Abstract
Aims
In this study, we aimed to investigate the effect of neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) on the rate of cognitive decline for both global cognition and specific cognitive domains in a cohort of patients from the Parkinson's Progression Markers Initiative (PPMI).
Method
Prospectively longitudinal data were obtained from the PPMI cohort. NPS, including depression, anxiety, apathy, psychosis, impulse control disorders (ICDs), and cognition ability, were evaluated by a series of questionnaires. Linear mixed‐effects models were used to investigate the relationship between NPS and the rate of cognitive decline. Generalized estimating equations (GEEs) were used to investigate the relationship between NPS and the occurrence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI).
Results
In total, 423 patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) were recruited at baseline and 395, 378, 366, 346, and 315 participants were followed up at 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 years, respectively. Depression, anxiety, apathy, and psychosis were associated with global cognitive decline. Except for those with ICDs, patients with psychosis, depression, anxiety, and apathy were more likely to meet the criteria for MCI. Patients with depression and anxiety showed a progressive decline in four major cognitive domains. Apathy and ICDs were separately associated with a progressive decline in processing speed‐attention and memory, respectively.
Conclusions
Neuropsychiatric symptoms, including psychosis, depression, anxiety, and apathy, could be used to predict future cognitive decline in patients with PD.
Keywords: cognitive impairment, neuropsychiatric symptoms, Parkinson's disease, prediction
This is the largest longitudinal study to study the relationship between cognitive impairment and neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS). NPS, including psychosis, depression, anxiety, and apathy, could be used to predict future cognitive decline in patients with PD. There is no relationship between impulse control disorders and cognitive decline in early PD patients.
1. INTRODUCTION
Cognitive impairment (CI) is one of the most important non‐motor symptoms experienced by patients with Parkinson's disease (PD). 1 It can present before or at the time of a diagnosis of PD 2 , 3 and gradually worsens as the disease progresses. 4 Even mild cognitive impairment (MCI) can impact a patient's activity of daily living and quality of life, thus causing a significant increase in the caregiver's burden. 5
Although numerous studies have investigated non‐motor symptoms in PD patients, especially cognitive decline, little is known about CI in PD, particularly compared to motor symptoms. Our ability to predict cognitive decline in PD remains very limited. Considering the importance of cognitive dysfunction in PD patients, it is critical to identify factors that could predict a faster rate of global cognitive decline, such as neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS). NPS included depression, anxiety, apathy, psychosis, and impulse control disorders (ICDs); these are regarded among the most common non‐motor symptoms in patients with PD. 6 Some studies reported NPS negatively impact cognitive abilities in PD patients. 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 However, in other studies, the relationship between NPS and cognitive function remained unclear. 3 , 12 Moreover, specific NPS may have differing effects on different cognitive domains. 9 , 13 In addition, most studies that investigated the relationship between cognitive dysfunction and NPS were cross‐sectional or case series studies that only included patients in the moderate to severe stages of disease. 13 Furthermore, high‐quality longitudinal cohort studies are scarce and lack evidence from the early stages of disease. Therefore, in the present study, we analyzed the data from the Parkinson's Progression Marker's Initiative (PPMI), a longitudinal cohort that included 423 newly diagnosed PD patients who were followed up annually for up to 5 years. Our aim was to confirm the relationship between cognitive dysfunction and NPS.
Previous studies have used the PPMI database to investigate the incidence and progression of NPS and CI in PD patients. These studies reported that compared with healthy controls, patients with PD were more depressed, anxious, apathetic, and psychotic 3 and that these symptoms increased significantly over time. 14 A previous study showed that NPS increased by 6.2%–20.9% and that CI increased by 2.7%–6.2% by year 5 relative to baseline. 15 No previous research has investigated the specific association between NPS and cognitive decline. Thus, the present study was designed to (i) confirm longitudinal relationships between NPS and cognitive decline in this longitudinal cohort and (ii) evaluate the influence of different NPS on cognitive domains over time.
