Abstract
Aversive reactivity to negative affect has been described as a transdiagnostic mechanism that links distal temperamental vulnerabilities to clinically relevant behaviors. However, the abundance of constructs reflecting aversive reactivity has resulted in a proliferation of models that may ultimately be redundant. We performed a circumscribed review of studies measuring associations between six constructs – anxiety sensitivity, experiential avoidance, distress intolerance, intolerance of uncertainty, thought-action fusion, and negative urgency – and ten relevant coping behaviors. Results suggested that most constructs were measured in relation to a limited number of coping behaviors. Additionally, constructs were most often measured in isolation, rather than with similar constructs. Implications and suggestions for future research and treatment are discussed.
Keywords: Transdiagnostic, Neuroticism, Behavior, Aversive Reactivity, Experiential Avoidance
Neuroticism, the tendency to experience negative emotions (Barlow et al., 2014), has been described as a higher-order risk factor for a range common psychiatric conditions (Brown, 2007; Brown & Barlow, 2009; Kessler et al., 2011; Griffith et al., 2010). Recently, Barlow and colleagues have described a functional model of “emotional disorders” (i.e., anxiety, depressive, and related disorders) to explain how neuroticism promotes various forms of psychopathology (Barlow et al., 2014; Bullis et al., 2019; Sauer-Zavala & Barlow, 2014). Emotional disorders are thought to develop from and be maintained by a negative reinforcement process in which: (1) frequent and intense negative emotions (neuroticism) are met with (2) aversive reactions to these emotional experiences (e.g., perceiving the emotions as intolerable, uncontrollable or as having negative consequences), leading to (3) behavioral efforts to escape, avoid, or reduce emotional intensity (Bullis et al., 2019; Gross et al., 2011).
In this model, aversive reactivity to emotions refers to the belief that negative emotions are unwelcome, dangerous, or otherwise intolerable. People who respond to negative affective experiences this way are likely to attempt to reduce or alter the intensity of their emotional experience through avoidant coping strategies. Although avoidant coping may offer temporary short-term relief (Campbell-Sills et al., 2006), this approach results in rebound effects where negative emotions become more frequent and intense over time (Abramowitz et al., 2001). In addition, people experience functional impairment as their lives become increasingly characterized by patterns of emotional avoidance (Hayes et al., 2006). Thus, in this model, aversive reactivity functions as the bridge connecting neuroticism with the avoidant behavioral coping that perpetuates a cycle of negative emotions and impairment.
Aversive Reactivity to Emotions: A Transdiagnostic Construct Studied as Isolated Components
Whereas neuroticism is well-accepted as a transdiagnostic vulnerability factor across a range of psychopathology (Barlow et al., 2014; Lahey, 2009; Sauer-Zavala et al., 2017), the study of aversive reactivity has been divided among several conceptually similar constructs, each with their own theoretical model and empirical research base. Examples of the varied processes reflecting aversive reactivity to emotions include anxiety sensitivity, experiential avoidance, distress intolerance, intolerance of uncertainty, thought-action fusion, and negative urgency (McHugh et al., 2011; Naragon-Gainey & Watson, 2018). Researchers have typically defined these constructs as distinctly associated with unique forms of psychopathology. For example, anxiety sensitivity and intolerance of uncertainty have historically been implicated in panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD; Naragon-Gainey & Watson, 2018), whereas thought-action fusion was initially studied in the context of obsessive-compulsive disorder (Shafran et al., 2006). Developing discrete theoretical models that assign unique risk factors to each DSM diagnosis is consistent with a categorical approach to classification. In contrast, experiential avoidance and distress intolerance have been viewed as transdiagnostic processes (McHugh et al., 2011; Naragon-Gainey & Watson, 2018; Spinhoven et al., 2014; Spinhoven et al., 2017); however, the existence of multiple constructs reflecting the same phenomenon has the potential to unnecessarily complicate the field’s understanding of psychopathology and its treatment through the creation of an abundance of conceptualizations and treatment approaches.
The goal of this review is to summarize and examine trends in how several distinct constructs representing aspects of aversive reactivity have been studied. We focused our efforts on the six aforementioned constructs in particular (i.e., anxiety sensitivity, experiential avoidance, distress intolerance, intolerance of uncertainty, thought-action fusion, and negative urgency) to explore (1) whether these constructs were more often studied together or separately and (2) the behavioral outcomes with which these constructs were most frequently studied. Although emotion dysregulation is an important and related process, and others have explored its overlap with constructs representing aversive reactivity (e.g., Conway et al., 2020), we chose to exclude it from our review. Emotion dysregulation research is characterized by a broad literature with inconsistent definitions of this construct; some (e.g., Trull et al., 2015; Linehan, 1993) view emotion dysregulation more akin to neuroticism (frequent, intensive, unstable emotions) whereas others (e.g., Gross et al., 2011) consider this construct to reflect ability to modulate emotional responses. These inconsistencies render it difficult to draw conclusions about the nature of the research on aversive reactivity as a unique process.
Anxiety Sensitivity
Anxiety sensitivity refers to the belief that the physical sensations associated with anxiety are inherently dangerous or will have serious negative consequences (Reiss et al., 1986). People higher in anxiety sensitivity view emotion-related physiological changes as dangerous, increasing the intensity of the emotional responses by activating the sympathetic nervous system (Reiss et al., 1986). Anxiety sensitivity includes three distinct manifestations, each related to a feared negative outcome: physical, cognitive, and social (Taylor et al., 2007). A person may notice increased heart rate and worry that they will have a heart attack (physical consequence; e.g., “it scares me when my heart beats rapidly”; Taylor et al., 2007), a loss of control (cognitive consequence; “It scares me when I am unable to keep my mind on a task”; Taylor et al., 2007), or that they will do something embarrassing in public (social consequence; e.g., “I worry that other people will notice my anxiety”; Taylor et al., 2007). Anxiety sensitivity leads to greater awareness and negative interpretations of anxiety symptoms, which promotes avoidant behavioral coping responses as people seek to escape from distressing physiological states (Wheaton et al., 2012). Since anxiety sensitivity is the belief that certain sensations are inherently dangerous, it is a form of aversive reactivity.
Anxiety sensitivity was originally investigated in the context of panic disorder (Reiss et al., 1986). Since then, anxiety sensitivity has been implicated in an increasingly wide range of psychopathology. High levels of anxiety sensitivity have been observed across the anxiety and related disorders, including obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; Raines et al., 2014), social anxiety disorder (Ak & Kılıç, 2017), GAD (Deacon & Abramowitz, 2006), and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Asmundson & Stapleton, 2008). People who engage in alcohol and drug use (Allan et al., 2015a), self-injurious behaviors (e.g., Allan et al., 2015b), and disordered eating (Anestis et al., 2008) also demonstrate elevated levels of anxiety sensitivity (e.g., Allan et al., 2015a). Since anxiety sensitivity was historically investigated exclusively in the context of anxiety disorders, recent work has only begun to break free of these historical limitations and consider the role of anxiety sensitivity across disorders. Anxiety sensitivity is most commonly measured using various iterations of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI; Reiss et al., 1986), including the ASI-R (Taylor & Cox, 1998) and ASI-3 (Taylor et al., 2007).
Experiential Avoidance
Experiential avoidance is a construct coined and popularized by the developers of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; Hayes et al., 2016) to combine a variety of responses to negative internal stimuli. Experiential avoidance has been defined by two primary aspects: (1) an “unwilling[ness] to remain in contact with particular private experiences (e.g., bodily sensations, emotions, thoughts, memories, behavioral predispositions)” and (2) taking “steps to alter the form or frequency of these events and the contexts that occasion them” (Hayes et al., 1996). Because experiential avoidance is defined as a functional process, it may present as any number of unique behavioral responses, depending on the stimulus the person is trying to avoid (e.g., substance use, withdrawal, or self-harm).
