Abstract
While the diversity and complexity of the links between creativity and emotional skills as well as their effects on cognitive processes are now established, few approaches to implementing them in schools have been evaluated. Within the framework of the enactive paradigm, which considers the complexity and dynamics of language as a cognitive process, we study how an approach based on performative theatre can synergistically stimulate creativity (artistic, bodily and linguistic), emotional skills (identifying and understanding emotions) and executive functions (especially inhibition, cognitive flexibility and emotional control), all as components defined in the context of oral communication. Stimulating this synergy in the context of foreign language teaching may be especially beneficial for children with communication disorders. This paper presents the first results of the CELAVIE pilot study (Creativity, Empathy and Emotions in Language learning with Autism for an Inclusive Education) through a case study of a pupil with a neurodevelopmental disorder included in a 4th-grade class. The results show a progression in oral communication in English as a Foreign Language (EFL), in emotional skills and creativity.
Keywords: creativity, emotional skills, communication, executive functions, performative theatre/drama, embodiment, English as foreign language (EFL), modern language didactics (MLD), inclusive education (EI), primary school
1. Introduction
“Creativity is a cognitive-emotional-manipulative experience that is accessible to all people. Creativity is cognitive because it is about innovating and developing ideas and occurs via specialized mental processes. It is emotional because emotions are integral and ‘loom large’ in the creative process”.
The diversity and complexity of the links between creativity and emotional skills as well as their effects on cognitive processes are now established (Besançon and Lubart 2015; Gnezda 2011; Lubart et al. 2015). We also know that there are complex links between emotions and language (Beck et al. 2012; Capron Puozzo and Piccardo 2013; Pasquier et al. 2022; Pons et al. 2003). Furthermore, creativity and language are closely related (Aden 2009; Cook 2000; Eschenauer 2019a; Jones 2015). It therefore seems interesting to explore the links between emotions, creativity and language as cognitive processes. However, few studies conducted in schools have assessed these links.
The main aim of the CELAVIE feasibility study (Creativity, Empathy and Emotions in Language learning for an Inclusive Education) presented here is to measure the effects of the drama-based performative approach on the oral communication skills of primary school students in English as a Foreign Language (EFL) in pupils with and without neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDS) as an inclusive goal. In this article, we explore through a case study of a student with NDDS how a performance theatre approach can develop communication through creative enactment, cognitive-emotional skills and oral production skills.
Like the majority of Western countries, France has followed the inclusive school model since the Salamanca Declaration (United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization [UNESCO] 1994), which defines both what Special Educational Needs are and the means of action to be implemented. Educational policies in France (Ministère de l’éducation nationale et de la jeunesse 2005, 2013) have thus enabled children with disabilities to have easier access to schooling in a mainstream environment. Indeed, the latest report by the French Ministry of Education and Research’s Department of Evaluation, Forecasting and Performance [DEPP] (Dauphin 2022) indicates that since the 2005 law, the number of children with disabilities enrolled in mainstream schools has risen by 6–7% per year. At the start of the 2021 school year, this involved almost 476,000 children or adolescents. On this subject, the report by the French National Council for School Evaluation (Conseil national d’évaluation du système scolaire [CNESCO] 2016) sets out a series of recommendations likely to promote the inclusion of pupils with disabilities. The role of collaborative practices and the issues surrounding training are put forward as key levers for inclusive schools.
We choose here to focus on a case study of a child with NDDS, that will allow us to explore the dynamics of cognitive-emotional and creative processes and how they relate to one another. For Lubart et al. (2015), creativity is defined as “the ability to produce something that is both novel and appropriate to the context in which it occurs” (p. 23). Furthermore, cognitive-emotional skills include the concept of emotional intelligence (Mayer and Salovey 1997), taken here in terms of emotional competencies rather than personality traits (Sander and Scherer 2019). There are complex links between creative and emotional processes (Lubart et al. 2015), which have been studied in different fields related to learning. We have chosen to study them in relation to communicative processes, in line with work carried out within the enaction paradigm (Varela 1989), applied in the context of foreign language learning (Aden 2009; Eschenauer 2019a, 2019b). Creativity and emotional skills are thus addressed in our study in a holistic, dynamic and integrative approach to modern foreign language learning processes in the context of inclusive education (EI). Our approach differs from more traditional acquisitional approaches that consider the components of communication (lexicon, syntax, prosody, interactions, body language, emotions, etc.) separately.
Varela proposes an integrative model of communication that is based on the complexity and dynamics of languaging as an embodied cognitive and creative process. “Embodied” includes, for example, emotions, posture, sensoriality, gestures, etc. For researchers in this field, there is no gap between the body and the mind (Noe 2016; Varela 2017). Thus, communication is not only information sent from a sender to a receiver, nor purely linguistic content consisting of lexicon and syntax. From an enactive perspective, language creativity is hence mainly concerned with the ability of speakers to adapt their behaviour and to produce new language appropriate to the context, but also with the originality of students’ communicative proposals (Aden 2009). Communication is a creative act since it allows for the infinite combination of a finite number of words and reinvents itself over time. Cook (2000) combines language manipulation with language play and language creativity. For him, EFL learners are using language creatively when they play with language forms, with semantic and pragmatic elements. For Carter and McCarthy (2004), pattern reformulation is also creative. Indeed, speakers manipulate existing patterns of prefabricated language to construct new creative sequences. Authors interested in the importance of creativity in foreign language acquisition agree on including humour, banter and joking as creative acts of language manipulation (Cook 2000; Kramsch 2009; Bell 2012). Hence communication is in itself a creative process, both in its language nature and from a behavioural point of view. Results from national and international studies show that creativity could be a lever for better EFL learning (Eschenauer 2019a; Jones 2015; Richards 2013)—and as many studies have shown, creativity can be learned (Bonnardel et al. 2023; Capron Puozzo 2016), for example, by practising acting (Hainselin et al. 2018).
Drama practice as an emotional and creative approach to language teaching can develop communication skills in EFL. It seems to be one of the best methods, as it balances thoughts and feelings (Gabitova et al. 2018). Yet, this approach is at odds with standard teaching practices in elementary schools. Indeed, primary school teachers focus more on the acquisition of lexicon and syntax and they most often decontextualise lessons (Dawoud et al. 2020). These practices have limitations that have been widely pointed out for the last thirty years. New approaches in France, such as role play (Care and Debyser 1991), were first proposed empirically and then researched and transformed into drama (Aden and Lovelace 2004), as remembered by Eschenauer et al. (2023). The positive effects of drama practice on foreign language learning have been noted (Abdelkader et al. 2013; Sambanis 2013; Schewe 1993), especially in relation to creative processes and emotional skills (Aden and Lovelace 2004). Effects of EFL instruction through drama have also been shown on prosodic production (Thienkalaya and Chusanachoti 2020). Furthermore, recent studies based on communities of practice highlight the contribution of drama-based practice to the development of social skills (Andréoléty 2015; Amonkar et al. 2021; Cook 2020; Corbett et al. 2019; Drill and Bellini 2022; Ioannou et al. 2020), basic learning (Mpella et al. 2019; Saint Martin (de) and Astier 2021) and communication skills in children with NDDS (Kumar and Raja 2009).
In view of these considerations, it now seems important to refer to definitions of communication in Modern Language Didactics (MLD) that integrate its complex and dynamic nature, including creativity and cognitive-emotional skills. This is why we develop in this paper an integrative model presenting what we believe to be three major components of oral communication (Figure 1). We assume that creativity and emotional literacy are both means (in the sense of processes) and communicative goals to be developed, as well as oral production.
In this model, we can see that oral production depends on cognitive-emotional skills and creativity and that embodied language lies at the intersection of these three elements.
