Abstract
Social adaptation arises from the interaction between the individual and the social environment. However, little empirical evidence exists regarding the relationship between social contact and social adaptation. We propose that loneliness and social networks are key factors explaining social adaptation. Sixty-four healthy subjects with no history of psychiatric conditions participated in this study. All participants completed self-report questionnaires about loneliness, social network, and social adaptation. On a separate day, subjects underwent a resting state fMRI recording session. A hierarchical regression model on self-report data revealed that loneliness and social network were negatively and positively associated with social adaptation. Functional connectivity (FC) analysis showed that loneliness was associated with decreased FC between the fronto-amygdalar and fronto-parietal regions. In contrast, the social network was positively associated with FC between the fronto-temporo-parietal network. Finally, an integrative path model examined the combined effects of behavioral and brain predictors of social adaptation. The model revealed that social networks mediated the effects of loneliness on social adaptation. Further, loneliness-related abnormal brain FC (previously shown to be associated with difficulties in cognitive control, emotion regulation, and sociocognitive processes) emerged as the strongest predictor of poor social adaptation. Findings offer insights into the brain indicators of social adaptation and highlight the role of social networks as a buffer against the maladaptive effects of loneliness. These findings can inform interventions aimed at minimizing loneliness and promoting social adaptation and are especially relevant due to the high prevalence of loneliness around the globe. These findings also serve the study of social adaptation since they provide potential neurocognitive factors that could influence social adaptation.
Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Social behaviour, Social neuroscience, Neuroscience, Amygdala, Prefrontal cortex
Introduction
Social adaptation refers to the extent to which individuals are motivated to participate in their various societal roles and interact with others under societal norms and expectations1,2. One crucial factor negatively affecting social adaptation is the experience of loneliness, defined as a distressing emotional state resulting from the subject's perception of separateness, regardless of the actual amount of social contact3,4. The maladaptive effect of loneliness is associated with an impoverished social network, poor well-being, disruptive social behavior5, and abnormal function in neural networks known to be involved in cognitive control, self-regulation, and social cognition6,7. Notably, impoverished social networks and associated limitations can explain the relationship between loneliness and decreased social adaptation. Conversely, social adaptation can significantly benefit from support resources from the social network8,9. Despite the above, little is known about the effect of loneliness and social networks on social adaptation, and even less is known about the neural aspects underlying such relationships. To approach this issue, we analyzed the relationship between multi-domain measures (i.e. self-report, resting-state functional connectivity) of loneliness and social network on social adaptation. A better understanding of social adaptation may constitute valuable input for public policymaking and interventions promoting development.
Social adaptation is intricately related to social engagement10,11. Interaction with other members of society and social institutions (i.e. socialization) allows individuals to gain knowledge about the roles, values, rights, and obligations within their society as well as facilitate coping with contextual demands12–15. These outcomes of social interaction foster adaptive behaviors within the social environment13,16–18. However, despite the recognized importance of social interaction for social adaptation, there is currently a lack of empirical evidence examining the relationship between these constructs. Consequently, the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying this relationship remain unknown.
One factor that can negatively affect social adaptation is the experience of loneliness. The emotional distress because of loneliness is linked to maladaptive outcomes, including poor physical and mental health19–21 and diminished well-being5,22,23 and limited ability to fulfill societal roles24. Loneliness can also affect social adaptation through increased social isolation. Reduced social interaction and participation reflect poor social integration (low social engagement), a core aspect of social adaptation25. On the one hand, decreased contact with others limits the beneficial effects of social interactions, such as obtaining social support23,26–30. Support resources are necessary to adjust contextual demands successfully15,31,32. Thus, limited access to such resources may result in socio-adaptive difficulties. This situation may perpetuate lonely individuals' tendency to experience reduced reward from social stimuli33, perpetuating loneliness and social isolation.
Additionally, Loneliness is associated with abnormal functional connectivity (FC) of attention (Dorsal and ventral networks)34,35 and cognitive control networks (Cingulo-opercular network and right middle/superior frontal gyrus)34,36. These alterations are known to underpin the maladaptive characteristics of loneliness such as reduced social affiliation (and increased aversion), negatively biased cognition, emotion dysregulation (high social pain), and impaired social cognition23,30,37–41. The above findings imply that the association between loneliness and social adaptation may be explained by altered patterns of brain connectivity and decreased social interactions.
On the other hand, one crucial factor that may facilitate social adaptation is the social network8,42 . The social network provides support through emotional, instrumental, and informational resources that facilitate social adjustment and coping ability while reducing stress8,43. People with better and more complex social networks report fewer daily hassles and lower stress9,44–46. At the neurocognitive level, these stress-buffering effects are modulated by prefrontal activation and fronto-limbic functional connectivity associated with adequate cognitive function, self-regulation, and social cognition32,47,48. Based on the evidence above, having a rich social network and its associated neurocognitive and stress-buffering effects would be expected to be essential for successful social adaptation. Conversely, it is reasonable to think that the socio-adaptive effects of the social network would be debilitated in subjects experiencing loneliness. However, previous research has not directly assessed the link between these factors.
Considering this background, this work aims to test the association between measures of loneliness, social network, and their FC correlates with social adaptation. We also evaluated the mediating role of the social network in the relationship between loneliness and social adaptation. Based on previous research on the adverse effects of loneliness on well-being and social functioning5,23 , we expect that loneliness is negatively associated with social adaptation. On the other hand, considering previous accounts of the stress-buffering hypothesis9,32,49, we expect that social networks facilitate social adaptation and explain the potential mechanism underlying the association between loneliness and social adaptation. Regarding the resting-state FC predictors of social adaptation, we expect loneliness-related abnormal function in attentional and cognitive control networks to be negatively associated with social adaptation. We also expect that FC correlates of social networks are positively associated with social adaptation. This study is the first to examine the behavioral and neural factors associated with social adaptation. This study also provides empirical evidence that can aid public policy and serve as a basis for interventions promoting social development.