2. METHODS
2.1. Study design
The data used in this study were acquired from the PPMI database (http://www.ppmi‐info.org). 16 The PPMI is a multicenter, international, longitudinal cohort study that was conducted at 33 clinical sites, including the United States, Europe, Israel, and Australia. The PPMI study and protocols were approved by the local ethics committees and review boards at 33 clinical sites. Written informed consent was acquired from each participant before being included in the study. All patients were drug‐naïve at baseline and underwent clinical assessments. Our longitudinal study was conducted using PPMI data collected between January 2011 and December 2017; all patients were followed up for 5 years. This research was approved by the ethics committee of the Beijing Rehabilitation Hospital. All methods in this study were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
2.2. Participants
A total of 423 PD patients were enrolled at baseline. At recruitment, participants were required to: (1) be over 30 years of age, (2) have dopamine transporter deficit on dopamine transporter imaging, (3) be competent to provide signed and informed consent, and (4) have been recently diagnosed with an asymmetric resting tremor or asymmetric bradykinesia or two of the following conditions: bradykinesia, resting tremor, and rigidity. All the PD patients recruited were drug‐naïve.
2.3. Motor assessment
Motor functions were assessed by the Movement Disorder Society‐Sponsored Revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS‐UPDRS). 17 The MDS‐UPDRS Part III is a disease‐relevant motor examination used to assess the motor experiences related to daily life. 17 The clinical stage of PD was evaluated by the Hoehn & Yahr (H&Y) stage. 18 According to Jankovic Subtype, 19 the participants were divided into tremor dominant (TD), indeterminate, and postural instability/gait dominant (PIGD) types.
2.4. Cognitive assessments
Cognitive assessments included a global cognitive screening test with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) 20 and a series of validated tests that examined four major areas of cognitive domains: memory, visuospatial functions, working memory‐executive functions, and attention‐processing speed. Memory was evaluated by the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test‐Revised (HVLT‐R), 21 while visuospatial function was assessed by the Benton Judgment of Line Orientation (JOLO). 22 Processing speed‐attention was evaluated by the Symbol‐Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) and working memory‐executive functions were evaluated by the Letter Number Sequencing and semantic fluency test. 23 For all these tests, a higher score implied a better performance. The participants were divided into three groups (normal cognition, MCI, and dementia) according to the cognitive function test results. MCI was defined as having two or more HVLT total recall, HVLT recognition discrimination, JOLO, Letter Number Sequencing, semantic fluency test, or SDMT scores exceeding 1.5 standard deviations (SD) below normal, and without functional impairment due to CI. 24 Dementia was defined as existing CI that could generate functional impairments that influenced the activity of daily living. 24 All these cognitive assessments were estimated at baseline and follow‐up visits.
2.5. Neuropsychiatric symptoms
Depression was evaluated by the 15 items Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS‐15); a score ≥5 indicated clinically significant depression. 25 Anxiety was evaluated by the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI); a score ≥40 on each subscale indicated clinically significant anxiety. 26 ICDs were evaluated by the short version of the Questionnaire for Impulsive‐Compulsive Disorders in PD for ICDs and related behaviors (gambling, sexual, buying, eating, punding, hobbyism, walkabout, and the compulsive use of medication). 27 Psychosis and apathy were separately assessed by the hallucinations/psychosis item and apathy item from the MDS‐UPDRS 17 Part I. Any psychosis or apathy symptom graded 1 or more was considered as the presence of psychosis or apathy symptom.
2.6. Other assessments
Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder (RBD) was measured with the RBD Sleep Questionnaire 28 ; a cutoff value of ≥6 was considered suggestive of REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD).
2.7. Statistical analysis
For the demographic and clinical characteristics at baseline, descriptive statistics were expressed as mean ± standard deviation (SD) for continuous variables and as percentage frequency for categorical variables. Data distribution and normality were evaluated with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test in patients with and without MCI at baseline. Age, duration, age at onset, education, H&Y stage, and MDS‐UPDRS part III were all normally distributed and were compared by independent samples t‐tests in patients with and without MCI at baseline. Gender, race, motor subtype, STAI trait, STAI state, GDS‐15, ICDs, apathy, and psychosis was categorical variables and were analyzed by the chi‐squared test in patients with and without MCI at baseline.
Linear mixed‐effects models were used to investigate the relationship between baseline NPS and the rate of cognitive decline, including psychosis, depression, anxiety, apathy, and ICDs. These models interpreted the correlations between repeated measures and variables over time. The predictive power of NPS on cognitive decline was analyzed via interactions with visit time, thus revealing the influence of NPS on cognitive score changes over time. The following terms were used as independent variables in each model: the presence/absence of NPS at baseline, visit, and visit*NPS interaction term. The repeated cognitive test measures were used as the dependent variables. The presence/absence of NPS and covariates as fixed effects and the intercept as a random effect. Covariates included age, age at onset, sex, education, H&Y stage, duration, tremor/PIGD phenotype, MDS‐UPDRS III score, and RBD score.