Given the breadth of this definition, experiential avoidance was initially conceptualized as an overarching transdiagnostic construct. Indeed, previous reviews (Chawla & Ostafin, 2007) demonstrate the role of experiential avoidance in various forms of psychopathology, including depression (Polusny et al., 2004), GAD (Roemer et al., 2005), PTSD (Tull et al., 2004), substance use (Forsyth et al., 2003), self-harm (Chapman et al., 2006), and BPD (Chapman et al., 2005). Experiential avoidance has been primarily measured in two ways: as a single dimension using the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (e.g., “I’m afraid of my feelings”; Hayes et al., 2004; Bond et al., 2011), and as six specific manifestations using the Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire (MEAQ; Gámez et al., 2011): behavioral avoidance (e.g., “I won’t do something if I think it will make me uncomfortable”), distress aversion (e.g., “If I could magically remove all of my painful memories, I would”), repression/denial (e.g., “I am able to ‘turn off’ my emotions when I don’t want to feel”), distraction/suppression (e.g., “When something upsetting comes up, I try very hard to stop thinking about it”), procrastination (e.g., “I tend to put off unpleasant things that need to get done”), and distress endurance (e.g., “People should face their fears”; Gámez et al., 2011).
Distress Intolerance
Distress intolerance is defined as a person’s ability to experience and withstand negative emotions (e.g., “I’ll do anything to stop feeling distressed or upset”; Simons & Gaher, 2005), encompassing the evaluation of and expectations around experiencing negative emotions, including tolerability, appraisal, emotion regulation, and disruption of functioning (Simons & Gaher, 2005). For example, people high in distress intolerance perceive distress to be intolerable, have difficulty accepting distress (i.e., feel ashamed of being distressed), use strategies aimed at immediately avoiding or alleviating negative emotions, and feel consumed by the experience of negative emotions, causing significant disruption of functioning (Simons & Gaher, 2005). Distress intolerance is a form of aversive reactivity, as it is characterized by a perceived inability to tolerate and regulate distressing emotional experiences.
Distress intolerance has been commonly studied in the context of substance use disorders. Individuals high in distress intolerance are more likely to experience problems related to alcohol and cannabis use (Buckner et al., 2007) and are prone to relapse sooner (Daughters et al., 2005) than those low in distress intolerance. Distress intolerance is also linked to eating disorders (e.g., Raykos et al., 2009), self-injurious behaviors (e.g., Nock & Mendes, 2008), suicidal ideation (Anestis et al., 2013), hoarding (e.g., Timpano et al., 2009), and rumination (e.g., Feldman et al., 2014). Low levels of distress intolerance have also been observed in emotional disorders such as PTSD (Tull et al., 2013), GAD, and major depressive disorder (Allan et al., 2014). Again, distress intolerance has only recently begun to receive attention in a broader range of emotional disorders outside of substance use disorders. Distress intolerance is most commonly measured using the Distress Tolerance Scale (DTS; Simons & Gaher, 2005)
Intolerance of Uncertainty
Intolerance of uncertainty is defined as difficulty withstanding the experience of not knowing (e.g., "Uncertainty makes me vulnerable, unhappy, or sad", "My mind can't be relaxed if I don't know what will happen tomorrow"; Buhr & Dugas, 2002). Although this construct has historically been considered a symptom of anxiety (Dugas et al., 1997), particularly of GAD, researchers have also described intolerance of uncertainty as a core mechanism maintaining a range of emotional disorders (e.g., Carleton, 2012; Carleton, 2016; Einstein, 2014). Intolerance of uncertainty may represent a specific form of aversive reactivity related to the cognitive features of an emotional experience (i.e., not knowing). The majority of intolerance of uncertainty research has historically centered on anxiety disorders (e.g., GAD [Ladouceur et al., 1999], social anxiety disorder [SAD; Boelen & Reijntjes, 2009], panic disorder [Smith et al., 2019]). Intolerance of uncertainty has since demonstrated relations with OCD (Hezel et al., 2019; Tolin et al., 2003) as well as with depression (Carleton, 2012; Gentes & Ruscio, 2011) and eating disorders (Brown et al., 2018).
In addition to correlational studies examining relations between intolerance of uncertainty and symptoms of various disorders, experimental manipulations that induce intolerance of uncertainty shed light on its behavioral consequences. For example, using a validated laboratory paradigm, Mosca et al. (2016) asked participants to progressively consider potential outcomes of a possible negative future life event and then read statements designed to induce high or intolerance of uncertainty. The authors reported significantly higher levels of worry in the high intolerance of uncertainty condition, relative to the low intolerance of uncertainty and control conditions. Indeed, worry represents a form of avoidant coping as it creates the illusion that one is reducing the likelihood of already low base rate negative events and may distract from negative affect and its associated arousal (Borkovec & Roemer, 1995; Llera & Newman, 2014). Intolerance of uncertainty is most commonly assessed using the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS; Buhr & Dugas, 2002).
Thought-Action Fusion
Thought-action fusion is a form of aversive reactivity that is specifically related to the cognitive component of an emotional experience (i.e., thoughts). There are two ways in which thought-action fusion can manifest (Shafran et al., 1996). First, likelihood thought-action fusion refers to the belief that simply having a thought about an event makes that event more likely to occur (e.g., “If I think of myself falling ill, this increases the risk that I will fall ill”; Shafran et al., 1996). Moral thought-action fusion is the belief that thinking about an action is morally equivalent to actually performing that behavior (e.g., “If I wish harm on someone, it is almost as bad as doing harm”; Shafran et al., 1996). Thought-action fusion is most commonly measured using the Thought-Action Fusion Scale (TAFS; Shafran et al., 1996). Indeed, thought-action fusion is thought to form an intermediate step between an intrusive thought and a compulsive behavior (Rachman, 1998), and is a form of aversive reactivity in the functional model of emotional disorders (e.g., Barlow et al., 2014; Bullis et al., 2019). Thought-action fusion is overwhelmingly most frequently associated with OCD, perhaps limiting our understanding of its transdiagnostic applicability, although it has also been linked to other anxiety disorders, as well as eating disorders (Thompson-Hollands et al., 2013).
Negative Urgency
Negative urgency refers to the tendency to react to emotional distress by engaging in impulsive behaviors (Keough et al., 2017). Initial factor analyses of impulsivity identified four distinct primary factors, including lack of premeditation (e.g., “I have a reserved and cautious attitude toward life” [reverse scored]) ,negative urgency (e.g., “When I feel bad, I will often do things I later regret in order to make myself feel better now”), sensation seeking (e.g., “I generally seek new and exciting experiences and sensations”), and lack of perseverance (e.g., “I tend to give up easily”; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). More recent iterations include a fifth factor, positive urgency, or the tendency to act impulsively as a result of intense positive emotions (e.g., “When I am very happy, I tend to do things that may cause problems in my life”; Lynam et al., 2006). While these individual aspects of impulsivity all refer in some way to acting without forethought (Whiteside et al., 2005), negative urgency specifically describes the functional process of negative affect as the driving force behind impulsive behaviors. In particular, negative urgency is associated with giving into cravings, and demonstrates strong associations with neuroticism (Whiteside et al., 2005).
Negative urgency has subsequently been linked to multiple high-risk behaviors including binge eating (Fischer et al., 2018), substance use, and non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI; Zhao et al., 2017), although there is a lack of research investigating the role of negative urgency with other emotional disorders outside of borderline personality disorder and substance use disorders (Whiteside et al., 2005). In sum, people high in negative urgency engage in impulsive behaviors to quickly relieve intense negative emotional distress despite possible negative long-term consequences (Fischer et al., 2018). Thus, negative urgency represents a form of aversive reactivity as negative evaluations of emotional experiences drive impulsive avoidant coping behaviors (Keough, et al., 2017). Negative urgency is most commonly assessed using the Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, Positive Urgency, Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS-P; Lynam et al., 2006).
