators; however, there is yet to be an ideal constellation of indicators for animal-based welfare assessment in small mammals. A systematic review was performed to identify and outline common animal-based indicators for animal welfare assessment in small mammals, specifically guinea pigs (Cavia Porcellus), mice (Mus musculus), rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), and rats (Rattus norvegicus). This review aims to provide the basis for further research into animal welfare indicators for these species and their application to improve animal welfare assessment, management, and strategies.

Abstract

The monitoring and assessment of animals is important for their health and welfare. The appropriate selection of multiple, validated, and feasible welfare assessment indicators is required to effectively identify compromises or improvements to animal welfare. Animal welfare indicators can be animal or resource based. Indicators can be collated to form assessment tools (e.g., grimace scales) or animal welfare assessment models (e.g., 5 Domains) and frameworks (e.g., 5 Freedoms). The literature contains a wide variety of indicators, with both types needed for effective animal welfare assessment; however, there is yet to be an ideal constellation of indicators for animal-based welfare assessment in small mammals such as guinea pigs (Cavia Porcellus), mice (Mus musculus), rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), and rats (Rattus norvegicus). A systematic review of grey and peer-reviewed literature was performed to determine the types of animal-based welfare indicators available to identify and assess animal health and welfare in these small mammals maintained across a wide variety of conditions. The available indicators were categorised and scored against a selection of criteria, including potential ease of use and costs. This review and analysis aim to provide the basis for further research into animal welfare indicators for these species. Future applications of this work may include improvements to animal welfare assessments or schemes, guiding better management, and implementing future strategies to enable better animal welfare.

Keywords: animal welfare, animal welfare indicators, animal welfare assessment, animal welfare monitoring, rabbit, guinea pig, mice, mouse, rat

1. Introduction

Appropriate animal welfare is foundational to the care and management of animals [1,2]. Animal care and use industries worldwide should be committed to providing high standards of welfare for all animals under their care [3]. There is growing public interest in animal welfare across all areas, including animal care, housing, and use, and increasing interest in evidence-based welfare assessments by veterinary professionals, researchers, animal carers, and stockpersons [4,5]. The standard of animal welfare can currently vary based on the purpose and species of the animal. Each species has its own specific inputs and strategies required for good welfare. During their life, animals may be exposed to a range of positive or negative experiences via human–animal interactions (i.e., handling), husbandry (i.e., diet), management practices (i.e., housing), disease (i.e., preventative care), and other inputs. The acknowledgement of sentience in many species has emphasised the need to ensure and assess good animal welfare through science-based evidence. Although there has been significant growth in animal welfare science, there are still gaps to be explored. One specific gap is in the assessment of animal welfare. There are several methods available for animal welfare assessment, but species-specific indicators are yet to be comprehensively identified, collated, and analysed [3,6]. Efforts have been undertaken in the development and harmonisation of animal welfare indicators for dogs, cats, some farm animals, and zoo animals. Some of the available indicators have been validated and may form part of wider welfare assessments or tools [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17]. Guinea pigs, mice, rabbits, and rats are commonly part of the pet ownership and laboratory animal industries. The ability to assess welfare more easily, objectively, and consistently in these animals is likely of interest to many animal care staff, pet owners, researchers, veterinarians, and others committed to the well-being of these animals.

The term “animal welfare” is used to describe the affective state of an individual animal, which goes beyond the perception, response, and ability to cope in different circumstances [1,2,18,19,20]. A contemporary and holistic approach to animal welfare is demonstrated in the Five Domains model, where good welfare is defined as an animal that is healthy, comfortable, well nourished, safe, can express behaviours for its physical and mental state, and attains an overall positive welfare experience [21,22,23]. Both positive and negative experiences of an animal are included, with good welfare achieved through the fulfilment of nutritional, physiological/physical, behavioural, and environmental requirements [18]. These experiences are often defined in terms of the affective states of an animal. Affective states are generally described as feelings, emotions, or moods and can include fear, pain, frustration, happiness, and satisfaction [24,25]. These affective states or experiences are explicitly included in the Five Domains model for animal welfare assessment [23,26,27] where animal welfare is considered a continuum of an animal’s affective states or experiences from positive, neutral, to negative [24,25]. Affective states incorporate behavioural, physiological, and cognitive components and are based on two dimensions: level of arousal, which indicates the level or strength of bodily activation (e.g., excited versus relaxed), and valence of the stimulus, which indicates the direction of the stimulus (e.g., positive versus negative) [24,25,28].

Using this approach, the affective state can be used to infer an animal’s welfare state [24,29,30]. For instance, animals are described as having good welfare when they are mainly experiencing positive states, such as pleasure and satisfaction. Conversely, poor welfare can occur when animals mainly experience negative states, such as fear and pain [24,29,30]. Animal welfare can be assessed over the long term and is influenced by a collection of affective states. Alternatively, it can also be viewed as a point in time and monitored with short-term observations [30]. For example, grimace scales can be used as a short-term observation tool to assess the presence and potential severity of pain in animals [31,32,33].

The assessment of animal health and welfare is an important component of veterinary medicine and research. Accurately assessing the welfare of animals is crucial and can be beneficial beyond simply maximising or maintaining a positive state of well-being. Accurate assessment of the welfare of companion animals by veterinarians can potentially increase owner awareness by improving their level of reflection with regards to their pets’ potential issues [34]. Regular welfare assessments can improve the prospect of detecting signs of pain or distress rapidly, consistently, and accurately, so appropriate intervention, such as timely administration of pain relief, is achieved [19]. In research, accurate welfare assessment can mean better detection if an intervention has an impact on animal well-being [35,36]. The routine assessment of animal welfare for research purposes supports the collection of high-quality scientific data [6]. Finally, assessing animal welfare offers an opportunity for continual improvements and a deeper understanding of the requirements of animals to help optimise housing and husbandry practices [19].

Animal welfare is generally assessed through a variety of means. As highlighted by Fraser (2008), the assessment of animal welfare (or the type of welfare assessment model or tools used) can be influenced by different definitions of “animal welfare” and which aspects or concepts of animal welfare are emphasised within the definition [26,37,38]. For the purposes of this paper, this review will use the affective states approach as the primary determinant of animal welfare [25]. Despite there being no direct measures of affective states in animals, affective states can be used to infer animal welfare states. Therefore, welfare measurements rely on drawing inferences from affective states, which are based on assessing animal-based welfare indicators, such as physiological, behavioural, and health indicators [19].

Animal welfare assessments should use a combination of resource-based and animal-based indicators [19]. Resource-based welfare indicators are closely associated with animal-based indicators and should be considered when assessing animal welfare, as environmental factors, human–animal interactions, and husbandry all play important roles in the welfare of an animal. However, using only resource-based assessments may not consistently correlate with the affective state of an animal [28]. Consequently, for this review, the evaluation of resource-based indicators has been excluded. Although some publications referenced in this paper may include these indicators, the focus of the review and analysis is on animal-based welfare indicators. To date, there has been no systematic review to appraise animal-based welfare measures, with an emphasis on physiological, behavioural, and physical health indicators for guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus), mice (Mus musculus), rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), and rats (Rattus norvegicus domestica). The animal-based welfare indicators found in this review were collated and analysed in regard to their potential practicality of use, such as time, training, cost, and equipment requirements. The analysis explored the breadth, depth, development, and application of animal welfare assessment indicators with the aim of identifying and providing options for their use and direction for future research.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Search Strategy and Eligibility Criteria

The article analyses the current literature with regards to animal-based welfare measures in accordance with PRISMA guidelines [39]. A systematic search was undertaken using four independent databases on each of the four animals of interest (rabbits, guinea pigs, rats, and mice). The four databases used in these searches were: CAB Abstracts, Scopus, Web of Science, and MDPI (Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute). A systematic search was performed on these databases using combinations of keywords: “welfare” and/or “welfare indicator” and/or “welfare assessment”, and/or “rabbit”, and/or “guinea pigs”, and/or “rats”, and/or “mice”, and/or “mouse”. Databases were searched as per the PRISMA guidelines using the eligibility criteria listed in Table 1 and the results described in Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3 and Figure 4. The PRISMA diagrams in Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3 and Figure 4 have been modified to account for the presence of grey literature. The search terms and results for each database search are presented in Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3 and Figure 4. The title and abstract of each citation were inspected to identify suitable articles based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria listed in Table 1. Hand-picked key texts or articles used by the authors in veterinary medicine and teaching were also selected alongside those found in Google Scholar. All database and hand-picked searches were conducted between April and August 2022, and there were no restrictions on the publication date; however, if there were two or more versions of any publication, the more recent publication was selected.

Table 1.

List of inclusion and exclusion criteria for database searches.

Inclusion Criteria Exclusion Criteria
Publications were considered if they were:
  • -

    Published in English or translated into English

  • -

    Primarily focused on rabbits, guinea pigs, rats, or mice

  • -
    Primarily focused on assessing welfare either directly or indirectly, including:
    • Animal-based measures or indicators of animal welfare (physiological, behavioural, physical health)
    • Animal welfare indicators that have been validated
  • -

    Studies key to the development of methods or tools for assessing welfare in rabbits, guinea pigs, rats, and mice

Publications were not considered if they were:
Duplicates
Not published in English
Did not address PICO question
Studies that did not use animal-based welfare indicators as their primary measure of welfare
Primarily focused on resource- or management-based measures or indicators of animal welfare

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Rabbit focused database searches—PRISMA flow diagram for articles sourced from CAB Abstracts, Scopus, Web of Science and MDPI databases using the following terms: TOPIC: (rabbit) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare indicator) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare assessment).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Guinea pig focused database searches—PRISMA flow diagram for articles sourced from CAB Abstracts, Scopus, Web of Science and MDPI databases using the following terms: TOPIC: (guinea pig) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare indicator) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare assessment).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Rat focused database searches—PRISMA flow diagram for articles sourced from CAB Abstracts, Scopus, Web of Science and MDPI databases using the following terms: TOPIC: (rat) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare indicator) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare assessment).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Mouse focused database searches—PRISMA flow diagram for articles sourced from CAB Abstracts, Scopus, Web of Science and MDPI databases using the following terms: TOPIC: (mice or mouse) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare indicator) AND/OR TOPIC: (welfare assessment).

2.2. Analysis of Indicators from the Literature

Publications meeting all eligibility criteria were reviewed. All animal-based welfare indicators found were analysed and placed into three separate tables labelled Table 2 with sections for rabbits (A), guinea pigs (B), as well as rats and mice (C). Due to the large proportion of shared or similar animal-based welfare indicators, rat and mouse welfare indicators were combined into a single table. Indicators were grouped into three categories: physiological, behavioural, and physical health indicators. Each category was subcategorised into its respective body systems, body parts, organs, or descriptors. An additional five parameters were included to denote the potential welfare state, ease, and practicality of the indicators. Animal welfare indicators were linked with a possible associated positive, neutral, or negative affective state (i.e., welfare state). In addition to affective state, four other parameters described the potential practicality and ease of use of the indicators in terms of “easy training”, “specialized equipment” required, approximate equipment and/or associated “costs”, and approximate “time to assess” the indicator. Training was deemed easy or not easy as a “yes or no” if it was thought it could be taught to a layperson or student within a 15 min consultation. Cost was categorised as “yes” or “no” and was deemed “high” if it cost more than AUD 100.00 to purchase the equipment or required the test to be performed in a specialised setting such as a laboratory or veterinary clinic. Special equipment was denoted “yes” or “no” if any equipment was required to undertake the assessment. Time was categorised as “yes” or “no” if it was likely to require more than five minutes to perform the assessment. Note that the allocated affective states as well as the ease and practicality criteria of each indicator in Table 2 are semi-subjective.

Table 2.

Animal-based welfare indicators grouped into physiological, behavioural and physical health and practical considerations..

