Skip to main content
The British Journal of Surgery logoLink to The British Journal of Surgery
. 2021 May 5;108(10):1145–1148. doi: 10.1093/bjs/znab120

Central importance of emotional and quality-of-life outcomes in the public’s perception of face transplantation

D C Murphy 1,2, V Hoyle 3, D Saleh 4, J Rees 5, F Bound Alberti 6,
PMCID: PMC10364884  PMID: 33954642

Abstract

Background

Face transplantation is a surgical innovation to manage people with severely interrupted facial function and form. How the public perceive face transplantation and its potential implications for the recipient, donor, and society is unclear. The aim of this study was to understand the public perception of face transplantation, including when it is appropriate, what information is required to feel adequately informed, and which factors influence a person’s willingness to donate their face.

Methods

This was a nationwide survey of participants representative of the GB public. A quantitative analysis was performed. Free-text qualitative responses were coded with thematic content analysis and a narrative analysis was constructed.

Results

The survey included 2122 participants. Face transplantation was considered worth the potential risks if it improved an individual’s quality of life, gave them a ‘normal life’, and/or increased their confidence and social interaction. Respondents were worried about the impact face transplantation might have on donor families, especially recipient families adapting to the identity of the donor. Respondents most concerned about the concept of face transplantation were aged at least 55 years (χ2(4) = 38.9, P < 0.001), women (χ2(1) = 19.8, P < 0.001) , and Indian/Asian (χ2(4) = 11.9, P = 0.016).

Conclusion

The public perceive emotional and psychological outcomes as equally as important as, or more important than, surgical outcomes when determining the appropriateness of face transplantation. Future research should focus on measuring and describing emotional and psychological outcomes after face transplantation.


This article explores public perceptions of face transplants, and highlights the importance of psychosocial as well as surgical outcomes. It shows how important qualitative research is in understanding and evaluating facial transplantation as a form of vascular composite allograft. It also highlights the importance of consulting the public, and facial difference communities, in order to account for lived experience and beliefs about face transplants.

Introduction

The first face transplantation (FT) was reported in 2005. Subsequently, 46 FTs have been performed globally1. As FTs become a viable intervention for severely interrupted facial function and form, the scientific literature is tending to focus on technical and quantitative assessments. Efforts to understand the psychological and quality-of-life impacts are less developed2–4. Questions about the appropriateness of FT, and its implications for recipients and donor families remain unresolved.

All transplants have ramifications for recipients and families, but additional complexities relating to identity, communication, and well-being accompany FT5,6. Although psychosocial issues are being raised repeatedly by ethicists, the validity of qualitative outcome measures is unclear, and recipient-centred reporting limited7–9.

Engaging the public in FT discussions is critical because willingness to donate is integral to developing a transplant service, and because sociocultural expectations contextualize recipients’ experiences. Factors that define success, psychosocially as well as physically, are required. Consultation with the public is also critical to understanding of the appropriateness of FT as a high-cost intervention in a publicly funded national health service.

The aim of this study was to undertake a nationwide survey to determine public understanding of the circumstances in which FT is appropriate; the most important outcome measures; information needed to feel informed; and factors that influence willingness to donate faces.

Methods

A cross-sectional survey was distributed to anonymous members of the GB (England, Scotland, and Wales) public using the British market research and data analytics firm, YouGov©10. Questions were identified by multidisciplinary discussions between surgical teams, psychologists, qualitative researchers, and patient representatives. Ten priority themes were chosen and all efforts were made to limit order-effects bias. Participants received instructions and a short, textual description of FT, citing potential outcomes and risks, without images.

Using a sampling algorithm that assessed eligible participants against the project’s demographic requirements, on 24 February 2020 YouGov© distributed the survey to participants weighted by demographics, including age, sex, and social class. Ethnicities were represented similarly to those of the UK population, although Caucasians were slightly over-represented11.

Quantitative analysis

Analysis was performed using SPSS® version 25 (IBM, Armonk, New York, USA). Characteristics and proportions were compared using the χ2 test, with P ≤ 0.050 indicating significance. For non-mutually exclusive questions, the percentage (with 95 per cent confidence interval) based on the total in that demographic group is provided.

Qualitative analysis

Free-text responses were aggregated and uploaded to NVivo 12™. Thematic content analysis coded data and themes were identified based on meanings, word choice, and context of responses. Codes were interrogated for the frequency of themes (N = X, where N refers to the number of responses), co-determinacy and juxtaposition, and a narrative analysis constructed.