Generalized estimating equations (GEEs) were used to investigate the relationship between baseline NPS and the occurrence of MCI. The GEE model could account for the reversion to normal that occurred in patients with MCI and the correlations among repeated measurements. 29 The GEEs utilized the same covariates as described above.
Data were analyzed using SPSS version 21 (SPSS Inc.) and p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
3. RESULTS
In total, 423 PD patients were recruited at baseline and 395, 378, 366, 346, and 315 patients were followed up at 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 years, respectively. The baseline demographic and clinical features of PD patients are shown in Appendix S1. The clinical characteristics were similar when compared between patients with and without CI, including age, race, duration, age at onset, education, H&Y stage, motor subtype, the severity of depression, QUIP, apathy, and psychosis. Patients with CI had a more severe UPDRS III score, and a higher proportion of females (p = 0.03), trait anxiety (p = 0.011), and state anxiety (p = 0.003) than those without CI.
Cognitive decline was correlated with depression during follow‐up (Table 1). The MOCA score in depressed patients decreased by 0.638 points per annum when compared with non‐depressed patients (p < 0.001) (Table 1). With regard to specific cognitive tests, depressed patients experienced greater declines in semantic fluency, SDMT, HVLT‐R recognition discrimination, HVLT‐R delayed free recall, and JOLO (Table 1). There was no significant difference in Letter‐Number Sequencing between patients with and without depression (Table 1). The odds of MCI was significantly greater among depressed patients in comparison with those of non‐depressed patients (odds ratio [OR] = 2.575; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.771–3.744, p < 0.001) in the GEE model (Figure 1).
TABLE 1.
The results of linear mixed‐effects models investigating the effect of depression on cognitive scores.
Dependent variable | β‐coefficient (95% CI) | p‐Value |
---|---|---|
MoCA | −0.638 (−0.968, −0.309) | <0.001 |
Semantic fluency | −1.757 (−2.868, −0.645) | 0.002 |
SDMT | −2.451 (−3.495, −1.406) | <0.001 |
HVLT‐R recognition discrimination | −0.323 (−0.643, −0.004) | 0.047 |
HVLT‐R delayed free recall | −0.749 (−1.078, −0.421) | <0.001 |
Letter‐Number Sequencing | −0.176 (−0.473, 0.121) | 0.245 |
JOLO | −0.430 (−0.694, −0.166) | 0.001 |
Note: In these models, GDS‐15 ≥5 is the independent variable, and the cognitive score is the dependent variable, Age, age at onset, sex, education, H&Y Stage, duration, tremor/PIGD phenotype, MDS‐UPDRS III score, and RBD score are covariates.
Abbreviations: GDS‐15, 15 item Geriatric Depression Scale; HVLT‐R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test‐Revised; JOLO, Benton Judgment of Line Orientation; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT, Symbol‐Digit Modalities Test.
FIGURE 1.
Forest plot showing the odds of MCI in patients with and without neuropsychiatric symptoms.
Cognitive decline is also correlated with apathy over time (Table 2). PD patients with apathy experienced a reduction in MOCA score of 0.296 points per annum compared with those without apathy (p = 0.031) (Table 2). Patients with apathy experienced a significantly greater reduction in the SDMT (Table 2). There was no significant difference in semantic fluency, HVLT‐R recognition discrimination, HVLT‐R delayed free recall, Letter‐Number Sequencing, and JOLO between patients with and without apathy (Table 2). The odds of MCI were significantly greater in patients with apathy when compared with those without apathy (OR = 1.554; 95% CI: 1.130–2.137; p = 0.007) in the GEE model (Figure 1).
TABLE 2.
The results of linear mixed‐effects models investigating the effect of apathy on cognitive scores.
Dependent variable | β‐coefficient (95% CI) | p‐Value |
---|---|---|
MoCA | −0.296 (−0.564, 0.028) | 0.031 |
Semantic fluency | −0.511 (−1.408, 0.387) | 0.264 |
SDMT | −0.994 (−1.834, −0.153) | 0.021 |
HVLT‐R recognition discrimination | −0.127 (−0.388, −0.134) | 0.388 |
HVLT‐R delayed free recall | −0.232 (−0.497, 0.034) | 0.087 |
Letter‐Number Sequencing | −0.215 (−0.455, −0.024) | 0.078 |
JOLO | −0.195 (−0.410, 0.019) | 0.075 |
Note: In these models, any positive apathy score is the independent variable, and the cognitive score is the dependent variable. Age, age at onset, sex, education, H&Y Stage, duration, tremor/PIGD phenotype, MDS‐UPDRS III score, and RBD score are covariates.