(A) Rabbit Animal-Based Welfare Indicators Grouped into Physiological, Behavioural and Physical Health and Practical Considerations.
Body-System, Body Part,
Organ, or Descriptor of
Indicator
Animal-Based Welfare Indicators Affective State Easy to Train High Costs (>AUD100) Special Equipment Time < 5 min Publications
CATEGORY 1: PHYSIOLOGICAL
Cardiovascular system Persistent increase or decrease or abnormality in heart rate Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [40,41,42]
Heart murmur or arrhythmia Negative,
Neutral
Yes Yes—stethoscope or electrocardiogram Yes—stethoscope or electrocardiogram Yes [42]
Poor/weak quality or asynchronous pulses Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [42]
Increased blood pressure Negative No Yes—Doppler or oscillometric device Yes—Doppler or oscillometric device Yes [40,42]
Prolonged capillary refill time Negative Yes No No Yes [42]
Respiratory system Persistent decrease or increase in respiratory rate and effort (laboured breathing/respiratory distress/dyspnoea/apnoea/abdominal breathing) Negative Yes No No Yes [40,41,42,43,44,45]
Persistent presence of respiratory sounds (including coughing, sneezing, sniffling, wheezes, and crackles) Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [42,43,45,46]
Alimentary, gastrointestinal, digestive system Reduced gut sounds Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [42]
Tender or painful abdomen Negative Yes No No Yes [42]
Distended abdomen Negative Yes No No Yes [42,45]
Presence of diarrhoea or soft faeces with an unpleasant smell Negative Yes No No Yes [40,42,43,44,47]
Changes in faecal output and appearance (including colour, consistency, frequency) Negative Yes No Yes—measuring utensils Yes [40,42,43,44,47]
Urogenital system Changes in urine output and appearance (incl. colour, consistency, frequency) Negative Yes No Yes—measuring utensils Yes [42,44,45]
Musculoskeletal,
nervous and vestibular system
Abnormalities in gait
  • Gait score

  • Abnormalities include lack of balance, stumbling, stiff-legged gait, reduced range of motion or reluctance to move

Negative No
Yes
No No Yes [42,46]
Nervous system,
Adrenal glands
Persistent increase or decrease in glucocorticoid levels and metabolites in comparison to normal. Negative,
Neutral
No Yes—corticosterone radioimmunoassay, etc. Yes—corticosterone radioimmunoassay, etc. No [41,42,43,48,49]
Body temperature Abnormal changes in body or ear temperature Negative Yes No Yes—thermometer Yes [40,41,42,44,45,46]
Hydration status Dehydration—presence of skin tenting, sunken and dull eyes Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [42]
CATEGORY 2: PHYSICAL HEALTH
Body weight Excess weight gain or loss Negative,
Neutral
Yes No Yes—scales Yes [40,42,43,45]
Body condition Body condition score Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [42,43,46,50]
Body symmetry Neutral,
Negative
Yes No No Yes [42]
Integument system Unkempt, dirty, matted coat, or urine/faecal stained fur including medial forelimbs Negative Yes No No Yes [40,42,43,45,46]
Piloerection Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [42,51,52]
Dandruff Negative, Neutral Yes No No Yes [42]
Damage to fur or skin
  • Includes discolouration, lesions, sores, scabs, scales, ulcerations, crusting or areas of hair loss

Negative Yes No No Yes [42,45,46]
Lumps under chin or mandible Negative Yes No No Yes [42]
Lumps in or under skin Negative, Neutral Yes No No Yes [42,45,46]
Eyes, nose, urogenital,
mammary glands
Presence of discharge from the eyes, nose, mammary glands, urinary or genital organs Negative Yes No No Yes [42,44,46]
Musculoskeletal, nervous and vestibular system Sudden head tilt or loss of balance Negative Yes No No Yes [42,44]
Facial asymmetry Negative, Neutral Yes No No Yes [42]
Facial paralysis Negative Yes No No Yes [42]
Swollen joints Negative Yes No No Yes [42,44]
Posture Hunched back Negative Yes No No Yes [40]
Tucked up appearance (including tucked abdomen) Negative Yes No No Yes [40,42,45,52]
Pressing abdomen to the floor Negative Yes No No Yes [40,42,51,52]
Oral cavity and dentition Dental malocclusion, overgrowth, malformation, discolouration and loose, fracture or absent teeth Negative No Yes—otoscope or oral speculum Yes—otoscope or oral speculum Yes [42]
Bleeding or evidence of trauma to the gingiva Negative Yes Yes—otoscope or oral speculum Yes—otoscope or oral speculum Yes [42]
Drooling (ptyalism) or wet chin Negative Yes No No Yes [42,45]
Absences or restricted lateral jaw movement Negative Yes No No Yes [42]
Eyes Wide/partially closed, sunken, or dull eyes Negative Yes No No Yes [40,42]
Redness, discolouration, masses or swelling of the periocular areas, eyelids, conjunctiva, or sclera Negative Yes No No Yes [42]
Protrusion of third eyelid Negative Yes No No Yes [42]
Ears Presence of excessive or abnormal coloured wax, ulcerations, scabbing, scaling, discolouration, or swellings Negative Yes Yes—otoscope Yes—otoscope Yes [42]
Pain-related signs Facial Grimacing (Grimace scale)
Grimace scale for Rabbits—Indicators:
  • Orbital tightening

  • Cheek flattening

  • Nostril shape (“V-shaped” nares)

  • Ear shape and position (ears drawn back)

  • Whisker shape and position

Negative Yes No No Yes [31,40,42,44,52,53,54]
Increased composite pain score Negative No No No Yes [55]
CATEGORY 3: BEHAVIOUR
Natural behaviours Binkying or frolicking (jumping rapidly whilst
shaking head and flinging hindlimbs to the side)
Positive Yes No No Yes [52,54]
Grooming (self-grooming, allo-grooming, mutual grooming) Positive Yes No No Yes [40,52,54,56]
Nocturnal/crepuscular behaviour Positive Yes No No Yes [52,54,57]
Nesting (for breeding does) Positive Yes No No Yes [52,54]
Regular eating with occasional drinking Positive Yes No No Yes [52,54]
Coprophagia Positive Yes No No Yes [52,54]
TERRITORIAL AND HIERARCHAL BEHAVIOURS
Scent marking by chinning objects Positive Yes No No Yes [52]
Cage guarding Positive, Neutral, Negative Yes No No Yes [52]
Marking territory with urine or faeces (spraying)
  • May be due to frustrated sexual behaviours of entire rabbits

Positive, Neutral, Negative Yes No No Yes [52]
SOCIAL AND EXPLORATORY BEHAVIOURS
Foraging Positive Yes No No Yes [52,54]
Investigative behaviour Positive Yes No No Yes [52,54]
Rearing or peri-scoping Positive Yes No No Yes [52,54]
Digging or burrowing Positive Yes No No Yes [52]
“Tooth purring” or “teeth chattering”
  • Different from tooth grinding (bruxism)

Positive Yes No No Yes [52]
RESTING BEHAVIOURS
Sprawling or stretching out Positive, Neutral Yes No No Yes [52]
Laying down or “flopped” on their side Positive, Neutral Yes No No Yes [52]
Abnormal behaviours ABNORMAL REPETITIVE BEHAVIOURS AND STEREOTYPIC BEHAVIOURS
Excessive scratching or rubbing Negative Yes No No Yes [45,52]
Barbering
  • Can be self-inflicted or inflicted by cage-mates

Negative Yes No No Yes [58]
ALTERED FOOD AND WATER CONSUMPTION
Decreased food and water consumption
  • Signs include refusal of usual fresh food or treats (for more than a day)

  • No spillage of food or water (indicating consumption)

Negative, Neutral Yes No No Yes [40,44,45,47,52]
Prolonged duration of uneaten caecotrophs or inability to eat caecotrophs Negative Yes No No Yes [52]
ALTERED INTERACTIONS WITH HUMAN HANDLERS AND COMPANIONS [41]
Hiding or refusal to leave hutch/hiding spots, or running away on approach Negative Yes No No Yes [40,50,52]
Self-isolation Negative Yes No No Yes [46,50]
Increased aggression to handler or companion Negative Yes No No Yes [40,50,52]
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOURS
Grunting Negative Yes No No Yes [52]
Thumping back legs Negative Yes No No Yes [40,52]
Lunging Negative Yes No No Yes [52]
Pain-related behaviours BEHAVIOURS ASSOCIATED WITH PAIN
Reduced or lack of responsiveness (including facing back of cage and immobility) Negative Yes No No Yes [51,52]
Reluctance to move Negative Yes No No Yes [40]
Loud tooth grinding (bruxism)
  • Different from “tooth purring” or “teeth chattering”

Negative Yes No No Yes [40,51,52]
Failure to groom or over-grooming Negative Yes No No Yes [40,52]
Squealing or shrieking Negative Yes No No Yes [42,52]
(B) Guinea Pig animal-based welfare indicators grouped into physiological, behavioural and physical health and practical considerations.
Body-system, Body part,
Organ, or Descriptor of Indicator
Animal-Based Welfare Indicators Affective State Easy To Train High Costs (>AUD100) Special Equipment Time <5 min Publications
CATEGORY 1: PHYSIOLOGICAL
Cardiovascular system Persistent increase or decrease or abnormality in heart rate Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [59]
Respiratory system Persistent decrease or increase in respiratory rate and effort Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [60]
Persistent presence of respiratory sounds (including coughing, sneezing, sniffling, wheezes, and crackles) Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [60]
Alimentary, gastrointestinal, digestive system Presence of diarrhoea or soft faeces with an unpleasant smell Negative Yes No No Yes [59,60]
Changes in faecal output and appearance (including colour, consistency, frequency) Negative Yes No Yes—measuring utensils Yes [59,60]
Urogenital system Poor breeding success (e.g., abortions, infertility) Negative No Yes—ultrasound, radiographs, haematology, biochemistry, etc. Yes—ultrasound, radiographs, haemotology, biochemistry, etc. Yes [60]
Changes in urine output and appearance (incl. colour, consistency, frequency) Negative Yes No Yes—measuring utensils Yes [60]
Musculoskeletal system Abnormal gait
  • Includes weight shifting, stiff gait, shuffling gait, limping or lameness

  • On palpation, joints may be hot and swollen

Negative Yes No No Yes [59,60,61]
Nervous system, adrenal glands Persistent increase or decrease in glucocorticoid levels and metabolites in comparison to normal Negative,
Neutral
No Yes—corticosterone radioimmunoassay, etc. Yes—corticosterone radioimmunoassay, etc. No [62]
Body temperature Extreme changes in rectal temperature Negative Yes No Yes—thermometer Yes [60,63]
Hot or swollen joints Negative Yes No No Yes [60]
CATEGORY 2: PHYSICAL APPEARANCE
Body weight Excess weight gain or loss Negative Yes No Yes—scales Yes [59,60,62]
Body condition Body condition score Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [59,60,62]
Integument system Unkempt, dirty, matted, erect coat or urine/faecal stained fur Negative Yes No No Yes [59,60]
Piloerection Negative Yes No No Yes [64]
Damage to fur or skin
  • Includes active bleeding from lesions, ulcerations, old scabbed over/scaly lesions, cheilitis (ulcerative scabbing lesions around lips)