Results

Some 2112 people responded to the survey (Table 1). Respondents were less likely to be concerned than not concerned about FT (χ2(1) = 12.0, P = 0.001 (Table 2). Women were more concerned than men (χ2(1) = 19.8, P < 0.001). Concern was more often expressed by respondents aged at least 55 years compared with other age groups (χ2(4) = 38.9, P < 0.001), and this group more often considered FT to be never appropriate (χ2(4) = 10.4, P = 0.034). Concern was more likely to be expressed by respondents of Indian/Asian ethnicity (χ2(4) = 11.9, P = 0.016).

Table 1.

Description of respondents who completed the nationwide survey

No. of patients
(n = 2112)
Age (years)
18–24 230 (10.9)
25–34 354 (16.8)
35–44 373 (17.7)
45–54 332 (15.7)
≥ 55 823 (38.9)
Sex ratio (M : F) 1019 : 1093
Ethnicity*
Caucasian 1968 (93.2)
Indian/Asian 62 (3.0)
Mixed race (white and other) 20 (0.9)
Afro-Caribbean 19 (0.9)
Arab 7 (0.3)
Other 20 (0.9)
Geographical location
North England 517 (24.5)
Midlands 356 (16.9)
East of England 216 (10.2)
London and South East England 554 (26.2)
South West England 189 (8.9)
Wales 97 (4.6)
Scotland 183 (8.7)
Face difference relationship
Personal facial difference 33 (1.5)
Family member with facial difference 60 (2.8)
Friend has facial difference 94 (4.4)
None of above 1830 (86.6)
Prefer not to say 107 (4.7)

Values in parentheses are percentages. *Sixteen respondents declined to state ethnicity.

Table 2.

Association between respondent characteristics and concerns about face transplantation

No. of participants Concerned Not concerned Never appropriate
Total 801 (46) 946 (54) 155 (7.3)
Age (years)
18–24 230 (10.9) 84 (45) 104 (55) 10 (4.2)
25–34 354 (16.8) 117 (41) 171 (59) 22 (6.3)
35–44 373 (17.7) 108 (37) 181 (63) 23 (6.4)
45–54 332 (15.7) 103 (38) 166 (62) 21 (6.4)
≥55 823 (38.9) 388 (54) 325 (46) 79 (9.4)
Sex ratio (M : F) 1019 : 1024 342 : 459 505 : 441 86 : 69
Ethnicity
Caucasian 1968 (93.2) 736 (45) 899 (55) 130 (7.0)
Indian/Asian 62 (3.0) 31 (67) 15 (33) 8 (14.3)
Mixed race (white and other) 20 (0.9) 16 (50) 16 (50) 2 (5.7)
Afro-Caribbean 19 (0.9) 5 (33) 10 (67) 0 (0.0)
Other 27 (1.2) 11 (61) 7 (39) 5 (15.6)
Face difference relationship
Personal facial difference 33 (1.5) 15 (49) 16 (51) 2 (6.2)
Family member with facial difference 60 (2.8) 26 (49) 27 (51) 2 (3.2)
Friend has facial difference 94 (4.4) 40 (47) 45 (53) 2 (2.3)
None of above 1830 (86.6) 697 (45) 844 (55) 133 (7.3)
Prefer not to say 107 (4.7) 28 (58) 20 (42) 16 (14.7)

Values in parentheses are percentages based on respondents who indicated a preference. Participants who responded ‘don’t know’ have been omitted. Percentages of those who said face transplant was never appropriate (never an appropriate intervention under any circumstances) are based on the total number of participants in that subcategory. Abbreviations: F: female; M: male; No.: number

Respondents were asked to describe the circumstances in which FT was an appropriate intervention (Table S1). Respondents perceived FT as appropriate when disfigurements were caused by attack, accident or condition (86 (95 per cent c.i. 84-88) per cent), and least appropriate when caused by self-inflicted injury (45 (43-47) per cent), particularly among respondents aged at least 55 years (36 (32-40) per cent), or of Afro-Caribbean (33 (11-55 per cent) or Indian/Asian (19 (6-32) per cent) ethnicity. Respondents with a facial difference agreed that FT was appropriate when psychological health was affected negatively (81 (66-96) per cent), but other groups less commonly held this view.

Narrative synthesis

Overall, 1610 participants (76 per cent) provided free-text answers in which they centralized emotional and psychological concerns. Potential benefits for recipients were most commonly raised (230 participants), and FT considered worthwhile if it improved quality of life (58), provided a ‘normal life’ (18) or increased confidence (11). Some 109 respondents felt that potential recipients should have autonomy in determining their needs.