Abbreviations: HVLT‐R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test‐Revised; JOLO, Benton Judgment of Line Orientation; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT, Symbol‐Digit Modalities Test.
Cognitive decline also correlated with psychosis over time (Table 3). Patients with psychosis experienced a reduction in the MOCA score of 0.708 points per annum compared with those without psychosis (p = 0.001). With regard to specific cognitive tests, there was no significant difference in semantic fluency, SDMT, HVLT‐R recognition discrimination, HVLT‐R delayed free recall, Letter‐Number Sequencing, or JOLO between patients with and without psychosis. The odds of MCI were significantly higher greater among patients with psychosis than those without psychosis (OR = 1.739; 95% CI: 1.048–2.884; p = 0.032) in the GEE model (Figure 1).
TABLE 3.
The results of linear mixed‐effects models investigating the effect of psychosis on cognitive scores.
Dependent variable | β‐coefficient (95% CI) | p‐Value |
---|---|---|
MoCA | −0.708 (−1.137, −0.280) | 0.001 |
Semantic fluency | −0.928 (−2.363, 0.507) | 0.205 |
SDMT | −0.427 (−1.779, 0.925) | 0.535 |
HVLT‐R recognition discrimination | −0.047 (−0.468, 0.373) | 0.826 |
HVLT‐R delayed free recall | −0.308 (−0.733, 0.118) | 0.156 |
Letter‐Number Sequencing | −0.011 (−0.395, 0.373) | 0.956 |
JOLO | −0.219 (−0.562, 0.124) | 0.211 |
Note: In these models, any positive psychosis score is the independent variable, and the cognitive score is the dependent variable. Age, age at onset, sex, education, H&Y Stage, duration, tremor/PIGD phenotype, MDS‐UPDRS III score, and RBD score are covariates.
Abbreviations: HVLT‐R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test‐Revised; JOLO, Benton Judgment of Line Orientation; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT, Symbol‐Digit Modalities Test.
Cognitive decline is also correlated with anxiety over time (Table 4). Patients with state anxiety experienced a reduction in MOCA score of 0.439 points per annum (p = 0.003) while patients with trait anxiety experienced a reduction in MOCA score of 0.346 points per annum (p = 0.029) when compared to those without state anxiety and trait anxiety. With regard to specific cognitive tests, patients with state anxiety experienced significantly greater declines in SDMT, HVLT‐R delayed free recall, Letter‐Number Sequencing, and JOLO (Table 4). Patients with trait anxiety experienced significantly greater declines in semantic fluency, SDMT, HVLT‐R delayed free recall, and JOLO (Table 4). The odds of MCI were significantly higher in patients with state anxiety when compared with those without state anxiety (OR = 2.462; 95% CI: 1.706–3.554; p < 0.001) and were significantly greater in patients with trait anxiety when compared with those without trait anxiety (OR = 2.605; 95% CI: 1.809–3.751; p < 0.001) in the GEE model (Figure 1).
TABLE 4.
The results of linear mixed‐effects models investigating the effect of STAI state and STAI trait on cognitive scores.
Dependent variable | STAI state | STAI trait | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
β‐coefficient (95% CI) | p‐Value | β‐coefficient (95% CI) | p‐Value | |
MoCA | −0.439 (−0.734, −0.145) | 0.003 | −0.346 (−0.656, −0.036) | 0.029 |
Semantic fluency | −0.913 (−1.906, 0.080) | 0.072 | −1.755 (−2.811, −0.698) | 0.001 |
SDMT | −0.961 (−1.893, −0.029) | 0.043 | −2.394 (−3.374, −1.415) | <0.001 |
HVLT‐R recognition discrimination | −0.174 (−0.461, 0.113) | 0.235 | −0.127 (−0.424, 0.170) | 0.401 |
HVLT‐R delayed free recall | −0.493 (−0.787, −0.199) | 0.001 | −0.530 (−0.839, −0.221) | 0.001 |
Letter‐Number Sequencing | −0.278 (−0.542, −0.013) | 0.040 | −0.263 (−0.542, 0.016) | 0.065 |
JOLO | −0.247 (−0.483, −0.010) | 0.041 | −0.529 (−0.778, −0.281) | <0.001 |
Note: In these models, STAI state ≥39 and STAI trait ≥39 is the independent variable and the cognitive score is the dependent variable. Age, age at onset, sex, education, H&Y Stage, duration, tremor/PIGD phenotype, MDS‐UPDRS III score, and RBD score are covariates.