Negative Yes No No Yes [59,60]
Lumps in or under the skin Negative Yes No No Yes [59,60]
Eyes, nose, ears, urogenital, mammary glands Presence of discharge from the eyes, nose, ears, mammary glands, urinary or genital organs Negative Yes No No Yes [59,60]
Oral cavity and dentition Dental malocclusion, overgrowth, malformation, discolouration and loose, fracture or absent teeth Negative No Yes—otoscope or oral speculum Yes—otoscope or oral speculum Yes [59]
Drooling (ptyalism) or wet chin Negative Yes No No Yes [59,60]
Pain-related signs Closed eyes or squinting Negative Yes No No Yes [64]
CATEGORY 3: BEHAVIOUR
Natural behaviours Social activity Positive Yes No No Yes [65,66]
Active for most of the 24 h period Positive Yes No No Yes [66]
Scent marking Positive Yes No No Yes [66]
Coprophagy Positive Yes No No Yes [61,65,66]
Thigmotaxic behaviour Positive Yes No No Yes [66]
Climb and jump Positive Yes No No Yes [61,66]
Grooming Positive Yes No No Yes [65]
Vocalisations Positive, Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [61]
Eating and drinking Positive Yes No No Yes [61]
Yawning Positive Yes No No Yes [61]
EXPLORATORY BEHAVIOURS
Appropriate digging, walking, running, stretching, lying down,
rearing, scratching, shaking, and standing
Positive Yes No No Yes [61]
Abnormal behaviours ABNORMAL REPETITIVE BEHAVIOURS AND STEREOTYPIC BEHAVIOURS
Excessive scratching or rubbing Negative Yes No No Yes [60]
Barbering
  • Can be self-inflicted or inflicted by cage-mates

Negative Yes No No Yes [59]
ALTERED FOOD AND WATER CONSUMPTION
Reduced or absence of food and water intake Negative Yes No No Yes [59,60,61]
Pain-related behaviours BEHAVIOURS ASSOCIATED WITH PAIN
Decreased movement or reluctant to move
  • Including forward or backward motion, turning the body or head, head or neck extension (rearing), ambulation

Neutral, Negative Yes No No Yes [59,64,65]
Decreased coprophagy Neutral,
Negative
Yes No No Yes [61,64,65]
Biting, chewing or licking at enclosure Neutral, Negative Yes No No Yes [65]
Changes in abdominal movements
  • Including pressing abdomen to the floor (belly pressing), abdominal contraction with back arching, writhe (slow contortion of abdominal flank muscles), twitching (rapid muscle contraction of back muscles) and weight shifting.

Neutral, Negative Yes No No Yes [61,64,65]
Incomplete or abnormal behaviours or frequent abrupt ceasing movement Neutral, Negative Yes No No Yes [64]
(C) Mice and rat animal-based welfare indicators grouped into physiological, behavioural, and physical health and practical considerations. Dark grey boxes represent welfare indicators displayed by rats; light grey represent welfare indicators displayed by mice; white boxes represent welfare indicators displayed by both rats and mice.
Body-system, Body part, Organ, or Descriptor of Indicator Animal-Based Welfare Indicators Affective State Easy to Train High Costs (>AUD 100) Special Equipment Time <5 min Publications
CATEGORY 1: PHYSIOLOGICAL
Cardiovascular system Persistent increase or decrease or abnormality in heart rate Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [67,68,69,70,71]
Presence of heart murmur or arrhythmia Negative,
Neutral
Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [67,68,69,70,71]
Respiratory system Persistent decrease or increase in respiratory rate and effort (laboured breathing/respiratory
distress/dyspnoea/apnoea/abdominal breathing)
Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [67,68,69,70,71,72,73]
Persistent presence of respiratory sounds
(including coughing, sneezing, sniffling, wheezes, and crackles)
Negative Yes Yes—stethoscope Yes—stethoscope Yes [67,68,70,71]
Alimentary, gastrointestinal, digestive system Presence of diarrhoea or soft faeces with an unpleasant smell Negative Yes No No Yes [67,68,70,71]
Changes in faecal output and appearance
(including colour, consistency, frequency)
Negative Yes No Yes—measuring utensils Yes [67,68,69,72]
Urogenital system Changes in urine output and appearance
(including colour, consistency, frequency)
Negative Yes No Yes—measuring utensils Yes [67,68,69,72]
Musculoskeletal, nervous and vestibular system Abnormalities in gait
  • Gait score

  • Abnormalities include lack of balance, stumbling, stiff-legged gait or reluctance to move

  • Lameness

Negative Yes
(variable)
No No Yes [68,69,70,71,72,74,75,76,77]
Nervous system, adrenal glands Persistent increase or decrease in glucocorticoid levels and their metabolites in comparison to normal. Negative,
Neutral
No Yes—corticosterone radioimmunoassay, etc. Yes—corticosterone radioimmunoassay, etc. No [67,68,69,72,78,79]
Body temperature Extreme changes in body temperature Negative Yes No Yes—thermometer Yes [67,68,72]
Hydration status Dehydration Neutral,
Negative
Yes No No Yes [67,68,72,74]
CATEGORY 2: PHYSICAL APPEARANCE
Body weight Excess weight gain or loss Negative Yes No Yes—scales Yes [68,69,70,71,72,74,77,80,81,82,83]
Body condition Body condition score Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [68,69,70,71,72,74,77,80,81,82,83]
Integument system Unkempt, dirty, matted, erect coat or urine/faecal stained fur Negative Yes No No Yes [68,69,70,71,72,77,84]
Piloerection Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [68,70,73,74,77,84]
Damage to fur or skin
  • Including the presence of bite wounds, discolouration, lesions such as sores, scabs, scales, ulcerations, crusting and areas of hair loss

Negative Yes No No Yes [67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,77,84]
Lumps under the chin or mandible Negative Yes No No Yes [70,71,72]
Lumps in or under the skin Negative Yes No No Yes [70,71,72]
Excessively loose skin Negative Yes No No Yes [70,71,72]
Eyes, nose, urogenital, mammary glands Presence of discharges from the eyes, nose, mammary glands, urinary or genital organs Negative Yes No No Yes [69,70,71,72,74]
Posture Altered abnormal postures
  • Hunched back, arched back with front paws tucked under the body, a lowered head and a tucked abdomen

  • Prostration or extension

Negative Yes No No Yes [68,69,70,71,72,74,77,84]
Abdomen Swollen or distended abdomen Negative Yes No No Yes [69,72,74]
Eyes and lacrimal glands Partially closed, sunken, or dull eyes Negative Yes No No Yes [68]
Chromodacryorrhea (porphyrin staining) Negative Yes No No Yes [68,69,77,85]
Pain-related signs Facial grimacing (Grimace scale)
Grimace scale for Rats—Indicators:
  • Orbital tightening

  • Nose/cheek flattening

  • Ear changes

  • Whisker change

Grimace scale for Mice—Indicators:
  • Orbital tightening

  • Nose bulge

  • Cheek bulge

  • Ear position

  • Whisker change

Negative Yes No No Yes [32,33,53,67,68,69,72,74,77,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95]
Increased composite pain score Negative Yes No No Yes [68,88,95,96,97,98,99]
CATEGORY 3: BEHAVIOURIAL
Natural
behaviours
Nocturnal/crepuscular behaviour Positive Yes No No Yes [100,101,102]
Avoidance of open spaces Positive Yes No No Yes [100,101,102]
EXPLORATORY BEHAVIOURS
Appropriate running, jumping, climbing, sniffing, stretching, foraging, digging, rearing, resting,
coprophagy, and chewing,
Positive,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [67,68,73,74,77,86,103]
Burrowing Positive,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [104,105,106]
SOCIAL AND PLAY BEHAVIOURS [49,74]
Object manipulation Positive,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [68,107]
Rough-and-tumble play with cage mates and human handlers Positive,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [68,107]
Social activity Positive Yes No No Yes [77,100,101,102]
Positive behaviour Positive Yes No No Yes [67,74,108]
GROOMING
Self-grooming and allo-grooming with cage mates Positive Yes No No Yes [68,73,77,86]
Abnormal
behaviours
Overgrooming Negative Yes No No Yes [68,70,73,84,86,109,110,111]
ALTERED FOOD AND WATER CONSUMPTION
Reduced or absence of food and water intake Negative Yes No No Yes [68,72,73,80,82,83,84,112]
ABNORMAL REPETITIVE AND STEREOTYPIC BEHAVIOURS
Locomotor or oral repetitive behaviours Negative Yes No No Yes [68,113]
Stereotypic behaviour which may be sex and strain related Negative Yes No No Yes [67,73,77,114,115,116]
Barbering
  • Self-inflicted or inflicted by cage-mates.

Negative Yes No No Yes [67,69,72,73,77,117]
ALTERED SOCIAL BEHAVIOURS
Self-isolation Negative Yes No No Yes [68,84,118,119]
Frequent agonistic severe behaviour towards cage mates Negative Yes No No Yes [68,73,74,77,84,118,119]
ALTERED ACTIVITY LEVELS
Altered, increased or decreased activity Negative Yes No No Yes [68,73,74,77,82,84,104,105,112]
Altered, increased or decreased alertness Negative,
Neutral,
Positive
Yes No No Yes [69,72]
Voluntary frequent wheel running Negative Yes No No Yes [120]
ALTERED INTERACTIONS WITH HUMAN HANDLERS
Avoidance or agonistic behaviour towards human handlers
  • Latency to approach

Negative Yes No No Yes [68,69,74]
Urinating or defecating during handling Negative Yes No No Yes [69,72,74]
ALTERATIONS IN SOUND WAVE FREQUENCY OF VOCALISATIONS [40,49,50,53,112]
20 kHz vocalisations Negative, Neutral Variable Yes Yes—sound level meter Yes [68,72,73,74,86,121,122]
50 kHz vocalisations Positive,
Neutral
Variable Yes Yes—sound level meter Yes [68,72,73,86,121,122,123]
ALTERATIONS IN BURROWING BEHAVIOUR
Decreased or absence spontaneous burrowing
behaviour
Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [67,68,86,103,104,105,106,120,124,125,126]
BREEDING-RELATED BEHAVIOURS
Excessive cornering, or wall-hugging behaviour Negative,
Neutral
Yes No No Yes [68,69,77,127]
Nest building performance
  • Time to integrate into nest and nesting consolidation scoring

Positive,
Neutral, Negative
Yes
(variable)
No No Yes [67,69,73,74,86,106,128,129,130]
Nursing, pup retrieval, and interaction with pups after nest disturbance Positive, Neutral Yes No No Yes [19]
Pup–pup interactions
Pup–adult interactions

Dark grey boxes represent welfare indicators studied to be displayed by rats; light grey represent welfare indicators studied to be displayed by mice; white boxes represent welfare indicators studied to be displayed by both rats and mice.

3. Results

3.1. Animal Welfare Based Indicators

This study identified a total of 190 animal-based welfare indicators across all four species. Of these 190 indicators, a total of 75 welfare indicators were found to be exhibited by rabbits. For guinea pigs, a total of 49 welfare indicators were identified. For rats and mice, a total of 66 welfare indicators were identified. Most of the indicators discovered were behavioural indicators, totalling 99 indicators. Physical health indicators totalled 52, and the fewest indicators were physiological, with a total of 39 indicators. Table 3 provides a further breakdown of the number of indicators identified for each animal based on the three categories: physiological, physical health, and behavioural.

Table 3.

Summarises the number of welfare indicators for each species as per one of three categories of animal-based welfare indicators.

Animal
Category of Animal-Based Welfare Indicators Rabbits Guinea Pigs Rats and Mice Total Number of
Indicators for Each
Category
Physiological indicators 17 11 11 39
Physical health indicators 27 10 15 52
Behavioural indicators 31 28 40 99
Total number of indicators for each animal 75 49 66 190

From these 190 indicators, there were a total of 9 physiological, 8 physical health, and 10 behavioural shared welfare indicators across all 4 species. Shared indicators are listed in Table 4.

Table 4.

Shared welfare indicators across guinea pigs, mice, rabbits, and rats categorised in physiological, physical health and behavioural.