Materials and methods
Participants
Based on an a priori power analysis, our integrative path model (see below), which includes 6 predictors, required data from a sample size of 59 subjects to detect a moderate effect size (F2 = 0.15) with a power of 0.90 and α = 0.05. To ensure adequate power, we recruited a sample of 64 subjects between 20 and 73 years old (M = 36.88; SD = 13.62; female (n = 38). They have an average of 11 years of education (M = 11.1, SD = 3.09, range 2–20 years of education) and reported no history of psychiatric or neurological conditions. Participants were recruited by accessibility from the general population.
Individual differences in executive function were controlled using the INECO frontal screening50. This instrument is sensitive for assessing executive dysfunction (see Supplementary instruments). The mean score for our sample was 20.92 points, just above the 18-point cut-off for the Chilean population51.
The Universidad Diego Portales ethics committee approved every procedure of this research. All participants signed an informed consent according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and received a payment as retribution for their collaboration.
Procedure
Participants were contacted via telephone or social network media and invited to attend the laboratory to complete various scales tapping on social adaptation, loneliness, and social network. fMRI scanning session was carried out on a different day using a Siemens Avanto 1.5 T scanner equipped with a standard head coil.
Self-report assessment
Social adaptation self-administered Scale (SASS)
The SASS52 is a 21-item scale that explores social adjustment and motivation. The SASS was initially developed to evaluate social functioning in clinical populations52. However, it has been increasingly used to assess social adaptation in non-clinical populations11,53–55. The items tap into levels of engagement with the environment (e.g., "Do you feel able to organize your environment according to your wishes and needs?"), family relationships (e.g., "How frequently do you seek contact with your family members?"), friendships (e.g., "What value do you attach to the relationship with others?"), and engagement to work ("How interested are you in your occupation?"), among others. Responses are recorded via a 4-point Likert scale (from 0 to 3). The total score ranges from 0 to 60, corresponding to minimal and maximal social adjustment. Scores between 35 and 52 are normal52. The instrument was reliable in our sample (α = 0.73).
University of California Loneliness Scale (UCLA)
The UCLA4 is a widely used measure of the subject's feelings of loneliness and levels of satisfaction with social experiences. For the present study, an abbreviated version of the UCLA was used56, comprising the following eight items: (1) "I can find companionship when I want it"; (2) "I feel left out"; (3) "I feel isolated from others"; (4) "I lack companionship"; (5) "There is no one I can turn to"; (6) "I am unhappy with being so withdrawn"; (7) "People are around me but not with me"; (8) "I am an outgoing person". Responses were recorded via a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (never) to 3 (always). The total score is obtained by inverting positive items and summarizing the score of all items. Thus, more significant scores indicate a more pronounced experience of loneliness. UCLA has shown good levels of reliability, as evidenced by an α coefficient of 0.894. We used a shorter 8-item version that showed good reliability levels in our sample (α = 0.85).
The revised Lubben Social Network Scale (LSNS-R)
The LSNS-R is a 12-item scale that measures the size and complexity of social relationships57 . It consists of two scales, one tapping on kinship/family ties (e.g., "How many relatives do you see or hear from at least once a month?"), and other evaluating non-kin / friendship ties (e.g., "How many friends do you feel close to such that you could call on them for help?"). Items are rated on a scale from 0 to 5, with 0 indicating the lowest frequency and number of contact with others and 5 indicating the highest frequency/number of contacts with others. The total score is obtained by summarizing all items' scores. The maximum total score is 60, with higher scores reflecting bigger and stronger social ties. The scale shows high reliability in old adults (α = 0.90)58 and young populations (α = 0.83)59. A similar level of reliability was obtained in our sample with a Cronbach coefficient of α = 0.85.
Images data collection
Images for this study were obtained from a Siemens Avanto 1.5 T scanner equipped with a standard head coil. We obtained 10-min resting-state fMRI recordings from 61 participants (data from 3 participants was excluded during pre-processing because of the low quality of their recordings). Functional spin-echo volumes were acquired sequentially ascendingly, parallel to the anterior–posterior commissures, covering the whole brain. The following parameters were used: TR = 3.3 s; TE = 50 ms; flip angle = 90°; number of slices = 36, matrix dimension 4 × 64; voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3.75 mm3; number of volumes = 190. Participants were instructed to lay still, keep their eyes closed, and not think about anything particular.
Data analyses
Self-report data
Descriptive data analysis for social adaptation, loneliness, social network, and executive functions are displayed in Table 1. We also conducted correlation analyses, including sociodemographic data and variables of interest (see Table 1).
Table 1.
Correlation matrix including sociodemographic data and variables of interest.