We have already pointed out that there is ample evidence of the complex links between emotions and creativity (Lubart et al. 2015). From a functionalist point of view, experiencing different emotional states (both positive and negative) is thought to enhance creativity (Abele-Brehm 1992; Zenasni and Lubart 2008). Executive functions are central to the psychological development of children, as they must learn to constantly adjust their behaviour to internal and environmental contingencies. This constant adjustment involves emotional regulation (Roy 2015). Mental flexibility underlies these behavioural and sensitive adaptations. The emotional state experienced could generate a level of arousal experienced as uncomfortable, which can be reduced by investing in creative activities (Adaman and Blaney 1995). Executive functions allow for the functioning of cognitive-emotional skills such as creativity. They are also needed to produce language (Roy 2015).
As emotional intensity, emotional expressiveness and emotional awareness (which refers to the abilities to perceive and understand emotions; Lane and Schwartz 1987) are positively correlated (Pasquier et al. 2009), it seems important to value didactic approaches that elicit strong emotions, their expression, and their (conscious) perception in connection with EFL—as possible, for example, through theatre. A motivating and innovative task such as a drama-based performance in a school context could evolve emotional awareness and creative engagement for the development of communicative skills in EFL.
Here we present a case that will be studied according to our three-component model of communication (see Figure 1). We chose the case of a child with NDDS who presents specifically communication disorders. We will explore how a performance theatre approach can develop communication through creativity, cognitive-emotional skills and oral production skills.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Design and General Procedures
2.1.1. A Test-Retest Research Design
We measured the evolution of three components of communication (cf. Figure 1) before and after the implementation of a performative theatre approach. The methodological approach of our research is interdisciplinary because we have developed a psycho-phenomenological methodology in accordance with the enaction paradigm (Varela 1996), which frames CELAVIE.
The study presented in this paper is based on the data collection of the feasibility phase of the larger CELAVIE project. This feasibility phase took place over 6 months, including one and a half months for the inclusion and pre-testing phases, 4 months of experimentation and 15 days for post-testing. The experimental approach is the practice of performance art (drama) in English for EFL learners.
Our data obtained through qualitative and quantitative measurement tools (self-reported and hetero-reported data, audio-video recordings) were qualitatively processed in a single case study.
We focus here on data related to cognitive-emotional skills (in particular emotional awareness and mental flexibility), which we link to the analysis of body language (especially body engagement), foreign language use and the creativity indicators of Fortin et al. (2014). The qualitative analysis from the interviews is based on content analysis (Bardin [1977] 2013; Mayring 2010). Coding for content analysis was performed double-blind with inter-rater agreement.
2.1.2. The Drama-Based Performative Approach
The students had been learning English at school for three years, for 1.5 h a week (pre-A1 level, starters). For our experimentation, an English-speaking actor set up 4 performative theatre workshops divided into 8 sessions of 2 h, i.e., 16 h over a period of 4 months. Both the actor and the teacher worked with the whole class, but activities could be performed in full class groups, half-groups, small groups of 3 or 4 or by a student alone. The teacher articulated these workshops with classroom language activities twice a week, in sessions of 30 min to 1 h, also based on the characteristics of performance (movement, space, voice, speech, etc. Examples of activities in the classroom were: presentation games with skits, vocabulary of emotions by acting out the emotions, then adding a sentence, etc.). In this way, language elements such as lexicon and syntax that had emerged during the creative drama activities could be reworked and formalised in the classroom. The EFL embodied experience was complemented by an awareness of how this new language works. The medium that linked the workshops to the classroom activities and from which the actor drew stories of encounters between the students was the children’s book Voices in the Park, by Anthony Browne. We chose this book for its aesthetic and creative proposition, as well as for the content related to our research questions. Indeed, it is a story about a walk in a park told by four different characters with four different emotions. Each story generates a different vision of the world, and the illustrations transform the park according to the subjective emotional experience of the narrator. This book is, therefore, very phenomenological. It allows us to work on the awareness of the characters’ emotions and the acting with them that links the children’s embodied knowledge (self-centred point of view) to reflection from the perspective of others (hetero-centred point of view). The didactisation of this book, originally written for native speakers, was not achieved by simplifying the text as teachers usually do, but through theatrical practice based on the illustrations and the decoding of the characters’ emotions.
Performative theatre differs from more canonical theatre practice in its emergent and relational nature. The theatre we call “canonical” is a stage interpretation of a text. The production is more linear: writing (often by a third person), learning and interpreting. Performative theatre is closer to stage creation, except that it can be performed in any space: in the street, in a museum, in a café, etc. Besides, it has always had an unpredictable dimension since the story is transforming itself through the relationship with the audience and the space. Performative theatre could be described with fractals (i.e., not linear): writing comes after the exploration of the space and the sharing of creative experiences and stories between participants. In a school setting, the audience may be part of the class, students from other classes, families, or even a wider audience for larger projects.
Performance is an embodied and phenomenological “social act” (Schechner 2003). Hence, performance also has an aesthetic dimension that engages the sensory and emotional experience of everyday life—but exacerbates it in its concentrated artistic form. This is why we choose this practice in schools: it brings everyday “real life” and creativity into the classroom rather than reproducing didactic schemes that disembody language and ignore its creative nature. The performative approach is not limited to theatre workshops. It articulates moments of workshop with moments of formalisation to work on the text, on the foreign language, and moments of reflexivity. The students explain what they have experienced and understood both from their own history and from what happened in the class or in the workshop between them. This reflexive aesthetic co-construction makes it possible to introduce informal forms of learning into the formal school setting (Eschenauer 2019b).
There is consequently no clear linear pattern for a performative approach in EFL teaching but rather a dynamic approach with some recurrent ingredients (warm-up, exploring spaces, working on the text, etc.), as we propose to schematise in Figure 2.
2.2. Participant: A Case Study
We choose here to present the case of Charles, aged 10 years and 6 months, who attends a mainstream school. He is attached to a reference class of his age group and benefits, according to his needs, from shared support time within a group of children with special needs (local unit for school inclusion, ULIS). He has been followed up in a medical-psychological-pedagogical centre for several years in speech therapy, psychomotricity and psychology. Charles has a neurodevelopmental disorder (NDD) characterised by a limitation in cognitive functioning and adaptation capacities. He has no language problems apart from a certain slowness of production. He is able to understand a variety of texts even though his fluency level is well below his age group. He can produce complex sentences and listen attentively to messages or instructions from an adult or a peer but has more difficulty in playing with the volume of his voice and posture, in role-playing and debating. He could benefit from shared English time in the ordinary class. In terms of cognitive skills, Charles has difficulty in learning situations that require planning and mental flexibility. His engagement in the activity is characterised by a certain slowness. On the other hand, he performs relatively well in tasks requiring sustained attention and memorisation. In view of his disorder, he has good adaptive behavioural skills. He needs a support worker to provide material assistance in situations that present a risk of cognitive overload, such as those requiring planning or mental flexibility.
2.3. Materials and Data Processing Procedure
Using a test-retest design, we measure the effects of a performative teaching approach on Charles’ communication skills, bearing in mind that oral communication relies on cognitive-emotional skills and creativity to develop oral production (through embodied language). Table 1 summarises the variables (and data collection tools) collected at the various stages of the study.
Table 1.
Pre-Test | during the Experiment | Post-Test | |
---|---|---|---|
- Executive functions (BRIEF, Gioia et al. 2013) - Cognitive-emotional skills (LEAS-C, Bajgar et al. 2005, semi-directive interviews with teachers) - Creativity (semi-directive interviews with teachers) - Oral production (audio-video recording with one camera from the language test in the classroom; semi-directive interviews with teachers). (See Section 3.4). |
- Cognitive-emotional skills, creativity and embodied Language (Audio-video recordings * during the 4 workshops with the actor: 8 sessions) | - Cognitive-emotional skills, creativity and oral production (BRIEF, LEAS-C, testimonials of the teachers + of the actor, semi-directive interviews with the teachers and with the participant) |
* We used 2 Canon XF105 cameras (A and B). The first one (A), static, was installed in a corner of the room. This camera was capturing wide shots. The second one, mobile, was shooting close-up, focused on the target child. Both cameras were synchronized. The microphone of Charles was wireless. All the audio streams were synchronized on both cameras, using a clap. These audio-video data help us to understand the links between the behaviour (language embodiment) and the oral production of the participants.