Category of Animal-Based Welfare Indicators Similar Animal-Based Welfare Indicators Shared across Rabbits, Guinea Pigs, Rats and Mice
Physiological indicators
  1. Persistent changes (especially increases) in heart rate

  2. Persistent changes (especially increases) in respiratory rate and effort

  3. Persistent presence of respiratory sounds

  4. Presence of diarrhoea or prolonged production of soft stools with unpleasant smell

  5. Changes in faecal output and appearance (colour, consistency and frequency)

  6. Changes in urine output and appearance (colour, consistency and frequency)

  7. Abnormal gait

  8. Persistent changes (especially increases) in glucocorticoid levels and their metabolites (e.g., corticosterone and cortisol)

  9. Extreme changes in body temperature

Physical health indicators
  1. Body weight changes

  2. Body condition score

  3. Unkempt, dirty, matted, erect coat or urine/faecal stained fur

  4. Piloerection

  5. Damage to fur or presence of skin lesions

  6. Lumps in or under the skin

  7. Presence of discharge from the eyes, nose, ears, mammary glands, urinary or genital organs

  8. Altered posture

Behavioural indicators
  1. Social behaviours

  2. Play behaviours

  3. Exploratory behaviours

  4. Grooming (allo-grooming, self-grooming, mutual grooming)

  5. Coprophagy

  6. Vocalisations

  7. Avoidance of open spaces (thigmotaxic behaviour)

  8. Abnormal repetitive behaviours and stereotypic behaviours—e.g., barbering

  9. Altered food and water consumption

  10. Pain-associated behaviours—e.g., reluctance to move, reduced responsiveness, over-grooming, or failure to groom

Based on the literature screened, studies infrequently focused on either the direct assessment of welfare or the validation of animal-based welfare indicators. Studies frequently aimed to evaluate non-animal welfare-based indicators such as management, environmental, or other resource-based factors as well as monitoring behaviour. Some studies focused on the physiological response to various stimuli for non-animal welfare purposes, such as pain studies and stress in biomedical research. Nonetheless, these studies were included and evaluated, as the findings can be utilised to assess aspects of animal welfare and strengthen the validity of certain indicators.

3.2. Animal Welfare Indicator Scoring Systems

While reviewing relevant publications, two commonly used scoring systems were found to be used for recording welfare indicators. Systems were either binary or numerically recorded, with their relative strengths and weaknesses outlined in Table 5.

Table 5.

Simple comparison of binary and numerical systems for animal welfare indicators with advantages and disadvantages.

Advantages Disadvantages
Binary
  • Less time consuming

  • Easier to use as based

  • Potential for less subjective assessment

  • May be more consistent if descriptions are clear

  • Limited description of intensity or severity of the indicator

  • Potential less complexity of data

Numerical
  • Severity and intensity can be scored

  • Potential for detailed data to be collected

  • Allows for tracking of trends over time

  • Potential for more subjectivity when assigning scores

  • Can be time consuming

  • Scores can under- or overestimate the severity of an indicator

4. Discussion

4.1. General Discussion

There is a need to study and identify best practices and new welfare assessments via the use of validated welfare indicators. The overall societal interest in animal welfare issues has increased, with a likely reciprocal desire among veterinarians, researchers, pet owners, and animal care staff to deliver improvements in animal welfare. Guinea pigs, mice, rabbits, and rats may be exposed to a range of stressful stimuli in their lifetime, such as transport, disease, poor husbandry, management, and/or experimental conditions, which can all disrupt their state of welfare and may result in an overall negative cumulative effect. While avoiding all stress may not be feasible or necessary to maintain an overall positive state of welfare, the ability to detect pain, stress, or distress enables appropriate interventions to alleviate or mitigate negative states of welfare. Most importantly, when determining the welfare state of animals, a carefully considered and multi-factorial approach is required.

This study has identified a range of animal-based welfare indicators, demonstrated in Table 1, that can or are currently being utilised as components of welfare assessment tools for guinea pigs, mice, rabbits, and rats. The systematic review and analysis of the literature found a selection of common welfare indicators shared amongst these species. In addition, many of these indicators are assessments of animal health, which is an important pillar of animal welfare; changes in these indicators may not correspond to changes in welfare. Thresholds for abnormalities and baselines of normality should also be considered when determining if changes in animal health indicators are potentially significant. The categories selected denote three main themes of welfare indicators, each with its own advantages and disadvantages. These indicators are of potential interest to those involved in the care and use of animals, including researchers, veterinarians, students, pet owners, and animal care staff. The tables highlight the breadth and depth of animal welfare assessment indicators and key considerations in their usage. This information may provide foundational support in future animal welfare assessment schemes for educators and animal inspectors in selecting appropriate species-specific and more generic animal-based indicators in these different contexts. The review and analysis of these indicators across these species were also able to highlight knowledge deficiencies across the different categories, types of indicators, and potential limitations that may provide direction for future research and development.

Animal welfare is widely studied across many species. Animal carers, animal use industries, and governments are moving away from the prevention of cruelty to the provision of positive animal welfare experiences. Critical to achieving this shift is the ability to identify positive and negative welfare states through appropriate monitoring and welfare indicators. However, there is currently no consensus regarding the animal-based welfare indicators to be assessed when evaluating the welfare of small mammals such as guinea pigs, mice, rabbits, and rats. Systematic review of the current literature demonstrated a large discrepancy in the quality and quantity of information provided by scientific articles and other literature. While a large portion of welfare indicators were found in the grey literature, such as in veterinary textbooks, on veterinary websites, pet websites, animal welfare society websites, and similar guidelines, only a small portion of welfare indicators were addressed in scientific papers. This suggests that welfare indicators may be selected based on anecdotal evidence and/or expert opinion. It is unknown if there may be issues or limitations with non-evidence-based or non-animal-specific indicators being used to assess the well-being of other animals.

Animal welfare assessment schemes can be derived from existing animal welfare frameworks such as the 5 Freedoms and 5 Domains and can provide a high-level approach to animal welfare assessment [22,23]. In guinea pigs, mice, rabbits, and rats, there are few publications available for validated, formal animal welfare assessments and no known work that outlines current utility. For rabbits, a recent study by Botelho and colleagues (2020) aimed to assess the welfare of commercially reared rabbits. These researchers developed a set of animal welfare assessment protocols based on the Welfare Quality Approach by using existing animal welfare indicators obtained from other studies [46]. In mice, a study by Campos-Luna and colleagues (2019) aimed to determine the most valid, reliable, and practicable welfare indicators for laboratory mice. The researchers in this study applied the Delphi consultation technique as a method of finding consensus amongst experts for mice [72]. There were no other similar studies involving rats and guinea pigs. This is of concern, given that guinea pigs are used as a food source in some countries, and both rats and guinea pigs are used in research and as companion animals. Although many mouse indicators are used to assess welfare in rats given their similarities in physiology, physical features, and behaviours, the tables demonstrated the important nuanced differences between the species. In the case of guinea pigs, there was an abundance of information found on pet websites but few scientific sources detailing guinea pig welfare assessment parameters, highlighting key gaps in scientific knowledge.

Some research companies and international animal research councils have developed publicly available welfare assessment procedures [19,67,68,131]. For example, Wageningen UR Livestock Research (2011) published a guideline for the assessment of commercially housed rabbits with potential animal- and resource-based measures based on the Welfare Quality Approach [131]. Other guidelines include the Animal Welfare Guidelines for Rats and Mice published by the Canadian Council on Animal Care (CCAC), which suggest a variety of welfare assessment tools for laboratory rats and mice [19,67,68]. These guidelines were useful in providing subsets of welfare indicators for analysis in this review. However, caution should be used when applying and assessing these indicators in other contexts, as many have been extracted from studies conducted on laboratory and commercial animals for non-animal welfare purposes, with many yet to be validated. For guinea pigs, the literature on welfare assessments is very sparse and still limited. The disparity in animal welfare knowledge and literature between the species, particularly for guinea pigs, is further supported in Table 2, which demonstrates guinea pigs possessed the fewest number of animal-based welfare indicators. Overall, there is a dearth across all species for life stage-specific indicators such as those for neonates, during pregnancy, or in geriatrics.

Animal welfare and assessment is multi-dimensional, and various welfare assessment frameworks have generated multiple tools; however, these tools are often developed for specific species or contexts [132]. As such, there is no universal approach for assessing animal welfare [132]. A total of 27 shared animal-based welfare indicators for different species were found. These similarities are listed in Table 3. There were numerous indicators that were also potentially specific to each species. For example, tooth purring or tooth chattering, which can be heard in content or sleeping rabbits but should not be confused with loud tooth grinding (bruxism), can also be heard in rabbits suffering from severe pain or discomfort [40,51,52]. Other indicators specific to rabbits and rodents (e.g., rats and mice) included burrowing/digging [52,74,86] and pain-related indicators such as facial grimacing [31,32,33,53] and altered posture [40,69,84]. Future studies developing animal welfare assessment protocols for small mammals could address the lack of harmonisation of these indicators to create a standardised list of key general welfare and species-specific assessment parameters via the list of indicators identified by this study.

4.2. Welfare Indicator Categories

Animal-based and resource-based assessments are frequently used simultaneously in many animal welfare assessment programs [19,46]. This holistic approach facilitates comprehensive yet feasible assessments; however, animal-based welfare indicators must be a prime focus of any welfare assessment program [19]. Animal-based welfare indicators are important as they directly assess the response (outcome) of an animal to its environment through physiological, physical, and behavioural measures [28,132,133]. Conversely, resource-based indicators evaluate indirect elements such as the animal’s environment and quality of husbandry. Both types of indicators play an important role in optimising and benchmarking acceptable practices for animal environments, husbandry, and overall animal welfare [133].

For the purposes of this review, animal-based welfare indicators were divided into three broad categories: Physiological indicators (Section 4.2.1), physical appearance or observable health indicators (Section 4.2.2), and behavioural indicators (Section 4.2.3). Together, these categories provide insight into the welfare of animals in a practical and sometimes scientific context. However, each category possesses advantages and disadvantages concerning its practical application.

4.2.1. Physiological Indicators

Physiological indicators can be used to detect potential stress responses in animals and can be assessed directly via vital signs or by analysing bodily fluids. These vital signs, such as heart rate (HR), respiratory rate (RR), blood pressure (BP), and body temperature, can represent a critical baseline of vital indicators of animal health and welfare [134]. The activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis in a stressful event creates a cascade of reactions, including changes in the secretion of glucocorticoid hormones from the adrenal glands [135]. The secretion of hormones and their metabolites, such as cortisol and corticosterone (in rodents), can be measured in different types of bodily fluids [33]. For example, paired or multiple blood plasma, serum, and saliva samples can be used to identify states of acute stress, while fur, urine, and faeces can be used to determine chronic stress [62,136,137,138,139]. Although these methods may be useful to indicate stress and potential welfare compromise, ongoing or repeated monitoring of these indicators is not always feasible, financially viable, or timely, which may limit their use. Physiological parameters must also be interpreted in context with the type of stress (distress vs. eustress). Nonetheless, deviations from baseline values should be cause for further review and assessment to ensure animals are not experiencing unnecessary negative affective states.

Laboratory physiological indicators can provide an abundance of information about an animal’s welfare. However, their applicability in practical and scientific contexts may be limited due to several reasons. The assessment of physiological indicators is often more invasive and disruptive compared to the assessment of other types of indicators. Physiological indicators such as serum and plasma cortisol levels are considered invasive as they require venepuncture for the collection of blood. Depending on the purpose of the study (i.e., determination of acute versus chronic stress), appropriate alternatives and substitutes to blood sampling can include fur, saliva, urine, or faecal sampling [42,43,48,49,62,69,72,78,79,139]. However, the collection of saliva and fur may be invasive based on the circumstances under which the samples are obtained (i.e., directly from the animal). The collection of urine and faeces from the enclosure may also be disruptive to the animal for similar reasons. In addition to physical contact, the collection of animal samples often requires animal handling and restraint. Sampling and animal handling procedures can act as stressors and affect cortisol concentrations, which can cause confounding issues when assessing acute stress [140,141]. On the other hand, some animals may be accustomed to handling and sampling procedures, and therefore, care must be taken when comparing and interpreting these results. Significant investment is required when analysing laboratory parameters, as this process can be costly, time consuming, and require specialised equipment.