Abbreviations: HVLT‐R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test‐Revised; JOLO, Benton Judgment of Line Orientation; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT, Symbol‐Digit Modalities Test; STAI, State–Trait Anxiety Inventory.
Cognitive decline was not correlated with QUIP over time in our model (Table 5). Only HVLT‐R recognition discrimination was associated with the cognitive decline and decreased by 0.280 points more per annum than those without QUIP (p = 0.043). There was no significant difference in MOCA, Semantic fluency, SDMT, HVLT‐R recognition discrimination, HVLT‐R delayed free recall, Letter‐Number Sequencing, and JOLO between patients with and without QUIP (Table 5). Finally, there is no significant difference in the odds of MCI with and without QUIP (OR = 1.173; 95% CI: 0.830–1.659; p = 0.366) in the GEE model (Figure 1).
TABLE 5.
The results of linear mixed‐effects models investigating the effect of ICDs on cognitive scores.
Dependent variable | β‐coefficient (95% CI) | p‐Value |
---|---|---|
MoCA | 0.017 (−0.262, −0.296) | 0.905 |
Semantic fluency | 0.192 (−0.736, 1.121) | 0.685 |
SDMT | 0.320 (−0.556, 1.196) | 0.474 |
HVLT‐R recognition discrimination | −0.280 (−0.552, −0.009) | 0.043 |
HVLT‐R delayed free recall | 0.071 (−0.206, 0.349) | 0.615 |
Letter‐Number Sequencing | −0.175 (−0.424, −0.074) | 0.167 |
JOLO | 0.091 (−0.132, 0.315) | 0.423 |
Note: In these models, any positive score is the independent variable, and the cognitive score is the dependent variable. Age, age at onset, sex, education, H&Y Stage, duration, tremor/PIGD phenotype, MDS‐UPDRS III score, and RBD score are covariates.
Abbreviations: HVLT‐R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test‐Revised; ICDs, impulse control disorders; JOLO, Benton Judgment of Line Orientation; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT, Symbol‐Digit Modalities Test.
4. DISCUSSION
In this study of early PD patients untreated at baseline, we examined the longitudinal relationship between NPS and global cognition changes in a PPMI cohort, as well as the association between NPS and four major cognitive domains, including memory, visuospatial functions, working memory‐executive functions, and attention‐processing speed over time. We identified significant associations between NPS at baseline and the rate of cognitive decline over time (in years).
NPS are important clinical manifestation for PD patients. 30 These symptoms are highly prevalent and can severely affect quality of life, for both the patients and their caregivers. Despite growing evidence suggesting a relationship between NPS and CI in PD patients, little is known of the specific aspects of this association. 9 Depression, anxiety, and psychosis, including visual hallucinations, are widely accepted as independently risk factors for subsequent CI or dementia in PD patients. 9 , 31 , 32 The independent influence of apathy on cognitive function has been investigated in previous studies 33 , 34 ; this research revealed that cognitive decline and/or dementia occurred more frequently in apathetic patients.
ICDs are common neuropsychiatric complications experienced by PD patients. 35 Related pathological behaviors, such as gambling, compulsive eating, shopping, or uninhibited sexual behavior, can have adverse effects on a patient's quality of life. However, the specific relationship between ICDs and cognitive decline has yet to be fully investigated. Some studies found that patients with ICDs experience a faster rate of cognitive decline, 36 , 37 whereas other studies did not identify this association. 38 In addition, Siri et al. reported that patients with ICDs were not associated with CI at baseline but manifested with a relatively lower degree of cognitive decline in long‐term cognitive follow‐up. 39 Our current findings agreed with that of Bentivoglio 38 in that we found that ICDs were not associated with global cognitive decline and that patients with ICDs were not more likely to develop MCI in the PPMI cohort. Although our study sheds some light on this issue, there is a clear need for further investigation.
In our study, we also investigated the influence of different NPS on cognitive domains over time. We found that patients with depression and anxiety had a lower score in attention‐processing speed and memory, along with visuospatial and executive tests. Apathy was found to be associated with a progressive decline in processing speed‐attention. QUIP also demonstrated a link with memory impairment, but with a large p‐value (p = 0.043); as such, this relationship should be interpreted cautiously. Psychosis was not associated with cognitive domain changes in this longitudinal cohort. In previous work, Pirogovsky‐Turk et al. 9 and Gryc et al. 40 also investigated the potential for this relationship in a longitudinal cohort. Pirogovsky‐Turk et al. found that anxiety and depression were the strongest predictors of longitudinal decline with regard to measures of verbal and visual learning; there was no significant association with a decline in other cognitive domains. 9 Gryc et al. found that psychosis was significantly associated with a diagnosis of PD dementia and with a reduced time to dementia longitudinally. 40 The differences between these earlier results and our current findings could be explained by different methodologies and study populations. 13 Because few longitudinal cohort studies have investigated these relationship, our current findings need to be verified further in other cohorts.