2.3.1. Executive Functions Assessment Procedure
We assessed the executive functions by scoring the teacher version of the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF, teacher version; Gioia et al. 2013). This inventory is an evaluation tool for ecological analysis of the executive functioning of children and adolescents through its repercussions in everyday school life.
In our study, we had to abandon the parents’ version, which posed the double difficulty of language (some parents have no or little command of the French language) and complexity (some parents themselves have cognitive disorders that prevent them from answering the questions). This questionnaire makes it possible to identify, in an ecological way, the executive functioning of children and adolescents.
This inventory is composed of 86 questions grouped into 8 scales, allowing the calculation of two indices: the behavioural regulation index and the metacognition index. The reliability indices indicate good internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranging from 0.80 to 0.97. Regarding construct validity, executive functions, assessed by the BRIEF, correlate with a set of other behavioural measures (Gioia et al. 2013).
Regarding the objective of the project, we focused our analyses on the three scales (inhibition, flexibility and emotional control) that allow us to calculate a behavioural regulation index score. To be precise, the scores obtained represent degradation scores, i.e., the lower the scores, the better the performances evaluated.
2.3.2. Cognitive-Emotional Skills Assessment Procedure
Cognitive-emotional skills are assessed through the Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale for Children, LEAS-C (Bajgar et al. 2005; Lane and Smith 2021), translated into French by the authors of the test themselves. This version has already shown its discriminatory qualities (Pasquier et al. 2022). It is a tool designed to assess the emotional awareness of children aged 8 and over. It consists of 12 scenarios, based on everyday social situations, each one involving two people, oneself and another person. Each participant is invited to reply, indicating what they and the other person could feel in each situation. (How would you feel? How would the other person feel?). The scenarios are based on four emotions (from the basic emotions described by Izard (1977): anger, fear, happiness and sadness. Each one is presented in three different situations in a mixed order. We decided to use an adapted version of the scale with only six scenarios selected from the 12 initially provided (Veirman et al. 2016). On the one hand, this allows the testing time to be reduced, and on the other hand, the adapted version also proves to be more reliable as only the most sensitive items are retained (Veirman et al. 2016). The scale’s rating is based on the complexity of the vocabulary related to emotions and their differentiation from each other by respondents. This questionnaire does not have a normative aim insofar as there are no expected “correct” responses, both from the point of view of form (grammatical and/or spelling errors) and substance. It is used to calculate three scores for each participant: an overall emotional awareness score (LEAS-T) and two subscores for intra-subjective emotional awareness (LEAS-S) and intersubjective emotional awareness (LEAS-O). The writing time does not exceed 20 min. If students have writing difficulties, they can tell an adult what they want to write.
We believe it is important to combine auto- and hetero-declarative assessment tools (LEAS-C, BRIEF) at pre- and post-test with behavioural observations (via video recordings). This allowed us to assess creativity through executive functions, cognitive-emotional scores, language embodiment and oral production.
2.3.3. Embodied Language: Body Involvement and Behaviour Coding Procedure
In order to better observe the links between executive functions, cognitive-emotional skills, creativity, and embodied language, we decided to code the participants’ language behaviour using the indicator of bodily engagement in the activity. We chose to code mainly sessions 1 (discovery, icebreaker) and 2 (first creative situations based on the children’s book Voices in the Park) and the final sessions (sessions 7: last improvisation and fixing of the stories invented by each group of pupils and 8: moment of visibility) of the theatre workshops to observe the evolution of the student’s body involvement.
Body involvement in the performative theatre activity was coded on Elan software (Sloetjes and Wittenburg 2008). A coding scheme of 4 labels was developed for this specific analysis: (1) “out of space”: when the participant leaves the space of the activity; (2) “in space but motionless”: when he is in the space of the activity but does not participate; (3) “in space with unexpected behaviour”: when he is in the space but acts differently from what is expected; and (4) “in space with expected behaviour”: when he is in the space and acts as expected.
Sessions 1, 2 and 7 were entirely coded by one coder, then Sessions 1 and 7 were counter-coded by another coder following Tellier’s method (Tellier 2014). Both codings were then compared (agreement reached 65%). The 35% of disagreement can be explained by the fact that the definition of label 3 (“in space with unexpected behaviour”) had to be clarified since it was sometimes misinterpreted by one of the coders. Every disagreement was discussed by both coders to determine which final label to adopt and reach total agreement. Our coding manual was also corrected for future data coding.
Finally, behavioural coding was also performed by triple-blind viewing of the videos. We focused our observation of Charles’ behaviour on two points: his behaviour in relation to himself and in relation to others. For his relationship with himself, we focused on the development of his spontaneity in creative activities (in the workshops with the actor). For the quality of individual behaviour, we defined three gradations: passive (accepts peer suggestions), semi-autonomous (indicates to a reference adult that he would like to make suggestions), and autonomous (makes spontaneous suggestions to peers, bodily and/or verbally). In terms of his relationship with others, we noted his tendency to collaborate and to construct the scenes by adapting his behaviour to the group’s proposals. Our observations were complemented by the teacher’s report on the evolution of Charles’ behaviour in class during the learning of English, which we coded using thematic discourse analysis. This coding allowed us to compare the evolution of the participant’s bodily involvement with the evolution of his oral production and creative proposals.
2.3.4. Oral Production Coding Procedure
To analyse the evolution of Charles’ language production, we conducted a qualitative study of the prosodic curve variation. We analysed the development of Charles’ oral competence by comparing his production in pre- and post-test foreign language. The evaluation criteria are based on the evolution of the prosodic production. We also quantified Charles’ EFL oral production in the pre- and post-test and analysed the degree of autonomy of his production. Prosody informs us elsewhere of the degree of awareness of emotional situations experienced or interpreted by the student. Prosody is an essential component of oral language because it plays an important role in the vocal communication of emotions that give meaning to speech (Petrone et al. 2016). Indeed, although the channels are different, the emotional perception of speech is directly related to the semantic processing of speech (Ben et al. 2016; Shakuf et al. 2022). Prosody facilitates the decoding of the semantic content of spoken language, as it can be used both for the recognition of the emotional content of speech (e.g., by increasing the speech rate or intonation of speech) and for linguistic processing (e.g., to disambiguate syntactic information).
Finally, we relied on the teacher’s testimony given at the end of the experimentation of the feasibility phase describing his academic evolution in English production, as well as the observation of audio-video recordings made during the drama sessions 1 (discovery, icebreaker), 2 (first creative situations based on the children’s book Voices in the Park) and 7 (last improvisation fixing the stories invented by each group of pupils). The EDA data can be used to complement the analysis performed by blind counter-coding. If the coding from the observation of the videos shows a change in attitude (body involvement) and a change in the use of English (see Figure 1 for the indicators: lexical richness and syntax accuracy; play with fluidity and rhythm of speech; word games; play with language forms, semantics and pragmatics; transformation of sentence’s models; humour, banter and joking) and/or in the prosody (tonal variation and accentuation), it is possible to go and check the electrodermal activity and observe whether indicators of strong emotions such as stress, joy, etc. appear too (peaks and valleys). An interview with the pupil about this moment in time provides the perceptual data necessary for the final interpretation.
2.3.5. Creativity
To study the development of the student’s creative skills, which is the subject of our case study, we first measure their evolution through the discourse analysis of the teacher’s and the actor’s testimonies after the experimentation, focusing on the linguistic aspects (body, verbal language) and on Charles’ scenic proposals (observed through the video recordings). The actor’s testimonial was written after having seen the videos from sessions 1, 2 and 7, 8. So she could remember what she observed directly during the sessions and indirectly analyse the videos.