Relationships Among Aversive Reactivity Constructs
In line with a more comprehensive transdiagnostic model of psychopathology, researchers have begun to explore the extent to which these forms of aversive reactivity are distinct. For instance, Spinhoven and colleagues (2017) demonstrated that experiential avoidance and anxiety sensitivity are indicators of a single underlying latent factor, suggesting that these constructs may function similarly. Similarly, Conway and colleagues (2020) performed factor analyses on measures reflecting distress tolerance, anxiety sensitivity, experiential avoidance, and emotion dysregulation, and found evidence supporting a single universal latent factor underlying all constructs. Moreover, some have argued that anxiety sensitivity, intolerance of uncertainty, and experiential avoidance are manifestations of neuroticism itself (Naragon-Gainey & Watson, 2018; Spinhoven et al., 2016). Others argue that these constructs are not synonymous with neuroticism but represent “proximal individual differences that can better describe who is likely to develop which specific symptoms beyond the broad risk conferred by affective traits” (Naragon-Gainey et al., 2018, p. 1177). Finally, given that many of the studies aimed at understanding relationships between aversive reactivity constructs and psychopathology tend to focus only on a portion of the possible relations (e.g., anxiety sensitivity and avoidance; distress intolerance and self-injury, etc.), it is unclear whether these constructs can differentially and meaningfully predict avoidant behavioral coping above and beyond neuroticism and each other.
Why Study Behaviors Instead of DSM Disorder Constructs?
As previously described, emotionally avoidant coping strategies contribute to a reinforcement cycle in which emotional experiences occur more frequently and are viewed more negatively, ultimately increasing the likelihood of continued avoidance (Bullis et al., 2019). Avoidant coping may manifest behaviorally (e.g., leaving an anxiety-provoking situation, using substances to dampen negative emotions) or cognitively (e.g., worrying, ruminating, suppressing unwanted thoughts). Although the typical strategies used may differ based on DSM disorder (e.g., people with panic disorder may be more likely to engage in agoraphobic avoidance, rumination may be more common in depression), all of these efforts function to escape from unwanted emotion and can occur across diagnoses (Barlow et al., 2014; Bullis et al., 2019). Unfortunately, because specific avoidant behaviors are typically associated with a particular disorder, the research to date relating various coping strategies to neuroticism and disorder-specific aversive reactivity constructs has been piecemeal. Recent efforts such as the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; Insel et al., 2010) encourage research that cuts across diagnostic categories to identify functional transdiagnostic processes. To better understand the shared and unique predictive utility of each aversive reactivity construct, it is necessary to conduct more comprehensive research that assesses multiple constructs related to aversive reactivity (e.g., anxiety sensitivity, experiential avoidance, distress intolerance, intolerance of uncertainty, thought-action fusion, and negative urgency) along with a wider range of behavioral outcomes that function to escape emotions (e.g., substance use, checking, self-injury). By examining specific effects captured by these broader assessments, researchers may develop more streamlined and empirically testable models of psychopathology with clear functional relations.
Additionally, the prediction of behavioral avoidance strategies, rather than disorder-based symptom severity, may be useful for the prevention of emotional disorders. Occasional emotionally avoidant behaviors are normative (Gross et al., 2011) and in some contexts, adaptive (Hofmann & Hay, 2018). For example, a person may need to distract themselves from suicidal urges in the moment to more adaptively approach and process their emotions in the future. However, the repeated, habitual use of avoidant behaviors in response to negative emotions lays the foundation for the development of an emotional disorder (Bullis et al., 2019). As described previously, engaging in short-term avoidance increases the intensity and distress of future negative emotions and can contribute to functional impairment (Abramowitz et al., 2001; Hayes et al., 2016). Over time, the cycle of negative emotions, aversive reactivity, and avoidant coping behaviors are thought to become more entrenched until a person ultimately crosses the dichotomous diagnostic threshold for a DSM disorder. Thus, although neuroticism represents a key vulnerability for the development of psychopathology, aversive reactivity may be the active mechanism contributing to the avoidant behavioral coping that eventually constitutes diagnostically significant syndromes.
Toward a Transdiagnostic Aversive Reactivity Construct: Trends in the Literature
In the section that follows, we review the frequency with which aversive reactivity constructs are studied alongside each other, as well as in the context of ten behaviors that function as avoidant responses to negative affect and that present across common DSM syndromes: alcohol use, drug use, compulsions, worry, rumination, binge eating/restricting, self-harm, behavioral withdrawal, situational avoidance, and reassurance-seeking. Although worry and rumination may be classified as cognitive avoidance strategies, we take a behaviorist approach in referring to them as behaviors in this review. By highlighting how frequently certain constructs are studied with specific behaviors (e.g., distress intolerance is most often assessed as a predictor of drug use), we demonstrate the isolated nature of this literature. We hope to underscore the need for more comprehensive investigations of the relations between aversive reactivity and avoidant behavioral coping that can precede and maintain emotional disorders.
Method
We conducted a circumscribed literature review to supplement our theoretical premise that the state of the research on aversive reactivity and avoidant behavioral coping is quite isolated. The scope of our review included a large number of constructs and measures, as well as specific statistical associations, rendering a conventional systematic review or meta-analysis unfeasible. Instead, we searched for peer-reviewed articles that included measures of any of the six aforementioned aversive reactivity constructs and at least one measure of the ten clinically relevant avoidant behaviors. We searched for articles within the APA PsycInfo, Web of Science, and Google Scholar databases by entering each combination of aversive reactivity construct and avoidant behavior (e.g., “experiential avoidance” + “rumination”). A complete list of search terms and the resulting eligible articles is available upon request. Eligibility for inclusion in this review was determined by the following criteria: (1) use of a validated measure of at least one aversive reactivity construct, (2) use of a measure that specifically captured behavioral manifestations of avoidant coping (e.g., Avoidance subscale of the PCL-5; Ruminative Response Scale), and not solely symptom severity, and (3) a reported association between the construct and avoidant behavior (e.g., correlation coefficient, R2 change, etc.). This list of measures is available in Appendix S1. After initial identification, an independent member of the research team reviewed each article to confirm its eligibility. We initially identified 561 possibly eligible articles. Of these original 561 articles, 26 were duplicates, and 70 more were deemed ineligible after further review by research team personnel. Of the 70 ineligible articles, 31 did not measure an aversive reactivity construct of interest, 31 did not measure a specific behavior of interest, and 8 did not report an association between an aversive reactivity construct and behavior, leaving 465 final studies included (see Figure S1).
Results
The frequency of studies that included associations between aversive reactivity constructs and avoidant behaviors is depicted in Figure 1. Darker colors indicate a higher number of articles that identified an association, whereas lighter colors indicate fewer articles documenting such associations. In addition, a graphical depiction of the frequency with which the aversive reactivity constructs were measured in conjunction with each other is presented in Figure 2. Again, darker colors indicate higher frequencies of articles, and lighter colors indicate lower frequencies. Values on the diagonal indicate the number of articles that only measured the indicated construct and no others. We summarize the results for each construct in the sections below.
Figure 1. Frequency of Studies Investigating Associations between Constructs Reflecting Aversive Reactivity and Avoidant Coping Behaviors.
Note. Darker colors represent high number of studies investigating the association. AS = Anxiety sensitivity; EA = Experiential avoidance; DI = Distress intolerance; IU = Intolerance of uncertainty; TAF = Thought-action fusion; NU = Negative urgency.
Figure 2. Frequency of Studies Investigating Associations between Constructs Reflecting Aversive Reactivity and Other Related Constructs.
Note. The numbers in the diagonal represent the number of studies that measured the construct of interest in isolation. Abbreviations for constructs are as follows, along with the total number of studies that measured each construct in parenthesis: AS = Anxiety sensitivity (k = 204); EA = Experiential avoidance (k = 91); DI = Distress intolerance (k = 85); IU = Intolerance of uncertainty (k = 81); TAF = Thought-action fusion (k = 26); NU = Negative urgency (k = 57).