Vital signs are considered a critical baseline set of indicators in the assessment of animal health and welfare. Persistent elevations in vital signs, including body temperature, BP, HR, RR, respiratory rhythm, effort, and sounds, can be early indicators of heat stress, disease, or other physiological complications [142]. These indicators can also be associated with the mental states of anxiety, fear, pain, discomfort, and distress [142]. The assessment of RR is highly recommended as it is easy-to-assess, non-invasive, and chest movements can be observed from a distance. On the contrary, the assessment of HR, BP, and body temperature requires handling, physical restraint, and contact with the animal. As guinea pigs, rabbits, and rodents are extremely sensitive to physical contact, restraint, and handling, transient changes in vital signs may be a confounder. Some technologies, such as the use of telemetry sensor implants in rodents, have been developed to try and avoid these issues, but they may be more aversive, thus negating their use [142]. Newer unobtrusive and contactless modalities for monitoring and assessing physiological parameters are being explored, which include the use of infrared thermography and motion tracking technologies [142,143,144,145]. The benefits of using these technologies are that monitoring is remote and passive, causing little to no disturbance to the animal and avoiding manual assessment of vital signs [142]. However, as these technologies are expensive, their use may not be limited at this time.

A primary advantage of physiological indices of assessment is the ability to measure and quantify animal responses [146]. The disadvantages are the existence of unlimited variability in physiological responses, contextualisation (e.g., heart rate increase due to excitement from feeding versus fear), and potential invasiveness [146]. Physiological responses can be influenced by factors such as species differences, age, sex, body condition, reproductive status, the animal’s history and past experiences, the animal’s mental state, geographic location, current energetic status, and the circumstances under which samples are collected [147]. In addition, examining and interpreting physiological indicators requires training, expertise, knowledge, and skills. This can potentially make comparisons and interpretations difficult even among experts in an academic context, which can prevent the common practical use of many of these indicators. Future research should focus on the further characterisation of non-invasive sampling techniques for practical use in the field and in real time. Apart from respiratory rate, non-invasive and real-time sampling methods are not commonly available for many of these techniques and may not yet be practical for routine welfare assessments.

4.2.2. Physical Health Indicators

Physical appearance and physical health measures are indicators of welfare that can be obtained through cage-side or pen-side visual observation and/or physical examination of an animal. Assessment of physical health and appearance may involve evaluating animal body weight, body condition score, and observing for signs of abnormalities such as physical injuries and/or discharges. It can also include observing physical signs of pain via alterations in posture. For some animals, pain can be scored via validated pain scoring systems such as facial grimace scales. Although assessing physical health and appearance is generally non-invasive, some indicators, such as body weight and body condition score, involve handling and manipulation of the animal. The weighing procedure also requires the use of a scale, which, for small animals, may be quite simple to use or access. Overall, the assessment of physical health using indicators such as body weight and body condition scores can be relatively quick, easy, and inexpensive and does not usually involve complex training.

A key disadvantage in evaluating physical appearance and observable health indicators of welfare is that many small mammals, including guinea pigs, mice, rabbits, and rats, are prey animals. Prey animals may be more likely to hide or minimally express clinical signs of disease, injury, weakness, and pain to elude predators and/or humans [148,149]. The absence of abnormal physical health indicators does not necessarily mean an animal is healthy, pain-free, and in a positive state of welfare. Issues may also arise when attempting to notice subtle deviations in health deterioration in group-housed laboratory animals, normally social animals that are individually housed, or when social hierarchies are disturbed. Additionally, while resource-based indicators of group housing, more space, and places to hide can provide potential welfare benefits, these elements may also limit direct visual inspection of some animals, especially in shy, stressed, or injured individuals [148]. A balanced (and creative) approach between potentially positive resource-based requirements and the need to use animal-based indicators to assess welfare should always be applied.

Unlike the assessment of physiological parameters, observing physical appearance is a non-invasive and effective method of contributing to the assessment of animal welfare. Several validated tools for visually detecting pain have been developed, including the Composite Pain Scores and Facial Grimace Scores [31,32,33,53]. Facial Grimace Scales are a specialised example of a physically observable assessment tool that can be used as a valid and reliable method for determining the presence of pain in rabbits, rodents, and other animals [31,35,40,150,151,152,153]. By observing simple changes in facial expression, valuable information can be provided on animal affective experiences/states [31,32,33,53]. However, like many other observable indicators, it must be interpreted in context and with other indicators to confirm negative states (i.e., pain) [36,153]. For example, rats in severe pain can demonstrate a pain face in conjunction with a hunched posture and tucked abdomen [40,84], but they can also express these signs during recovery from anaesthesia, aggression, and sleep [32,33,154,155]. As suggested previously, the application of various imaging, scanning, or video technologies may be beneficial when assessing the physical state of an animal to determine its mental state [32,53,156,157]. At times, the use of advanced technologies may not be feasible. However, human observation of physical health, paired with regular examinations (e.g., BCS, weights), can be critical in detecting early concerns about animal welfare. The provision of visual aids (e.g., video, photos) and appropriate staff training is also likely to enhance the application of these possible welfare-improving techniques. The appropriate selection of physical health indicators and the best way to train humans in the use of these indicators are areas warranting additional work to further advance animal welfare and assessment.

4.2.3. Behavioural Indicators

Behavioural indicators of welfare were found to be the most common and well-studied of the three welfare indicator categories. This is demonstrated by the large proportion of behaviour-based welfare indicators in Table 1 compared to physiological and physical health indicators. This is perhaps because they are the easiest to assess in both practical and experimental contexts. As behavioural indicators of welfare are normally based on observations, they might be the most minimally invasive and least disruptive of the three welfare indicator categories. Behavioural indicators were subcategorised into natural and abnormal behaviours. There is a collection of several natural behaviours that may be used to infer a positive affective state [158]. Natural exploratory behaviours such as foraging, sniffing, burrowing, play, object manipulation/approach, resting, laying down, and social interactions such as mutual grooming. Abnormal behaviours encompass a range of abnormal repetitive or stereotypic behaviours, such as excessive scratching or rubbing, as well as behaviours related to states of disease or pain, such as reluctance to move, lack of responsiveness, and failure to groom or overgrooming. However, behavioural abnormalities can be complex. For example, an animal demonstrating abnormal behaviour may have used this behaviour as a coping mechanism in a barren environment but may continue this “learned” behaviour even after being moved into an appropriate enriched environment. Therefore, caution may be warranted, along with a good understanding of prior negative experiences and context, when assessing affective states.

Challenges to incorporating behavioural indicators into welfare assessments include, but are not limited to, general interpretation and difficulties in assessing animals in some scenarios. A holistic approach is important for animal welfare assessments, as utilising a single welfare indicator separately may be inaccurate [140]. The use of a few incorrectly chosen indicators can be misleading, and welfare compromise can exist in the absence of behavioural, physiological, or physical changes [146]. Additionally, some behaviours, such as mating, may be life-stage or time-sensitive specific and should (or can) only be assessed at specific times [158].

Another consideration is that interactions between social, exploratory, and play behaviours may be difficult to assess in animals captively managed in complex environments or intentionally isolated for short durations (e.g., transport or veterinary care). It may take time and familiarity to distinguish normal vs. abnormal use of hiding holes or shelters due to pain or stress in animals [150]. Separation of social animals from their conspecifics may also result in temporary behavioural changes. Several publications have found that social isolation can induce (locomotor) hyperactivity in adult rats; however, when returning previously isolated rats to their group, this behaviour was ameliorated [104,105]. Either set of changes may be interpreted as positive or negative behaviour, pending the social hierarchies and/or nuances of the animal and the environment. Consequently, careful interpretation and context are needed when assessing the welfare of individual animals surrounded by a group of animals of the same species, and vice versa. An additional characteristic that may be useful to explore and apply further is determining when animal welfare indicators should be used and if they are applicable to individual and/or population-based assessments.

4.3. Animal Welfare Scoring Systems

When working with animal welfare indicators, the scoring system has the potential to affect scores and animal welfare outcomes. Indicators can be scored and recorded via a binary or numerical system. In the case of the binary system, a simple “yes” or “no” is recorded to denote the presence or absence of an indicator [158]. This type of scoring system is simple yet effective when more detailed information or scaled data is not required. However, this type of scoring system may not be appropriate when a gradient of scoring is required or if the indicator/question is not suitable for binary scoring. If this occurs, it may be difficult to determine whether the score is valid and/or result in the loss of valuable information.

In contrast, a numerical system offers the possibility of scoring severity and/or intensity [158]. Numerical scores can offer a better opportunity to track trends and provide a greater depth of information. Simple versions of the numerical scores found in Table 2 included weight and BCS. It is tempting to collectively sum numerical scores from various animal welfare indicators to give a total score to denote “good” or “bad” welfare or offer a strict numerical delineation to determine when intervention or further action is required. However, the summation of scores may not always be accurate or justified and may sometimes prove inadvertently detrimental to animal welfare by reducing sensitivity and/or specificity. Additionally, not all indicators are necessarily equally important to animal welfare, and a weighted score may be more appropriate.

When developing or using a scoring system for animal welfare assessment, it is essential to have appropriate language, minimal overlap between outcomes, training of high-quality assessors, and consistency. Accurate, consistent scoring by observers is potentially more likely to occur if assessors are appropriately trained and possess appropriate experience and knowledge of the species being assessed. Assessor attributes, such as teamwork, empathy, and communication, can be as important as the scoring system [158]. The provision of evaluation guidelines with sufficient instructions and pictures can also assist in greater inter-observer reliability [44,104,105]. Consistency can permit comparisons, trend analysis, and benefit animal research and welfare [158]. Scores obtained from either system (i.e., binary or numerical) require careful interpretation and awareness of any limitations. Depending on the indicators and context, either system may be appropriate [94]. A good understanding of both systems and associated factors is important when selecting animal welfare indicators for overall animal welfare assessment and in more formal animal welfare assessment schemes.

4.4. Limitations

The aim of the review was to collate as many known indicators as possible with the understanding that there might be limitations in demonstrating their validity but that they could offer potential areas for future exploration. Limitations in the study include the potential search methodology and the associated number of relevant publications selected and reviewed. Not all publications concerning the objective of the current work may have been identified by the systematic literature search. For example, some publications may not have been found in the databases searched or if indicators or assessment tools used were not stated in the title or abstract (e.g., biomedical studies). Furthermore, due to the vast array of synonyms for the search terms used and the subsequent large number of irrelevant results, keywords and synonyms were sparingly used. Although this technique offered the best return on relevant search results, it may have limited the detection of publications in which the term “welfare” and its synonyms were not included in the title or abstract, thus highlighting the difficulties and nuances of keywords in systematic searches. In addition, the automation tools (e.g., exclusion tools) incorporated into the search databases used to eliminate irrelevant/ineligible papers may also limit the detection of relevant publications. Finally, given the scarcity of available peer-reviewed published papers pertaining to valid, reliable, and feasible animal-based welfare assessment protocols, the tables relied on a higher than preferred portion of grey literature. The combination of utilising primary and secondary sources from grey literature and obtaining information from these global citation databases has proven to be a suitable starting point and approach for the purposes of this review paper.