In a second step, we triangulate all the data presented for this article and synthesise the results in a table. The indicators of Charles’ creative skills are taken from Fortin et al.’s (2014) work on assessing creative skills in dance as a performance art. Thus, this framework provides valuable indicators for analysing the development of Charles’ creative skills in theatre performance. Fortin and her colleagues propose 5 components to measure the development of the creative process in the performance arts. These 5 components are broken down into indicators, which we have used as an analysis tool to position all the data from the different collection tools on the markers of creativity. We complete with a 6th component relating to a verbal dimension specific to drama-based performance as we practice it in the experiment:
Quality of presence to oneself, to the world, to experience (physical, emotional and intellectual availability; imagination; listening, observation; acceptance of the new, of the unknown).
Quality of the exploration of postural-mimicry and gestural expression: performance (risk-taking by creating new worlds: ability of students to create and express themselves with the body; appropriation of instructions; commitment).
Quality of the organisation of time, space and materials for creation (anticipation of the whole project; taking initiatives).
Quality of collaboration with partners (ability of students to adapt their behaviour; contribution to teamwork; establishing a relationship climate; listening to the ideas of others).
Quality of reflection on one’s work and the group’s work (analysing one’s own work and that of others; verbalising opinions; consideration of comments and criticism; renewal of one’s representations and perceptions).
Quality of the formatting of the performance (quality of the verbal and bodily interpretation; selection of material: gestural, verbal, musical, etc.; consideration of technical limitations; harmonisation of the different components of the work).
We added:
Quality of verbal expression (risk-taking: ability of speakers to produce new language appropriate to the context, originality of the verbal language, i.e., new words or new combinations of words).
Fortin’s and colleagues’ definition of “competence” or creative “skills” meets ours insofar as it means “the mobilisation of resources to resolve situations in an optimal way”. For them, developing one’s creativity and competence in the field of performance arts means,
“on the one hand, developing personal resources that will specifically favour artistic creation and, on the other hand, developing one’s ability to mobilise these same resources in a complex way by engaging in a creative process and in the shaping of artistic and discursive productions” (p. 13).
2.4. Links between Cognitive-Emotional Skills and Creativity
The links between cognitive-emotional skills and creativity will be analysed by triangulating the data after detailing each of the measures. In our study, language is at the interface of emotions and creativity. We will therefore end the presentation of our results with a summary of the various indicators relating to cognitive-emotional skills and/or creativity. Taken together, these elements will enable us to draw conclusions about the development of oral communication skills (see Figure 1) in our case study.
3. Results
The results show an increase in Charles’s communication skills.
3.1. Executive Functions: BRIEF (Teacher Version)
Looking at the pre- and post-test data (Table 2), we can see that Charles’ scores have improved on all the indices. Concerning the behavioural regulation index, which includes three scales: inhibition, flexibility and emotional control, Charles shows more ability to adjust his behaviour in a school context. The change in raw scores on the indices between the pre-test and the post-test is greater than one standard deviation from the French data for boys aged 10 and 11. As Charles is 10 years and 6 months old, we have included both types of data (Table 3 and Table 4).
Table 2.
Pre-Test | Post-Test | |
---|---|---|
Inhibition | 22 | 16 |
Flexibility | 29 | 26 |
Emotional control | 24 | 17 |
Behavioural regulation index | 75 | 59 |
Table 3.
Mean | Standard Deviation | |
---|---|---|
Inhibition | 14.05 | 4.47 |
Flexibility | 12.46 | 3.00 |
Emotional control | 11.17 | 3.78 |
Behavioural regulation index | 37.68 | 9.75 |
Table 4.
Mean | Standard deviation | |
---|---|---|
Inhibition | 12.53 | 3.86 |
Flexibility | 12.08 | 2.87 |
Emotional control | 10.43 | 2.99 |
Behavioural regulation index | 35.08 | 8.39 |
As we can see in Table 2 on the Inhibition scale, there is a difference of 6 points between the pre- and post-test for Charles, which is more than one standard deviation for the 10 (Table 3) and 11 (Table 4) year-old boys. For the Flexibility Index, the difference between the two test conditions is 3 points, which is also one standard deviation. For the Emotional Control Index, the data show a difference of 7 points, well over one standard deviation. For the Behavioural Regulation Index, the difference between the two conditions is 16 points, or 2 standard deviations. The data, therefore, indicate that Charles made significant progress between the pre-test and the post-test.
Indeed, in view of the observations reported by his teacher, Charles seems to be more at ease in controlling his impulses and behaviours at the right moments in the face of constraints imposed by the teacher in a learning context (inhibition scale).
On the other hand, he was more frequently able to move freely from one situation to another according to the demands of the situation, without necessarily needing support from an adult, showing more flexible behaviour in the post-test situation (flexibility scale). He is also able to modulate his emotions more appropriately (emotional control scale).
These observations converge with the qualitative analyses and his results in the language class, which are improving both in terms of production and in terms of his behaviour when faced with the unknown. We measure these two elements through language tests on the one hand, and the study of the pupil’s bodily engagement on the other, as an indicator of the child’s ability to inhibit negative emotions (such as anger) that usually prevent him from interacting and carrying out activities.
3.2. Cognitive-Emotional Skills: LEAS-C
Charles’ pre- and post-test LEAS-C scores (Figure 3) show an improvement in inter-individual levels of emotional awareness after training.
In more detail, Charles’ pre-test LEAS-C scores show heterogeneity between intra- and inter-individual levels of emotional awareness. His intra-individual awareness level is average (12 points out of 24), while his inter-individual awareness is well below average (7 points out of 24). He obtained a total score of 12 points out of 30, allowing us to place him at a level of development where the sensory aspects of subjective experience still predominate and the beginning of the translation of the sensible into simple emotional language. It seems that his identification and language expression skills concern mainly personal emotions. The emotions of others are poorly identified and differentiated from internal experience. This clinical picture seems to be consistent with that usually encountered in children with NDDS. Indeed, he has difficulties processing social information, which would explain the difficulties usually found in the expression and recognition of emotional expressions in children with NDDS (see Baurain and Nader-Grosbois 2011 for more details on this subject).
Charles’ LEAS-C scores at the post-test still show heterogeneity, but this time in reverse. The level of intra-individual emotional awareness fell slightly below the average (10 points out of 24), indicating a slight drop in performance during this second phase of the test. However, we are talking here about performance and not ability, insofar as the test conditions were particular (the adult who usually helps him was not present) compared to the pre-test situation. This context could explain his difficulty in focusing on his feelings (which also impacted the total score level, which reached 15 points out of 30 on the post-test). However, his level of inter-individual emotional awareness exceeded the average (15 points out of 24) by doubling the score obtained in phase 1. This time Charles answers all the questions and uses a more precise vocabulary, even if it is a lexicon describing simple emotions. He also seems to be able to consider different emotions in the two protagonists of the same scenario. These results suggest that there was a clear progression in the ability to identify and express the emotions of others between the pre- and post-test LEAS-C. Again, our data are in line with the literature, which points to the need to work on emotional skills in a lively and interactive way in order to make the best progress for the child with an NDD (e.g., Baurain and Nader-Grosbois 2009).
3.3. Embodied Language: Body Involvement by Languaging
The participant’s involvement in the performance workshop evolves over time. The following chart (Figure 4) presents the duration of each behaviour (out of space, in space but motionless, in space with unexpected behaviour, in space with expected behaviour). In Session 1, the participant’s behaviour was coded for a duration of 2031 s; in Session 2, for 2331 s; and in Session 7, for a total duration of 2776 s. We thus present the descriptive statistics in percentage for a comparison between the 3 sessions.