Variable | M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Age | 36.88 | 13.62 | |||||
2. Years of education | 11.07 | 3.09 |
− 0.50** [− 0.67, − 0.27] |
||||
3. SASS | 42.27 | 7.12 |
− 0.01 [− 0.26, 0.25] |
0.07 [− 0.19, 0.32] |
|||
4. UCLA | 8.65 | 5.27 |
0.08 [− 0.18, 0.33] |
0.04 [− 0.22, 0.29] |
− 0.56** [− 0.71, − 0.36] |
||
5. LSNS_R | 34.52 | 10.47 |
− 0.21 [− 0.47, 0.08] |
− 0.04 [− 0.32, 0.25] |
0.48** [0.24, 0.67] |
− 0.43** [− 0.63, − 0.17] |
|
6. IFS Total score | 20.92 | 3.80 |
− 0.30* [− 0.52, − 0.04] |
0.45** [0.21, 0.63] |
0.03 [− 0.22, 0.28] |
0.02 [− 0.23, 0.27] |
0.23 [− 0.05, 0.48] |
Correlation analysis for our sample of 64 subjects (37 female, 27 male). M and SD are used to represent mean and standard deviation, respectively. Values in square brackets indicate the 95% confidence interval for each correlation. The confidence interval is a plausible range of population correlations that could have caused the sample correlation (Cumming, 2014). Note that * indicates p < 0.05. ** indicates p < 0.01. Abbreviations: SASS (social adaptation self-administered scale), UCLA (University of California loneliness scale), LSNS_R (Lubben social network scale).
Behavioral data were analyzed with R studio60. We first conducted a hierarchical multiple regression to evaluate the predictive value of loneliness and social network on social adaptation. Hierarchical multiple regression models help evaluate and compare the predictability of groups of independent variables entered at different steps of the analysis61. In other words, the main idea of the analysis is to test whether variables entered in subsequent steps have better predictive value than those entered in a former step of the analysis. As for the present analysis, we first specified a base model including our control variables (executive functions, age, education, and gender). These variables did not have any significant effect on social adaptation. In a subsequent step, our measure of loneliness was incorporated into the group of variables. For the last step, we specified a model that included the social network measure (LSNS scores).
Resting-state fMRI data
Pre-processing
First, we discarded the first three volumes of each subject's resting-state recording to ensure that magnetization achieved a steady state. Images were then pre-processed using the Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF V2.3)62. This open-access toolbox generates an automatic pipeline for fMRI analysis. The DPARFS processes the data recruiting the Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12) and the Resting-State fMRI Data Analysis Toolkit (REST V.1.7). In line with recommendations63, pre-processing included slice-timing correction (using the middle slice of each volume as the reference scan) and realignment to the first scan of the session to correct head movement (SPM functions). To reduce the effect of motion during image acquisition as well as physiological artifacts64, we controlled two motion parameters (i.e. Translation, rotation; See Supplementary table 1), CFS, and WM signals (REST V1.7 toolbox). Motion parameters were estimated during realignment, and data from three subjects were discarded due to exceeding the maximum head movement (3 mm and 3°). After discarding the three subjects, the subsample left showed acceptable motion parameters in movements (M = 0.05, SD = 0.04) and rotations (M = 0.05, SD = 0.03). CFS and WM masks were derived from the tissue segmentation of each subject's T1 scan in native space with SPM12 (after co-registration of each subject's structural image with the functional image). Then, images were normalized to the MNI space using the echo-planar imaging (EPI) template from SPM65, smoothed using an 8-mm full-width-at-half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel (SPM functions), and bandpass filtered between 0.01 and 0.08 Hz. None of the participants showed movements greater than 3 mm (M = 0.05, SD = 0.04) or rotations higher than 3° (M = 0.05, SD = 0.03).
Functional connectivity analyses
We explored associations between resting-state functional connectivity data and scores from our predictor variables, loneliness (UCLA scores) and social network (LSNS-R score). First, for each subject, we extracted the mean time course of the BOLD signal in each of the 116 regions of the Automated Anatomical Labelling Atlas (AAL)66 by averaging the signal in all voxels comprising each region. Second, we constructed a connectivity matrix for each subject, indicating the strength of association between all pairs of regions (Pearson's correlation coefficient; DPARSF toolbox). Third, we performed a Fisher z-transformation. The resulting functional connectivity (FC) correlation coefficients between all pairs of regions (AAL atlas) were used to perform Spearman's correlations with the scores of each predictor: loneliness (UCLA score) and social network (LSNS-R score). Following procedures from recent research67–69, the significance thresholding of neuroimaging results was set to p ≤ 0.001 (uncorrected). Less stringent, uncorrected statistical thresholds, as supported by previous research70–72, can be valuable in reducing false negative results, particularly in modest sample sizes like ours. This approach allows for the consideration of genuine effects that might have otherwise been disregarded with stricter thresholds.
Principal component analyses (PCA) of fMRI data
We conducted PCA for each predictor separately to reduce the dimensionality of the FC correlates of the two predictors (loneliness and social network). In the case of loneliness, we analyzed the rho values of the pairs of regions that significantly correlated with UCLA scores (see Supplementary table 2). For social networks, we also analyzed the rho values of the pairs of regions that significantly correlated with LSNS.R scores (see Supplementary table 3). Then, the component that captured the most variance associated with each construct was retained and included in the posterior integrative path analysis.
Integrative path analysis
We performed a path analysis using the Laavan package73 in JASP statistical software to evaluate the combined effect of self-report and brain indicators of social adaptation. This technique tests a theoretical model comprising (a priori) hypothesized relationships between variables. Based on this approach, we constructed a model that integrates self-report and neurophysiological (FC) data as predictors of social adaptation. Specifically, we hypothesize that at the behavioral (self-report) level, loneliness scores are associated with lower performance on the social adaptation scale (SASS). Social network scores will mediate this relationship. These patterns of associations will be replicated at the neurophysiological level, meaning that FC correlated to loneliness will predict low performance in the social adaptation scale. In contrast, FC correlated to social networks will mediate in such relationships. The fit is evaluated by various parameters, including the X2 statistic (non-significant), NFI (> 0.95), GFI (> 0.95), CFI (0.95–1.00), RMSEA (< 0.08), IC (≤ 0.05), SRMR (< 0.08).