4.5. Future Directions

Key gaps in the literature reviewed demonstrated a lower number of studies and indicators available for guinea pigs and rats, which indicates a potential risk to animal welfare due to a potential lack of validated knowledge. Across the grey literature, mouse indicators are commonly used for rats, but it is unknown if this is accurate and valid for all available indicators as most peer-reviewed publications are centred around mice. Additionally, determining the ease of use of these indicators across a variety of animal care settings (laboratory versus pet) may also be useful. However, there appears to be a baseline of shared indicators that could be useful across all species and other potential species of animals. Future research should potentially be directed towards a deeper understanding of these animals, as this would support a better interpretation of experimental results in animals for both welfare and non-welfare purposes.

More knowledge about animal welfare, indicators, and the utilisation of collectives of indicators is likely to enhance animal welfare. Moving forward, using the list of welfare indicators identified in this study, future studies should aim to determine the ideal set of welfare indicators for these animals. As the Delphi consultation technique has been shown to be a valuable tool in selecting the ideal set of mouse welfare indicators, applying the same technique to rabbit, guinea pig, and rat welfare indicators may be worth considering. Determining ideal constellations of key general and species-specific indicators could help support better training, education, and standardisation of animal welfare requirements across animal care and use industries and promote better animal welfare for all species.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, animal welfare is an area of increasing interest for animal care and use staff, industry, the research community, government bodies, and the public. It is important for those working with animals to continually enhance animal welfare. It is essential to use evidence-based assessment tools, including animal-based indicators, to evaluate animal welfare. A large number (190) of potential animal-based welfare indicators for guinea pigs, mice, rabbits, and rats and mice were identified. The literature was reviewed and categorised to outline potential animal welfare indicators based on their physiological, physical health, and behavioural elements. This research may be useful in the future when developing assessment protocols. Additional analysis of the possible ease and practicality of their use was performed, reviewing costs, equipment, time, and training requirements. With the help of experts, a more harmonised basic animal welfare assessment system may be developed to better evaluate animal welfare in day-to-day practice as well as in experimental settings. The development of a welfare assessment protocol or welfare scheme from this work should be species specific, well considered (including context), trialled, validated, and recognise negative, neutral, and positive states of welfare to facilitate improvements in animal welfare outcomes.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.C.; methodology, S.C. and C.H.; formal analysis, C.H. and S.C.; writing—original draft preparation, C.H.; writing—review and editing, S.C.; visualization C.H. and S.C.; supervision, S.C.; project administration S.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Funding Statement

This research received no external funding.