Anxiety Sensitivity and Behavioral Coping
We identified 204 articles that measured anxiety sensitivity along with at least one avoidant behavioral response. Out of these 204 studies, anxiety sensitivity was most frequently investigated with alcohol use (k = 73) and drug use (k = 60), indicating the central role anxiety sensitivity is thought to play in the use of substances to avoid uncomfortable sensations associated with anxiety.
Anxiety sensitivity was also studied relatively frequently with worry (k = 35), compulsions (k = 31), self-harm (k = 28), and behavioral or situational avoidance (k = 20). Worry, compulsions, and avoidance are all commonly associated symptoms or sequelae of anxiety disorders; therefore, the frequency of these associations is consistent with the conceptualization of anxiety sensitivity as a bridge between anxious arousal and subsequent clinical syndromes. However, the moderately high number of studies regarding deliberate self-harm was somewhat unexpected given the initially limited scope of anxiety sensitivity. Given that self-harm is often a maladaptive response to intense negative affect, catastrophic interpretations characteristic of anxiety sensitivity may be an important factor in the use of self-harm to escape or distract from these affective experiences.
Anxiety sensitivity was least frequently studied with rumination (k = 15), binge eating/restricting (k = 13), behavioral withdrawal (k = 5), and reassurance seeking (k = 1), underscoring the conceptual silo between anxiety sensitivity and non-anxiety-related disorders. Indeed, the fear of anxiety-related physical sensations may not, at face value, appear relevant to self-harm, rumination, disordered eating, behavioral withdrawal, or reassurance seeking. Yet when considered from the perspective of the negative reinforcement cycle that defines emotional disorders, these behaviors function to escape perceived distress.
Anxiety Sensitivity with Other Constructs Reflecting Aversive Reactivity
Anxiety sensitivity was measured more frequently than other aversive reactivity constructs (k = 204), and most often in isolation (k = 149; Figure 2). The most commonly paired construct with anxiety sensitivity was distress intolerance (k = 22), which is also highly linked to alcohol and drug use. In descending order, the next constructs most frequently studied with anxiety sensitivity were experiential avoidance (k = 14), intolerance of uncertainty (k = 14), and negative urgency (k = 12). No studies assessed both anxiety sensitivity and thought-action fusion (k = 0). Thus, despite the likelihood that anxiety sensitivity and the other aversive reactivity constructs represent conceptually similar functional mechanisms, these associations have not been investigated in a systematic fashion.
Experiential Avoidance and Behavioral Coping
Consistent with its broad applicability, researchers have studied experiential avoidance in the context of a range of behavioral outcomes across 91 articles (Figure 1). Unlike other measures of aversive reactivity, a similar number of studies have been conducted assessing experiential avoidance and each behavioral outcome assessed here except for withdrawal (k = 2) and reassurance-seeking (k = 0): alcohol use (k = 14), drug use (k = 10), compulsions (k = 15), worry (k = 17), rumination (k = 18), binge eating and restricting (k = 15), self-harm (k = 13), and behavioral avoidance (k = 14). This pattern suggests that researchers have considered the transdiagnostic functional nature of experiential avoidance by testing its relations with a wide range of putatively avoidant behaviors.
Experiential Avoidance with Other Constructs Reflecting Aversive Reactivity
Although researchers have studied experiential avoidance with a wide range of behaviors, the vast majority of research conducted on experiential avoidance has not included other constructs reflecting aversive reactivity (k = 91; Figure 2). Specifically, experiential avoidance was most frequently studied alongside anxiety sensitivity (k = 14), yet hardly studied alongside negative urgency (k = 2), distress intolerance (k = 4), intolerance of uncertainty (k = 4), and thought-action fusion (k = 0). Researchers have tended to test the relations between avoidant behaviors and experiential avoidance alone (k = 70), rather than in tandem with other relevant constructs. This tendency has made it difficult to establish the degree of statistical overlap between experiential avoidance and other aversive reactivity constructs. Given that experiential avoidance was initially purported to function as an overarching, transdiagnostic process, researchers may not have considered it necessary to include other forms of aversive reactivity, since the broad definition of experiential avoidance was thought to capture these theorized processes.
Distress Intolerance and Behavioral Coping
We identified 85 articles that measured Distress Intolerance along with at least one avoidant behavior. Distress intolerance was most frequently investigated with alcohol use (k = 38) and drug use (k = 21), highlighting the significance of distress intolerance in the use of substances to avoid distressing emotions. Distress intolerance was also relatively frequently investigated with self-harm (k = 12), compulsions (k = 9), worry (k = 8), and binge eating/restricting (k = 8). Given that self-harm and binge eating/restricting can provide immediate relief from intense, negative emotions, researchers may be more likely to examine the role of distress intolerance specifically. Distress intolerance was least frequently studied with behavioral avoidance (k = 5), rumination (k = 3), behavioral withdrawal (k = 0), and reassurance seeking (k = 0), which are more common among anxiety and depressive disorders and generally represent less immediate forms of avoidant coping.
Distress Intolerance with Other Constructs Reflecting Aversive Reactivity
Distress intolerance was most commonly studied in isolation (k = 54). When it was studied with another aversive reactivity construct, distress intolerance was most frequently measured with anxiety sensitivity (k = 22), reflecting a similar aversion to physical sensations brought on by intense negative emotions. In descending order, the next constructs most frequently studied with distress intolerance were negative urgency (k = 9), experiential avoidance (k = 4), and intolerance of uncertainty (k = 3). No studies investigated associations between distress intolerance and thought-action fusion (k = 0).
Intolerance of Uncertainty and Behavioral Coping
We identified 81 articles that assessed relationships between intolerance of uncertainty and at least one avoidant behavior. Unsurprisingly, intolerance of uncertainty was most frequently investigated with worry (k = 54). Worry may provide a sense of control over uncertain outcomes, functioning to escape from the aversive state of not knowing (Borkovec, 1994). Intolerance of uncertainty was studied relatively frequently with compulsions (k = 20) and rumination (k = 10). Given that compulsive behaviors and rumination serve to avoid negative emotions, it makes conceptual sense that researchers would explore associations between these variables. In contrast, intolerance of uncertainty was rarely studied with other forms of behavioral avoidance. This is unfortunate as some have hypothesized that specific forms of uncertainty may lead to different types of avoidant behaviors (e.g., situational avoidance, social withdrawal) and ultimately to the onset of different clinical conditions (Shihata et al., 2017). However, more comprehensive research is needed to draw these conclusions.
Intolerance of Uncertainty with Other Constructs Reflecting Aversive Reactivity
In relation to other constructs representing aversive reactivity to emotion, intolerance of uncertainty was most often studied in isolation (k = 63). The most commonly paired construct was anxiety sensitivity (k = 14), followed by experiential avoidance (k = 4), distress tolerance (k = 3), and thought-action fusion (k = 1). No studies investigated associations between intolerance of uncertainty and negative urgency. The piecemeal nature of the work examining relations between aversive reactivity constructs makes it difficult to draw conclusions about the extent to which these constructs are unique in predicting specific forms of avoidant behavioral coping.
Thought-Action Fusion and Behavioral Coping
We identified 26 articles that assessed relations between thought-action fusion and at least one avoidant behavior. The relatively low number of articles assessing this construct is likely because thought-action fusion is often studied in the context of OCD symptoms. Studies that reported the association between thought-action fusion and the frequency of compulsions, the behavioral avoidance strategy associated with OCD, were relatively uncommon (k = 18). The other forms of behavioral coping studied in tandem with thought-action fusion include binge eating (k = 6), worry (k = 5), alcohol use (k = 3), and drug use (k = 1). Worry makes conceptual sense in this context because, like obsessional thinking, it is a form of repetitive thought. It is also worth noting that a body image-specific form of thought-action fusion (i.e., thought-shape fusion; TSF) was tested in relation to the frequency of eating behaviors (e.g., Shafran & Robinson, 2004); the specific construct of TSF indicates that the tendency to view thoughts as dangerous facts is indeed transdiagnostic, although separate measures contribute to fragmentation of this literature.