Path models can be used to extend the multiple regression model by simultaneously analyzing the relationships between the independent and dependent variables74 . Based on this approach, we proposed a model incorporating brain variables with purely behavioral ones to generate an integrated model.
Ethics approval
The Universidad Diego Portales ethics committee approved every procedure of this research. All participants signed an informed consent according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and received a payment as retribution for their collaboration and time.
Results
Correlations for our analyzed variables are displayed in Table 1 below.
Self-report measures
Multiple hierarchical regression: loneliness and social network as predictors of social adaptation
We performed a hierarchical multiple regression to evaluate the predictive value of loneliness and social network on social adaptation (see Table 2). Before the hypothesized model, we conducted the first analysis step by considering sociodemographics and EF as control variables. Results showed that gender (β = 0.13, p = 0.41), age (β = 0.04, p = 0.81) years of education (β = 0.09, p = 0.67) and executive function (β = − 0.04, p = 0.82) did not have significant effects over social adaptation. In the second step, after controlling the intervening variables, only loneliness (β = − 0.58, p < 0.001) emerged as a significant predictor of Social adaptation[F(1, 38) = 17.82, p < 0.001]. In a third step, the social network was added to the existing predictors; this model revealed that both loneliness (β = − 0.41, p < 0.005) and social network (β = 0.43, p < 0.005) significantly predicted social adaptation. Interestingly, this model shows that the effect of loneliness is still significant after including the social network. After comparing the R2adj. values for the last two steps, the third step explains 13% more variance associated with social adaptation than the second. Finally, regarding the effects of potentially confounding variables, we confirmed that the associations amongst the variables of interest remained significant after controlling for gender, age, years of education, and EFs.
Table 2.
Multiple hierarchical regression for loneliness and social network as predictors of social adaptation.
Demographic | Loneliness | Social network | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Std. Beta | Standardized CI | p | Std. Beta | Standardized CI | p | Std. Beta | Standardized CI | p | |
(Intercept) | 0.00 | − 0.32–0.32 | < 0.001* | 0.00 | − 0.26–0.26 | < 0.001* | 0.00 | − 0.24–0.24 | 0.002* |
Gender | 0.13 | − 0.19–0.46 | 0.411 | − 0.02 | − 0.30–0.26 | 0.884 | − 0.04 | − 0.29–0.22 | 0.783 |
Age | 0.04 | − 0.33–0.42 | 0.818 | 0.16 | − 0.16–0.47 | 0.325 | 0.25 | − 0.05–0.54 | 0.099 |
Years of education | 0.09 | − 0.33–0.51 | 0.673 | 0.12 | − 0.23–0.47 | 0.500 | 0.26 | − 0.07–0.59 | 0.123 |
Executive functions | − 0.04 | − 0.42–0.33 | 0.828 | 0.04 | − 0.28–0.35 | 0.803 | − 0.08 | − 0.38–0.22 | 0.596 |
UCLA | − 0.59 | − 0.87 to − 0.31 | < 0.001* | − 0.41 | − 0.70 to − 0.13 | < 0.05* | |||
LSNS | 0.43 | 0.14–0.71 | < 0.05* | ||||||
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.025/− 0.075 | 0.336/0.249 | 0.466/0.380 |
Note The table shows standardized coefficients and p-values for each step comprising the hierarchical multiple regression model. The demographic column shows parameter estimates for our control variables (i.e. sociodemographic data and executive functions). The middle column shows the model evaluating the effects of loneliness on social adaptation after controlling for non-interest variables. After controlling for demographic variables, the final column shows the model evaluating the effects of loneliness and social network on social adaptation. Asterisks indicate significant effects. For more details, refer to Supplementary table 4.
UCLA University of California Loneliness Scale, LSNS Lubben Social Network scale.
Significant values are in bold.
Functional connectivity results
Significant functional connectivity for loneliness and social network is represented in Fig. 1A and B, respectively (for the list of specific areas involved, please see Supplementary tables 2 and 3). Loneliness was negatively associated with the functional connectivity between the amygdala and frontal regions (i.e. orbitofrontal gyrus, superior and inferior frontal gyrus) and the supramarginal gyrus in the parietal cortex. Fronto-insular connectivity was also negatively correlated to loneliness (see Fig. 1A and Supplementary table 2). The social network was associated with increased functional connectivity between the frontotemporal and temporoparietal areas and connectivity between the insula, frontal areas, and basal ganglia (Fig. 1B and Supplementary table 3).
Figure 1.
(A) Resting-state functional connectivity negatively associated with loneliness (UCLA score), (B) Resting-state functional connectivity positively associated with Social networks (LSNS-R score). The figure above shows pairs of areas with a threshold of p ≤ 0.001. *Lines connecting the nodes illustrate the valence of the association (between self-reported scores and pairs of areas), with blue lines reflecting decreased connectivity and red lines reflecting increased connectivity. Also, note that the color of the nodes (circles indicating the areas) have no other meaning than to facilitate the differentiation of each region. For more details on the areas depicted in this figure, see Supplementary tables 2 and 3
Principal component analyses (PCA) on Functional connectivity (FC) data
To obtain a summarized index of the functional connectivity correlates of each predictor, we conducted PCA and retained the component that captured the most variance. For loneliness, the first component explained 47% of the variance of its functional connectivity. For social networks, the first component explained 70% of the variance associated with its functional connectivity. These components for both loneliness (PCLonely) and social network (PCSocNet) were included in the subsequent Integrative Path analysis (see Fig. 2).
Figure 2.