Footnotes

Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

References

  • 1.Brown M., Winnicker C. Chapter 39—Animal Welfare. In: Fox J.G., Anderson L.C., Otto G.M., Pritchett-Corning K.R., Whary M.T., editors. Laboratory Animal Medicine. 3rd ed. Academic Press Elsevier; San Diego, CA, USA: 2015. pp. 1653–1672. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.NHMRC . Australian Code for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes. Volume 8. National Health and Medical Research Council; Canberra, Australia: 2013. p. 3. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Everitt S. Veterinary Clinical Research–Legal, Ethical and Welfare Considerations. Volume 54. Wiley Online Library; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 2013. pp. 117–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Coleman G. Public animal welfare discussions and outlooks in Australia. Anim. Front. 2018;8:14–19. doi: 10.1093/af/vfx004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Phillips C., Izmirli S., Aldavood S., Alonso M., Choe B., Hanlon A., Handziska A., Illmann G., Keeling L., Kennedy M. Students’ attitudes to animal welfare and rights in Europe and Asia. Anim. Welf.-UFAW J. 2012;21:87. doi: 10.7120/096272812799129466. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Prescott M.J., Leach M.C., Truelove M.A. Harmonisation of welfare indicators for macaques and marmosets used or bred for research. F1000 Res. 2022;11:272. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.109380.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kiddie J.L., Collins L.M. Development and validation of a quality of life assessment tool for use in kennelled dogs (Canis familiaris) Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014;158:57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.05.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Polgár Z., Blackwell E.J., Rooney N.J. Assessing the welfare of kennelled dogs—A review of animal-based measures. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019;213:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2019.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Arena L., Berteselli G., Lombardo F., Candeloro L., Dalla Villa P., De Massis F. Application of a welfare assessment tool (Shelter Quality Protocol) in 64 Italian long-term dogs’ shelters: Welfare hazard analysis. Anim. Welf. 2019;28:353–363. doi: 10.7120/09627286.28.3.353. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Barnard S., Pedernera C., Candeloro L., Ferri N., Velarde A., Dalla Villa P. Development of a new welfare assessment protocol for practical application in long-term dog shelters. Vet. Rec. 2016;178:18. doi: 10.1136/vr.103336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Noble C.E., Wiseman-Orr L.M., Scott M.E., Nolan A.M., Reid J. Development, initial validation and reliability testing of a web-based, generic feline health-related quality-of-life instrument. J. Feline Med. Surg. 2019;21:84–94. doi: 10.1177/1098612X18758176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Brondani J.T., Luna S.P.L., Padovani C.R. Refinement and initial validation of a multidimensional composite scale for use in assessing acute postoperative pain in cats. Am. J. Vet. Res. 2011;72:174–183. doi: 10.2460/ajvr.72.2.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Richmond S.E., Wemelsfelder F., De Heredia I.B., Ruiz R., Canali E., Dwyer C.M. Evaluation of animal-based indicators to be used in a welfare assessment protocol for sheep. Front. Vet. Sci. 2017;4:210. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2017.00210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Zufferey R., Minnig A., Thomann B., Zwygart S., Keil N., Schüpbach G., Miserez R., Zanolari P., Stucki D. Animal-Based Indicators for On-Farm Welfare Assessment in Sheep. Animals. 2021;11:2973. doi: 10.3390/ani11102973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Muri K., Stubsjøen S., Valle P. Development and testing of an on-farm welfare assessment protocol for dairy goats. Anim. Welf. 2013;22:385–400. doi: 10.7120/09627286.22.3.385. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Napolitano F., Grasso F., Bordi A., Tripaldi C., Saltalamacchia F., Pacelli C., De Rosa G. On-farm welfare assessment in dairy cattle and buffaloes: Evaluation of some animal-based parameters. Ital. J. Anim. Sci. 2005;4:223–231. doi: 10.4081/ijas.2005.223. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dalla Costa E., Murray L., Dai F., Canali E., Minero M. Equine on-farm welfare assessment: A review of animal-based indicators. Anim. Welf. 2014;23:323–341. doi: 10.7120/09627286.23.3.323. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.ANSES Proposes a Definition of Animal Welfare and Sets the Foundation for Its Research and Expert Appraisal Work. [(accessed on 12 July 2022)]. Available online: https://www.anses.fr/en/content/anses-proposes-definition-animal-welfare-and-sets-foundation-its-research-and-expert.
  • 19.CCAC . CCAC Guidelines: Animal Welfare Assessment. Canadian Council on Animal Care; Ottawa, ON, Canada: 2021. pp. 1–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.OIE . Terrestrial Animal Health Code. 28th ed. Volume 1. World Organisation for Animal Health; Paris, France: 2019. Chapter 7.1. Introduction to the Recommendations for Animal Welfare; p. 355. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.WOAH . Terrestrial Animal Health Code. 31st ed. Volume 1. World Organisation for Animal Health; Paris, France: 2022. Chapter 7.1. Introduction to the Recommendations for Animal Welfare; pp. 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mellor D.J. Updating animal welfare thinking: Moving beyond the “Five Freedoms” towards “a Life Worth Living”. Animals. 2016;6:21. doi: 10.3390/ani6030021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mellor D.J., Beausoleil N.J., Littlewood K.E., McLean A.N., McGreevy P.D., Jones B., Wilkins C. The 2020 five domains model: Including human–animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals. 2020;10:1870. doi: 10.3390/ani10101870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Boissy A., Manteuffel G., Jensen M.B., Moe R.O., Spruijt B., Keeling L.J., Winckler C., Forkman B., Dimitrov I., Langbein J. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:375–397. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Brydges N.M., Braithwaite V.A. Measuring animal welfare: What can cognition contribute. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Sci. 2008;10:91–103. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fraser D. Understanding Animal Welfare: The Science in Its Cultural Context. Wiley-Blackwell; Bognor Regis, UK: 2008. (UFAW Animal Welfare Series). [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mellor D.J., Beausoleil N.J. Extending the ‘Five Domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Anim. Welf. 2015;24:241. doi: 10.7120/09627286.24.3.241. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Benn A.L., McLelland D.J., Whittaker A.L. A review of welfare assessment methods in reptiles, and preliminary application of the welfare quality® protocol to the pygmy blue-tongue skink, Tiliqua adelaidensis, using animal-based measures. Animals. 2019;9:27. doi: 10.3390/ani9010027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ahloy-Dallaire J., Espinosa J., Mason G. Play and optimal welfare: Does play indicate the presence of positive affective states? Behav. Process. 2018;156:3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Whittaker A.L., Golder-Dewar B., Triggs J.L., Sherwen S.L., McLelland D.J. Identification of animal-based welfare indicators in captive reptiles: A delphi consultation survey. Animals. 2021;11:2010. doi: 10.3390/ani11072010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Keating S.C., Thomas A.A., Flecknell P.A., Leach M.C. Evaluation of EMLA cream for preventing pain during tattooing of rabbits: Changes in physiological, behavioural and facial expression responses. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e44437. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Sotocinal S.G., Sorge R.E., Zaloum A., Tuttle A.H., Martin L.J., Wieskopf J.S., Mapplebeck J.C., Wei P., Zhan S., Zhang S. The Rat Grimace Scale: A partially automated method for quantifying pain in the laboratory rat via facial expressions. Mol. Pain. 2011;7:1744–8069. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-7-55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Langford D.J., Bailey A.L., Chanda M.L., Clarke S.E., Drummond T.E., Echols S., Glick S., Ingrao J., Klassen-Ross T., LaCroix-Fralish M.L. Coding of facial expressions of pain in the laboratory mouse. Nat. Methods. 2010;7:447–449. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Yeates J., Main D. Assessment of companion animal quality of life in veterinary practice and research. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2009;50:274–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-5827.2009.00755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cho C., Michailidis V., Lecker I., Collymore C., Hanwell D., Loka M., Danesh M., Pham C., Urban P., Bonin R.P. Evaluating analgesic efficacy and administration route following craniotomy in mice using the grimace scale. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:359. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-36897-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Miller A.L., Clarkson J.M., Quigley C., Neville V., Krall C., Geijer-Simpson A., Flecknell P.A., Leach M.C. Evaluating pain and analgesia effectiveness following routine castration in rabbits using behavior and facial expressions. Front. Vet. Sci. 2022;9:782486. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.782486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Whittaker A., Marsh L. The role of behavioural assessment in determining’positive’affective states in animals. CAB Rev. 2019;14:1–13. doi: 10.1079/PAVSNNR201914010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Veerasamy S., Lakritz J., Ezeji T., Lal R. Assessment methods and indicators of animal welfare. Asian J. Anim. Vet. Adv. 2011;6:301–315. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Page M.J., McKenzie J.E., Bossuyt P.M., Boutron I., Hoffmann T.C., Mulrow C.D., Shamseer L., Tetzlaff J.M., Akl E.A., Brennan S.E. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Int. J. Surg. 2021;88:105906. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2021.105906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Benato L., Murrell J., Blackwell E., Saunders R., Rooney N. Pain and analgesia in pet rabbits: A survey of the attitude of veterinary nurses. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2020;61:576–581. doi: 10.1111/jsap.13186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Baias A., Bodnariu A., Nichita I., Cristina R.T. Stress in laboratory juvenile rabbits: Physiological indicators. J. Anim. Sci. Biotechnol. 2012;45:142–145. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Meredith A., Lord B. BSAVA Manual of Rabbit Medicine. British Small Animal Veterinary Association; Quedgeley, UK: 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Trocino A., Menegon F., Zomeño C., Pasqualin D., Cunial G., Xiccato G., Pirrone F., Bertotto D., Bortoletti M., Dorigo F. A pilot study about on-farm assessment of health and welfare in rabbits kept in different housing systems. Front. Vet. Sci. 2022;9:936643. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.936643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Sickness-How to Tell If Your Rabbit Is Sick. [(accessed on 27 April 2022)]. Available online: https://rabbit.org/care/sickness/
  • 45.Rabbits. [(accessed on 27 April 2022)]. Available online: https://myresources.education.wa.edu.au/docs/default-source/resources/animal-ethics/rabbits8c896fe0-8d53-497c-9ba2-d1f780105ba0.pdf?sfvrsn=79843459_3.
  • 46.Botelho N., Vieira-Pinto M., Batchelli P., Pallisera J., Dalmau A. Testing an animal welfare assessment protocol for growing-rabbits reared for meat production based on the welfare quality approach. Animals. 2020;10:1415. doi: 10.3390/ani10081415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Weaver L.A., Blaze C.A., Linder D.E., Andrutis K.A., Karas A.Z. A model for clinical evaluation of perioperative analgesia in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2010;49:845–851. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Prola L., Cornale P., Renna M., Macchi E., Perona G., Mimosi A. Effect of breed, cage type, and reproductive phase on fecal corticosterone levels in doe rabbits. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2013;16:140–149. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2013.768903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Watson R., Munro C., Edwards K.L., Norton V., Brown J.L., Walker S.L. Development of a versatile enzyme immunoassay for non-invasive assessment of glucocorticoid metabolites in a diversity of taxonomic species. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 2013;186:16–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2013.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Dalmau A., Moles X., Pallisera J. Animal welfare assessment protocol for does, bucks, and kit rabbits reared for production. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020;7:445. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2020.00445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Mayer J. Use of behavior analysis to recognize pain in small mammals. Lab. Anim. 2007;36:43–48. doi: 10.1038/laban0607-43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Price N., Feigen Z. Rabbit Clinical Manual 2018. Melbourne Rabbit Clinic; Melbourne, Australia: 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Cohen S., Beths T. Grimace scores: Tools to support the identification of pain in mammals used in research. Animals. 2020;10:1726. doi: 10.3390/ani10101726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Grimace Scale-Rabbit. [(accessed on 28 April 2022)]. Available online: https://www.nc3rs.org.uk/3rs-resources/grimace-scales/grimace-scale-rabbit.
  • 55.Benato L., Murrell J., Knowles T.G., Rooney N.J. Development of the Bristol Rabbit Pain Scale (BRPS): A multidimensional composite pain scale specific to rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0252417. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Wemelsfelder F., Mullan S. Applying ethological and health indicators to practical animal welfare assessment. OIE Sci. Tech. Rev. 2014;33:111–120. doi: 10.20506/rst.33.1.2259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Jilge B. The rabbit: A diurnal or a nocturnal animal? J. Exp. Anim. Sci. 1991;34:170–183. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Jenkins J.R. Skin disorders of the rabbit. Vet. Clin. North Am. Exot. Anim. Pract. 2001;4:543–563. doi: 10.1016/S1094-9194(17)30042-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Meredith A., Redrobe S. BSAVA Manual of Exotic Pets. British Small Animal Veterinary Association; Quedgeley, UK: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Guinea Pigs. [(accessed on 28 April 2022)]. Available online: https://myresources.education.wa.edu.au/docs/default-source/resources/animal-ethics/guinea-pigsc5633af3-fa91-447a-a0e6-b688e53088b7.pdf?sfvrsn=9228ff73_3.
  • 61.Ellen Y., Flecknell P., Leach M. Evaluation of using behavioural changes to assess post-operative pain in the guinea pig (Cavia porcellus) PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0161941. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Powell D.M., Kozlowski C.P., Clark J., Seyfried A., Baskir E., Franklin A.D. Physical and physiological indicators of welfare in guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) serving as ambassador animals. Animals. 2020;10:815. doi: 10.3390/ani10050815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Levy I., Di Girolamo N., Keller K. Rectal temperature is a prognostic indicator in client-owned guinea pigs. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2021;62:861–865. doi: 10.1111/jsap.13388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Oliver V.L., Athavale S., Simon K.E., Kendall L.V., Nemzek J.A., Lofgren J.L. Evaluation of pain assessment techniques and analgesia efficacy in a female guinea pig (Cavia porcellus) model of surgical pain. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2017;56:425–435. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Dunbar M.L., David E.M., Aline M.R., Lofgren J.L. Validation of a behavioral ethogram for assessing postoperative pain in guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2016;55:29–34. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Guinea Pig Housing & Handling-Evidence-Based Refinements for Guinea Pigs. [(accessed on 27 April 2022)]. Available online: https://www.na3rsc.org/guinea-pigs/
  • 67.CACC . CCAC Guidelines-Mice. Canadian Council on Animal Care; Ottawa, ON, Canada: 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.CACC . CCAC Guidelines-Rats. Canadian Council on Animal Care; Ottawa, ON, Canada: 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Leach M., Thornton P., Main D. Identification of appropriate measures for the assessment of laboratory mouse welfare. Anim. Welf. 2008;17:161–170. doi: 10.1017/S0962728600027676. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Rats. [(accessed on 30 April 2022)]. Available online: https://myresources.education.wa.edu.au/docs/default-source/resources/animal-ethics/rats321b720a-48e9-470d-902d-b78889e9c64d.pdf?sfvrsn=32431de5_3.
  • 71.Mice. [(accessed on 30 April 2022)]. Available online: https://myresources.education.wa.edu.au/docs/default-source/resources/animal-ethics/mice58014827-f7d0-46ff-89b6-8756e9fe2646.pdf?sfvrsn=87f04a6c_3.