We can see that, in Session 1, Charles spends more than 1/3 of the time out of the space of the activity. It can be moments when he stands out of the circle or even moments when he leaves the centre of the room completely to sit against a wall (see Figure 5a for an example). In Session 2, a large amount of the session is still spent out of the space of the activity (47.79%). This kind of behaviour is considerably reduced over time (only 8.47% in Session 7). His involvement in the performance activities changes too, as the chart shows. The most striking result concerns when he acts as expected (i.e., following the instructions and waiting for his turn): only 19.64% of the time in Session 1, then 38.81% in Session 2 and 54.77% in Session 7 (see Figure 5b for an example).
This analysis could be confirmed through the content analysis of the teacher’s testimonial, which explains the social and cognitive-emotional changes observed in the class after the experiment:
He is willing to work with other students (he develops social skills).
He even makes scenic proposals (he develops creativity), both in terms of postural and gestural play, in terms of language creativity.
And in terms of the emotional interpretation of the character being played (he develops emotional skills). He tells his classmate that he wants to perform his playlet with the anger he feels after a disappointment. He puts this emotion at a distance thanks to the theatrical expression and remains in the language activity.
3.4. Creativity
Charles’ creativity increased at the end of the experiment, at the same time as he became more involved in the activities. The cross-analysis of all the measures attempts to highlight the link between the different skills developed: cognitive-emotional, verbal, embodied and creative.
Table 5 represents a synthesis of all the analyses. To complete it, as mentioned in our methodology section, we triangulated the results of the tests with a thematic analysis of the discourse from the interviews with the teacher and the testimony of the actor. The “themes” of the analysis correspond to the 6 components of creativity skills (5 components described by Fortin et al. and the verbal component that we added). In this way, we can analyse and organise our data relating to cognitive (BRIEF analysis) and emotional (LEAS-C) skills, as well as linguistic skills (prosody in the pre- and post-language test + analysis of classroom results on language skills tests, described by the teacher in her testimony) and embodied language (study of Charles’ physical engagement using Elan software). The analysis through the creativity marker shows how drama potentially enabled Charles to progress in cognitive-emotional skills, oral production skills in English and creativity.
Table 5.
Creating Skills | |
1—Quality of presence to oneself, to the world, to experience (indicators: physical, emotional and intellectual availability; imagination; listening, observation; acceptance of the new, of the unknown). | |
Before training | After training |
Low quality of presence (a) Awareness of himself
|
Higher quality of presence (a) Awareness of himself
|
2—Quality of the exploration of the postural, mimicry and gestural expression (indicators: risk-taking by creating new worlds: ability of students to create and express themselves with the body; appropriation of instructions; commitment) | |
Before training | After training |
Low quality of exploration
|
Higher quality of exploration
|
3—Quality of the organisation of time, space and materials for creation (indicators: anticipation of the whole project; taking initiatives) | |
Before training | After training |
Low quality of organisation His material organisation’s score by the BRIEF test is low (83 points). |
Low quality of organisation Despite a very slight improvement in the score (79 points: reversed scores), the material organisation’s score from Charles did not change significantly. We do not have sufficient data to position Charles’ behaviour on this indicator. At this stage, it seems that Charles is not more able to anticipate future events, to plan ahead, to put in place appropriate measures in advance to carry out an action or task, or to carry out tasks in a more systematic way. |
4—Quality of collaboration with partners (indicators: ability of students to adapt their behaviour; contribution to teamwork; establishing a relationship climate; listening to the ideas of others) | |
Before training | After training |
Low quality of collaboration
|
Higher quality of collaboration
|
5—Quality of reflection on one’s work and the group’s work (indicators: analysing one’s own work and that of others; verbalising opinions; consideration of comments and criticism; renewal of one’s representations and perceptions) | |
Before training | After training |
Low quality of reflection
|
Higher quality of reflection
|
6—Quality of the formatting of the work (indicators: quality of the role play; interpretation) | |
Before training | After training |
Low quality of formatting
|
Higher quality of formatting
|
7—Quality of the verbal expression (indicators: ability of speakers to produce new language appropriate to the context, originality of the verbal language, i.e., new words or new combination of words) | |
Before training | After training |
Low quality of verbal expression
|
Higher quality of verbal expression
|
Nota bene: The bold highlights in the textual quotations correspond to the coded elements of the corpus, which allowed us to sort them into the categories corresponding to the 6 observables of creativity. This coding was carried out in a double-blind setting.
3.5. Emotions and Creativity
The following Table 6 (Table summarises the main results concerning the development of Charles’ cognitive-emotional skills (BRIEF and LEAS-C) in relation to the development of his creativity skills (Fortin et al.’s framework). The three most important indicators of the 6 creativity indicators analysed previously (see Section 3.4) are shown here. Finally, this table aims to show the role of cognitive-emotional skills and creativity in Charles’ ability to express himself in a modern foreign language.
Table 6.
Emotions | Creativity | |
---|---|---|
Cognition |
|
|
Behaviour |
|
|
Language (EFL) |
|
4. Discussion and Conclusions
Our main aim was to explore how a performative theatre tool could develop communication skills in a student with NDDS through creativity and cognitive-emotional skills. Analysis of Charles’ pre- and post-test scores shows a clear progression in cognitive-emotional skills (executive functions and emotional awareness of self and especially others), supported by greater involvement in the performance workshop. This involvement, particularly bodily involvement, in the language produced by Charles is visible through the analysis of behavioural indicators during the performance workshop. Charles’ oral EFL production in class also increased following the implementation of the performative approach. And the intersection of all these data allowed us to make a qualitative analysis of Charles’ creativity. According to the case study here, creativity is a skill developed through performative theatre, and at the same time, it participates in the development of other skills (cognitive-emotional, including verbal skills). This finding is consistent with the work of other researchers who have shown the benefits of drama-based approaches in education for developing various skills related to creativity (Celume et al. 2019).
Once again, we are led to believe that an oral language teaching tool in a natural context of exchange around emotions is effective in developing emotional and language skills in a school context (see Eschenauer 2014, 2019b; Pasquier et al. 2022).
Regarding the use of theatre, the data presented here seem to be in line with previous studies that have shown the effectiveness of drama methods in the development of a second language (Galante and Thomson 2017), communication skills (Gabitova et al. 2018), and the positive effects of a theatre performance on emotional literacy and its link to the development of students’ creativity and EFL skills (Aden and Eschenauer 2020; Eschenauer 2014; Eschenauer and Voise 2017).
More specifically, the case study seems to show that a performative theatrical approach is an interesting modality for the development of socio-emotional and linguistic skills through the initiated and also developed creativity process. Indeed, in the case of Charles, we are led to believe that this performative theatre approach in a school context allowed him to experience a variety of emotions in socially interactive situations for learning EFL. These situations contributed to enriching his understanding of emotions (emotional competence) while promoting a high level of arousal and better regulation of his emotions (greater commitment to activities despite strong emotions such as anger. It could mean that the training may have led to better regulation of emotional processes), leading him to engage in creative activities (Adaman and Blaney 1995). Indeed, the approach does not seem to have had the effect of crossing the threshold of tolerance for too high a level of arousal, which would have made the experience more vulnerable than creative. The framework of the school approach seems to have provided sufficient stabilising and containing aspects to channel the creativity vulnerability factors (Lubart et al. 2015) and allow Charles’ creative communicative production to develop.
Furthermore, Charles’ engagement in creative activities seems to have helped him to internalise the foreign language with which, after the experiment, he allowed himself to play and even generate humorous situations. Before the experiment, Charles reproduced linguistic patterns, but the teachers had never witnessed his independent use of language, let alone the production of humour in English.