Schematic representation of the integrative path analysis exploring the association between self-reported and FC data with social adaptation. The relationships between the variables are expressed in standardized estimates. Continuous lines represent significant results, while discontinuous lines reflect non-significant effects. The curved lines represent the correlation between two variables (i.e. self-report scores with their FC correlates). Abbreviations: PCLonely: Principal component reflecting functional connectivity negatively associated with loneliness; Lonely: scores in the self-report measure of loneliness (UCLA); PCSocNet: Principal component reflecting functional connectivity positively associated with Social networks; SocNet: scores in the self-report measure of social network (LSNS); SocAdap: scores in the self-report measure of social adaptation (SASS). For more details, see Supplementary Table 5.
Integrative path analysis
A graphic account of the model and results can be seen in Fig. 2. Loneliness negatively relates to social networks (β = − 0.430, p < 0.001) and does not have effects on social adaptation (β = − 0.226, p = 0.098). Social networks, in turn, have a positive relationship with social adaptation (β = 0.284, p < 0.05). The PCA of brain connectivity of loneliness (PCLonely) had positive effects on social adaptation (β = 0.428, p < 0.05).
Additionally, PCLonely showed significant positive effects on the PCA of social network (PCSocNet) (β = 0.705, p < 0.001), which in turn had non-significant effects on social adaptation (β = − 0.038, p = 0.814).
Regarding the model's fit, the χ2 test statistic was not significant, suggesting that the model had a good fit (χ2 [4, N = 47] = 2.16, p = 0.71). Overall, the fit indices were very good; NFI = 0.99 (values above 0.95 is good); GFI = 0.99 (values above 0.95 is good); CFI = 1.00 (values in the range of 0.95–0.99 considered as excellent fit, and a value of 1 considered as exact fit); RMSEA = 0.00, IC = 0.00–0.16 (values ≤ 0.05 considered as good fit); SRMR = 0.04 (a value less than 0.08 is generally considered a good fit).
Discussion
This study is the first to assess the relationship between loneliness, social network, and their FC correlates with social adaptation. We hypothesized that loneliness and its functional brain connectivity (FC) correlates were negatively associated with social adaptation, while social network and brain correlates would explain such maladaptive effects. Our hierarchical multiple regression indicates that loneliness was associated with low scores on the social adaptation scale. The model also revealed that the effect of loneliness on social adaptation decreased after including the social network measure, which positively affected social adaptation. The model containing both factors as predictors explained the most variance associated with social adaptation.
Loneliness revealed a decreased functional connectivity of the amygdala and fronto-parietal areas. These regions have previously been shown to underpin social pain, emotional disorders, cognitive control, and mentalizing impairment. On the other hand, the social network was positively associated with intrinsic FC between previously reported hubs for global cognitive functioning and self-regulation. However, this FC did not have predictive value over social adaptation. The final integrative path analysis model showed that at the behavioral level (self-reported data), social networks mediated the effects of loneliness over social adaptation. At a neurophysiological level, social adaptation was predicted solely by the resting-state FC correlates of loneliness. These FC correlates of loneliness explained most of the variance associated with social adaptation when considering behavioral and brain factors. These findings offer new insight into behavioral, affective, socio-cognitive, and, more importantly, neurophysiological predictors of social adaptation. Our findings may also serve as an empirical basis for interventions to improve well-being and development.
Previous research has highlighted the maladaptive impact of loneliness on health, cognitive functioning, and social behavior5,23,41 and the stress-buffering role of the social network8,43. In line with this evidence, our hierarchical regression results suggest that feeling lonely or left out might hinder social adaptation while having a rich social network might facilitate social adaptation. Importantly, two particularities of this model can lead to interesting interpretations. First, the relationship between SA and loneliness or social network remained significant after controlling for each other, indicating that these constructs are differentially associated with social adaptation. Based on the literature cited above, we suggest that the "loneliness-adaptation" link may reflect increased social pain (reduced well-being, and mental health). In contrast, the "social network-adaptation" link reflects enhanced coping capabilities (i.e. mainly due to support resources). The second observation is that introducing the social network measure captures some loneliness-SA variance, suggesting that social networks might explain a mechanism underlying such a relationship.
Decreased contact with others may limit support resources necessary to cope with stress and life demands8,32,49, hindering social adaptation. This antecedent is consistent with our finding that social networks mediated the association between loneliness and social adaptation (see Fig. 2). The evidence suggests that loneliness affects social adaptation through isolation from the social group and decreased instances of social support. In this sense, contact with the social network (and obtained social support) may promote social adaption by minimizing emotional distress and fomenting self-regulation and coping8,43. These findings highlight previously unaddressed socio-affective and behavioral factors associated with social adaptation.
At the Neurophysiological level, our integrative path model yielded exciting results. Specifically, we found that loneliness-related decreased activity in front-amygdalar and fronto-parietal networks was associated with lower social adaptation scores. Past research shows that the strength of front-amygdalar coupling modulates emotion regulation processes75, affective aspects of social perception, and affiliative behaviors76. The inability of frontal cortical regions to modulate amygdala reactivity results in emotional dysfunctions, anxious and depressive symptomatology, and isolation exhibited by lonely individuals41. In line with the above, our results suggest that decreased front-amygdalar connectivity might indicate poor social adaptation through emotion regulation impairments and poor mental health.
On the other hand, fronto-parietal areas are part of a widely distributed cognitive control network77. Cognitive control enables the appropriate selection of thoughts, emotions, and behaviors based on contextual demands78. Another aspect associated with fronto-parietal function is the modulation of socio-cognitive abilities (i.e. mentalizing, empathy)79,80. Our results suggest that dampened front-amygdalar and fronto-parietal activity in loneliness may hinder social adaptation. This process could be through emotion regulation impairments, inability to cope with daily life stressors, and affected social cognition (i.e. Negative appraisal of social stimuli and poor empathic abilities).