A key question to consider is why patients with NPS are more likely to experience cognitive decline; the mechanisms underlying this association remain unclear. One possible reason might be due to PD‐related disruption in frontostriatal circuits. 9 , 41 This phenomenon might be due to the common neuroanatomical substrates associated with NPS and CI. 42 , 43 , 44 , 45 , 46 A significant relationship has been detected between an elevated total neuropsychiatric inventory score and the frontal atrophy level in PD patients. 43 , 44 , 46 Furthermore, frontal cortical thinning was reported to represent a useful marker for the conversion of early dementia 45 , 47 ; abnormalities in the frontostriatal pathway have also been reported to contribute to CI in PD patients. 48 Common changes in the frontal cortex may explain the greater susceptibility of PD patients with NPS to cognitive decline. Another possible reason may be due to dysregulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA)‐axis in PD patients. 9 , 49 , 50 , 51 The HPA‐axis is a key neuroendocrine signaling system involved in physiological homeostasis and stress response. 49 Disturbances of this system lead to chronically elevated levels of cortisol; this can subsequently damage neurons in the hippocampus, an anatomical region that is critical for cognitive function. 9 Therefore, patients are prone to develop cognitive dysfunction after presenting with NPS.
Some limitations of this study must be acknowledged. First, both NPS and CI can be influenced by dopamine replacement therapy and the use of antipsychotic medications. Although the participants were drug‐naïve PD patients at baseline, we still cannot neglect the potential influence of drugs in this study. Second, as this was a longitudinal cohort study that featured 5 years of follow‐up, some questionnaires and rating scales were missing due to the long follow‐up time. Finally, multiple NPS frequently co‐occur in PD patients, especially depression and anxiety; it is difficult to differentiate between these multiple manifestations. 13 , 52 However, despite these limitations, our research findings are still of great significance and may help to identify PD patients for special trial interventions to prevent cognitive decline.
In conclusion, our study confirmed that NPS, including depression, anxiety, and apathy and psychosis exhibit longitudinal relationships with CI in PD patients. These patients experienced greater rates of cognitive decline over time. The specific relationships between ICDs and CI warrant further investigation. Our study also demonstrated an association between NPS (especially depression and anxiety) and several impaired cognitive domains. Although dysfunction of the front striatal circuits and HPA‐axis could explain this phenomenon, the underlying mechanisms warrant further study.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Study design: BYF; MDT; data acquisition: MDT; data analysis: MDT; statistical analysis: MDT; manuscript editing: MDT, BYF, JZH, and WYX; manuscript review: BYF, JZH, and WYX. All authors approved the final version of the study.
FUNDING INFORMATION
This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (82004431) and the Science and Technology Development Fund of Beijing Rehabilitation Hospital, Capital Medical University (2021‐005). The funding body had no role in protocol design, statistical analysis, and manuscript preparation.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supporting information
Appendix S1
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Parkinson's Progression Markers Initiative database (https://www.ppmi‐info.org/access‐data‐speciments/download‐data). For up‐to‐date information on the study, visit www.ppmi‐info.org.PPMI—a public‐private partnership—is funded by the Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson's Research funding partners 4D Pharma, Abbvie, Acurex Therapeutics, Allergan, Amathus Therapeutics, ASAP, Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Bial Biotech, Biogen, BioLegend, Bristol‐Myers Squibb, Calico, Celgene, Dacapo Brain Science, Denali, The Edmond J. Safra Foundation, GE Healthcare, Genentech, GlaxoSmithKline, Golub Capital, Handl Therapeutics, Insitro, Janssen Neuroscience, Lilly, Lundbeck, Merck, Meso Scale Discovery, Neurocrine Biosciences, Pfizer, Piramal, Prevail, Roche, Sanofi Genzyme, Servier, Takeda, Teva, UCB, Verily, BlueRock Therapeutics and Voyager Therapeutics.