Thought-Action Fusion with Other Constructs Reflecting Aversive Reactivity
With regard to co-occurrence of thought-action fusion and other forms of aversive reactivity, only one study with behavioral outcomes included another construct (k =1), indicating that thought-action fusion was most commonly studied in isolation (k = 25). This pattern suggests that thought-action fusion has historically been seen as its own unique construct, relevant only in a few narrow contexts, and separate from other forms of aversive reactivity. However, given that thought-action fusion may indeed function similarly as a means of escaping or avoiding negative affect, the paucity of literature that measures thought-action fusion in conjunction with other relevant constructs limits the applicability of findings.
Negative Urgency and Behavioral Coping
We identified 57 articles that measured negative urgency along with at least one avoidant behavior. The most commonly investigated behavior was alcohol use (k = 23), highlighting researchers’ interest in alcohol use as an impulsive response to negative emotions. Similarly, several studies included assessments of both negative urgency and drug use (k = 11). The next most commonly investigated behaviors studied with negative urgency were binge eating/restricting (k = 16) and self-harm (k = 10), which are often impulsive responses to distress that fit with the conceptual notion of negative urgency. Negative urgency was studied least frequently with compulsions (k = 5), worry (k = 2), rumination (k = 2), behavioral withdrawal (k = 0), behavioral avoidance (k = 0), and reassurance-seeking (k = 0).
Negative Urgency with Other Constructs Reflecting Aversive Reactivity
Negative urgency was the second-least frequently studied construct in our review (k = 57); however, like many of the other constructs, it was also more often studied in isolation (k = 37) than with other constructs. The most commonly paired construct with negative urgency was anxiety sensitivity (k = 12), which also has a strong literature base in alcohol and drug use. Next, negative urgency was investigated with distress intolerance (k = 9) and experiential avoidance (k = 2) to a lesser extent. Distress intolerance and negative urgency research overlaps in several behavioral domains, including alcohol/drug use and self-harm. However, the paucity of research involving negative urgency and experiential avoidance, as well as the complete lack of studies investigating negative urgency with both intolerance of uncertainty and thought-action fusion suggest that these constructs are not historically perceived to be related, despite the functional similarities in aversive reactivity.
Discussion
In this review, we conceptualize aversive reactivity to emotions as a transdiagnostic, functional mechanism linking temperamental risk factors (i.e., neuroticism) and clinical syndromes. We contend that six supposedly unique psychological processes (i.e., anxiety sensitivity, experiential avoidance, distress intolerance, intolerance of uncertainty, thought-action fusion, and negative urgency) all reflect the tendency to find emotional experiences aversive. Experiential avoidance, for instance, is considered a general unwillingness to experience unpleasant internal states; similarly, distress intolerance is the perceived inability to tolerate subjective distress. Several further constructs reflect aversive reactions to specific aspects of an affective experience. For example, anxiety sensitivity refers to distress over anxiety-related physical sensations, whereas both intolerance of uncertainty and thought-action fusion represent negative evaluations of cognitions. Finally, negative urgency is the impulsive urge to engage in behaviors that will provide relief from strong negative emotions. In our estimation, these constructs all serve similar functional purposes as the intermediate bridge between negative affect and maladaptive coping behaviors.
However, the foundational research trends on which these constructs are based have not reflected the conceptual similarities between constructs and continue to perpetuate isolated research strategies, contributing to an unnecessary overabundance of psychopathological models that hinders transdiagnostic research and clinical development. For example, categorial diagnostic models such as the DSM have produced numerous conceptualizations and models for each distinct diagnostic category, despite findings that several of the most common emotional disorders may simply represent variations of shared underlying mechanisms (Blashfield et al., 2014; Lilienfeld, 2014). Clinically speaking, the existence of many disorder-specific interventions (i.e., single-disorder protocols [SDPs]) places additional burden on clinicians to learn and administer different SDPs, contributing to clinician burnout and preventing patients from receiving care that they need (Kazdin, 2008; Kazdin & Blase, 2016; McHugh & Barlow, 2010). Recently, researchers have begun to more comprehensively investigate the degree to which aversive reactivity constructs relate to neuroticism (Naragon-Gainey & Watson, 2018), to one another (Conway et al., 2020; Spinhoven et al., 2017), and to other related constructs such as emotion dysregulation (Conway et al., 2020; Juarascio et al., 2020). From a clinical perspective, empirical efforts to compare transdiagnostic cognitive-behavioral interventions (i.e., the Unified Protocol [UP]) to disorder-specific protocols demonstrated that the UP resulted in equivalent symptom reduction, suggesting equal effectiveness with less attrition and less clinician burden (Barlow et al., 2017). Thus, although the isolated nature of these constructs has been perpetuated for decades, it is clear that researchers are moving toward more empirical comparisons and integrations of these constructs.
As a supplement to our theoretical review, we sought to characterize overarching patterns in the study of constructs reflecting aversive reactivity to negative emotions alongside clinically relevant avoidant behaviors by conducting a circumscribed literature review. In support of the view that this literature has been conducted in a piecemeal fashion, our findings demonstrate that aversive reactivity constructs were most often studied in isolation. This suggests that researchers view these constructs as primarily circumscribed to particular DSM diagnoses and associated behaviors. It is possible that this isolated study of aversive reactivity constructs is a result of theoretical/empirical traditions; in other words, foundational studies in particular fields may have emphasized relations between one aversive reactivity construct and one DSM disorder (e.g., early research on panic disorder and anxiety sensitivity; Reiss, 1991), resulting in follow-up studies that perpetuate this pattern. Indeed, most constructs were most commonly associated with one or two primary behaviors, except for experiential avoidance, again reinforcing the perception that most constructs are seen as tied to specific disorders based on established research paradigms.
Of course, we do not assume that the aversive reactivity constructs are entirely overlapping; for example, anxiety sensitivity may indeed demonstrate incremental power in predicting agoraphobic avoidance. However, without comprehensive assessment of all six aversive reactivity constructs alongside a wide swath of behavioral outcomes, the points of convergence and divergence remain unclear. We encourage future researchers to investigate the degree to which these aversive reactivity constructs differentially predict specific avoidant behaviors. For example, using longitudinal factor analysis to assess the relations between the multiple aspects of aversive reactivity and avoidant behaviors has the potential to illuminate these relations and the degree to which aversive reactivity constructs overlap or demonstrate unique factor structures. Such work has the possibility to accomplish three aims. First, it may replicate and extend previous cross-sectional work showing the relatively unitary nature of aversive reactivity. Second, it would allow researchers to explore the added degree of variability, if any, in avoidant behaviors that is explained by specific aspects of aversive reactivity. Third, it would allow researchers to better validate the construct of aversive reactivity by decomposing state and trait components of its variability.
Such fundamental and naturalistic studies further have the potential to improve the measurement and assessment of mechanisms that maintain emotional disorders and further streamline psychological interventions to target these factors. For example, evidence that the full range of emotional disorders are maintained by a general aversive reactivity factor would warrant a single transdiagnostic treatment. In this vein, transdiagnostic treatments such as the UP (Barlow et al., 2017; Cassiello-Robbins et al., 2020) and ACT (Hayes et al., 2016) demonstrate efficacy across diagnostic categories, equivalent to single-disorder protocols, supporting the utility of transdiagnostic interventions to treat a range of psychopathology. However, if certain constructs are uniquely relevant to particular clinical presentations, targeted interventions may be necessary. If future research indicated that specific forms of behavioral avoidance were most commonly predicted by a particular facet or subfactor of an overarching aversive reactivity factor, then targeted interventions could be developed to address these facets. Patients exhibiting difficulties in these specific areas could then receive these targeted interventions, either in addition to or in place of treatment targeting aversive reactivity more broadly. The assessment of underlying mechanisms has the potential to reduce therapist burden by streamlining interventions to treat transdiagnostic processes that can reach patients across diagnostic categories (Southward & Sauer-Zavala, 2020). Clinicians could select from a small number of mechanism-focused interventions to treat nearly all patients in general practice (Sauer-Zavala et al., 2020).