These maladaptive neurocognitive patterns might be explained via lowered oxytocin neuropeptide levels. It is well known that loneliness is associated with attenuated normal oxytocinergic responsiveness, with subjects with higher loneliness having lower oxytocin levels in response to social stimuli81,82. This oxytocinergic underproduction could be potentially linked to neurocognitive abnormalities leading to poor social networks and, consequently, maladaptation. For example, research shows that oxytocin neuropeptide modulates emotion regulation by increasing amygdala-prefrontal cortex connectivity83. Oxytocin also enhances trust and social approaching by modulating insula reactivity to positive social stimuli84.
Regarding sociocognitive impairments, evidence shows that oxytocin weakens negativity bias within regions involved in the theory of mind (temporoparietal junction) and identification of emotional cues in social perception (right fusiform)85. The above might offer insights into the potential neuroendocrine mechanisms by which loneliness reduces social networks and affect social adaptation. However, this interpretation is merely speculative, and future studies should directly test the role of oxytocin in the relationship between loneliness, social networks, and social adaptation.
Concerning potential limitations, it is crucial to emphasize that the findings presented in this study are derived from a correlational design. Although there is strong supporting evidence for the proposed relationships, caution should be exercised when inferring causal relationships between variables. On the other hand, we acknowledge that self-report variables may not entirely reflect the construct under investigation. However, according to the theory, such variables showed a pattern of expected relationships.
Word of caution
Given that this is the first study on this particular topic, we have taken a less stringent approach to explore the association of functional connectivity with self-reported data, as done in previous studies67–69,86. However, it is important to note that the significance threshold used in our analysis (p < 0.001, uncorrected) may be considered liberal, which warrants caution when interpreting our findings. To ensure the reliability of the results, future research should aim at replicating our findings using more stringent significance criteria.
Conclusion
Little has been done to evaluate the aspects that predict and facilitate social adaptation from a socio-affective and neural perspective. This work aimed to predict the effects of socio-affective, cognitive, and brain predictors of social adaptation. Our findings suggest that the negative impact of loneliness on social adaptation involves impairments in social functioning, affective disorders, and decreased functional connectivity in networks associated with cognitive control, self-regulation, and social cognition. However, support from the social network is a critical factor that protects against these maladaptive behavioral and brain patterns and foments adequate social adaptation. Considering this, future research should aim at two primary goals. One is to incorporate measures of the above processes as indicators of social adaptation. The second is to explore further the various contributions of social integration to the social adaptation processes. The findings here reported contributing to the understanding of mechanisms involved in social adaptation. A better understanding of these mechanisms can inform public policymaking and promote social development interventions.
Supplementary Information
Author contributions
Contributors D.F., J.P.M.S., D.H., R.G.G., S.F., and A.I. conceived and designed the project, contributed analysis tools, performed the analysis, and participated in drafting the final manuscript. D.F., D.H.A., R.M., and J.A. collected the data. D.F., D.H., J.P.M.S., R.G.G., S.F., and A.I. conducted data analysis and interpretation and participated in drafting the final manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.
Funding
David Huepe is supported by ANID/FONDECYT Regular (1231117, 1201486, 1171200). Agustin Ibañez is supported by CONICET; FONCYT-PICT (2017-1818, 2017-1820); ANID/FONDECYT Regular (1210195, 1210176, 1220995) ANID/FONDAP (15150012); ANID/PIA/ANILLOS ACT210096; and the Multi-Partner Consortium to Expand Dementia Research in Latin America (ReDLat), funded by the National Institutes of Aging of the National Institutes of Health under award number R01AG057234, and Alzheimer's Association grant (SG-20-725707-ReDLat), the Rainwater Foundation, and the Global Brain Health Institute. The content is solely the authors' responsibility and does not represent the official views of these institutions.
Data availability
The data supporting this study's findings are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-023-38244-0.
References
- 1.Herrera López M, Romera Félix EM, Ortega Ruiz R, Ortiz OG. Influence of social motivation, self-perception of social efficacy and normative adjustment in the peer setting. Psicothema. 2016 doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Samadi M, Sohrabi N. mediating role of the social problem solving for family process, family content, and adjustment. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2016.02.141. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jeste DV, Lee EE, Cacioppo S. Battling the modern behavioral epidemic of loneliness: suggestions for research and interventions. JAMA Psychiat. 2020;77:553–554. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Russell D, Peplau LA, Cutrona CE. The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1980 doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S. Social relationships and health: The toxic effects of perceived social isolation. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass. 2014 doi: 10.1111/spc3.12087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lam JA, et al. Neurobiology of loneliness: A systematic review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2021;4611(46):1873–1887. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01058-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Tomova LKT, Saxe R. The neuroscience of unmet social needs. Soc. Neurosci. 2021;16:221–231. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2019.1694580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Berkman LF, Glass T. Social integration, social networks, social support, and health. Soc Epidemiol. 2000;1:137–173. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Londono Y, Mcmillan DE, Rn M, Candidate P. Psychosocial adaptation: an evolutionary concept analysis exploring a common multidisciplinary language. J. Adv. Nurs. 2015;71:2504–2519. doi: 10.1111/jan.12723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Franco-O’Byrne D, et al. The impact of loneliness and social adaptation on depressive symptoms: Behavioral and brain measures evidence from a brain health perspective. Front. Psychol. 2023;14:1096178. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1096178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Becker, H. S. The self and adult socialization. In Sociological work 289–304 (Routledge, 2017).
- 13.Terziev, V. The dependence social adaptation-socialization. Acad. Russ. Acad. Nat. Hist.13, (2019).