This engagement was supported by executive processes such as emotional flexibility, which, as Lubart et al. (2015, p. 37) point out, “is implicated in creativity because it reflects the mobility and flexibility of thinking and the ability and willingness to change registers”. These registers can be linguistic, bodily, emotional or cognitive. In this sense, emotional flexibility (as a “hot” basic executive process) operates in contexts with an affective, emotional and/or motivational component, as defined by Zelazo et al. (2005). Emotional flexibility and creativity could be the engines of communication, especially in foreign languages where it is necessary to change linguistic and cultural referents.
“Everyone has the potential to be creative, but not everyone fulfils that potential” (Runco 2014, p. 39). Charles seems to have been able to do this.
However, at this stage of the study, it is not possible to conclude that the theatrical approach alone could be the source of all of Charles’ progress. Indeed, every child in inclusion progresses because of his inclusion, as shown by various research studies (Gray et al. 2021), and every so-called “ordinary” child also progresses. Nevertheless, the very nature of training through drama performance suggests that there is a positive training effect.
From a methodological point of view, it is also necessary to mention the limitations of the case study.
First of all, as far as the interpretation of the data is concerned, we have, on the one hand, proceeded to a qualitative treatment of the data, which usually supports a quantitative treatment (BRIEF, LEAS-C). It seems that the productions obtained (answers written by the participant for the LEAS-C or by the teacher for the BRIEF) allowed this finer and more specific processing inherent to the case study. On the other hand, we decided to rely on the skill repositories to triangulate the previous data with those from the videos and interviews with double-blind analyses in order to avoid interpretation bias by proceeding with a meticulous description of the specificity of Charles’ behaviours in the situation. In this respect, Fortin’s Creative Skills Development Analysis Tool was very useful, as it lists relevant indicators of creativity for artistic performance.
However, despite our rigour, we believe that it is necessary in the future to complement our measurement of creativity with the use of a tool such as the EPoC (Evaluation of Children’s Creative Potential) test, which assesses divergent-exploratory and convergent-integrative thinking in the graphic and verbal domains (Besançon et al. 2011), as creative processes can vary not only between children but also within children depending on the domains considered (Barbot et al. 2017). In addition, the results show the importance of taking into account emotional prosody as a marker of the meaning given to speech. To this end, a prosodic reception test and the development of an adapted verbal production test to measure more precisely the development of the participants’ language skills are to be implemented for the next phase. We also believe that it is necessary to test the effect of the performative approach on a larger sample. Therefore, we have started the second pilot phase of the CELAVIE study, with the same objective, according to a test-re-test research design, this time with an experimental group practicing performance theatre (n = 23) and a control group (n = 24) of children of the same age group and from the same city, attending school in a neighbourhood, and practicing English through table games.
In conclusion, this first phase of the CELAVIE research was very useful in that it allowed us to verify the feasibility of the study. The case study with Charles allowed us to refine the methodology of data collection and analysis in order to answer our research questions in the most controlled way possible, despite our desire to conduct the study in an environmentally friendly way so as not to take the students out of their daily formal learning environment. Despite the few limitations identified at this stage of the study, the initial results are promising and encourage us to extend our research with the necessary adaptations.
Acknowledgments
We would also like to warmly thank the coordinators of the réseau Sainte Victoire Linda Ben Jemaa and Clémentine Tavant, the IEN Rachel Eyssautier and Magali Lahondes and the educational coordinator Eric Delgiovine, the director of the elementary school, the teacher, the parents and the children for their active participation in this collaborative research project. Last but not least: we warmly have to thank the speech experimentation centre CEP (Centre d’expérimentation de la parole), which actively supports the project from a logistical, material and human point of view, in particular Carine André (audio-video recordings and editing), Sabrina Kadri (audio-video recordings) and Alain Ghio (sound recording, audio-video recording design).
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, S.E.; methodology, S.E., R.T., T.L., M.T., A.T. and A.P.; software, T.L. and C.A.; validation, S.E., R.T., T.L., M.T., A.T., I.B. and A.P.; formal analysis, S.E., R.T., T.L., M.T., A.T., I.B. and A.P.; investigation, S.E., R.T., T.L., M.T., I.B., C.A. and A.P.; resources, S.E., R.T., T.L., M.T., C.A. and A.P.; data curation, S.E., R.T., T.L., C.A., I.B. and A.P.; writing—original draft preparation, S.E.; writing—review and editing, S.E., R.T., T.L., M.T., A.T., I.B. and A.P.; visualization, S.E.; supervision, S.E.; project administration, S.E.; funding acquisition, S.E., R.T. and A.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of COMITE DE PROTECTION DES PERSONNES Sud Est V (protocol code N° ID RCB: 2021-A01111-40. Date of approval: 29 June 2021. Contact with the class was established via the national educational institution (IEN). Although the pathological dimension was ruled out in this preliminary study, the use of physiological variables (HR, EDA) raised questions that placed the research at the intersection of Humanities and Biological studies.
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all parents/legal representatives and participants involved in the study. Written informed consent was obtained from parents/legal guardians and the participant to publish this article.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Funding Statement
This work was carried out within the pilot centre Ampiric, funded by the French State’s Future Investment Program (PIA3/France Relance) as part of the “Territories of Educational Innovation” action. It was also supported by the Institute of Creativity and Innovation from Aix-Marseille University (InCIAM); the Institute of Language, Communication and the Brain (ILCB), and the Structure Fédérative d’Études et de Recherches en Éducation de Provence (SFERE-Provence, FED4238). The CELAVIE team thanks these Institutions for their support.
Footnotes
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
References
- Abdelkader Yamna, Bazile Sandrine, Fertat Omar. Pour un Théâtre-Monde: Plurilinguisme, interculturalité, transmission. Presses Universitaires de Bordeaux; Bordeaux: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Abele-Brehm Andrea. Positive and negative mood influences on creativity: Evidence for asymmetrical effects. Polish Psychological Bulletin. 1992;23:203–21. [Google Scholar]
- Adaman Jill E., Blaney Paul H. The Effects of Musical Mood Induction on Creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior. 1995;29:95–108. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1995.tb00739.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Aden Joëlle. La créativité artistique à l’école: Refonder l’acte d’apprendre. Synergies Europe. 2009;4:173–80. [Google Scholar]
- Aden Joëlle, Lovelace Kester. 3, 2, 1… Action!: Le Drama Pour Apprendre L’anglais au Cycle 3. Canopé—CRDP de Créteil; Champigny-sur-Marne: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Aden Joëlle, Eschenauer Sandrine. Translanguaging: An Enactive-Performative Approach to Language Education. In: Moore Emilee, Bradley Jessica, Simpson James., editors. Translanguaging as Transformation: The Collaborative Construction of New Linguistic Realities. Multilingual Matters; Bristol: 2020. pp. 102–17. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Amonkar Nidhi, Su Wan-Chun, Bhat Anjana N., Srinivasan Sudha M. Effects of Creative Movement Therapies on Social Communication, Behavioral-Affective, Sensorimotor, Cognitive, and Functional Participation Skills of Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2021;12:722874. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.722874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andréoléty Léa. La pratique du théâtre avec un public atteint de déficience intellectuelle. Université Stendhal Grenoble 3; Sciences de l’Homme et Société: 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Bajgar Jane, Ciarrochi Joseph, Lane Richard, Deane Franck P. Development of the Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale for Children (LEAS-C) British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2005;23:569–86. doi: 10.1348/026151005X35417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbot Baptiste, Besançon Maud, Lubart Todd. Creativity and Creative Pedagogies in the Early and Primary Years. Routledge; London: 2017. Creative potential in educational settings: Its nature, measure, and nurture; pp. 12–22. [Google Scholar]