Many respondents were concerned about the risk of FT; 112 felt it was too risky, and 110 highlighted dangers of rejection and failure. Negative effects on identity and psychology (33 participants), and the trauma of facial loss for donors and families (39), were described. Many recognized that FT required complex risk–benefit analyses (117 participants) and, where benefits outweighed risks, this reflected positive psychological outcomes.

When asked what further information they required, respondents stressed understanding recipient outcomes (166 participants), rates of success (78), and psychological effects (23). Thirty-seven participants requested testimonies from transplant recipients.

The psychological implications of FT were critical for decisions regarding donation. When considering donation, 107 respondents worried about the impact on their families, especially if recipients resembled donors (68 participants), although this concept is incorrect clinically12–14.

Seventy-two participants considered the face too personal to donate; for this reason, 53 would not donate their face, even if they donated other organs. Twenty-seven said this was because the face was visible and personal, unlike internal organs. When asked about donating the face of a loved one, despite 111 expressing a desire to help others, there was significant resistance. Where people would not donate a family member’s face, this was because the idea was upsetting, unbearable or uncomfortable (113 participants).

Discussion

This large survey of the GB public provides new insights and lay beliefs about FT, and how the public defines success. Emotional and psychological outcomes were equally as important as, or more important than, surgical outcomes. This contradicts existing literature, which largely defines the success of FT in terms of postoperative surgical outcomes.

The importance of recipient autonomy was emphasized by respondents, who believed FT was appropriate if recipients felt that the benefits outweighed the risks. Requests for information about aftercare, outcomes, and prognoses highlights an awareness of a recipient’s longitudinal life experience, especially given the risks of chronic rejection.

It has been argued that individuals cannot give fully informed consent while FT remains experimental; potential recipients are vulnerable, and long-term biopsychosocial outcomes are unknown15–19. The present findings suggest that the emotional needs of patients are paramount in public views of FT, and qualitative assessments should investigate the diverse meanings of risk in different contexts.

One area where more clarity is needed is the relationship between cosmetic and functional repair. Respondents overwhelmingly considered FT as intended to improve cosmesis, despite being advised of functional benefits, and requested before-and-after images20. Historically, with exceptions, FTs have been reserved for individuals with extreme facial differences and after reconstruction has failed. Most commonly, facial damage results from ballistic injuries, animal attacks, burns, and diseases such as neurofibromatosis. Current indications for FT are more complex than simply cosmesis, and these complexities must form part of organ donor education and risk evaluation for patient and donor families21–24.

This cross-sectional survey has measured views in a snapshot rather than over time. Response rates to some questions were low; for example, one-third responded when asked what information would benefit them in the future. Analyses based on low response rates (50 or fewer responses) should be interpreted with caution. The results may be influenced by order-effects bias. Responses are culturally and socially situated in GB. A comparative international survey is needed for generalization.

Defining and measuring emotional outcomes are paramount in the innovation of FT. This will require patient-centred qualitative analysis in combination with clinical data and international collaboration that acknowledges diverse social and cultural contexts. Awareness of social prejudice also needs to be acknowledged, through involvement of patient groups25,26.

Funding

This research was funded by UKRI, via the Future Leaders Fellowship project AboutFace: The Affective and Cultural History of Face Transplants (MR/S017356/1). The funder had no role in the study design, data collection, analysis or interpretation, in the writing of the report or in the decision to submit it for publication.

Supplementary Material

znab120_Supplementary_Data

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge the guidance of the AboutFace Advisory Board and AboutFace Lived Experience Advisory Panel in the design of the survey; and N. Rumsey, who provided feedback on an early version of this paper. They thank members of the Newcastle Facial Research Group for their contribution to research design, including: P. Hodgkinson, M. Ragbir, O. Ahmed, A. Bashir, S. Brown, M. Kennedy, J. Adams, D. Manas, D. Sainsbury, and S. Butterworth. Data can be made available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

YouGov statement: All figures, unless otherwise stated, are from YouGov Plc.Total sample size was 2112 adults. Fieldwork was undertaken between 24 and 25 February 2020. The survey was carried out online. The figures have been weighted and are representative of all GB adults (aged 18 years and over).

D.S. reports grants and non-financial support from Stryker UK Honorarium, personal fees from MTech consultancy, and personal fees from Sanofi Oncology. None of these are relevant to the present study.

Disclosure. The authors declare no other conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at BJS online.

Contributor Information

D C Murphy, Institute of Genetic Medicine, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK; Northumbria NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.

V Hoyle, Department of History, University of York, York, UK.

D Saleh, Department of Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.

J Rees, School of Psychology, University of Sunderland, Sunderland, UK.

F Bound Alberti, Department of History, University of York, York, UK.