- 14.Crisogen DT. Types of socialization and their importance in understanding the phenomena of socialization. Eur. J. Soc. Sci. Educ. Res. 2015;5:331. doi: 10.26417/ejser.v5i1.p331-336. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Cohen, Sheldon, and G. M. Social support, stress and the buffering hypothesis: A theoretical analysis. In Handbook of Psychology and Health vol. 4 (Routledge, 2020).
- 16.Reker GT, Woo LC. Personal meaning orientations and psychosocial adaptation in older adults. SAGE Open. 2011;1:1–10. doi: 10.1177/2158244011405217. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wong PTP. The Human Quest for Meaning: Theories, Research, and Applications. New York: Routledge; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Terziev, V. Conceptual framework of social adaptation. IJASOS- Int. E-journal Adv. Soc. Sci.5, 13 (2019).
- 19.Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychol. Aging. 2010;25:453–463. doi: 10.1037/a0017216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hawkley LC, Thisted RA, Masi CM, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness predicts increased blood pressure: 5-year cross-lagged analyses in middle-aged and older adults. Psychol. Aging. 2010;25:132–141. doi: 10.1037/a0017805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hawkley LC, Thisted RA, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses. Health Psychol. Off. J. Div. Health Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. 2009;28:354–363. doi: 10.1037/a0014400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S, Boomsma DI. Evolutionary mechanisms for loneliness. Cogn. Emot. 2014 doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.837379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann. Behav. Med. 2010 doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tokac U, Razon S. Nursing professionals’ mental well-being and workplace impairment during the COVID-19 crisis: A network analysis. J. Nurs. Manag. 2021;29:1653–1659. doi: 10.1111/jonm.13285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Londono Y, McMillan DE. Psychosocial adaptation: an evolutionary concept analysis exploring a common multidisciplinary language. J. Adv. Nurs. 2015;71:2504–2519. doi: 10.1111/jan.12723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Emerson E, Fortune N, Llewellyn G, Stancliffe R. Loneliness, social support, social isolation and wellbeing among working age adults with and without disability: Cross-sectional study. Disabil. Health J. 2021;14:100965. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2020.100965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Scarfe B, et al. Maintaining social and emotional wellbeing among older adults during periods of increased social isolation: lessons from the COVID-19 pandemic. Aust. J. Psychol. 2022;74:2141584. doi: 10.1080/00049530.2022.2141584. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Xiong, Y., Hong, H., Liu, C. & Zhang, Y. Q. Social isolation and the brain: Effects and mechanisms. Mol. Psychiatry28, 191–201 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 29.Clair R, Gordon M, Kroon M, Reilly C. The effects of social isolation on well-being and life satisfaction during pandemic. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2021;8:28. doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-00710-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cacioppo JT, Hawkley L. Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Shinn M, Lehmann S, Wong NW. Social interaction and social support. J. Soc. Issues. 1984 doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1984.tb01107.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hostinar CE, Gunnar MR. Social support can buffer against stress and shape brain activity. AJOB Neurosci. 2015;6:34. doi: 10.1080/21507740.2015.1047054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cacioppo JT, Norris CJ, Decety J, Monteleone G, Nusbaum H. In the eye of the beholder: individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009 doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Layden EA, et al. Perceived social isolation is associated with altered functional connectivity in neural networks associated with tonic alertness and executive control. Neuroimage. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L, et al. Loneliness and meaning in life are reflected in the intrinsic network architecture of the brain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2019;14:423–433. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Feng C, Wang L, Li T, Xu P. Connectome-based individualized prediction of loneliness. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2019;14:353–365. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Cacioppo JT, Norris CJ, Decety J, Monteleone G, Nusbaum H. Loneliness affects how the brain operates. ScienceDaily. 2009 doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cristofori I, et al. The lonely brain: Evidence from studying patients with penetrating brain injury. Soc. Neurosci. 2019 doi: 10.1080/17470919.2018.1553798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.DeWall N, Richman S. Social exclusion and the desire to reconnect. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass. 2011;5:919–932. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00383.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Woolgar A, Afshar S, Williams M, Rich A. Flexible coding of task rules in frontoparietal cortex: an adaptive system for flexible cognitive control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015;27:1895–1911. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Cacioppo S, Capitanio JP, Cacioppo JT. Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychol. Bull. 2014;140:1464–1504. doi: 10.1037/a0037618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Nguyen AW, Chatters LM, Taylor RJ, Mouzon DM. Social support from family and friends and subjective well-being of older African Americans. J. Happiness Stud. 2016 doi: 10.1007/s10902-015-9626-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.French KA, Dumani S, Allen TD, Shockley KM. A meta-analysis of work-family conflict and social support. Psychol. Bull. 2018;144:284. doi: 10.1037/bul0000120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Janicki Deverts D, Cohen S, Doyle WJ. Dispositional affect moderates the stress-buffering effect of social support on risk for developing the common cold. J. Pers. 2017 doi: 10.1111/jopy.12270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.McQuaid RJ, et al. Relations between plasma oxytocin and cortisol: The stress buffering role of social support. Neurobiol. Stress. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Szkody E, McKinney C. Appraisal and social support as moderators between stress and physical and psychological quality of life. Stress Health. 2020 doi: 10.1002/smi.2957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Costa-Cordella S, Arevalo-Romero C, Parada FJ, Rossi A. Social support and cognition: A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 2021;12:637060. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.637060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sato W, Kochiyama T, Uono S, Sawada R, Yoshikawa S. Amygdala activity related to perceived social support. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:2951. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59758-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cohen S. Social relationships and health. Am. Psychol. 2004 doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Torralva T, Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Lopez P, Manes F. INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): A brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009;15:777–786. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ihnen J, Antivilo A, Muñoz-Neira C, Slachevsky A. Chilean version of the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS-Ch): Psychometric properties and diagnostic accuracy. Dement. Neuropsychol. 2013 doi: 10.1590/s1980-57642013dn70100007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bosc M, Dubini A, Polin V. Development and validation of a social functioning scale, the social adaptation self-evaluation scale. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 1997 doi: 10.1016/S0924-977X(97)00420-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Neely-Prado A, van Elk M, Navarrete G, Hola F, Huepe D. Social adaptation in context: The differential role of religiosity and self-esteem in vulnerable versus non-vulnerable populations–a registered report study. Front. Psychol. 2021 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.519623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Neely-Prado A, Navarrete G, Huepe D. Socio-affective and cognitive predictors of social adaptation in vulnerable contexts. PLoS ONE. 2019 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Carriedo, N. et al. Cognitive, emotional, and social factors promoting psychosocial adaptation: a study of latent profiles in people living in socially vulnerable contexts. (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 56.Hays RD, Dimatteo MR. A short-form measure of loneliness. J. Pers. Assess. 1987 doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5101_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lubben JE. Assessing social networks among elderly populations. Fam. Community Health. 1988 doi: 10.1097/00003727-198811000-00008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Wells M. Resilience in older adults living in rural, suburban, and urban areas. Online J. Rural Nurs. Health Care. 2010 doi: 10.14574/ojrnhc.v10i2.55. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Villas-Boas S, Lima De Oliveira A, Ramos N, Montero I. Social support and generational diversity: The potential of the LSNS-6. Pedagog. Soc. 2018 doi: 10.7179/PSRI_2018.31.14. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.R core team. A language and environment for statistical computing. https://www.r-project.org/. (2020).
- 61.Petrocelli JV. Hierarchical multiple regression in counseling research: Common problems and possible remedies. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 2003;36:9–22. doi: 10.1080/07481756.2003.12069076. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Chao-Gan Y, Yu-Feng Z. DPARSF: A MATLAB toolbox for ‘pipeline’ data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2010 doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Parker DB, Razlighi QR. The benefit of slice timing correction in common fMRI preprocessing pipelines. Front. Neurosci. 2019 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Goto M, et al. Head motion and correction methods in resting-state functional MRI. Mag. Resonance Med. Sci. 2016 doi: 10.2463/mrms.rev.2015-0060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1999 doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:4<254::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002 doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Garcia-Cordero I, et al. Metacognition of emotion recognition across neurodegenerative diseases. Cortex. 2021;137:93–107. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.12.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Birba A, et al. Multimodal neurocognitive markers of naturalistic discourse typify diverse neurodegenerative diseases. Cereb. Cortex. 2021 doi: 10.1093/CERCOR/BHAB421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Ibañez A, et al. Predicting and characterizing neurodegenerative subtypes with multimodal neurocognitive signatures of social and cognitive processes. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2021;83:227–248. doi: 10.3233/JAD-210163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Jabbi M, Bastiaansen J, Keysers C. A common anterior insula representation of disgust observation, experience and imagination shows divergent functional connectivity. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e2939. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Kanske P, Böckler A, Trautwein F-M, Parianen Lesemann F, Singer T. Are strong empathizers better mentalizers? Evidence for independence and interaction between the routes of social cognition. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016;11:1383–1392. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Zang Y-F, et al. Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Dev. 2007;29:83–91. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2006.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Rosseel, Y. et al. Package ‘lavaan’. at (2017).
- 74.Garson DG. Path analysis. Statistical Associates Publishing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 75.Xie X, et al. How do you make me feel better? Social cognitive emotion regulation and the default mode network. Neuroimage. 2016;134:270–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Bickart KC, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF. The amygdala as a hub in brain networks that support social life. Neuropsychologia. 2014;63:235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Harding IH, Yücel M, Harrison BJ, Pantelis C, Breakspear M. Effective connectivity within the frontoparietal control network differentiates cognitive control and working memory. Neuroimage. 2015;106:144–153. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Dixon ML. Cognitive control, emotional value, and the lateral prefrontal cortex. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:758. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Schurz M, Maliske L, Kanske P. Cross-network interactions in social cognition: A review of findings on task related brain activation and connectivity. Cortex. 2020;130:142–157. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Dumas G, Moreau Q, Tognoli E, Kelso JAS. The human dynamic clamp reveals the fronto-parietal network linking real-time social coordination and cognition. Cereb. Cortex. 2020;30:3271–3285. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Lieberz J, et al. Loneliness and the social brain: how perceived social isolation impairs human interactions. Adv. Sci. 2021;8:2102076. doi: 10.1002/advs.202102076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Morr M, Liu X, Hurlemann R, Becker B, Scheele D. Chronic loneliness: neurocognitive mechanisms and interventions. Psychother. Psychosom. 2022;91:227–237. doi: 10.1159/000524157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Xin F, et al. Oxytocin differentially modulates amygdala responses during top-down and bottom-up aversive anticipation. Adv. Sci. 2020;7:2001077. doi: 10.1002/advs.202001077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Yao S, et al. Oxytocin facilitates approach behavior to positive social stimuli via decreasing anterior insula activity. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;21:918–925. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyy068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Gozzi M, Dashow EM, Thurm A, Swedo SE, Zink CF. Effects of oxytocin and vasopressin on preferential brain responses to negative social feedback. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;42:1409–1419. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Díaz-Rivera MN, et al. Multidimensional inhibitory signatures of sentential negation in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Cereb. Cortex. 2023;33:403–420. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data supporting this study's findings are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.