- Bardin Laurence. L’analyse de Contenu. 2nd ed. Presses Universitaires de France; Paris: 2013. Quadrige. First published 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Baurain Céline, Nader-Grosbois Nathalie. Évaluer la régulation émotionnelle, la résolution de problèmes socio-émotionnels et les compétences sociales d’enfants présentant une déficience intellectuelle: Études de cas. [Assess the emotional regulation, the strategies of socio-emotional problems-solving and the social competence of children with intellectual disability: Case study.] Revue Francophone de la Déficience Intellectuelle. 2009;20:123–47. [Google Scholar]
- Baurain Céline, Nader-Grosbois Nathalie. La théorie de l’esprit. 1st ed. De Boeck Supérieur; Louvain-la-Neuve: 2011. Compétences émotionnelles, régulation émotionnelle en contextes interactifs et ToM-émotions chez des enfants typiques et avec déficience intellectuelle; pp. 213–40. Questions de personne. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Beck Luna, Kumschick Irina R., Eid Michael, Klann-Delius Gisela. Relationship between language competence and emotional competence in middle childhood. Emotion. 2012;12:503–14. doi: 10.1037/a0026320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell Nancy. Comparing Playful and Nonplayful Incidental Attention to Form. Language Learning. 2012;62:236–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00630.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ben David Boaz M., Multani Namita, Shakuf Vered, Rudzicz Frank, Lieshout Pascal H. H. M. van. Prosody and Semantics Are Separate but Not Separable Channels in the Perception of Emotional Speech: Test for Rating of Emotions in Speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2016;59:72–89. doi: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-H-14-0323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Besançon Maud, Lubart Todd. La créativité de l’enfant. Évaluation et développement. Mardaga; Wavre: 2015. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)]. PSY-Évaluation, Mesure, Diagnostic. Available online: https://www.cairn.info/la-creativite-de-l-enfant--9782804701987.htm. [Google Scholar]
- Besançon Maud, Barbot Baptiste, Lubart Todd. Évolution de l’évaluation de la créativité chez l’enfant de Binet à nos jours. Recherches & éducations. 2011;5:215–26. doi: 10.4000/rechercheseducations.840. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnardel Nathalie, Girandola Fabien, Bonetto Eric, Lubart Todd. La créativité en situations: Théories et applications. Dunod; Malakoff: 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Capron Puozzo Isabelle. Créativité et apprentissage: Dilemme et harmonie. Revue française de pédagogie. 2016;197:5–12. doi: 10.4000/rfp.5130. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Capron Puozzo Isabelle, Piccardo Enrica. L’émotion et l’apprentissage des langues. Revue de linguistique et de didactique des langues—Lidil. 2013;48:3305. doi: 10.4000/lidil.3305. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Care Jean-Marc, Debyser Francis. Jeu, langage et créativité. Hachette Français Langue Etrangère; Paris: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Carter Ronald, McCarthy Michael. Talking, Creating: Interactional Language, Creativity, and Context. Applied Linguistics. 2004;25:62–88. doi: 10.1093/applin/25.1.62. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Celume Macarena-Paz, Besançon Maud, Zenasni Franck. Fostering Children and Adolescents’ Creative Thinking in Education. Theoretical Model of Drama Pedagogy Training. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)];Frontiers in Psychology. 2019 9:2611. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02611. Available online: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conseil national d’évaluation du système scolaire [CNESCO] Quelle école pour les élèves en situation de handicap? Dossier de synthèse. 2016. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)]. Available online: http://www.cnesco.fr/fr/dossier-handicap/
- Cook Angelle. A post-intentional phenomenological investigation of inclusivity in a therapeutic theatre production. Drama Therapy Review. 2020;7:169–91. doi: 10.1386/dtr_00072_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cook Guy. Language Play, Language Learning. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Corbett Blythe A., Ioannou Sara, Key Alexandra P., Coke Catherine, Muscatello Rachael, Vandekar Simon, Muse Ian. Treatment Effects in Social Cognition and Behavior Following a Theater-Based Intervention for Youth with Autism. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2019;44:481–94. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2019.1676244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dauphin Laurence. In: Élèves en situation de handicap. 2nd ed. Rosenwald Fabienne., editor. Direction de l’évaluation, de la prospective et de la performance [DEPP]/ministère de l’Éducation nationale, de la Jeunesse et des Sports; Paris: 2022. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)]. Available online: https://www.education.gouv.fr/synthese-de-la-depp-105296. [Google Scholar]
- Dawoud Laila K. A., Hasim Zuwati, Saad Mohd R. M. Creative Drama in Teaching English Language: A Systematic Literature Review. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)];PalArch’s Journal of Archaeology of Egypt/Egyptology. 2020 17:11752–68. Available online: https://archives.palarch.nl/index.php/jae/article/view/4568. [Google Scholar]
- Drill Rochelle B., Bellini Scott. Combining Readers Theater, Story Mapping and Video Self-Modeling Interventions to Improve Narrative Reading Comprehension in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2022;52:1–15. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-04908-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eschenauer Sandrine. Performing Arts in Language Learning. Edizioni Novacultur; Roma: 2014. Combining foreign, native and national Languages through Theatre in Schools: An ‘enactive Approach’ to Education; pp. 65–71. [Google Scholar]
- Eschenauer Sandrine. Conseil National d’Evaluation du Système scolaire. CNESCO; Paris: 2019a. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)]. Pourquoi et en quoi la créativité a-t-elle sa place en classe de langue? pp. 1–17. Available online: http://www.cnesco.fr/wp-content/uploads/2019/04/CCLV_ESCHENAUER_MEF-v2.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Eschenauer Sandrine. Translanguaging and Empathy: Effects of performative approach to language learning. In: Fleiner Olivier Mentz et Micha., editor. The Arts in Language Teaching. International Perspectives: Performative—Aesthetic—Transversal. LIT Verlag; Berlin, Vienna, Zürich and London: 2019b. pp. 231–60. [Google Scholar]
- Eschenauer Sandrine, Voise Anne-Marie. De l’école au collège, quelles continuités dans l’enseignement-apprentissage des langues? Enjeux, faisabilité, perspectives. Les Langues Modernes; Paris: 2017. Partager ses langues et ses cultures en Cycle 3. APLV, 34–43. Les Langues Modernes, numéro 2-2017. [Google Scholar]
- Eschenauer Sandrine, Rollinat-Levasseur Eve-Marie, Clark Steven, Aden Joëlle. La recherche sur les pratiques théâtrales en éducation en France: Contextes et enjeux. Journal de recherche en éducations artistiques (JREA) 2023;1:55–70. doi: 10.26034/vd.jrea.2023.3585. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fortin Sylvie, Trudelle Sylvie, Gosselin Pierre, St-Denis Elaine, Murphy Serge, Gagnon-Bourget Francine. Référentiel pour le développement et l’évaluation de la compétence à créer en danse au collège et à l’université. Québec: UQAM. 2014. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)]. Available online: https://competenceacreer.uqam.ca/upload/files/referentiel_danse.pdf.
- Gabitova Linara, Shayakhmetova Leysan, Beisembayeva Zhanargul. The Effectiveness of Drama Methods in the Development of Communication Skills. Revista Publicando. 2018;5:308–15. [Google Scholar]
- Galante Angelica, Thomson Ron I. The Effectiveness of Drama as an Instructional Approach for the Development of Second Language Oral Fluency, Comprehensibility, and Accentedness. TESOL Quarterly. 2017;51:115–42. doi: 10.1002/tesq.290. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia Gerard A., Isquith Peter K., Guy S. C., Kenworthy Lauren. In: BRIEF: Inventaire d’Évaluation Comportementale des Fonctions Exécutives. Roy Arnaud, Fournet Nathalie, Legall Didier, Roulin Jean-Luc., translators. PAR/Hogrefe; Paris: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Gnezda Nicole M. Cognition and Emotions in the Creative Process. Art Education. 2011;64:47–52. doi: 10.1080/00043125.2011.11519111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gray Peter, Norwich Brahm, Webster Rob. Review of Research about the Effects of Inclusive Education: A Summary. SEN Policy Research Forum. 2021. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)]. Available online: https://senpolicyresearchforum.co.uk/wp-content/uploads/Review-of-inclusion-effects-research-final-Feb-21-.pdf.