Presented to a Head, neck and face virtual meeting organised by the British Association of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery on 10th December 2020

References

  • 1. Dubernard JM, Lengelé B, Morelon E, Testelin S, Badet L, Moure C.  Face transplants: an international history. N Engl J Med  2007;357:2451–2460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Aycart MA, Kiwanuka H, Krezdorn N, Alhefzi M, Bueno EM, Pomahac B et al. Quality of life after face transplantation: outcomes, assessment tools, and future directions. Plast Reconstr Surg  2017;139:194–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Rifkin WJ, Kantar RS, Ali-Khan S, Plana M, Diaz-Siso JR, Tsakiris M et al. Facial disfigurement and identity: a review of the literature and implications for facial transplantation. AMA J Ethics  2018;20:309–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Dicks SG, Northam H, van Haren FMP, Boer DP.  An exploration of the relationship between families of deceased organ donors and transplant recipients: a systematic review and qualitative synthesis. Heal Psychol Open  2018;5:2055102918782172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Westphal J.  The transplant patient: biological, psychiatric, and ethical issues in organ transplantation. Ann Intern Med  2001;134:628. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Baylis F.  A face is not just like a hand: Pace barker. Am J Bioeth  2004;4:30–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Tasigiorgos S, Kollar B, Krezdorn N, Bueno EM, Tullius SG, Pomahac B.  Face transplantation—current status and future developments. Transpl Int  2018;31:677–688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kimberly LL, Alfonso AR, Kantar RS, Ramly EP, Caplan AL, Rodriguez ED.  How to integrate lived experience into quality-of-life assessment in patients considering facial transplantation. AMA J Ethics  2019;21:980–987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Brill SE, Clarke A, Veale DM, Butler PEM.  Psychological management and body image issues in facial transplantation. Body Image  2006;3:1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.YouGov. What the World Thinks. https://yougov.co.uk/ (accessed 5 August 2020).
  • 11.CIA World Factbook. United Kingdom Demographics Profile  2019. https://www.indexmundi.com/united_kingdom/demographics_profile.html (accessed 5 August 2020).
  • 12. Shanmugarajah K, Hettiaratchy S, Clarke A, Butler PEM.  Clinical outcomes of facial transplantation: a review. Int J Surg  2011;9:600–607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Butler PEM, Clarke A, Hettiaratchy S.  Facial transplantation. BMJ  2005;331:1349–1350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Coghlan A.  Face transplant recipients don’t look like donors. New Scientist  2008;199:13. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Morris PJ, Bradley JA, Doyal L, Earley M, Hagan P, Milling M et al. Facial transplantation: a working party report from the Royal College of Surgeons of England. Transplantation  2004;77:330–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Freeman M, Jaoudé PA, Justifying surgery’s last taboo: the ethics of face transplants. J Med Ethics  2007;33:76–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Brett KE, Ritchie LJ, Ertel E, Bennett A, Knoll GA.  Quality metrics in solid organ transplantation. Transplantation  2018;102:e308–e330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Diver AJ.  Investigation of risk acceptance in facial transplantation. Plast Reconstr Surg  2007;119:2317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Theodorakopoulou E, Meghji S, Pafitanis G, Mason KA.  A review of the world’s published face transplant cases: ethical perspectives. Scars Burn Heal  2017;3:205951311769440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Bound Alberti F.  Face transplants as surgical acts and psychosocial processes. Lancet  2020;395:1106–1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Rodrigue JR, Tomich D, Fleishman A, Glazier AK. Vascularized composite Allograft donation and transplantation: a survey of public attitudes in the United States. Am J Transplant  2017;17:2687–2695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Denadai R, Araujo KC, Pinho AS, Denadai R, Araujo GH, Raposo-Amaral CE.  Public’s knowledge and attitudes toward vascularized composite allotransplantation and donation. Aesthetic Plast Surg  2016;40:301–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Plana NM, Kimberly LL, Parent B.  The public face of transplantation: the potential of education to expand the face donor pool. Plast Reconstr Surg  2018;141:176–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Sarwer DB, Ritter S, Reiser K, Spitzer JC, Baumann BM, Patel SN et al. Attitudes toward vascularized composite allotransplantation of the hands and face in an urban population. Vascularized Composite Allotransplantation  2014;1:22–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Face Equality International. International Media Standard on Disfigurement. https://faceequalityinternational.org/thecampaign/mediastandard/ (accessed 5 August 2020).
  • 26. Hartman RG.  Face value: challenges of transplant technology. Am J Law Med  2005;31:7–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

znab120_Supplementary_Data

Articles from The British Journal of Surgery are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES