Abstract
The problem of entrainment is central to circadian biology. In this regard, Drosophila has been an important model system. Owing to the simplicity of its nervous system and the availability of powerful genetic tools, the system has shed significant light on the molecular and neural underpinnings of entrainment. However, much remains to be learned regarding the molecular and physiological mechanisms underlying this important phenomenon. Under cyclic light/dark conditions, Drosophila melanogaster displays crepuscular patterns of locomotor activity with one peak anticipating dawn and the other anticipating dusk. These peaks are characterized through an estimation of their phase relative to the environmental light cycle and the extent of their anticipation of light transitions. In Drosophila chronobiology, estimations of phases are often subjective, and anticipation indices vary significantly between studies. Though there is increasing interest in building flexible analysis software tools in the field, none incorporates objective measures of Drosophila activity peaks in combination with the analysis of fly activity/sleep in the same program. To this end, we have developed PHASE, a MATLAB-based program that is simple and easy to use and (i) supports the visualization and analysis of activity and sleep under entrainment, (ii) allows analysis of both activity and sleep parameters within user-defined windows within a diurnal cycle, (iii) uses a smoothing filter for the objective identification of peaks of activity (and therefore can be used to quantitatively characterize them), and (iv) offers a series of analyses for the assessment of behavioral anticipation of environmental transitions.
Keywords: circadian analysis, MATLAB, entrainment, activity phase, Drosophila, Trikinetics, DAM, Savitzky-Golay filter
Background/Introduction
Entrainment to environmental cycles is a key property of circadian clocks and is critical for maintaining the adaptive advantage of rhythmic phenomena across taxa (Pittendrigh, 1981; Vaze and Sharma, 2013). Critical markers of entrainment are (i) the stable and reproducible daily timing of behavior (phase of entrainment), (ii) systematic changes of these phases under various zeitgeber regimes, and (iii) anticipation of predictable environmental transitions (for instance, lights-ON and lights-OFF) (Pittendrigh, 1981; Moore-Ede et al., 1982; Roenneberg et al., 2005). Therefore, measures of phases and estimates of anticipation are critical to characterizing entrained behavior of organisms.
Drosophila has proved to be an insightful model system in which many of the molecular and neuronal underpinnings of entrainment have been revealed, owing to its ease of maintenance, its relatively simple nervous system, and the availability powerful genetic tools (Hardin, 2011; Helfrich-Förster, 2020). The daily morning and evening peaks of activity are the most prominent features of the Drosophila locomotor activity rhythm under light/dark (LD) cycles. The timing and amounts of activity underlying these peaks are therefore of significant interest to those using the fly to understand entrainment (Hamblen-Coyle et al., 1992; Renn et al., 1999; Grima et al., 2004; Stoleru et al., 2004). Thus, the objective estimation of the phases of these peaks and the extent of their anticipation of environmental transitions is critical for the characterization of the entrained circadian system of flies.
Recently, there has been an increasing interest in the development of open-source software and teaching tools to analyze and understand features of circadian rhythms (Schmid et al., 2011; Cichewicz and Hirsh, 2018; Geissmann et al., 2019; Abhilash and Sheeba, 2019; Cenek et al., 2020; Kostadinov et al., 2021; Hammad et al., 2021). However, to the best of our knowledge, a program that supports the standard analysis and visualization of Drosophila activity and sleep rhythms that is also easily accessible, incorporates objective analysis of phases of activity, and includes measures of anticipation is not currently available. To this end, we present PHASE – a freely available MATLAB program to analyze Drosophila Phase, Activity, and Sleep under Entrainment.
PHASE: System Requirements, Installation, and the GUI
PHASE is a freely-available, open access, and stand-alone program written for MATLAB and hosted on GitHub (https://github.com/ajlopatkin/PHASE/tree/master/Installer%20Downloads). However, users can also run PHASE in MATLAB, which requires a current MATLAB license (Runtime R2020 or later; The MathWorks, Inc, Natick, MA). The PHASE bundle can be downloaded from GitHub (https://github.com/ajlopatkin/PHASE) and instructions in the supplemental manual can be used to install and run the program on Windows (7 or later) and Mac (10.13 or later) computers. Only data acquired using Drosophila Activity Monitor (DAM) systems via DAMSystem3 and processed using the DAMFileScan can be used as input for PHASE (Trikinetics, Waltham, MA; https://www.trikinetics.com). The DAM system is the most commonly used apparatus for recording locomotor activity in flies, and is also widely used to measure sleep in the fly (Chiu et al., 2010; Cichewicz and Hirsh, 2018; Kostadinov et al., 2021).
The graphical user-interface (GUI) of PHASE can be divided into five sections (Fig. 1A). The first is where users choose the directory where all their files for analysis are stored (Fig. 1A–i). Input files for the analysis must be DAM-scanned channel files. The second section of the interface is “Data Settings” and is one of the most critical components of PHASE (Fig. 1A–ii). From left-to-right, “Data Settings” consists of the following: a display window which lists all the monitors that are available in the user-chosen folder (specified in the first section), a display window showing all the channels within the monitor files, a display window that shows which monitor and channels have been selected by the user for analysis, and input settings for the desired analysis (Fig. 1A–ii). Details regarding how to use these input settings are included in the figure legends and the supplemental user manual. The third section is where the user chooses which of the PHASE functions to execute (Figs. 1A-iii, B and C). The user chooses a scaling option for the y-axis in the plots, the folder in which to save all the plots and spreadsheets generated by PHASE, and the name of the output files in section four (Fig. 1A–iv). The buttons to execute the functions chosen in Fig. 1A-iii are in the fifth section of the user-interface (Fig. 1A–v). The three available buttons are “Sleep Analysis”, “Normalized Activity Analysis” and “Averaged Activity Analysis.” Each of these buttons will use a different data set to perform all the user-chosen analysis. In the case of “Sleep Analysis,” PHASE will use standard sleep data (see Shafer and Keene, 2021). For “Normalized Activity,” PHASE uses activity data normalized to the total activity per cycle, and for “Averaged Activity,” it uses activity data averaged over the length of the user-defined bin (see Fig. 1A-iii). We encourage users to read the figure legends and consult the user manual for detailed instructions.
Activity and Sleep Analysis
PHASE allows users to view activity and sleep profiles in ways that facilitate easy visual inspection of inter-individual and cycle-to-cycle variations in temporal patterns (Figs. 2 and 3). It also computes and exports all underlying data to spreadsheets that also contain commonly used metrics for assessment of sleep and activity, such as the number of beam crossings (i.e., activity) and the number and duration of sleep bouts in the day and night-phases (see manual). Distinct types of sleep and activity plots can be generated in PHASE using different approaches (see Fig. 1A-iii): averaging data across flies, days, and/or both (Figs. 2A–C and 3A–C). When averaging across “Days” is chosen, one plot for every individual fly (i.e., channel) is generated wherein activity/sleep is averaged across all cycles indicated by the user (Figs. 2A and 3A). PHASE generates one PDF file for every 12 channels. Thus, the 32 channels of a standard DAM monitor are saved as three PDF files. When averaging across “Flies” is chosen, a single averaged plot of the activity or sleep timeseries is generated for the days indicated in 1A-iii and data are averaged across all the channels chosen by the user (Figs. 2B and 3B). When averaging across “Both,” a single plot of activity/sleep profile is generated, wherein data are averaged across all the days and flies indicated by the user (Figs. 2C and 3C).
As previously described by many investigators, activity was significantly higher during the day in wild-type Canton S (CS) male flies under LD12:12 (Fig. 2D) and total sleep was higher during the night (Hamblen-Coyle et al., 1992; Majercak et al., 1999; Hendricks et al., 2000; Shaw et al., 2000). Increased nighttime sleep was accompanied by increased bout numbers at night (Fig. 3E) with no differences in the bout duration between night and day (Fig. 3F). Sexual dimorphism in sleep is well documented (Huber et al., 2004; Andretic and Shaw, 2005), with females sleeping less during the day than males. We quantified sleep within a three-hour window around mid-day (ZT05-ZT08) to highlight how window-specific analysis of sleep/activity can be carried out using PHASE (Fig. 3G). As expected, this analysis revealed that daytime sleep was significantly higher in males than in females (Fig. 3H).
Savitzky-Golay filtering for the quantification of behavioral phase
One of the earliest methods used to objectively characterize the timing of activity peaks in Drosophila was described by (Hamblen-Coyle et al., 1992). These authors employed a Butterworth filter to remove high-frequency components of beam crossing time-course data and used a search algorithm to look for peaks within specific windows of interest from the filtered data. Other objective measures have included the use of a non-recursive digital filter (Helfrich-Förster, 1998), and dual smoothing (non-recursive digital filter + moving averages) (Rieger et al., 2003). Recent studies have used various alternative methods of peak detection (Potdar and Vasu, 2012; De et al., 2013; Das et al., 2015; Liang et al., 2019), which supports the need for a robust peak detection method and software to implement it. While it is useful to use smoothed data to quantify the timing of behavioral peaks, there are limitations to using filters like the Butterworth because such filters work well only for stationary time-series (wherein frequency and amplitude are maintained from cycle-to-cycle). Behavioral rhythms are typically non-stationary and therefore require tools that work well for such time-series (Leise, 2013; Leise, 2017). One such digital filter is the Savitzky-Golay (SG) filter (Savitzky and Golay, 1964; Schafer, 2011). The SG filter is known for its ability to preserve amplitude of the signal while de-noising the data set, thereby making it extremely useful for the identification and characterization of peaks and the estimation of their phases. While it has been used in other sub-disciplines of biology (Azami et al., 2012; Chu et al., 2014), it has not, to our knowledge, been applied to circadian data.
Mathematically, the SG filter fits a polynomial of order p, to a window of size n over the original timeseries using linear least-squares. This window of the timeseries then slides across the entire length of the timeseries, fitting a polynomial of order p for each window. In the case of equally spaced data, as is the case with Drosophila activity and sleep time-series, there exists an analytical solution to this set of least-squares equations as a single set of convolution coefficients, thereby making this a fast and computationally straightforward filter. These coefficients can then be used to derive signal estimates (e.g., activity or sleep) at a given time t. This smoothed, estimated signal can then be used to make peak calls and characterize anticipation of environmental transitions (see below). For its implementation in PHASE, it is important to note that the sliding window must consist of an odd number of timepoints, whose units must be in minutes.
To demonstrate the effect of polynomial order and window length on smoothing, we used SG filters with varying values of p and n and overlaid smoothed estimates over the raw data (Suppl. Fig. 1). These results show that, for a given filter order, increasing window lengths result in smoother time-series. Furthermore, for a given window length, increasing the filter order increases the ability of the filter to capture nuances of the time-series (Suppl. Fig. 1). Therefore, the most useful filter order and frame-length will depend on the nature of both the raw data and the question being posed, which can be established using an iterative process to determine the most appropriate parameters. As an example, we have documented the phases of morning and evening peaks of activity, the associated inter-individual variation, and the amplitude of morning and evening peaks of CS males under LD12:12 at 25 °C using different polynomial orders (p) and filter frame lengths (n) (Suppl. Tables 1–3). It is immediately clear that although the mean values of morning and evening peak phases do not vary much, the coefficients of variation across individual flies differ vastly. For instance, lower polynomial orders and longer filter frame lengths have higher inter-individual variation, which may indicate that phase calls were made with reduced accuracy (Suppl. Tables 1–2). In the case of amplitude, the maximum activity counts around the morning and evening peaks of activity in the average profile were 1.48 and 2.11/minute. From Suppl. Table 3 it is clear that lower and higher polynomial orders and filter frame lengths, in most cases, provide estimates of amplitude that are further away from the actual values of activity. Using such measures, users could vary the polynomial order and filter frame length to determine which combinations best capture their raw data most closely and use that for downstream analyses.
We used a filter order of three and a frame-length of 241 minutes to estimate peak times in the analysis presented here to illustrate how this filter can be used to characterize activity peaks in Drosophila behavioral time-series (employing the iterative approach discussed above). Filtering with these parameters captures the activity patterns of wild-type CS males entrained to three LD cycles of differing daylengths well (Fig. 4A-left). For equinox and short-day cycles (LD 8:16, 8 hours of light and 16 hours of darkness), the morning peaks of activity immediately follow lights-on but occur about an hour after lights-on under long-day conditions (Fig. 4B). Evening peaks of activity are clearly adjusted to occur just before dusk (Fig. 4C) in CS males, as previously described (e.g., Qiu and Hardin, 1996). In contrast, per01 mutants, which lack a functional molecular clock, are characterized by evening peaks that occur after lights-off under all day-lengths (Figs. 4A-right and 4C), suggesting that these are masked peaks of activity and not clock-driven activity peaks. This illustrates the utility of the SG filter for determining if behavioral peaks anticipate or follow environmental transitions.
We used a second fly mutant to demonstrate the utility of the SG filter in the characterization of behavioral timing. As previously shown, pdf01 mutants have an advanced evening peak of activity and a reduced or absent morning peak of activity under LD cycles (Renn et al., 1999). Comparing the top and bottom rows of Fig. 5A reveals that the morning peaks of activity are highly reduced in pdf01 flies compared to their genetic control background, w1118. We extracted peak information with PHASE using the order and window length values described above and quantified both the phases and heights of activity peaks for both genotypes. As expected from previous work (Renn et al., 1999), the morning peaks of pdf01 mutants were delayed (Fig. 5B) and their heights significantly reduced (Fig. 5C), whereas the evening peak phases were significantly advanced (Fig. 5D) and their heights significantly reduced compared to w1118 controls (Fig. 5E). These examples further illustrate the utility of smoothing time-series data with the SG filter to characterize the timing and magnitude of activity peaks. A similar approach can be used to estimate and characterize peaks of sleep in flies using PHASE.
The Use of Slopes for Estimating Anticipation
The behavioral anticipation of predictable environmental transitions depends on a functional and entrained circadian system. One of the first methods of estimating anticipation was proposed by (Stoleru et al., 2004) who used an estimate of anticipation that was directly proportional to the gradual, stepwise increase in activity prior to environmental transitions, inversely proportional to the startle response due to the transition. Only a few studies have made use of this method (Stoleru et al., 2004; Stoleru et al., 2007; Sheeba et al., 2010). A simpler approach to the estimation of anticipation was proposed by (Harrisingh et al., 2007), who used the ratio of activity in a three-hour window before a transition of interest and the activity in a six-hour window before the same transition. The expected value of this metric in case of a complete lack of anticipation is 0.5. As anticipation increases, the value of this metric is also expected to increase. This approach has been used in many studies (Im and Taghert, 2010; Sheeba et al., 2010; Potdar and Vasu, 2012; De et al., 2013; Prakash et al., 2017; Baik et al., 2018; Schlichting et al., 2019). A recently published approach to quantifying anticipation made use of slopes from least-squares linear regression modeling of the increase in activity before specific environmental transitions (Fernandez et al., 2020). While both the above discussed measures of anticipation can be easily calculated from the activity data exported by PHASE, the slope method requires additional steps. We therefore implemented slope calculations into our analysis package to ease the use of this approach for Drosophila behavioral time-series data.
For the calculation of slopes, PHASE allows users to choose the duration of the time-window and the temporal resolution of time-series data sets to conduct linear regression analysis. Using this method, we compared the activity of wild-type (CS) and per01 mutant males in the hours before lights-on and light-off under LD12:12 cycles. Best fitting lines for each fly reveal that CS flies (Fig. 6A) display steeper increases in activity (i.e., higher slopes) before both the morning and evening transitions compared to per01 mutants (Fig. 6B). We quantified morning and evening slopes for every fly and compared average slope values for each genotype. The results revealed that, as expected, per01 mutants do not display significantly positive slopes in the hours leading to light transitions, indicating that there is no morning or evening anticipation, consistent with the absence of circadian timekeeping (Fig. 6C and legend therein). Furthermore, CS flies consistently displayed significantly positive slopes that were on average much higher than those of per01 flies, consistent with the presence of robust anticipatory activity in CS controls (Fig.6C). It is important to note here that this functionality is available only for the “Averaged Activity Analysis” option (see Fig. 1A–v) and not for the “Normalized Activity Analysis” option. It is also useful to contrast this method to the method of estimating anticipation described by Harrisingh et al., 2007. We found that using Harrisingh’s method for per01 mutants indicates anticipation in both morning and evening windows. Further, while CS flies did not show higher anticipation than per01 flies in the morning window, they had higher statistically significant anticipation index compared to per01 flies in the evening window (Suppl. Fig. 2). These results show that, at least in this context, the slope method of estimating anticipation is better at capturing the differences between mutants and controls than the previously used metrics of anticipation.
The SG filter can also identify the highest and lowest points of activity in the smoothed data within the user-defined window (Fig. 6D). PHASE uses these values to estimate the duration of anticipatory activity and the area under the curve between the minimum and maximum data points identified by PHASE (Fig. 6D). Based on these measurements (see manual for details), we found that the percent time spent in anticipation was significantly higher in CS controls compared to per01 mutants for both morning (Fig. 6E) and evening (Fig. 6F) transitions. We also found that the area under the curve was significantly larger for CS controls for both transitions (Figs. 6G and H), consistent with significantly reduced anticipation in per01 mutants. Additionally, the slope method is utilized by PHASE to calculate anticipation of environmental transitions for “Normalized Activity” and latency of sleep following environmental transitions using the smoothed normalized activity and sleep data, respectively (see manual for details). We note that each method of measuring anticipation has its own set of limitations and the ability to implement multiple measures in a program like PHASE will be helpful for researchers interested in quantitively characterizing anticipation in fly behavioral time-series.
In conclusion, we offer PHASE as a platform to support the visualization and analysis of behavioral time-series through user-defined, window-based measurements of activity and sleep data. Furthermore, PHASE implements a powerful smoothing filter to model fly activity and sleep rhythms, which allows for the quantitative characterization of activity peaks and anticipation, a feature that is not currently widely available. All codes for PHASE are open-source and available at: https://github.com/ajlopatkin/PHASE. The software bundle for installation can be downloaded from the same link. A detailed user manual is available as supplementary online material. This user manual contains detailed information regarding the interface, suggestions to make analyses easier, details about the graphs generated, and descriptions of common errors and how to troubleshoot them, all of which we hope will make this program useful for the circadian fly community and support future breakthroughs in our understanding of circadian entrainment.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) NINDS grant (R01NS077933) and an NSF IOS grant (1354046) to OTS, an NIH NINDS grant (R01NS118012) to MPF and OTS, and start-up funds from Barnard College to MPF. We thank members of the Fernandez and Shafer labs for feedback on the PHASE software.
References
- Abhilash L and Sheeba V (2019) RhythmicAlly: Your R and Shiny–Based Open-Source Ally for the Analysis of Biological Rhythms. J Biol Rhythms 074873041986247. doi: 10.1177/0748730419862474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andretic R and Shaw P (2005) Essentials of sleep recordings in Drosophila: moving beyond sleep time. Meth Enzymol 393:759–772. doi: 10.1016/S0076-6879(05)93040-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azami H, Mohammadi K and Bozorgtabar B (2012) An Improved Signal Segmentation Using Moving Average and Savitzky-Golay Filter. 2012:39–44. doi: 10.4236/JSIP.2012.31006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baik L, Recinos Y, Chevez J and Holmes T (2018) Circadian modulation of light-evoked avoidance/attraction behavior in Drosophila. PLoS One. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0201927 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cenek L, Klindziuk L, Lopez C, McCartney E, Martin Burgos B, Tir S, Harrington M and Leise T (2020) CIRCADA: Shiny Apps for Exploration of Experimental and Synthetic Circadian Time Series with an Educational Emphasis. J Biol Rhythms 35:214–222. doi: 10.1177/0748730419900866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiu JC, Low KH, Pike DH, Yildirim E and Edery I (2010) Assaying locomotor activity to study circadian rhythms and sleep parameters in Drosophila. J of Vis Exp: JoVE. doi: 10.3791/2157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chu L, Liu GH, Huang C and Liu QS (2014) Phenology detection of winter wheat in the Yellow River delta using MODIS NDVI time-series data. 2014 The 3rd International Conference on Agro-Geoinformatics, Agro-Geoinformatics 2014. doi: 10.1109/AGRO-GEOINFORMATICS.2014.6910664 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cichewicz K and Hirsh J (2018) ShinyR-DAM: a program analyzing Drosophila activity, sleep and circadian rhythms. Comm Biol 2018 1:1 1:1–5. doi: 10.1038/s42003-018-0031-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Das A, Holmes TC and Sheeba V (2015) dTRPA1 Modulates Afternoon Peak of Activity of Fruit Flies Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One 10:e0134213. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0134213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De J, Varma V, Saha S, Sheeba V and Sharma VK (2013) Significance of activity peaks in fruit flies, Drosophila melanogaster, under seminatural conditions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:8984–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220960110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez MP, Pettibone HL, Bogart JT, Roell CJ, Davey CE, Pranevicius A, Huynh K v., Lennox SM, Kostadinov BS and Shafer OT (2020) Sites of Circadian Clock Neuron Plasticity Mediate Sensory Integration and Entrainment. Curr Biol 30:2225–2237.e5. doi: 10.1016/J.CUB.2020.04.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geissmann Q, Rodriguez LG, Beckwith EJ and Gilestro GF (2019) Rethomics: An R framework to analyse high-throughput behavioural data. PLoS One 14:e0209331. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0209331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grima B, Chélot E, Xia R and Rouyer F (2004) Morning and evening peaks of activity rely on different clock neurons of the Drosophila brain. Nature 431:869–873. doi: 10.1038/nature02935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamblen-Coyle MJ, Wheeler DA, Rutila JE, Rosbash M and Hall JC (1992) Behavior of period-altered circadian rhythm mutants of Drosophila in light:dark cycles (Diptera: Drosophilidae). J Insect Behav 5:417–446. doi: 10.1007/BF01058189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hammad G, Reyt M, Beliy N, Baillet M, Deantoni M, Lesoinne A, Muto V and Schmidt C (2021) pyActigraphy: Open-source python package for actigraphy data visualization and analysis. PLoS Comput Biol 17:e1009514. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PCBI.1009514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardin PE (2011) Molecular genetic analysis of circadian timekeeping in Drosophila. Adv Genet 141–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrisingh MC, Wu Y, Lnenicka GA and Nitabach MN (2007) Intracellular Ca2+ Regulates Free-Running Circadian Clock Oscillation In Vivo. J Neurosci 27:12489–12499. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3680-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfrich-Förster C (1998) Robust circadian rhythmicity of Drosophila melanogaster requires the presence of lateral neurons: a brain-behavioral study of disconnected mutants. J Comp Physiol A 182:435–453. doi: 10.1007/S003590050192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfrich-Förster C (2020) Light input pathways to the circadian clock of insects with an emphasis on the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A 206:259. doi: 10.1007/S00359-019-01379-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendricks J, Finn S, Panckeri K, Chavkin J, Williams J, Sehgal A and Pack A (2000) Rest in Drosophila is a sleep-like state. Neuron 25:129–138. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80877-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huber R, Hill S, Holladay C, Biesiadecki M, Tononi G and Cirelli C (2004) Sleep homeostasis in Drosophila melanogaster. Sleep 27:628–639. doi: 10.1093/SLEEP/27.4.628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Im S and Taghert P (2010) PDF receptor expression reveals direct interactions between circadian oscillators in Drosophila. J Comp Neurol 518:1925–1945. doi: 10.1002/CNE.22311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kostadinov B, Lee Pettibone H, Bell EV, Zhou X, Pranevicius A, Shafer OT and Fernandez MP (2021) Open-source computational framework for studying Drosophila behavioral phase. STAR Protocols 2:100285. doi: 10.1016/J.XPRO.2020.100285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leise TL (2017) Analysis of Nonstationary Time Series for Biological Rhythms Research: http://dx.doi.org/101177/0748730417709105 32:187–194. doi: 10.1177/0748730417709105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leise TL (2013) Wavelet analysis of circadian and ultradian behavioral rhythms. J Circ Rhythms 11:5. doi: 10.1186/1740-3391-11-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liang X, Ho MCW, Zhang Y, Li Y, Wu MN, Holy TE and Taghert PH (2019) Morning and Evening Circadian Pacemakers Independently Drive Premotor Centers via a Specific Dopamine Relay. Neuron 102:843–857.e4. doi: 10.1016/J.NEURON.2019.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Majercak J, Sidote D, Hardin PE and Edery I (1999) How a circadian clock adapts to seasonal decreases in temperature and day length. Neuron 24:219–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore-Ede MC, Sulzman FM and Fuller CA (Charles A (1982) The clocks that time us : physiology of the circadian timing system. Harvard University Press [Google Scholar]
- Pittendrigh CS (1981) Circadian Systems: Entrainment. Biological Rhythms. Springer US, Boston, MA, pp 95–124 [Google Scholar]
- Potdar S and Vasu S (2012) Large Ventral Lateral Neurons Determine the Phase of Evening Activity Peak across Photoperiods in Drosophila melanogaster. J Biol Rhythms 27:267–279. doi: 10.1177/0748730412449820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prakash P, Nambiar A and Sheeba V (2017) Oscillating PDF in termini of circadian pacemaker neurons and synchronous molecular clocks in downstream neurons are not sufficient for sustenance of activity rhythms in constant darkness. PLoS One 12:e0175073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qiu J and Hardin PE (1996) per mRNA cycling is locked to lights-off under photoperiodic conditions that support circadian feedback loop function. Mol Cell Biol 16:4182–4188. doi: 10.1128/MCB.16.8.4182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renn SCP, Park JH, Rosbash M, Hall JC and Taghert PH (1999) A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell 99:791–802. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81676-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rieger D, Stanewsky R and Helfrich-Förster C (2003) Cryptochrome, compound eyes, Hofbauer-Buchner eyelets, and ocelli play different roles in the entrainment and masking pathway of the locomotor activity rhythm in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J Biol Rhythms 18:377–391. doi: 10.1177/0748730403256997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roenneberg T, Dragovic Z and Merrow M (2005) Demasking biological oscillators: properties and principles of entrainment exemplified by the Neurospora circadian clock. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:7742–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501884102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abraham Savitzky and Golay MJE(1964) Smoothing and Differentiation of Data by Simplified Least Squares Procedures. Anal Chem 36:1627–1639. doi: 10.1021/AC60214A047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer RW (2011) What is a savitzky-golay filter? IEEE Signal Processing Magazine 28:111–117. doi: 10.1109/MSP.2011.941097. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schlichting M, Díaz MM, Xin J and Rosbash M (2019) Neuron-specific knockouts indicate the importance of network communication to Drosophila rhythmicity. eLife. doi: 10.7554/eLife.48301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmid B, Helfrich-Förster C and Yoshii T (2011) A New ImageJ Plug-in “ActogramJ” for Chronobiological Analyses. J Biol Rhythms 26:464–467. doi: 10.1177/0748730411414264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shafer OT and Keene AC (2021) The Regulation of Drosophila Sleep. Curr Biol 31:R38–R49. doi: 10.1016/J.CUB.2020.10.082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw PJ, Cirelli C, Greenspan RJ and Tononi G (2000) Correlates of Sleep and Waking in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 287:1834–1837. doi: 10.1126/SCIENCE.287.5459.1834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheeba V, Fogle KJ and Holmes TC (2010) Persistence of Morning Anticipation Behavior and High Amplitude Morning Startle Response Following Functional Loss of Small Ventral Lateral Neurons in Drosophila. PLoS One 5:e11628. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0011628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoleru D, Nawathean P, Fernández M de la P, Menet JS, Ceriani MF and Rosbash M (2007) The Drosophila Circadian Network Is a Seasonal Timer. Cell 129:207–219. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.02.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoleru D, Peng Y, Agosto J and Rosbash M (2004) Coupled oscillators control morning and evening locomotor behaviour of Drosophila. Nature 2004 431:7010 431:862. doi: 10.1038/nature02926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaze KM and Sharma VK (2013) On the Adaptive Significance of Circadian Clocks for Their Owners. Chronobiol Int 30:413–433. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.754457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.