- Hainselin Mathieu, Aubry Alexandre, Bourdin Béatrice. Improving Teenagers’ Divergent Thinking with Improvisational Theater. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)];Frontiers in Psychology. 2018 9:1759. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01759. Available online: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ioannou Sara, Key Alexandra P., Muscatello Rachael A., Klemencic Mark, Corbett Blythe A. Peer Actors and Theater Techniques Play Pivotal Roles in Improving Social Play and Anxiety for Children with Autism. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11:908. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izard Carroll E. Human Emotions. Plenum Press; New York: 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Jones Rodney H. Creativity in Language Teaching. Routledge; London: 2015. Creativity and Language. [Google Scholar]
- Kramsch Claire. The Multilingual Subject. 1st ed. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Kumar S. Praveen, Raja B. William Dharma. Enhancing the Performance of Learning-Disabled Children by Dint of Theatre Education. Journal on School Educational Technology. 2009;5:8–14. doi: 10.26634/jsch.5.2.1026. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lane Richard D., Schwartz Gary E. Levels of Emotional Awareness: A Cognitive-Developmental Theory and Its Application to Psychopathology. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1987;144:133–43. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.2.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane Richard D., Smith Ryan. Levels of Emotional Awareness: Theory and Measurement of a Socio-Emotional Skill. Journal of Intelligence. 2021;9:42. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence9030042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubart Todd, Mouchiroud Christophe, Tordjman Sylvie, Zenasni Franck. Psychologie de la créativité. 2nd ed. Armand Colin; Paris: 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Mayer John D., Salovey Peter. What is emotional intelligence? In: Salovey P., Sluyter D. J., editors. Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Educational Implications. Harper Collins; New York: 1997. pp. 3–34. [Google Scholar]
- Mayring Philipp. Qualitative Inhaltsanalyse: Grundlagen und Techniken. Beltz; Weinheim: 2010. Neuausgabe, 11. [Google Scholar]
- Ministère de l’éducation nationale et de la jeunesse Loi n° 2005–102 du 11 février 2005 pour l’égalité des droits et des chances, la participation et la citoyenneté des personnes handicapées. JORF n°36 du 12 février 2005. 2005. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)]. Available online: https://www.legifrance.gouv.fr/
- Ministère de l’éducation nationale et de la jeunesse Loi n°2013–595 du 8 juillet 2013 d’orientation et de programmation pour la refondation de l’École de la République. JORF n°0157 du 9 juillet 2013. 2013. [(accessed on 30 June 2023)]. Available online: https://www.legifrance.gouv.fr/
- Monette Sébastien, Bigras Marc. La mesure des fonctions exécutives chez les enfants d’âge préscolaire. [The measurement of the executive functions in pre-school aged children.] Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne. 2008;49:323–41. doi: 10.1037/a0014000. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mpella Maria, Evaggelinou Christina, Koidou Eirini, Tsigilis Nikolaos. The Effects of a Theatrical Play Programme on Social Skills Development for Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. International Journal of Special Education. 2019;33:828–45. [Google Scholar]
- Noe Alva. Strange Tools: Art and Human Nature. Farrar, Straus and Giroux; New York: 2016. Reprint. [Google Scholar]
- Pasquier Aurélie, Bonnet Agnès, Pedinielli Jean-Louis. Fonctionnement cognitivo-émotionnel: Le rôle de l’intensité émotionnelle chez les individus anxieux. Annales Medico-psychologiques. 2009;167:649–56. doi: 10.1016/j.amp.2007.11.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pasquier Aurélie, Ponthieu Guillaume, Papon Lola, Rezzi Nathalie. Working on the identification and expression of emotions in primary schools for better oral production: An exploratory study of links between emotional and language skills. Education 3–13. 2022;50:1107–20. doi: 10.1080/03004279.2021.1938167. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Petrone Caterina, Carbone Francesca, Champagne-Lavau Maud. Effects of Emotional Prosody on Skin Conductance Responses in French. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody. 2016;2016:425–29. doi: 10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-87. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pons Francisco, Lawson Joanne, Harris Paul L., Rosnay Marc de. Individual differences in children’s emotion understanding: Effects of age and language. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2003;44:347–53. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards Jack C. Creativity in Language Teaching. Iranian Journal of Language Teaching Research. 2013;1:19–43. [Google Scholar]
- Roy Arnaud. “Approche neuropsychologique des fonctions exécutives de l’enfant”: État des lieu et éléments de prospective. Revue de Neuropsychologie. 2015;7:245–56. doi: 10.3917/rne.074.0245. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Runco Mark A. Creativity: Theories and Themes: Research, Development, and Practice. 2nd ed. Academic Press Inc; Amsterdam: 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Saint Martin (de) Claire, Astier Marie. Le théâtre pour éveiller un regard inclusif. Revue francophone de la déficience intellectuelle. 2021;31:14–23. doi: 10.7202/1085198ar. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sambanis Michaela. Fremdsprachenunterricht und Neurowissenschaften. 1st ed. Narr Dr. Gunter; Tübingen: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Sander David, Scherer Klaus. Traité de psychologie des émotions. Dunod; Malakoff: 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Schechner Richard. Performance Theory. 1st ed. Routledge; London: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Schewe Manfred. Fremdsprache inszenieren: Zur Fundierung einer dramapädagogischen Lehr- und Lernpraxis. BIS-Verlag; Oldenburg: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Shakuf Vered, Ben-David Boaz, Wegner Thomas G. G., Wesseling Patricia B. C., Mentzel Maya, Defren Sabrina, Allen Shanley E. M., Lachmann Thomas. Processing Emotional Prosody in a Foreign Language: The Case of German and Hebrew. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science. 2022;6:251–68. doi: 10.1007/s41809-022-00107-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloetjes Han, Wittenburg Peter. Annotation by Category—ELAN and ISO DCR; Paper presented at 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2008); Marrakech, Morocco. May 28–30; 2008. pp. 816–20. [Google Scholar]
- Tellier Marion. Quelques orientations méthodologiques pour étudier la gestuelle dans des corpus spontanés et semi-contrôlés. Discours. 2014;15 doi: 10.4000/discours.8917. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thienkalaya Chanukrit, Chusanachoti Ruedeerath. Improving the English Reading Prosody of L2 Learners Through Readers Theater. LEARN Journal: Language Education and Acquisition Research Network. 2020;13:306–20. [Google Scholar]
- United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization [UNESCO] Salamanca statement and Framework for Action on Special Needs Education. Unesco; Paris: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Varela Francisco. Autonomie et connaissance: Essai sur le vivant. Seuil; Paris: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Varela Francisco. Neurophenomenology. A Methodological Remedy for the Hard Problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 1996;3:330–49. [Google Scholar]
- Varela Francisco. In: Le cercle créateur: Écrits (1976–2001) Bitbol Michel., editor. Éditions du Seuil; Paris: 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Veirman Elke, Fontaine Johnny R. J., Ryckeghem Dimitri M. L. Van. A componential emotion approach for the assessment of emotional awareness in youth. Psychological Assessment. 2016;28:1416–26. doi: 10.1037/pas0000261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zelazo Philip D., Qu Li, Müller Ulrich. Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development. In: Schneider W., Schumann-Hengsteler R., Sodian B., editors. Young Children’s Cognitive Development: Interrelationships among Executive Functioning, Working Memory, Verbal Ability, and Theory of Mind. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; Pittsburgh: 2005. pp. 71–93. [Google Scholar]
- Zenasni Franck, Lubart Todd I. Emotion-Related Traits Moderate the Impact of Emotional State on Creative Performances. Journal of Individual Differences. 2008;29:157–67. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001.29.3.157. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.