We conceptualized our circumscribed literature review as a supplement to our theoretical thesis to provide a “snapshot” of quite a large literature. However, limitations of this work still warrant comment. First, our methods may have resulted in the omission of some studies, despite efforts to be comprehensive. Given the number of constructs and behaviors within the scope of this review, it would have been impractical to carry out a more systematic review. For example, whereas constructs such as emotion dysregulation and stress reactivity may not fall precisely within the scope of aversive reactivity, they are at the very least neighboring constructs that would benefit from additional exploration to more fully determine their relations to aversive reactivity and avoidant behavioral coping.
Additionally, given our focus on behavioral outcomes (not symptom severity), it is possible that avoidant coping strategies that are more amenable to measurement may have been overemphasized. For example, alcohol and drug use, which were consistently some of the most commonly measured behaviors across constructs, are more easily assessed as discrete behaviors than worry or rumination, for instance. Other behavioral strategies like reassurance-seeking or rumination were less commonly measured overall and are more typically assessed indirectly through the lens of symptom severity or as markers of subjective distress. Furthermore, certain behaviors like reassurance-seeking and withdrawal were rarely measured across constructs, likely due to the fact that there are few assessment tools that specifically tap into these behaviors. These measurement issues may explain the imbalance in capturing certain behaviors relative to others. Furthermore, our review was limited in that the field relies primarily on self-report measures of behaviors. However, given their potential importance to the development of clinically relevant psychopathology, we encourage future researchers to develop psychometrically valid and reliable ways of measuring these behaviors.
Overall, our review suggests that the majority of research trends to date fail to make headway in better understanding comorbidity and contribute to the problem of having an overabundance of models related to only one disorder or problem behavior. Research progress suffers from a lack of parsimony as a result of unwieldy theoretical models, which further translates to limited clinical utility. The overall point of this review is to shed light on the fragmented state of the research. By more comprehensively understanding these relations, we can move toward a better conceptualization of the functional processes that maintain emotional disorders, streamline assessment procedures, and help refine treatment interventions.
Supplementary Material
Public Health Statement.
The current study reviewed the existing literature to observe how often researchers measured associations between possibly redundant constructs reflecting aversive reactivity to affect (i.e., the tendency to view negative emotions as intolerable) and clinically relevant coping behaviors (e.g., avoidance, substance use, etc.). Most of the time, these constructs were only measured with a small number of behaviors, and rarely with other similar constructs. Future research should consider if these constructs are truly different from one another, in order to help develop more streamlined and universally applicable treatments.
Role of Funding Sources
Funding for Caitlyn O. Hood’s doctoral studies was provided by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (T32 DA035200) through the National Institutes of Health (NIH). NIH had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
References
- Abramowitz JS, Tolin DF, & Street GP (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(5), 683–703. 10.1016/s0272-7358(00)00057-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ak S, & Kılıç C (2017). Does gender affect the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and social anxiety? Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 28(4), 1–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allan NP, Albanese BJ, Norr AM, Zvolensky MJ, & Schmidt NB (2015). Effects of anxiety sensitivity on alcohol problems: Evaluating chained mediation through generalized anxiety, depression and drinking motives. Addiction, 110(2), 260–268. 10.1111/add.12739 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allan NP, Capron DW, Raines AM, & Schmidt NB (2014). Unique relations among anxiety sensitivity factors and anxiety, depression, and suicidal ideation. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(2), 266–275. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allan NP, Macatee RJ, Norr AM, & Schmidt NB (2014). Direct and interactive effects of distress tolerance and anxiety sensitivity on generalized anxiety and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(5), 530–540. 10.1007/s10608-0149623-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Allan NP, Norr AM, Boffa JW, Durmaz D, Raines AM, & Schmidt NB (2015). Examining the unique relations between anxiety sensitivity factors and suicidal ideation and past suicide attempts. Psychiatry Research, 228(3), 441–447. 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
- Andrews G (1996). Comorbidity and the General Neurotic Syndrome. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 168(S30), 76–84. 10.1192/S0007125000298449 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anestis MD, Holm-Denoma JM, Gordon KH, Schmidt NB, & Joiner TE (2008). The role of anxiety sensitivity in eating pathology. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(3), 370–385. 10.1007/s10608-006-9085-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Anestis MD, Knorr AC, Tull MT, Lavender JM, & Gratz KL (2013). The Importance of High Distress Tolerance in the Relationship between Nonsuicidal Self-Injury and Suicide Potential. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 43(6), 663–675. 10.1111/sltb.12048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asmundson GJG, & Stapleton Jennifer A. (2008). Associations between dimensions of anxiety sensitivity and PTSD symptom clusters in active-duty police officers. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 37(2), 66–75. 10.1080/16506070801969005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow DH, Farchione TJ, Bullis JR, Gallagher MW, Murray-Latin H, Sauer-Zavala S, Bentley KH, Thompson-Hollands J, Conklin LR, Boswell JF, Ametaj A, Carl JR, Boettcher HT, & Cassiello-Robbins C (2017). The unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders compared with diagnosis-specific protocols for anxiety disorders: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(9), 875. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow DH, Sauer-Zavala S, Carl JR, Bullis JR, & Ellard KK (2014). The nature, diagnosis, and treatment of neuroticism: Back to the future. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(3), 344–365. 10.1177/2167702613505532 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blashfield RK, Keeley JW, Flanagan EH, & Miles SR (2014). The cycle of classification: DSM-I through DSM-5. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 25–51. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153639 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boelen PA, & Reijntjes A (2009). Intolerance of uncertainty and social anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(1), 130–135. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borkovec TD (1994). The nature, functions, and origins of worry. In Davey GC & Tallis F (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 5–33). John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Borkovec TD, & Roemer L (1995). Perceived functions of worry among generalized anxiety disorder subjects: Distraction from more emotionally distressing topics? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26(1), 25–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TA (2007). Temporal course and structural relationships among dimensions of temperament and DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorder constructs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(2), 313–328. 10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TA, Avery JC, Jones MD, Anderson LK, Wierenga CE, & Kaye WH (2018). The impact of alexithymia on emotion dysregulation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa over time. European Eating Disorders Review, 26(2), 150–155. 10.1002/erv.2574 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TA, & Barlow DH (2009). A proposal for a dimensional classification system based on the shared features of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders: Implications for assessment and treatment. Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 256–271. 10.1037/a0016608 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TA, Campbell LA, Lehman CL, Grisham JR, & Mancill RB (2001). Current and lifetime comorbidity of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders in a large clinical sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(4), 585–599. 10.1037//0021-843x.110.4.585 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner JD, Keough ME, & Schmidt NB (2007). Problematic alcohol and cannabis use among young adults: The roles of depression and discomfort and distress tolerance. Addictive Behaviors, 32(9), 1957–1963. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.12.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buhr K, & Dugas MJ (2002). The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale: Psychometric properties of the English version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(8), 931–945. 10.1016/s0005-7967(01)00092-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullis JR, Boettcher H, Sauer-Zavala S, Farchione TJ, & Barlow DH (2019). What is an emotional disorder? A transdiagnostic mechanistic definition with implications for assessment, treatment, and prevention. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 26(2), e12278. 10.1111/cpsp.12278 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH, Brown TA, & Hofmann SG (2006). Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1251–1263. 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carleton RN (2012). The intolerance of uncertainty construct in the context of anxiety disorders: Theoretical and practical perspectives. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 12(8), 937–947. 10.1586/ern.12.82 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carleton RN (2016). Into the unknown: A review and synthesis of contemporary models involving uncertainty. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 39, 30–43. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassiello-Robbins C, Southward MW, Wilner Tirpak J, & Sauer-Zavala S (2020). A systematic review of Unified Protocol applications: Facilitating widespread dissemination via adaptability. Clinical Psychology Review, 78, 101852. 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman AL, Gratz KL, & Brown MZ (2006). Solving the puzzle of deliberate self-harm: The experiential avoidance model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(3), 371–394. 10.1016/j.brat.2005.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman AL, Specht MW, & Cellucci T (2005). Borderline personality disorder and deliberate self-harm: Does experiential avoidance play a role? Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 35(4), 388–399. 10.1521/suli.2005.35.4.388 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chawla N, & Ostafin B (2007). Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63(9), 871–890. 10.1002/jclp.20400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chien L-L, Ko H-C, & Wu JY-W (2007). The five-factor model of personality and depressive symptoms: One-year follow-up. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(5), 1013–1023. 10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chmielewski M, Clark LA, Bagby RM, & Watson D (2015). Method matters: Understanding diagnostic reliability in DSM-IV and DSM-5. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 764–769. 10.1037/abn0000069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conway CC, Naragon-Gainey K, & Harris MT (2020). The structure of distress tolerance and neighboring emotion regulation abilities. Assessment, 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuijpers P, Karyotaki E, Weitz E, Andersson G, Hollon SD, van Straten A (2014). The effects of psychotherapies for major depression in adults on remission, recovery and improvement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disordorders, 159, 118–26. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cyders MA, Smith GT, Spillane NS, Fischer S, Annus AM, & Peterson C (2007). Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: Development and validation of a measure of positive urgency. Psychological Assessment, 19(1), 107–118. 10.1037/1040-3590.19.1.107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daughters SB, Lejuez CW, Bornovalova MA, Kahler CW, Strong DR, & Brown RA (2005). Distress tolerance as a predictor of early treatment dropout in a residential substance abuse treatment facility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 729–734. 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.729 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deacon B, & Abramowitz J (2006). Anxiety sensitivity and its dimensions across the anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(7), 837–857. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dugas MJ, Freeston MH, & Ladouceur R (1997). Intolerance of Uncertainty and Problem Orientation in Worry. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21(6), 593–606. 10.1023/A:1021890322153 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Einstein DA (2014). Extension of the transdiagnostic model to focus on intolerance of uncertainty: A review of the literature and implications for treatment. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 21(3), 280–300. 10.1111/cpsp.12077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman G, Dunn E, Stemke C, Bell K, & Greeson J (2014). Mindfulness and rumination as predictors of persistence with a distress tolerance task. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 154–158. 10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer S, Wonderlich J, Breithaupt L, Byrne C, & Engel S (2018). Negative urgency and expectancies increase vulnerability to binge eating in bulimia nervosa. Eating Disorders, 26(1), 39–51. doi: 10.1080/10640266.2018.1418253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forsyth JP, Parker JD, & Finlay CG (2003). Anxiety sensitivity, controllability, and experiential avoidance and their relation to drug of choice and addiction severity in a residential sample of substance-abusing veterans. Addictive Behaviors, 28(5), 851–870. 10.1016/s0306-4603(02)00216-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frances AJ, & Widiger T (2012). Psychiatric diagnosis: Lessons from the DSM-IV past and cautions for the DSM-5 future. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 109–130. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gámez W, Chmielewski M, Kotov R, Ruggero C, & Watson D (2011). Development of a measure of experiential avoidance: The Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 692–713. 10.1037/a0023242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentes EL, & Ruscio AM (2011). A meta-analysis of the relation of intolerance of uncertainty to symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 923–933. 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffith JW, Zinbarg RE, Craske MG, Mineka S, Rose RD, Waters AM, & Sutton JM (2010). Neuroticism as a common dimension in the internalizing disorders. Psychological Medicine, 40(7), 1125–1136. 10.1017/S0033291709991449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ, Sheppes G, & Urry HL (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: A distinction we should make (carefully). Cognition & Emotion, 25(5), 765–781. 10.1080/02699931.2011.555753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Luoma JB, Bond FW, Masuda A, & Lillis J (2006). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 1–25. 10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, & Wilson KG (2016). Acceptance and commitment therapy: The process and practice of mindful change (2nd ed.). Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Wilson KG, Gifford EV, Follette VM, & Strosahl K (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(6), 1152–1168. 10.1037//0022-006x.64.6.1152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hezel DM, Stewart SE, Riemann BC, & McNally RJ (2019). Standard of proof and intolerance of uncertainty in obsessive-compulsive disorder and social anxiety disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 64, 36–44. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Bagby RM, Gralnick T, Ro E, Ruggero C, Mullins-Sweatt S, Kotov R, Bach B, Cicero DC, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Chmielewski M, DeYoung CG, Docherty AR, Eaton NR, Forbush KT, Ivanova MY, Latzman RD, Pincus AL, … Zimmermann J (2019). Integrating psychotherapy with the hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP). Journal of Psychotherapy Integration. 10.1037/int0000156 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, Sanislow C, & Wang P (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry; Am J Psychiatry. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juarascio AS, Parker MN, Manasse SM, Barney JL, Wyckoff EP, & Dochat C (2020). An exploratory component analysis of emotion regulation strategies for improving emotion regulation and emotional eating. Appetite, 150, 104634. 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazdin AE (2008). Evidence-based treatment and practice: New opportunities to bridge clinical research and practice, enhance the knowledge base, and improve patient care. The American Psychologist, 63(3), 146–159. 10.1037/0003-066X.63.3.146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazdin AE, & Blase SL (2011). Rebooting psychotherapy research and practice to reduce the burden of mental illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 6(1), 21–37. 10.1177/1745691610393527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keough MT, Hendershot CS, Wardell JD, & Bagby RM (2017). Investigating the mediational role of negative urgency in the anxiety sensitivity pathway to cannabis problems and dependence symptoms among postsecondary students. Journal of American College Health, 66(2), 69–75. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2017.1369093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Ormel J, Petukhova M, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Russo LJ, Stein DJ, Zaslavsky AM, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Andrade L, Benjet C, de Girolamo G, de Graaf R, Demyttenaere K, Fayyad J, Haro JM, Hu C. yi, Karam A, … Ustün TB (2011). Development of lifetime comorbidity in the World Health Organization world mental health surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(1), 90–100. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khan AA, Jacobson KC, Gardner CO, Prescott CA, & Kendler KS (2005). Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186(3), 190–196. 10.1192/bjp.186.3.190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D, Achenbach TM, Althoff RR, Bagby RM, Brown TA, Carpenter WT, Caspi A, Clark LA, Eaton NR, Forbes MK, Forbush KT, Goldberg D, Hasin D, Hyman SE, Ivanova MY, Lynam DR, Markon K, … Zimmerman M (2017). The Hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(4), 454–477. 10.1037/abn0000258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ladouceur R, Dugas MJ, Freeston MH, Rhéaume J, Blais F, Boisvert J-M, Gagnon F, & Thibodeau N (1999). Specificity of generalized anxiety disorder symptoms and processes. Behavior Therapy, 30(2), 191–207. 10.1016/S0005-7894(99)80003-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lahey BB (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism. The American Psychologist, 64(4), 241–256. 10.1037/a0015309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lilienfeld SO (2014). DSM-5: Centripetal scientific and centrifugal antiscientific forces. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 21(3), 269–279. 10.1111/cpsp.12075 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Llera SJ, & Newman MG (2014). Rethinking the role of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: Evidence supporting a model of emotional contrast avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 45(3), 283–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynam DR, Smith GT, Whiteside SP, & Cyders MA (2006). The UPPS-P: Assessing five personality pathways to impulsive behavior. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University, 10. [Google Scholar]
- Maser JD, Norman SB, Zisook S, Everall IP, Stein MB, Schettler PJ, & Judd LL (2009). Psychiatric nosology is ready for a paradigm shift in DSM-V. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 16(1), 24–40. 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01140.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McHugh RK, & Barlow DH (2010). The dissemination and implementation of evidence-based psychological treatments. A review of current efforts. The American Psychologist, 65(2), 73–84. 10.1037/a0018121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McHugh RK, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW, Murray HW, Hearon BA, Gorka SM, & Otto MW (2011). Shared variance among self-report and behavioral measures of distress intolerance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35(3), 266–275. 10.1007/s10608-010-9295-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosca O, Lauriola M, & Carleton RN (2016). Intolerance of uncertainty: A temporary experimental induction procedure. PLoS ONE, 11(6). 10.1371/journal.pone.0155130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naragon-Gainey K, McMahon TP, & Park J (2018). The contributions of affective traits and emotion regulation to internalizing disorders: Current state of the literature and measurement challenges. American Psychologist, 73(9), 1175–1186. 10.1037/amp0000371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naragon-Gainey K, & Watson D (2018). What lies beyond neuroticism? An examination of the unique contributions of social-cognitive vulnerabilities to internalizing disorders. Assessment, 25(2), 143–158. 10.1177/1073191116659741 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nock MK, & Mendes WB (2008). Physiological Arousal, Distress Tolerance, and Social Problem–Solving Deficits Among Adolescent Self-Injurers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(1), 28–38. 10.1037/0022-006X.76.1.28 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polusny MA, Rosenthal MZ, Aban I, & Follette VM (2004). Experiential avoidance as a mediator of the effects of adolescent sexual victimization on negative adult outcomes. Violence and Victims, 19(1), 109–120. 10.1891/vivi.19.1.109.33238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachman S (1998). A cognitive theory of obsessions: Elaborations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(4), 385–401. 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10041-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raines AM, Oglesby ME, Capron DW, & Schmidt NB (2014). Obsessive compulsive disorder and anxiety sensitivity: Identification of specific relations among symptom dimensions. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 3(2), 71–76. 10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Raykos BC, Byrne SM, & Watson H (2009). Confirmatory and exploratory factor analysis of the distress tolerance scale (DTS) in a clinical sample of eating disorder patients. Eating Behaviors, 10(4), 215–219. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Regier DA, Narrow WE, Kuhl EA, & Kupfer DJ (2009). The conceptual development of DSM-V. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(6), 645–650. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiss S (1991). Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clinical Psychology Review, 11(2), 141–153. 10.1016/0272-7358(91)90092-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reiss S, Peterson RA, Gursky DM, & McNally RJ (1986). Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24(1), 1–8. 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90143-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roemer L, Salters K, Raffa SD, & Orsillo SM (2005). Fear and avoidance of internal experiences in GAD: Preliminary tests of a conceptual model. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 71–88. 10.1007/s10608-005-1650-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sauer-Zavala S, & Barlow DH (2014). The case for borderline personality disorder as an emotional disorder: Implications for treatment. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 21(2), 118–138. 10.1111/cpsp.12063 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sauer-Zavala S, Southward MW, & Semcho SA (2020). Integrating and differentiating personality and psychopathology in cognitive behavior therapy. Journal of Personality. Advance online publication. 10.1111/jopy.12602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauer-Zavala S, Wilner JG, & Barlow DH (2017). Addressing neuroticism in psychological treatment. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 8(3), 191–198. 10.1037/per0000224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shafran R, & Robinson P (2004). Thought-shape fusion in eating disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43(4), 399–408. 10.1348/0144665042389008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shafran R, Thordarson DS, & Rachman S (1996). Thought-action fusion in obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10(5), 379–391. 10.1016/0887-6185(96)00018-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Shihata S, McEvoy PM, & Mullan BA (2017). Pathways from uncertainty to anxiety: An evaluation of a hierarchical model of trait and disorder-specific intolerance of uncertainty on anxiety disorder symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 45, 72–79. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons J, & Gaher R (2005). The Distress Tolerance Scale: Development and Validation of a Self-Report Measure. Motivation and Emotion, 29(2), 83–102. 10.1007/s11031-005-7955-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smith NS, Albanese BJ, Schmidt NB, & Capron DW (2019). Intolerance of uncertainty and responsibility for harm predict nocturnal panic attacks. Psychiatry Research, 273, 82–88. 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.01.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Southward MW, & Sauer-Zavala S (2020). Experimental manipulations to test theory-driven mechanisms of cognitive behavior therapy. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, Article 603009. 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.603009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinhoven P, Drost J, de Rooij M, van Hemert AM, & Penninx BW (2014). A longitudinal study of experiential avoidance in emotional disorders. Behavior Therapy, 45(6), 840–850. 10.1016/j.beth.2014.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinhoven P, Drost J, de Rooij M, van Hemert AM, & Penninx BWJH (2016). Is experiential avoidance a mediating, moderating, independent, overlapping, or proxy risk factor in the onset, relapse and maintenance of depressive disorders? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40(2), 150–163. 10.1007/s10608-015-9747-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinhoven P, van Hemert AM, & Penninx BWJH (2017). Experiential avoidance and bordering psychological constructs as predictors of the onset, relapse and maintenance of anxiety disorders: One or many? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(6), 867–880. 10.1007/s10608-017-9856-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Springer KS, Levy HC, & Tolin DF (2018). Remission in CBT for adult anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 61, 1–8. 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor S, & Cox BJ (1998). An expanded anxiety sensitivity index: Evidence for a hierarchic structure in a clinical sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 12(5), 463–483. 10.1016/s0887-6185(98)00028-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, Abramowitz JS, Holaway RM, Sandin B, Stewart SH, Coles M, Eng W, Daly ES, Arrindell WA, Bouvard M, & Cardenas SJ (2007). Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: Development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychological Assessment, 19(2), 176–188. 10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson-Hollands J, Farchione TJ, & Barlow DH (2013). Thought-action fusion across anxiety disorder diagnoses: Specificity and treatment effects. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 201(5), 407–413. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31828e102c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timpano KR, Buckner JD, Richey JA, Murphy DL, & Schmidt NB (2009). Exploration of anxiety sensitivity and distress tolerance as vulnerability factors for hoarding behaviors. Depression and Anxiety, 26(4), 343–353. 10.1002/da.20469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tolin DF, Abramowitz JS, Brigidi BD, & Foa EB (2003). Intolerance of uncertainty in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17(2), 233–242. 10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00182-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trull TJ, Lane SP, Koval P, & Ebner-Priemer UW (2015). Affective dynamics in psychopathology. Emotion Review, 7(4), 355–361. 10.1177/1754073915590617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tull MT, Gratz KL, Coffey SF, Weiss NH, & McDermott MJ (2013). Examining the interactive effect of posttraumatic stress disorder, distress tolerance, and gender on residential substance use disorder treatment retention. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(3), 763–773. 10.1037/a0029911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tull MT, Gratz KL, Salters K, & Roemer L (2004). The role of experiential avoidance in posttraumatic stress symptoms and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and somatization. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192(11), 754–761. 10.1097/01.nmd.0000144694.30121.89 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheaton MG, Deacon BJ, McGrath PB, Berman NC, & Abramowitz JS (2012). Dimensions of anxiety sensitivity in the anxiety disorders: Evaluation of the ASI-3. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26(3), 401–408. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside SP, & Lynam DR (2001). The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(4), 669–689. 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside SP, Lynam DR, Miller JD, & Reynolds SK (2005). Validation of the UPPS impulsive behaviour scale: A four-factor model of impulsivity. European Journal of Personality, 19(7), 559–574. 10.1002/per.556 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao J, Tomasi D, Wiers CE, Shokri-Kojori E, Demiral ŞB, Zhang Y, Volkow ND, & Wang G (2017). Correlation between traits of emotion-based impulsivity and intrinsic default-mode network activity. Neural Plasticity, 2017, Article 9297621. 10.1155/2017/9297621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.