Meng D, Jin Z, Wang Y, Fang B. Longitudinal cognitive changes in patients with early Parkinson's disease and neuropsychiatric symptoms. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2023;29:2259‐2266. doi: 10.1111/cns.14173
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
REFERENCES
- 1. Aarsland D, Batzu L, Halliday GM, et al. Parkinson disease‐associated cognitive impairment. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2021;7(1):47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Fengler S, Liepelt‐Scarfone I, Brockmann K, Schaffer E, Berg D, Kalbe E. Cognitive changes in prodromal Parkinson's disease: a review. Mov Disord. 2017;32(12):1655‐1666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Weintraub D, Simuni T, Caspell‐Garcia C, et al. Cognitive performance and neuropsychiatric symptoms in early, untreated Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2015;30(7):919‐927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Shahid M, Kim J, Leaver K, et al. An increased rate of longitudinal cognitive decline is observed in Parkinson's disease patients with low CSF Aß42 and an APOE ε4 allele. Neurobiol Dis. 2019;127:278‐286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Leroi I, McDonald K, Pantula H, Harbishettar V. Cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease: impact on quality of life, disability, and caregiver burden. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2012;25(4):208‐214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Weintraub D, Aarsland D, Chaudhuri KR, et al. The neuropsychiatry of Parkinson's disease: advances and challenges. Lancet Neurol. 2022;21(1):89‐102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Jones JD, Kurniadi NE, Kuhn TP, Szymkowicz SM, Bunch J, Rahmani E. Depressive symptoms precede cognitive impairment in de novo Parkinson's disease patients: analysis of the PPMI cohort. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(8):1111‐1120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Ehgoetz Martens KA, Silveira CRA, Intzandt BN, Almeida QJ. State anxiety predicts cognitive performance in patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology. 2018;32(8):950‐957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Pirogovsky‐Turk E, Moore RC, Filoteo JV, et al. Neuropsychiatric predictors of cognitive decline in Parkinson disease: a longitudinal study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2017;25(3):279‐289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Dissanayaka NNW, Lawson RA, Yarnall AJ, et al. Anxiety is associated with cognitive impairment in newly‐diagnosed Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2017;36:63‐68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Szymkowicz SM, Jones JD, Timblin H, Ryczek CA, Taylor WD, May PE. Apathy as a within‐person mediator of depressive symptoms and cognition in Parkinson's disease: longitudinal mediation analyses. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2021;30:664‐674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Reynolds GO, Hanna KK, Neargarder S, Cronin‐Golomb A. The relation of anxiety and cognition in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):596‐604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Alzahrani H, Venneri A. Cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review. J Neurol Sci. 2015;356(1–2):32‐44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. de la Riva P, Smith K, Xie SX, Weintraub D. Course of psychiatric symptoms and global cognition in early Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2014;83(12):1096‐1103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Weintraub D, Caspell‐Garcia C, Simuni T, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive abilities over the initial quinquennium of Parkinson disease. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2020;7(4):449‐461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Marek K, Jennings D, Lasch S, et al. The Parkinson Progression Marker Initiative (PPMI). Prog Neurobiol. 2011;95(4):629‐635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, et al. Movement Disorder Society‐sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS‐UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord. 2008;23(15):2129‐2170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967;17(5):427‐442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Stebbins GT, Goetz CG, Burn DJ, Jankovic J, Khoo TK, Tilley BC. How to identify tremor dominant and postural instability/gait difficulty groups with the movement disorder society unified Parkinson's disease rating scale: comparison with the unified Parkinson's disease rating scale. Mov Disord. 2013;28(5):668‐670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695‐699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Shapiro A, Benedict R, Schretlen D, Brandt J. Construct and concurrent validity of the Hopkins verbal learning test‐revised. Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;13(3):348‐358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Benton AL, Varney NR, Hamsher KD. Visuospatial judgment. A clinical test. Arch Neurol. 1978;35(6):364‐367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Gladsjo JA, Schuman CC, Evans JD, Peavy GM, Miller SW, Heaton RK. Norms for letter and category fluency: demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment. 1999;6(2):147‐178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Litvan I, Goldman JG, Troster AI, et al. Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: Movement Disorder Society Task Force Guidelines. Mov Disord. 2012;27(3):349‐356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Weintraub D, Oehlberg KA, Katz IR, Stern MB. Test characteristics of the 15‐item geriatric depression scale and Hamilton depression rating scale in Parkinson disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(2):169‐175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Knight RG, Waal‐Manning HJ, Spears GF. Some norms and reliability data for the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory and the Zung Self‐Rating Depression Scale. Br J Clin Psychol. 1983;22(Pt 4):245‐249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Weintraub D, Hoops S, Shea J, et al. Validation of the questionnaire for impulsive‐compulsive disorders in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2009;24(10):1461‐1467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Stiasny‐Kolster K, Mayer G, Schäfer S, Möller J, Heinzel‐Gutenbrunner M, Oertel W. The REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire – a new diagnostic instrument. Mov Disord. 2007;22(16):2386‐2393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Chahine LM, Xie SX, Simuni T, et al. Longitudinal changes in cognition in early Parkinson's disease patients with REM sleep behavior disorder. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2016;27:102‐106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Martinez‐Martin P, Leentjens AF, de Pedro‐Cuesta J, Chaudhuri KR, Schrag AE, Weintraub D. Accuracy of screening instruments for detection of neuropsychiatric syndromes in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2016;31(3):270‐279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Marinus J, Zhu K, Marras C, Aarsland D, van Hilten JJ. Risk factors for non‐motor symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17(6):559‐568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Ramirez‐Ruiz B, Junque C, Marti MJ, Valldeoriola F, Tolosa E. Cognitive changes in Parkinson's disease patients with visual hallucinations. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;23(5):281‐288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Butterfield LC, Cimino CR, Oelke LE, Hauser RA, Sanchez‐Ramos J. The independent influence of apathy and depression on cognitive functioning in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology. 2010;24(6):721‐730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Dujardin K, Sockeel P, Delliaux M, Destée A, Defebvre L. Apathy may herald cognitive decline and dementia in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2009;24(16):2391‐2397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Drew DS, Muhammed K, Baig F, et al. Dopamine and reward hypersensitivity in Parkinson's disease with impulse control disorder. Brain. 2020;143(8):2502‐2518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Poletti M, Lucetti C, Del Dotto P, Berti C, Logi C, Bonuccelli U. Relationship between neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive performance in de novo Parkinson's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2012;24(4):E22‐E23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Martini A, Weis L, Fiorenzato E, et al. Impact of cognitive profile on impulse control disorders presence and severity in Parkinson's disease. Front Neurol. 2019;10:266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Bentivoglio AR, Baldonero E, Ricciardi L, De Nigris F, Daniele A. Neuropsychological features of patients with Parkinson's disease and impulse control disorders. Neurol Sci. 2012;34(7):1207‐1213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Siri C, Cilia R, Reali E, et al. Long‐term cognitive follow‐up of Parkinson's disease patients with impulse control disorders. Mov Disord. 2015;30(5):696‐704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Gryc W, Roberts KA, Zabetian CP, et al. Hallucinations and development of dementia in Parkinson's disease. J Parkinsons Dis. 2020;10(4):1643‐1648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Owen AM. Cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson's disease: the role of frontostriatal circuitry. Neuroscientist. 2004;10(6):525‐537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Hanganu A, Monchi O. Interaction between neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive performance in Parkinson's disease: what do clinical and neuroimaging studies tell us? Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2018;18(12):91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Ye BS, Jeon S, Yoon S, et al. Effects of dopaminergic depletion and brain atrophy on neuropsychiatric symptoms in de novo Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2018;89(2):197‐204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. O'Callaghan C, Shine JM, Lewis SJ, Hornberger M. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in Parkinson's disease: fronto‐striatal atrophy contributions. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20(8):867‐872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Chung SJ, Yoo HS, Lee YH, et al. Frontal atrophy as a marker for dementia conversion in Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40(13):3784‐3794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Feldmann A, Illes Z, Kosztolanyi P, et al. Morphometric changes of gray matter in Parkinson's disease with depression: a voxel‐based morphometry study. Mov Disord. 2008;23(1):42‐46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, et al. Cognitive deficits in progressive supranuclear palsy, Parkinson's disease, and multiple system atrophy in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57(1):79‐88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Brown G, Hakun J, Lewis MM, et al. Frontostriatal and limbic contributions to cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease. J Neuroimaging. 2023;33(1):121‐133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Du X, Pang TY. Is dysregulation of the HPA‐Axis a core pathophysiology mediating co‐morbid depression in neurodegenerative diseases? Front Psych. 2015;6:32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Khatri DK, Choudhary M, Sood A, Singh SB. Anxiety: an ignored aspect of Parkinson's disease lacking attention. Biomed Pharmacother. 2020;131:110776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Soares NM, Pereira GM, Altmann V, de Almeida RMM, Rieder CRM. Cortisol levels, motor, cognitive and behavioral symptoms in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2019;126(3):219‐232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Szatmari S, Illigens BM, Siepmann T, Pinter A, Takats A, Bereczki D. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in untreated Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2017;13:815‐826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Appendix S1
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.