  • 72.Campos-Luna I., Miller A., Beard A., Leach M. Validation of mouse welfare indicators: A Delphi consultation survey. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:10249. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-45810-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Kappel S., Hawkins P., Mendl M.T. To group or not to group? Good practice for housing male laboratory mice. Animals. 2017;7:88. doi: 10.3390/ani7120088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Spangenberg E.M., Keeling L.J. Assessing the welfare of laboratory mice in their home environment using animal-based measures–a benchmarking tool. Lab. Anim. 2016;50:30–38. doi: 10.1177/0023677215577298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Gabriel A., Marcus M., Honig W., Walenkamp G., Joosten E. The CatWalk method: A detailed analysis of behavioral changes after acute inflammatory pain in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2007;163:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Lakes E.H., Allen K.D. Gait analysis methods for rodent models of arthritic disorders: Reviews and recommendations. Osteoarthr. Cartil. 2016;24:1837–1849. doi: 10.1016/j.joca.2016.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Leach M., Main D. An assessment of laboratory mouse welfare in UK animal units. Anim. Welf. 2008;17:171–187. doi: 10.1017/S0962728600027688. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Kroll T., Kornadt-Beck N., Oskamp A., Elmenhorst D., Touma C., Palme R., Bauer A. Additional assessment of fecal corticosterone metabolites improves visual rating in the evaluation of stress responses of laboratory rats. Animals. 2021;11:710. doi: 10.3390/ani11030710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Wright-Williams S., Flecknell P.A., Roughan J.V. Comparative effects of vasectomy surgery and buprenorphine treatment on faecal corticosterone concentrations and behaviour assessed by manual and automated analysis methods in C57 and C3H mice. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e75948. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Faraday M.M. Rat sex and strain differences in responses to stress. Physiol. Behav. 2002;75:507–522. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00645-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Hickman D.L., Swan M. Use of a body condition score technique to assess health status in a rat model of polycystic kidney disease. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2010;49:155–159. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Liles J.H., Flecknell P.A. The effects of surgical stimulus on the rat and the influence of analgesic treatment. Br. Vet. J. 1993;149:515–525. doi: 10.1016/S0007-1935(05)80036-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Monteiro F., Abraham M., Sahakari S., Mascarenhas J. Effect of immobilization stress on food intake, body weight and weights of various organs in rat. Indian J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 1989;33:186–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Hart B.L. Biological basis of the behavior of sick animals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1988;12:123–137. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(88)80004-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Mason G., Wilson D., Hampton C., Würbel H. Non-invasively assessing disturbance and stress in laboratory rats by scoring chromodacryorrhoea. Altern. Lab. Anim. 2004;32:153–159. doi: 10.1177/026119290403201s25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Jirkof P., Rudeck J., Lewejohann L. Assessing affective state in laboratory rodents to promote animal welfare—What is the progress in applied refinement research? Animals. 2019;9:1026. doi: 10.3390/ani9121026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Oliver V., De Rantere D., Ritchie R., Chisholm J., Hecker K. Psychometric Assessment of the Rat Grimace Scale and Development of an Analgesic Intervention Score. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e97882. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Miller A.L., Kitson G.L., Skalkoyannis B., Flecknell P.A., Leach M.C. Using the mouse grimace scale and behaviour to assess pain in CBA mice following vasectomy. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016;181:160–165. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Miller A.L., Leach M.C. The mouse grimace scale: A clinically useful tool? PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0136000. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Leung V., Zhang E., Pang D.S. Real-time application of the Rat Grimace Scale as a welfare refinement in laboratory rats. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:31667. doi: 10.1038/srep31667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Grimace Scale-Rat. [(accessed on 1 May 2022)]. Available online: https://www.nc3rs.org.uk/3rs-resources/grimace-scales/grimace-scale-rat.
  • 92.Grimace Scale-Mouse. [(accessed on 1 May 2022)]. Available online: https://www.nc3rs.org.uk/3rs-resources/grimace-scales/grimace-scale-mouse.
  • 93.Hohlbaum K., Corte G.M., Humpenöder M., Merle R., Thöne-Reineke C. Reliability of the mouse grimace scale in c57bl/6jrj mice. Animals. 2020;10:1648. doi: 10.3390/ani10091648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Descovich K.A., Wathan J., Leach M.C., Buchanan-Smith H.M., Flecknell P., Framingham D., Vick S.-J. Facial expression: An under-utilised tool for the assessment of welfare in mammals. Altex. 2017 doi: 10.14573/altex.1607161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Leach M.C., Klaus K., Miller A.L., Scotto di Perrotolo M., Sotocinal S.G., Flecknell P.A. The assessment of post-vasectomy pain in mice using behaviour and the Mouse Grimace Scale. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e35656. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Roughan J., Flecknell P. Effects of surgery and analgesic administration on spontaneous behaviour in singly housed rats. Res. Vet. Sci. 2000;69:283–288. doi: 10.1053/rvsc.2000.0430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Roughan J.V., Flecknell P.A. Evaluation of a short duration behaviour-based post-operative pain scoring system in rats. Eur. J. Pain. 2003;7:397–406. doi: 10.1016/S1090-3801(02)00140-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Roughan J., Flecknell P. Behaviour-based assessment of the duration of laparotomy-induced abdominal pain and the analgesic effects of carprofen and buprenorphine in rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 2004;15:461–472. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200411000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Roughan J.V., Flecknell P.A. Training in behaviour-based post-operative pain scoring in rats—An evaluation based on improved recognition of analgesic requirements. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006;96:327–342. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.06.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Rat Housing & Handling-Evidence-Based Refinements for Rats. [(accessed on 4 July 2022)]. Available online: https://www.na3rsc.org/rats-housing-handling/
  • 101.Mice Housing & Handling-Evidence-Based Refinements for Mice. [(accessed on 4 July 2022)]. Available online: https://www.na3rsc.org/mice-housing/
  • 102.A Guide to Defining and Implementing Protocols for the Welfare Assessment of Laboratory Animals: Eleventh Report of the BVAAWF/FRAME/RSPCA/UFAW Joint Working Group on Refinement. [(accessed on 5 July 2022)]. Available online: https://www.rspca.org.uk/webContent/staticImages/Downloads/WelfareAssessmentProtocolsFull.pdf. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 103.Chartier L.C., Hebart M.L., Howarth G.S., Whittaker A.L., Mashtoub S. Affective state determination in a mouse model of colitis-associated colorectal cancer. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0228413. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Begni V., Sanson A., Pfeiffer N., Brandwein C., Inta D., Talbot S.R., Riva M.A., Gass P., Mallien A.S. Social isolation in rats: Effects on animal welfare and molecular markers for neuroplasticity. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0240439. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Fone K.C., Porkess M.V. Behavioural and neurochemical effects of post-w.eaning social isolation in rodents—Relevance to developmental neuropsychiatric disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2008;32:1087–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Jirkof P. Burrowing and nest building behavior as indicators of well-being in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2014;234:139–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Lampe J.F., Burman O., Würbel H., Melotti L. Context-dependent individual differences in playfulness in male rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 2017;59:460–472. doi: 10.1002/dev.21509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Hurst J.L., West R.S. Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nat. Methods. 2010;7:825–826. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Abbott F.V., Franklin K.B., Westbrook R.F. The formalin test: Scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats. Pain. 1995;60:91–102. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Fernández-Teruel A., Estanislau C. Meanings of self-grooming depend on an inverted U-shaped function with aversiveness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:591. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Song C., Berridge K.C., Kalueff A.V. ‘Stressing’rodent self-grooming for neuroscience research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:591. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Flecknell P., Liles J. The effects of surgical procedures, halothane anaesthesia and nalbuphine on locomotor activity and food and water consumption in rats. Lab. Anim. 1991;25:50–60. doi: 10.1258/002367791780808239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Kelley A.E. Measurement of rodent stereotyped behavior. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. 1998;4:8.8.1–8.8.13. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.ns0808s04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Bechard A., Meagher R., Mason G. Environmental enrichment reduces the likelihood of alopecia in adult C57BL/6J mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2011;50:171–174. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Bechard A., Nicholson A., Mason G. Litter size predicts adult stereotypic behavior in female laboratory mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2012;51:407–411. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Gross A.N., Richter S.H., Engel A.K.J., Würbel H. Cage-induced stereotypies, perseveration and the effects of environmental enrichment in laboratory mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2012;234:61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Tynes V.V. Behavioral dermatopathies in small mammals. Vet. Clin. Exot. Anim. Pract. 2013;16:801–820. doi: 10.1016/j.cvex.2013.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Peters S.M., Pinter I.J., Pothuizen H.H., de Heer R.C., van der Harst J.E., Spruijt B.M. Novel approach to automatically classify rat social behavior using a video tracking system. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2016;268:163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Wood G.E., Young L.T., Reagan L.P., McEwen B.S. Acute and chronic restraint stress alter the incidence of social conflict in male rats. Horm. Behav. 2003;43:205–213. doi: 10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00026-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Weegh N., Füner J., Janke O., Winter Y., Jung C., Struve B., Wassermann L., Lewejohann L., Bleich A., Häger C. Wheel running behaviour in group-housed female mice indicates disturbed wellbeing due to DSS colitis. Lab. Anim. 2020;54:63–72. doi: 10.1177/0023677219879455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Brudzynski S.M. Ethotransmission: Communication of emotional states through ultrasonic vocalization in rats. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013;23:310–317. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Wöhr M., Schwarting R.K. Affective communication in rodents: Ultrasonic vocalizations as a tool for research on emotion and motivation. Cell Tissue Res. 2013;354:81–97. doi: 10.1007/s00441-013-1607-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Wöhr M. Ultrasonic communication in rats: Appetitive 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations as social contact calls. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2018;72:14. doi: 10.1007/s00265-017-2427-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Andrews N., Legg E., Lisak D., Issop Y., Richardson D., Harper S., Pheby T., Huang W., Burgess G., Machin I. Spontaneous burrowing behaviour in the rat is reduced by peripheral nerve injury or inflammation associated pain. Eur. J. Pain. 2012;16:485–495. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Wodarski R., Delaney A., Ultenius C., Morland R., Andrews N., Baastrup C., Bryden L.A., Caspani O., Christoph T., Gardiner N.J. Cross-centre replication of suppressed burrowing behaviour as an ethologically relevant pain outcome measure in the rat: A prospective multicentre study. Pain. 2016;157:2350. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Shepherd A.J., Cloud M.E., Cao Y.-Q., Mohapatra D.P. Deficits in burrowing behaviors are associated with mouse models of neuropathic but not inflammatory pain or migraine. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018;12:124. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Gaskill B.N., Pritchett-Corning K.R. Effect of cage space on behavior and reproduction in Crl: CD (SD) and BN/Crl laboratory rats. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2015;54:497–506. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Gaskill B.N., Karas A.Z., Garner J.P., Pritchett-Corning K.R. Nest building as an indicator of health and welfare in laboratory mice. J. Vis. Exp. 2013;82:e51012. doi: 10.3791/51012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Oliver V.L., Thurston S.E., Lofgren J.L. Using cageside measures to evaluate analgesic efficacy in mice (Mus musculus) after surgery. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2018;57:186–201. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Rock M.L., Karas A.Z., Rodriguez K.B.G., Gallo M.S., Pritchett-Corning K., Karas R.H., Aronovitz M., Gaskill B.N. The time-to-integrate-to-nest test as an indicator of wellbeing in laboratory mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2014;53:24–28. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.de Jong I.C., Reuvekamp B.F., Rommers J.M. A Welfare Assessment Protocol for Commercially Housed Rabbits. Wageningen UR Livestock Research; Wageningen, The Netherlands: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Jones N., Sherwen S.L., Robbins R., McLelland D.J., Whittaker A.L. Welfare Assessment Tools in Zoos: From Theory to Practice. Vet. Sci. 2022;9:170. doi: 10.3390/vetsci9040170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Sherwen S.L., Hemsworth L.M., Beausoleil N.J., Embury A., Mellor D.J. An animal welfare risk assessment process for zoos. Animals. 2018;8:130. doi: 10.3390/ani8080130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Blokhuis H.J., Miele M., Veissier I., Jones B. Improving Farm Animal Welfare: Science and Society Working Together: The Welfare Quality Approach. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Von Borell E., Langbein J., Després G., Hansen S., Leterrier C., Marchant-Forde J., Marchant-Forde R., Minero M., Mohr E., Prunier A. Heart rate variability as a measure of autonomic regulation of cardiac activity for assessing stress and welfare in farm animals—A review. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:293–316. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Stewart P.M. Tissue-specific Cushing’s syndrome, 11b-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenases and the redefinition of corticosteroid hormone action. Endocrinology. 2003;149:163–168. doi: 10.1530/eje.0.1490163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Bamberg E., Palme R., Meingassner J. Excretion of corticosteroid metabolites in urine and faeces of rats. Lab. Anim. 2001;35:307–314. doi: 10.1258/0023677011911886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Joëls M., Karst H., Sarabdjitsingh R. The stressed brain of humans and rodents. Acta Physiol. 2018;223:e13066. doi: 10.1111/apha.13066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Nohara M., Tohei A., Sato T., Amao H. Evaluation of response to restraint stress by salivary corticosterone levels in adult male mice. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 2016;78:775–780. doi: 10.1292/jvms.15-0610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Mason G.J., Mendl M. Why is there no simple way of measuring animal welfare? Anim. Welf. 1993;2:301–319. doi: 10.1017/S0962728600016092. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Mormède P., Andanson S., Aupérin B., Beerda B., Guémené D., Malmkvist J., Manteca X., Manteuffel G., Prunet P., van Reenen C.G. Exploration of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal function as a tool to evaluate animal welfare. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:317–339. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Barbosa Pereira C., Kunczik J., Zieglowski L., Tolba R., Abdelrahman A., Zechner D., Vollmar B., Janssen H., Thum T., Czaplik M. Remote welfare monitoring of rodents using thermal imaging. Sensors. 2018;18:3653. doi: 10.3390/s18113653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.González-Sánchez C., Fraile J.-C., Pérez-Turiel J., Damm E., Schneider J.G., Zimmermann H., Schmitt D., Ihmig F.R. Capacitive sensing for non-invasive breathing and heart monitoring in non-restrained, non-sedated laboratory mice. Sensors. 2016;16:1052. doi: 10.3390/s16071052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Mutlu K., Rabell J.E., Del Olmo P.M., Haesler S. IR thermography-based monitoring of respiration phase without image segmentation. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2018;301:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Pereira C.B., Czaplik M., Blanik N., Rossaint R., Blazek V., Leonhardt S. Contact-free monitoring of circulation and perfusion dynamics based on the analysis of thermal imagery. Biomed. Opt. Express. 2014;5:1075–1089. doi: 10.1364/BOE.5.001075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Rushen J. Problems associated with the interpretation of physiological data in the assessment of animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1991;28:381–386. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(91)90170-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Munksgaard L. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica. Section A. Animal Science. Taylor and Francis AS; Oxford, UK: 1996. The use of open field tests in the assessment of welfare of cattle. Supplementum (Denmark) [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Carbone L. Do “Prey Species” hide their pain? Implications for ethical care and use of laboratory animals. J. Appl. Anim. Ethics Res. 2020;2:216–236. doi: 10.1163/25889567-BJA10001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Arras M., Rettich A., Cinelli P., Kasermann H.P., Burki K. Assessment of post-laparotomy pain in laboratory mice by telemetric recording of heart rate and heart rate variability. BMC Vet. Res. 2007;3:16. doi: 10.1186/1746-6148-3-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Vojtkovská V., Voslářová E., Večerek V. Methods of assessment of the welfare of shelter cats: A review. Animals. 2020;10:1527. doi: 10.3390/ani10091527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Waite M.E., Tomkovich A., Quinn T.L., Schumann A.P., Dewberry L., Totsch S.K., Sorge R.E. Efficacy of common analgesics for postsurgical pain in rats. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2015;54:420–425. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Matsumiya L.C., Sorge R.E., Sotocinal S.G., Tabaka J.M., Wieskopf J.S., Zaloum A., King O.D., Mogil J.S. Using the Mouse Grimace Scale to reevaluate the efficacy of postoperative analgesics in laboratory mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2012;51:42–49. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Faller K.M., McAndrew D.J., Schneider J.E., Lygate C.A. Refinement of analgesia following thoracotomy and experimental myocardial infarction using the Mouse Grimace Scale. Exp. Physiol. 2015;100:164–172. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2014.083139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Miller A.L., Golledge H.D., Leach M.C. The influence of isoflurane anaesthesia on the rat grimace scale. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0166652. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Defensor E.B., Corley M.J., Blanchard R.J., Blanchard D.C. Facial expressions of mice in aggressive and fearful contexts. Physiol. Behav. 2012;107:680–685. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Andresen N., Wöllhaf M., Hohlbaum K., Lewejohann L., Hellwich O., Thöne-Reineke C., Belik V. Towards a fully automated surveillance of well-being status in laboratory mice using deep learning: Starting with facial expression analysis. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0228059. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Ernst L., Kopaczka M., Schulz M., Talbot S.R., Zieglowski L., Meyer M., Bruch S., Merhof D., Tolba R.H. Improvement of the Mouse Grimace Scale set-up for implementing a semi-automated Mouse Grimace Scale scoring (Part 1) Lab. Anim. 2020;54:83–91. doi: 10.1177/0023677219881655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Bracke M.B., Hopster H. Assessing the importance of natural behavior for animal welfare. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics. 2006;19:77–89. doi: 10.1007/s10806-005-4493-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.


Articles from Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES