Abstract
Neurodevelopmental disorders have been associated with genetic mutations that affect cellular function, including chromatin regulation and epigenetic modifications. Recent studies in humans have identified mutations in KMT2C, an enzyme responsible for modifying histone tails and depositing H3K4me1 and H3K4me3, as being associated with Kleefstra syndrome 2 and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, the precise role of KMT2C mutations in brain disorders remains poorly understood. Here we employed CRISPR/Cas9 gene editing to analyze the effects of KMT2C brain specific knockout on animal behavior. Knocking out KMT2C expression in cortical neurons and the mouse brain resulted in decreased KMT2C levels. Importantly, KMT2C brain specific knockout animals exhibited repetitive behaviors, social deficits, and intellectual disability resembling ASD. Our findings shed light on the involvement of KMT2C in neurodevelopmental processes and establish a valuable model for elucidating the cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying KMT2C mutations and their relationship to Kleefstra syndrome 2 and ASD.
Keywords: ASD, KMT2C, epigenetics, CRISPR/Cas9, behavior
Introduction
Neurodevelopmental disorders are characterized by impairments in brain development that consequently affect behavior, social interactions, communication, cognitive function, and learning abilities, having a significant impact on the life of individuals. Among neurodevelopmental disorders is Kleefstra syndrome 2 that is characterized by intellectual disability, facial dysmorphisms, and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Koemans et al., 2017). ASD is a highly variable condition characterized by deficits in social interactions and communication, repetitive behaviors, and restricted interests, which may be associated with comorbid psychiatric, neurological, physical, and/or intellectual disabilities (Lord et al., 2020). Genetic mutations are a prominent factor contributing to the development of ASD. These mutations can lead to the loss of function in a diverse array of genes, resulting in disrupted gene function through mechanisms such as alterations in the reading frame or the creation of premature termination codons (Iossifov et al., 2012; O’Roak et al., 2012; Rubeis et al., 2014; Satterstrom et al., 2020; Tuncay et al., 2022). According to the SFARI GENE database, to date, there are more than 1,392 genes associated to ASD that are distributed in all human chromosomes (Banerjee-Basu and Packer, 2010). These genes have many different functions in the cell including neuronal communication, cytoskeleton formation and dynamics, and chromatin regulation and control of gene expression (Iossifov et al., 2012; Rubeis et al., 2014; Stessman et al., 2017; Grove et al., 2019; Ruzzo et al., 2019; Satterstrom et al., 2020). Among chromatin regulating genes, studies using transgenic animals knock-out (KO) for the epigenetic enzymes Chromodomain helicase DNA binding protein 8 (CHD8), Lysine demethylase 6A (KDM6A) or Lysine demethylase 6B (KDM6B) show complex behavioral phenotypes that resemble what is observed in humans carrying the mutations in them (Platt et al., 2017; Tang et al., 2017; Gao et al., 2022), establishing model systems to study molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying ASD.
Among the enzymes that control gene expression by chromatin remodeling, and more specifically by altering histone tail modifications is Lysine methyltransferase 2C (KMT2C) also known as MLL3 (Koemans et al., 2017; Eshraghi et al., 2018; Faundes et al., 2018; Satterstrom et al., 2020). KMT2C is a histone lysine methyltransferase enzyme that deposits the H3K4me1 mark, associated with active enhancers, or the H3K4me3 mark related to transcriptionally active regions (Ruthenburg et al., 2007; Jozwik et al., 2016). KMT2C belongs to the complex proteins associated with Set1 (COMPASS complex) that contributes to essential functions in eukaryotic developmental signaling pathways (Fagan and Dingwall, 2019; Lavery et al., 2020; Cenik and Shilatifard, 2021). Using fruit flies it has been shown that KMT2C binds to promoter regions of genes involved in neuronal processes, and its loss of function produce severe deficits in memory formation (Koemans et al., 2017). In humans, mutations in KMT2C have been found in individuals with intellectual disabilities including Kleefstra syndrome 2 and ASD (Iossifov et al., 2012; Stessman et al., 2017; Lavery et al., 2020; Satterstrom et al., 2020; Dhaliwal et al., 2021; Siano et al., 2022; Tuncay et al., 2022; Zhou et al., 2022). Interestingly only eleven patients have been described with mutations in KMT2C (Siano et al., 2022), with only one long term report showing long lasting phenotypes (Wu and Li, 2022). Thus, phenotypes associated to KMT2C mutants have not been fully characterized.
In this study, we utilized the CRISPR/Cas9 system to generate KMT2C KO models in both cultured cells and mice to investigate the phenotypic consequences of KMT2C loss of function. Using adeno-associated viruses (AAV) as delivery vectors we targeted exon 3 of KMT2C to produce the KO of the gene. As expected, KMT2C KO produced a decrease in the abundance of H3K4me1 and H3K4me3 histone tail marks in cultured neurons. In KMT2C KO mice, we conducted a series of behavioral tests and observed deficits in social interaction, absence of anxiety-like behavior, increased repetitive behaviors, and significant impairments in memory formation.
These phenotypic changes are relevant to the human conditions of ASD and Kleefstra Syndrome 2, both of which are associated with neurodevelopmental disorders that can be attributed to the loss of function of KMT2C. Our findings demonstrate the utility of the CRISPR/Cas9 technology to generate an animal model of KMT2C brain specific KO that can be used to investigate the cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying the pathogenesis of KMT2C knockout-mediated disorders.
Materials and methods
Primary neuronal cultures
Postnatal day 0 Cas9 KI mice (C57BL/6J; JAX 026179) were euthanized by decapitation and the whole brain was extracted in ice cold Ca2+/Mg2+-free Hank’s balanced salt solution (HBSS). Meninges were removed, the tissue was minced and incubated with Papain (20 U) for 15 min at 37°C. Cells were rinsed twice with HBSS, resuspended by mechanical agitation through fire-polished glass Pasteur pipettes of decreasing diameters, and plated over poly-L-lysine-coated culture plates or cover slips. Cultures were maintained at 37°C in 5% CO2 in growth media [Neurobasal-A (Life technologies 1088802) supplemented with B27 (Life technologies 17504044), 2 mM L-glutamine (Life technologies 25030-081), 100 U/ml penicillin/streptomycin (Life technologies 15070-063)]. Half of the media was replaced every 3 days. Neuronal cultures were transduced at 3 days in vitro (DIV) using concentrated AAV particles.
Plasmids
For the expression of CRISPR/Cas9, plasmids were generated in AAV backbones. For sgRNAs 20nt target sequences were selected contiguous to a 5′-NGG photospacer-adjacent motif (PAM). sgRNAs for KMT2C were designed against exon 3 (K1: 5′ GGAAATCAAAGAACAATCTG 3′; K2: 5′ GGAGGATGCTGAAACAGAAG 3′) and cloned into a custom AAV plasmid under the control of the U6 promoter, and coding for tdTomato under the control of the human synapsin1 promoter. For viral packaging we used a plasmid coding for the PHP.eB capsid and pAdDeltaF6 plasmid to express adenovirus E4, E2A and VA genes. pUCmini-iCAP-PHP.eB was a gift from Viviana Gradinaru (Addgene plasmid # 103005; http://n2t.net/addgene:103005; RRID:Addgene_103005), pAdDeltaF6 was a gift from James M. Wilson (Addgene plasmid # 112867; http://n2t.net/addgene:112867; RRID:Addgene_112867).
AAV production
AAV particles coding for sgRNA against KMT2C under the control of a U6 promoter, together with the red fluorescent protein tdTomato controlled by the hSyn1 promoter were packed using the PHP.eB capsid (Bustos et al., 2017; 2023; Chan et al., 2017). High titer viral particles were purified as described in (Challis et al., 2019). Briefly, HEK 293T were transfected with PEI and PHP.eB capsid plasmids, the vector with KMT2C sgRNA-tdTomato, and the helper plasmid DF6. After 24 h of transfection, the media was replaced for DMEM 1% FBS. 72 h later, medium was collected from the plates and replaced with fresh DMEM 1% FBS. The collected medium was stored at 4°C. To collect the viruses, 120 h after transfection, the cells were detached from the plate and transferred to 250 mL conical tubes, as well as the collected media. They were centrifuged for 10 min at 2,000 g, the supernatant was removed and saved for later use. The pellet was resuspended in SAN digestion buffer (5 mL of 40 mM Tris, 500 mM NaCl and 2 mM MgCl2 pH 8.0) containing 100 U/mL of Salt Active Nuclease (SAN) from Arcticzymes and incubated at 37°C for 1 h. To the supernatant that was saved, a 5x stock solution of 40% PEG 8000 (Sigma) in 2.5 M NaCl was added, incubated on ice for 2 h and centrifuged at 4,000 g for 30 min in 250 mL bottles. The supernatant was collected and was placed in an Optiprep gradient and ultracentrifuged at 41.000 rpm for 4 h. The phase containing the AAV was rescued and frozen at −80°C for later use.
Genomic DNA extraction and T7 endonuclease I assay
Genomic DNA from transduced primary cortical neurons or brain tissue from transduced animals was extracted using Quick-DNA Miniprep Kit (Zymo Research, United States) following manufacturer recommendations. To test KMT2C edition, PCR with primers encompassing the edited region (5′ TACGTTGACCTCAAGGCACAGT 3′, 5′ TAAAACTGTCTCTTGGCCCCCG 3′) were used to determine edition of the locus. PCR products were run in agarose gels and purified by Gel extraction kit (Qiagen). 400 ng of gDNA was used for T7 endonuclease assay (NEB Cat#M0302). Assays were run in TBE polyacrylamide gels and visualized using Gel Red.
RNA extraction and RT-qPCR
RNA was isolated from the tissues and cell cultures using TRIzol (Life Technologies) according to the manufacturer’s instructions as in (Henriquez et al., 2013; Bustos et al., 2017; 2023). To obtain complementary DNA (cDNA), 400 ng of RNA was used. cDNA quantification was performed by qPCR using 3 μL of the cDNA mix, 6 μL Fast Evagreen qPCR Master Mix (Biotium, 31003), 2 μL of nuclease-free water, and 1 μL of 10 mM primers with the program recommended by the maker. The relative abundance was measured by the ddCt method using the GAPDH gene as a control. Transcript detection was performed with specific primers for messenger RNA (mRNA): KMT2C: Fw 5′ TGTTCACAGTGTGGTCAATGTT 3′; Rv 5′ GAGGGTCTAGGCAGTAGGTATG 3′; GAPDH: Fw 5′ ATGGTGAAGGTCGGTGTGAA 3′; Rv 5′ CATTCTCGGCCTTGACTGTG 3′.
Nuclear protein extraction
Nuclear proteins to determine histone tail modification abundance by western blot were isolated as in (Henriquez et al., 2013). Briefly, cultured cells were washed with cold PBS, centrifuged at 5,000 rpm for 5 min and the pellet was resuspended in 5 volumes of cell lysis buffer (50 mM Hepes pH 7.9, 3 mM MgCl2, 20 mM KCl, 0.1% NP-40, 1 mM DTT and Protease Inhibitor Cocktail) and incubated on ice for 10 min. Solution was homogenized with 30 strokes of the pestle and centrifuged at 6,000 rpm for 15 min at 4°C to separate the cytosolic and nuclear fractions. The pellet was resuspended in 1 volume of Buffer C (10 mM Hepes pH 7.9, 420 mM NaCl, 1.5 mM MgCl2, 25% Glycerol, 0.2 mM EDTA, 1 mM DTT and protease inhibitor cocktail) and incubated for 1 h. Extracts were sonicated at 50% amplitude in cycles of 30 s ON/30 s OFF, centrifuged at 12,000 rpm for 15 min at 4°C. Supernatant was recovered and frozen at −80°C to be subsequently quantified by Bradford.
Western blot analysis
Nuclear fractions were separated on polyacrylamide gels and transferred to PVDF membranes (Millipore, United States). Membranes were blocked and incubated overnight at 4°C with primary antibodies. After rinsing, the membranes were incubated with secondary antibodies for 30 min at room temperature, rinsed and developed using chemiluminescence (Cell Signaling Technology, United States). Primary antibodies used: H3K4me1 (Diagenode, C15410194), H3K4me3 (Diagenode, C15410003), and H3pan (Diagenode C15410324) as loading control. For detection HRP-conjugated secondary anti-bodies were used (Cell Signaling Technology, United States).
Animals
All animal procedures and experiments were performed according to the NIH and ARRIVE guidelines and were approved by the animal ethics committee from Universidad Andrés Bello (020/2018). Newborn Cas9 KI mice (C57BL/6J; JAX 026179) were cryoanesthetized in a cold aluminium plate and injected with 1 μL of concentrated AAV (1 × 1011 vg), containing sgRNA or empty vector, in each cerebral ventricle at a depth of 3 mm in the animal’s head at 2/5 of the intersection between lambda and the eye with a 10 μL HAMILTON syringe (Hamilton, 7653-01) and a 32 G needle (Hamilton, 7803-04). After the injection, P0 mice were placed in a heating pad until they recovered their color and temperature, then they were returned to their cage with the mother (Passini and Wolfe, 2001; Kim et al., 2014). 3 weeks after birth, mice from both conditions were weaned off and separated by sex in cages with a 12/12 light/dark cycle with free access to food and water. A chip (p-chips, Pharmseq) was put in the tail of each animal for easy tracking during behavioral test. Behavior tests were performed between 9:00 am and 6:00 pm. At the end of the battery of behavioral tests, the animals were euthanized using isoflurane for subsequent molecular analyses.
Behavioral overview
All behavioral tests on mice were carried out 8 weeks after AAV injection. Before each test, mice cages were transported to the behavior room and habituated for 30 min in the dark. After completing a test, equipment and devices used were cleaned with 70% ethanol. Tests were recorded and analyzed with ANY-Maze software and/or Graphpad Prism software.
Rotarod
Motor coordination and capacity was assessed in the Rotarod test. Mice were placed on an elevated accelerating rod for three trials. Each trial lasted for a maximum of 3 min, during which the Rotarod underwent a linear acceleration of 4 rpm per min. Mice weights were registered before the test. Mean time and speed from each animal were registered before falling off.
Open field
Mice were tested in an open field (45 × 45 cm) virtually divided into central and peripheral regions with ANY-Maze software. Apparatus were illuminated from above with 300 lux in center and 250 lux in periphery. Animals were allowed to roam freely for 10 min. The total distance traveled and time in center and periphery were analyzed.
Light and dark
The apparatus used for this test is a 40 cm box split in half with 390 lux on light side and 0–2 lux on the dark side. Mice were placed in the dark chamber and were allowed to freely explore both chambers for 10 min. Distance traveled, time spent and number of entrances to the light side were analyzed.
Elevated zero-maze
The apparatus consisted of a 46 cm diameter circular runway and raised 54 cm off the ground. The runway was divided equally into four alternating quadrants of open arcs and closed arcs, with 15 cm walls. Mice started in the center of an open arm and were recorded by video tracking for 10 min. Measures of cumulative open and closed arc times, total open arm entries, distance in each arm and total distance traveled were analyzed.
Marble burying test
Mice were tested in a 45 × 45 cm box filled with 5 cm deep wood chips and 49 marbles distributed in a 7 × 7 pattern. Animals were placed in the test cage and allowed to explore and bury the marbles during a 30 min session that was videotaped. At the end of the session the subject was removed and the number of marbles buried (2/3 marble covered by wood chip) was counted.
Contextual fear conditioning
UGO-BASILE apparatus controlled by ANY-Maze was used. This equipment consisted of a sound attenuating box, fan, light (visible/I.R.), a speaker, a USB camera, a single on-board controller, and a mouse cage. All trials were recorded and all mice underwent a habituation, conditioning and testing phase (Pandian et al., 2020; Bustos et al., 2023). In the habituation (day 1): mice were placed in the fear conditioning cage to explore freely for 5 min and then returned to their cage. During the conditioning phase (day 2): subject mouse was placed in the fear conditioning cage, let explore freely for 2 min and then subjected to an electric shock of 0.75 mA for 2 s. It was allowed to explore freely for 3 min and returned to its cage. On the test phase (day 3): Twenty-four hours after the conditioning phase, the animals were tested for contextual memory. Each mouse was placed in the fear conditioning box, allowed to freely explore for 5 min, and returned to its cage. The number of freezing episodes and freezing time was registered.
Barnes maze
A non-reflective gray circular platform (91 cm diameter) with 20 holes (5 cm diameter) evenly distributed along the perimeter, with one hole containing a metal escape tunnel was used. Three exogenous visual cues (length/width ∼30 cm) were used around the platform: black circle, blue triangle and a yellow square. The light was adjusted to 1,000 lux in the center of the platform. All animals underwent a phase of habituation, spatial acquisition and testing (Bolivar et al., 2007; Pitts, 2018). For habituation (day 1): each mouse was placed in the center of the platform, directed towards the escape hole, and allowed to remain there for 1 min. Then it was taken and allowed to freely explore the maze for 5 min, and was again allowed to spend 1 min inside the escape hole. If the mouse did not enter within 5 min, it was gently guided near the escape hole selected randomly on the table. During training phase (day 2–4): each animal was introduced into the start box, left in the center of the platform for 10 s and the start box was removed, and simultaneously a 16,000 Hz sound was played. The test ended at 3 min or when the mouse has found the escape hole. This procedure was repeated 2 times per day. During those days the following was recorded: Primary latency: Time to review the escape hole for the first time; Time in the zone of interest; Total distance traveled. In the test phase (day 5): the position of the escape tunnel was changed, and the animal was brought in the start box to the center of the platform, left for 10 s and sound reproduction was started. The test ended at 90 s or when the mouse found the escape tunnel. The number of primary and total errors, primary and total latency, and total distance before finding the gap were recorded. The number of visits to each hole was also measured to show preference.
Three-chamber sociability and social novelty test
This was performed in a transparent acrylic three-chambered apparatus with the following dimensions: 61.5 × 43.5 × 22 cm. Each outer chamber was 20 × 43.5 cm. We used small cages of 8 cm diameter and 18 cm height to put the social (unknown WT mice of the same sex and similar age) and non social stimulus (plastic block of 8 × 4 × 4 cm). A 20 lux illumination was used in this test. On the habituation phase (day 1): stimulus holders were placed in the center of outer chambers. The subject mouse was placed in the central chamber and allowed to explore freely for 10 min. Apparatus and stimulus holders were cleaned between mice with 70% ethanol. During pre-test (day 2): two clean paper balls were prepared and introduced inside each stimulus holder. The mouse was placed in the central chamber and allowed to explore freely for 10 min. Apparatus and stimulus holders were cleaned between mice with 70% ethanol. For the social preference test (day 3): a control mouse was placed in a stimulus holder to be used as a social stimulus and changed for another one every 2 test runs to avoid burnout or social fatigue (Rein et al., 2020). A plastic block was placed in the stimulus holder as a non social stimulus. Once social and non social objects were put in the outer chambers, mouse was placed in the central chamber and allowed to explore freely for 10 min. Behavior was video recorded. Time amount and distance traveled in the social and non social chamber was registered, as well as the interaction time with each stimulus. Discrimination index was calculated by (time with novel animal–time with novel object)/(time with novel animal + time with novel object).
Social interactions
In this test, an animal from the control condition or KMT2C bKO with a control animal of the same sex and similar age, were placed in a 30 × 30 × 30 cm box for 10 min. Aggressive behavior (e.g., biting, mounting or aggressive grooming) and the amount of social interactions (nose-nose sniffing, nose-face, nose-anogenital area, and grooming) between them were quantified.
Tube dominance
Tube test apparatus consisted of a smooth transparent acrylic tube of 30 cm length and internal diameter of 2.5 cm. Mice were habituated for 3 days. On day 1, each animal interacted and explored the tube freely for 30 min and was then returned to its cage where a habituation tube was placed with a 10 cm length with an internal diameter of 2.5 cm. A small amount of gel food (Diet Gel Boost) was placed at the end of the cage habituation tube and they were deprived of their common diet food. On day 2, tube inlet was closed and gel food was placed at the end. Animals were allowed to explore freely for 30 min. They were fed and starved again of their common diet for 12 h and a small amount of gel food (Diet Gel Boost) was placed at the end of the cage habituation tube. On day 3, the same procedure of Day 2 was repeated, and mice were returned to their common diet. On the training phase (day 4 and 5): mice were taken by the tail and allowed to freely explore for approximately 1 min on the table where the tube was located. Then they were taken from the tail and put on one end of the tube and when the animal entered the tail, it was released. If the animal did not move for several seconds, it was gently prodded with a wooden stick. This step was repeated 5 times per side, so that the mouse passed through the tube a total of 10 times. The same procedure was repeated on day 4 and 5. On the test phase (day 6–9): two mice, one control and one KMT2C bKO, were taken and brought to the ends of the tube by the tail. When they entered completely and reached the middle of the tube, their tails were released to begin the confrontation. The test was repeated for 4 days. The mouse that pushes and removes its opponent from the tube was considered the winner and the one that is removed from the tube was considered the loser. All confrontations were video recorded to analyze the times each animal won and lost. The test stopped when the loser had all 4 paws out of the tube. The total number of wins and the number of wins per day were compared between control and KMT2C bKO conditions.
Brain sectioning and mounting
After behaviors to assess brain transduction, animals were deeply anesthetized, and half of the brain was extracted and fixed by immersion PBS + 4% PFA + 4% Sucrose into 30 mL flasks for 24 h. After fixation, a Leica VT1000s vibratome was used to cut 100 μm coronal sections. Slices were kept in PBS and mounted using Fluoromont G (EMS, Hatfield, PA) to preserve the fluorescence signal. Brain images were captured with a Nikon Eclipse TE2000 epifluorescence microscope (Nikon, United States).
Statistical analysis
Values are presented as mean ± standard error of mean (SEM) for 3 or more independent experiments. Statistical analyzes with Student’s t-test was performed. Values of p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. All statistical analyzes were performed using Graphpad Prism (GraphPad Software Inc.).
Results
Gene editing of KMT2C results in knockout in primary culture of cortical neurons and in the mouse brain
To determine the role of KMT2C in the induction of ASD-like behaviors, we used CRISPR/Cas9 technology to knock out gene expression (Cong et al., 2013; Swiech et al., 2014). First, we designed sgRNAs targeting the coding sequence of KMT2C. We targeted exon 3 of the gene since it is a common exon for all splice variants (Figure 1A). Cortical neuron cultures of Cas9 KI mice were transduced using AAVs coding for TdTomato alone as a control or together with sgRNA K1 or K2. Ten days after transduction, genomic DNA was extracted, and the T7 endonuclease I assay was performed. We observed that both sgRNAs targeting KMT2C exon 3 were able to edit the genomic locus, shown by the smaller size DNA bands (Figure 1B). Additionally, total RNA was extracted, and RT-qPCR was performed to determine changes in KMT2C expression. We observed that both K1 and K2 sgRNAs significantly reduced KMT2C expression by more than 60% in primary cortical neurons (Figure 1C). KMT2C is a component of the COMPASS chromatin remodeling complex that can increase the histone tail modifications H3K4me1 and H3K4me3 at enhancers and promoters to induce gene expression (Jozwik et al., 2016). Therefore, we tested the abundance of these histone tail marks in nuclear extracts of transduced cortical neurons. We found that the transduction of neurons with K1 or K2 decreased the total abundance of H3K4me1 and H3K4me3 (Figure 1D). Quantification reveals a significant reduction in the relative expression over H3 of both H3K4me3 (Figure 1E) and H3K4me1 (Figure 1F). This data shows that the CRISPR/Cas9 system can edit the KMT2C genomic locus, reduce its expression, and decrease the presence of histone tail modifications associated with its function in KMT2C KO cultured neurons.
In humans, loss-of-function mutations in KMT2C have been associated to the appearance of ASD-like behaviors and Kleefstra syndrome 2 (Koemans et al., 2017; Satterstrom et al., 2020; Siano et al., 2022; Wu and Li, 2022), however limited information is available about the phenotype. To determine whether KO of KMT2C in mice can produce ASD-like behaviors, we used AAVs to express the previously characterized K2 sgRNA in Cas9 knock-in animals. We selected K2 sgRNA from our culture experiments because it showed a more significant reduction in mRNA expression compared to control neurons and K1 (Figure 1C). The fluorescence reporter TdTomato was used to show transduction efficiency. AAVs were packed using the PHP.eB capsid (Chan et al., 2017) to efficiently transduce the whole brain after injection into the cerebral ventricles of P0-P1 animals (Bustos et al., 2023). At 8 weeks, animals were subjected to behavioral testing. To be included in the final behavioral analyses, animals needed to meet three parameters after euthanizing and dissecting their brains: 1) a strong fluorescence signal widely spread in the brain (Figure 1G); 2) Gene editing by T7 endonuclease I assay (Figure 1H); and 3) RT-qPCR analyses showing a >50% reduction in KMT2C expression levels (Figure 1I). A total of 9 animals met these criteria, and the following results for KMT2C CRISPR injected animals to produce KMT2c brain specific KO (henceforth KMT2C bKO mice) are only based on these 9 animals and 15 animals used as control. Fluorescence imaging of brain sections, show high and broad expression of tdTomato (Figure G). All KMT2C bKO (KO1-9) animals show lower size bands in the T7 endonuclease I assay, showing that gene editing was successful (Figure 1H, KO1-9) compared to 5 representative control animals (Figure 1H, Control 1-5). In addition, RT-qPCR from brain tissue showed >50% reduction in the expression levels of KMT2C in injected animals (Figure 1I). Similary results were observed using hippocampal tissue from KMT2C bKO animals (Supplementary Figures S1A, B). These results demonstrate that CRISPR/Cas9 system can edit the KMT2C genomic locus and reduce its expression in the mouse brain.
KMT2C bKO animals exhibit no signs of behaviors associated with anxiety
Eight weeks after injection, KMT2C bKO and Cas9-KI littermates (Control) animals underwent behavioral testing. Importantly, KMT2C bKO did not result in any discernible differences in growth. To assess whether KMT2C bKO animals had any locomotion deficits that could impact their performance in the upcoming battery of behavioral tests, the rotarod test was performed. KMT2C bKO animals did not exhibit any locomotion difficulties compared to their control littermates, as both groups spent similar amounts of time on the rotarod apparatus (Figure 2A). One of the hallmarks of both human and animal ASD cases is the manifestation of anxiety-like behaviors (Silverman et al., 2010; Lord et al., 2020; Pandian et al., 2020; Bustos et al., 2023). To assess whether bKO of KMT2C could produce similar behaviors in mice, we subjected KMT2C bKO animals to behavioral analyses after 8 weeks. In the open field test, animals were allowed to explore for 10 min, and the time spent in the center or periphery of the apparatus and the distance traveled was measured (Figure 2B). We observed no significant differences between KMT2C bKO animals and control littermates in total distance traveled (Figure 2C), time spent in the center (Figure 2D), or time spent in the periphery (Figure 2E). However, we when we looked in more detail at the time spent in the center, we found significant reduction in the distance travelled in the center by KMT2C bKO animals (Figure 2F), suggesting appearance of anxiety since animals freeze in the zone.
Next, animals were tested in the light-dark apparatus, where they were placed in the dark chamber and allowed to explore for 10 min. We observed no significant difference in the number of crosses to the light compartment (Figure 3B) or time spent in the illuminated field (Figure 3C). Similarly, in the elevated zero maze, KMT2C bKO animals did not differ significantly from control littermates in the time spent in the open zone (Figure 3E) or distance traveled (Figure 3F) after exploring the platform for 10 min. Finally, to determine whether KMT2C bKO animals showed signs of anxiety or repetitive behavior, we used the marble burying test, in which KMT2C bKO and Control animals were placed in a box with 49 marbles to be buried in a 30-min interval. We found that KMT2C bKO animals buried more marbles than Control littermates within the given timeframe (Figure 3H). Taken together, our data show that KMT2C bKO animals exhibit repetitive behaviors, as observed in the marble burying test, and display almost no signs of anxiety-like behaviors that could be measured using these behavioral tests.
KMT2C KO animals show impaired social behaviors
Individuals with ASD often exhibit difficulties in social interaction, which can manifest as a lack of aptitude or skill in performing sociabilization (Silverman et al., 2010; Lord et al., 2020). To assess the ability of KMT2C bKO mice to engage in social interaction, we employed the three-chamber social interaction test leaving the animal to explore for 10 min (Bolivar et al., 2007; Defensor et al., 2010; Pandian et al., 2020; Bustos et al., 2023). KMT2C bKO and Control animals spent similar time in the novel mouse region (NM) (Figure 4A) and novel object region (NO) (Figure 4B). However, when the time spent directly interacting with NM or NO was analyzed in more detail, we found that KMT2C bKO animals spent significantly more time engaging directly with the NM (Supplementary Figure S2A) and NO (Supplementary Figure S2B) than their control littermates. However, discrimination index shows no significant difference in preference between WT and KMT2C bKO animals (Figure 4C). This suggests that KMT2C bKO mice do not exhibit alterations in social behavior compared to Control mice in the 3 chamber social test.
To gain insight into the social behavior observed, we used the social interaction test. Pairs of animals of the same sex from KMT2C bKO and Control animals were let to interact in a clear box for 10 min and recorded to quantify interactions. The quantification of social sniffing in animal pairs showed no significant difference in the frequency of nose-nose (N-N) interactions (Figure 5A) or nose-head (N-H) interactions (Figure 5B). However, we observed a significant decrease in the number of nose-anogenital (N-A) interactions in KMT2C bKO mice (Figure 5C), indicating a decrease in social investigation activity. Additionally, we found that KMT2C bKO animals displayed a significant increase in self-grooming episodes compared to their control littermates (Figure 5D). Lastly, we used the tube dominance test to determine social hierarchy and interaction among animals. Results show that KMT2C bKO animals won a significantly greater number of times against control animals in the test tube (Figure 5E). This data demonstrates that KMT2C bKO animals exhibit social impairments, as evidenced by a decrease in social investigation, an increase in self-grooming episodes to avoid interactions, and avoidance of direct contact with other animals. These behaviors suggest that KMT2C bKO animals have difficulty engaging in social interactions and may display aggression when faced with such situations.
KMT2C bKO mice exhibit severe memory formation deficits
Cognitive impairments are a crucial feature of ASD phenotypes. Human with mutations in KMT2C have shown to have mild to severe intellectual disability, phenotype also associated to ASD (Koemans et al., 2017; Siano et al., 2022; Wu and Li, 2022). To determine whether KMT2C bKO animals have impaired capacity to form memories, we utilized two behavioral tests. First, we employed the contextual fear conditioning paradigm to analyze long-term memory formation after 24 h. Animals were tested for 5 min, and we observed that KMT2C bKO animals froze for a significantly reduced amount of time compared to Control animals (Figure 6A). Next, we used the Barnes maze apparatus to assess spatial learning and memory (Pitts, 2018; Gawel et al., 2019). Similarly, to what is observed in the open field apparatus, we found no significant difference in the total distance travelled between KMT2C bKO and control animals (Figure 6B). However, when we quantified the primary latency—time to reach the escape hole for the first time—we observed a significant increase in the time required by KMT2C bKO animals, and moreover, some of them never reached the escape hole (Figure 6C). Finally, to assess whether KMT2C bKO animals showed deficits in spatial memory, we moved the escape hole to a new location and quantified the time spent in the region where the original escape hole had been located. We observed a significant decrease in the time spent in the target region by KMT2C bKO animals compared to Controls (Figure 6D). Taken together, our data suggests that KMT2C bKO animals exhibit reduced memory formation capacity and significant cognitive impairments in both behavioral tests used.
Discussion
Neurodevelopmental disorders, including ASD and Kleefstra Syndrome 2, have been associated with mutations in genes involved in chromatin regulation (Rubeis et al., 2014; Eshraghi et al., 2018; Satterstrom et al., 2020; Chen et al., 2021; Siano et al., 2022). Some of these genes encode enzymes responsible for modulating histone tail modifications, which play a crucial role in gene expression regulation (Bannister and Kouzarides, 2011).
In this study, we utilized CRISPR/Cas9 technology to disrupt the expression of KMT2C, an H3K4 methyltransferase enzyme, both in vitro and in vivo. KMT2C whole animals KO are lethal and die around birth without any abnormalities in embryonic development (Lee et al., 2013). Using a postnatal injection into the cerebral ventricles of CRISPR/Cas9 system to KO KMT2C, allows the study of gene function in early development. Even though, we cannot fully recapitulate the whole-body KO by this methodology, and we produce mosaics editions in the brain of animals as shown by (Swiech et al., 2014; Bustos et al., 2023), we can study KMT2C function and the development of ASD like phenotypes by its loss of function in the brain. Our results demonstrate a significant reduction in KMT2C expression in vitro, accompanied by a concomitant decrease in the overall levels of H3K4me1 and H3K4me3, histone tail modifications associated with the function of KMT2C (Shilatifard, 2012; Faundes et al., 2018). This reduction in histone tail marks indicates the successful gene editing of KMT2C. Furthermore, the changes in these histone tail marks suggest a mechanism by which KMT2C KO may impact gene networks involved in the observed phenotype. To further elucidate these alterations, future ChIP-Seq experiments will be conducted to examine changes in histone marks associated with promoters and analyze the specific genes and gene networks involved in the manifestation of the phenotype.
In humans, studies have demonstrated a strong association between mutations in KMT2C and carcinogenesis, highlighting the connection between epigenetic regulation and the development of cancer (Fagan and Dingwall, 2019). However, the understanding of the impact of KMT2C mutations on brain disorders remains limited. In humans, loss of function mutations of KMT2C causes ASD and Kleefstra syndrome 2, characterized by intellectual disability and ASD-like behaviors (Koemans et al., 2017). Only eleven cases of Kleefstra syndrome 2 have been reported in literature, thus not much evidence on the behavioral phenotype is described (Koemans et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2021; Siano et al., 2022; Wu and Li, 2022). Using AAV intracerebral ventricular injections at postnatal day 1, we injected CRISPR/Cas9 to produce KMT2C bKO. We observed a robust decrease in the expression of KMT2C following gene editing in vivo, which enabled us to investigate the behavioral changes associated with ASD. We subjected the animals to a battery of test that are design to test ASD-like behaviors (Pandian et al., 2020; Bustos et al., 2023). Similar experiments have been conducted to characterize the behavioral phenotypes in mutant animals for KMD6A and KMD6B (Tang et al., 2017; Gao et al., 2022). KMT2C bKO animals did not show signs of affected growth or locomotor abilities, thus effects on the posterior tests are not due to locomotive problems. Using the open field test, light and dark apparatus, and elevated zero maze, we did not observe significant differences in anxiety-related behaviors. This observation is supported by the phenotypes observed in humans carrying KMT2C mutations where anxiety is not described as a main characteristic of the individuals (Koemans et al., 2017; Grove et al., 2019; Siano et al., 2022; Wu and Li, 2022). Repetitive behaviors are a recognized characteristic of ASD phenotypes, as evidenced by studies conducted on other mouse models (Silverman et al., 2010; Lord et al., 2020; Pandian et al., 2020; Bustos et al., 2023). To assess repetitive behaviors in KMT2C bKO animals, we employed the marble burying test and quantified grooming behaviors. Our results indicate that KMT2C animals exhibit increased repetitive behaviors. While this specific characteristic has not been described in humans with KMT2C mutations, it is important to note that repetitive behaviors are complex and may be associated with the presence of comorbid conditions such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as observed in individuals with KMT2C mutations (Wu and Li, 2022).
In the social tests performed, KMT2C bKO animals displayed increased social interactions and demonstrated dominance over Control animals in the tube dominance test. While this specific phenotype has not been described in humans due to limited available data, it highlights a novel observation that warrants further investigation in individuals with KMT2C mutations. By exploring this phenotype in human subjects with KMT2C mutations, we may gain valuable insights into the role of KMT2C in social behavior and its potential implications for neurodevelopmental disorders. Intellectual disability is a well-documented phenotype observed in all human patients with KMT2C mutations, as supported by previous studies (Koemans et al., 2017; Siano et al., 2022; Wu and Li, 2022). To determine whether KMT2C bKO animals exhibit impairments in memory formation, which is closely associated with intellectual disability, we conducted the fear conditioning and Barnes maze tests. Results revealed severe deficits in memory formation in KMT2C bKO animals, with some individuals failing to learn the required tasks altogether. This finding aligns with the phenotypes observed in humans with KMT2C mutations and suggests that KMT2C plays a critical role in cognitive processes, including memory formation, thereby contributing to intellectual disability.
To the best of our knowledge, this study represents the first comprehensive characterization of the behavioral phenotype of KMT2C bKO animals. Particularly, using CRISPR/Cas9 gene editing we were able to KO KMT2C in the entire brain to assess the behavioral phenotype. The phenotypic similarities observed between KMT2C KO animals and humans with KMT2C mutations indicate the relevance and translational potential of our approach. This animal model will allow to investigate the underlying cellular and molecular mechanisms involved in the development of ASD and Kleefstra syndrome 2 associated with KMT2C mutations.
In summary, our study contributes to the understanding of KMT2C-related neurodevelopmental disorders. In addition, the generated animal model offers a promising avenue for investigating therapeutic interventions and advancing our knowledge of the molecular pathways involved in ASD and Kleefstra syndrome 2.
Acknowledgments
We thank Lorena Varela-Nallar for critical reading of the manuscript and helpful feedback.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by: ANID Fondecyt Iniciacion 11180540 (FJB), ANID PAI 77180077 (FJB), ANID-FONDECYT 1220480 to GA, UNAB DI-02-22/REG (FJB), DI-03-21/APP UNAB (BB).
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The animal study was reviewed and approved by Animal ethics committee from Universidad Andrés Bello, Chile.
Author contributions
BB performed experimental work and analyzed data. NM-V and CA-M performed experimental work. GA analyzed data, and wrote the manuscript. FJB designed and conceptualized the study, acquired and analyzed data, and wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fcell.2023.1227723/full#supplementary-material
References
- Banerjee-Basu S., Packer A. (2010). SFARI gene: An evolving database for the autism research community. Dis. Model Mech. 3, 133–135. 10.1242/dmm.005439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bannister A. J., Kouzarides T. (2011). Regulation of chromatin by histone modifications. Nat. Publ. Group 21, 381–395. 10.1038/cr.2011.22 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolivar V., Walters S., Phoenix J. (2007). Assessing autism-like behavior in mice: Variations in social interactions among inbred strains. Behav. Brain Res. 176, 21–26. 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bustos F. J., Ampuero E., Jury N., Aguilar R., Falahi F., Toledo J., et al. (2017). Epigenetic editing of the Dlg4/PSD95 gene improves cognition in aged and Alzheimer’s disease mice. Brain 140, 3252–3268. 10.1093/brain/awx272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bustos F. J., Pandian S., Haensgen H., Zhao J.-P., Strouf H., Heidenreich M., et al. (2023). Removal of a genomic duplication by double-nicking CRISPR restores synaptic transmission and behavior in the MyosinVA mutant mouse Flailer. Biorxiv, 2023.04.28.538685. 10.1101/2023.04.28.538685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cenik B. K., Shilatifard A. (2021). COMPASS and SWI/SNF complexes in development and disease. Nat. Rev. Genet. 22, 38–58. 10.1038/s41576-020-0278-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Challis R. C., Kumar S. R., Chan K. Y., Challis C., Beadle K., Jang M. J., et al. (2019). Systemic AAV vectors for widespread and targeted gene delivery in rodents. Nat. Protoc. 14, 379–414. 10.1038/s41596-018-0097-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan K. Y., Jang M. J., Yoo B. B., Greenbaum A., Ravi N., Wu W.-L., et al. (2017). Engineered AAVs for efficient noninvasive gene delivery to the central and peripheral nervous systems. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1172–1179. 10.1038/nn.4593 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen C.-H., Huang A., Huang Y.-S., Fang T.-H. (2021). Identification of a rare novel KMT2C mutation that presents with schizophrenia in a multiplex family. J. Pers. Med. 11, 1254. 10.3390/jpm11121254 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cong L., Ran F. A., Cox D., Lin S., Barretto R., Habib N., et al. (2013). Multiplex genome engineering using CRISPR/Cas systems. Science 339, 819–823. 10.1126/science.1231143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Defensor E. B., Pearson B. L., Pobbe R. L. H., Bolivar V. J., Blanchard D. C., Blanchard R. J. (2010). A novel social proximity test suggests patterns of social avoidance and gaze aversion-like behavior in BTBR T+ tf/J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 217, 302–308. 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhaliwal J., Qiao Y., Calli K., Martell S., Race S., Chijiwa C., et al. (2021). Contribution of multiple inherited variants to autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in a family with 3 affected siblings. Genes-basel 12, 1053. 10.3390/genes12071053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eshraghi A. A., Liu G., Kay S.-I. S., Eshraghi R. S., Mittal J., Moshiree B., et al. (2018). Epigenetics and autism spectrum disorder: Is there a correlation? Front. Cell Neurosci. 12, 78. 10.3389/fncel.2018.00078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fagan R. J., Dingwall A. K. (2019). COMPASS Ascending: Emerging clues regarding the roles of MLL3/KMT2C and MLL2/KMT2D proteins in cancer. Cancer Lett. 458, 56–65. 10.1016/j.canlet.2019.05.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faundes V., Newman W. G., Bernardini L., Canham N., Clayton-Smith J., Dallapiccola B., et al. (2018). Histone lysine methylases and demethylases in the landscape of human developmental disorders. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 102, 175–187. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.11.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao Y., Aljazi M. B., He J. (2022). Kdm6b haploinsufficiency causes ASD/ADHD-Like behavioral deficits in mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 905783. 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.905783 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gawel K., Gibula E., Marszalek-Grabska M., Filarowska J., Kotlinska J. H. (2019). Assessment of spatial learning and memory in the Barnes maze task in rodents—Methodological consideration. Schmiedeb. Arch. Pharmacol. 392, 1–18. 10.1007/s00210-018-1589-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grove J., Ripke S., Als T. D., Mattheisen M., Walters R. K., Won H., et al. (2019). Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Genet. 51, 431–444. 10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henriquez B., Bustos F. J., Aguilar R., Becerra A., Simon F., Montecino M., et al. (2013). Ezh1 and Ezh2 differentially regulate PSD-95 gene transcription in developing hippocampal neurons. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 57, 130–143. 10.1016/j.mcn.2013.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iossifov I., Ronemus M., Levy D., Wang Z., Hakker I., Rosenbaum J., et al. (2012). De novo gene disruptions in children on the autistic spectrum. Neuron 74, 285–299. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jozwik K. M., Chernukhin I., Serandour A. A., Nagarajan S., Carroll J. S. (2016). FOXA1 directs H3K4 monomethylation at enhancers via recruitment of the methyltransferase MLL3. Cell Rep. 17, 2715–2723. 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.11.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim J.-Y., Grunke S. D., Levites Y., Golde T. E., Jankowsky J. L. (2014). Intracerebroventricular viral injection of the neonatal mouse brain for persistent and widespread neuronal transduction. J. Vis. Exp. 51863, 51863. 10.3791/51863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koemans T. S., Kleefstra T., Chubak M. C., Stone M. H., Reijnders M. R. F., Munnik S. de, et al. (2017). Functional convergence of histone methyltransferases EHMT1 and KMT2C involved in intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorder. Plos Genet. 13, e1006864. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavery W. J., Barski A., Wiley S., Schorry E. K., Lindsley A. W. (2020). KMT2C/D COMPASS complex-associated diseases [KCDCOM-ADs]: An emerging class of congenital regulopathies. Clin. Epigenetics 12, 10. 10.1186/s13148-019-0802-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J.-E., Wang C., Xu S., Cho Y.-W., Wang L., Feng X., et al. (2013). H3K4 mono- and di-methyltransferase MLL4 is required for enhancer activation during cell differentiation. eLife 2, e01503. 10.7554/elife.01503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord C., Brugha T. S., Charman T., Cusack J., Dumas G., Frazier T., et al. (2020). Autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 6, 5. 10.1038/s41572-019-0138-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Roak B. J., Vives L., Girirajan S., Karakoc E., Krumm N., Coe B. P., et al. (2012). Sporadic autism exomes reveal a highly interconnected protein network of de novo mutations. Nature 485, 246–250. 10.1038/nature10989 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pandian S., Zhao J.-P., Murata Y., Bustos F. J., Tunca C., Almeida R. D., et al. (2020). Myosin va brain-specific mutation alters mouse behavior and disrupts hippocampal synapses. Eneuro 7, ENEURO.0284. 10.1523/eneuro.0284-20.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passini M. A., Wolfe J. H. (2001). Widespread gene delivery and structure-specific patterns of expression in the brain after intraventricular injections of neonatal mice with an adeno-associated virus vector. J. Virol. 75, 12382–12392. 10.1128/jvi.75.24.12382-12392.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitts M. (2018). Barnes maze procedure for spatial learning and memory in mice. Bio-protocol 8, e2744. 10.21769/bioprotoc.2744 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Platt R. J., Zhou Y., Slaymaker I. M., Shetty A. S., Weisbach N. R., Kim J.-A., et al. (2017). Chd8 mutation leads to autistic-like behaviors and impaired striatal circuits. Cell Rep. 19, 335–350. 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.03.052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rein B., Ma K., Yan Z. (2020). A standardized social preference protocol for measuring social deficits in mouse models of autism. Nat. Protoc. 15 (10), 3464–3477. 10.1038/s41596-020-0382-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubeis S. D., He X., Goldberg A. P., Poultney C. S., Samocha K., Cicek A. E., et al. (2014). Synaptic, transcriptional and chromatin genes disrupted in autism. Nature 515, 209–215. 10.1038/nature13772 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruthenburg A. J., Allis C. D., Wysocka J. (2007). Methylation of lysine 4 on histone H3: Intricacy of writing and reading a single epigenetic mark. Mol. Cell 25, 15–30. 10.1016/j.molcel.2006.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruzzo E. K., Pérez-Cano L., Jung J.-Y., Wang L., Kashef-Haghighi D., Hartl C., et al. (2019). Inherited and de novo genetic risk for autism impacts shared networks. Cell 178, 850–866.e26. 10.1016/j.cell.2019.07.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satterstrom F. K., Kosmicki J. A., Wang J., Breen M. S., Rubeis S. D., An J.-Y., et al. (2020). Large-scale exome sequencing study implicates both developmental and functional changes in the neurobiology of autism. Cell 180, 568–584.e23. 10.1016/j.cell.2019.12.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shilatifard A. (2012). The COMPASS family of histone H3K4 methylases: Mechanisms of regulation in development and disease pathogenesis. Annu. Rev. Biochem. 81, 65–95. 10.1146/annurev-biochem-051710-134100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siano M. A., Maggio I. D., Petillo R., Cocciadiferro D., Agolini E., Majolo M., et al. (2022). De novo mutation in KMT2C manifesting as Kleefstra syndrome 2: Case report and literature review. Pediatr. Rep. 14, 131–139. 10.3390/pediatric14010019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman J. L., Yang M., Lord C., Crawley J. N. (2010). Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 490–502. 10.1038/nrn2851 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stessman H. A. F., Xiong B., Coe B. P., Wang T., Hoekzema K., Fenckova M., et al. (2017). Targeted sequencing identifies 91 neurodevelopmental-disorder risk genes with autism and developmental-disability biases. Nat. Genet. 49, 515–526. 10.1038/ng.3792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swiech L., Heidenreich M., Banerjee A., Habib N., Li Y., Trombetta J., et al. (2014). In vivo interrogation of gene function in the mammalian brain using CRISPR-Cas9. Nat. Biotechnol. 33, 102–106. 10.1038/nbt.3055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang G.-B., Zeng Y.-Q., Liu P.-P., Mi T.-W., Zhang S.-F., Dai S.-K., et al. (2017). The histone H3K27 demethylase UTX regulates synaptic plasticity and cognitive behaviors in mice. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 10, 267. 10.3389/fnmol.2017.00267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tuncay I. O., Parmalee N. L., Khalil R., Kaur K., Kumar A., Jimale M., et al. (2022). Analysis of recent shared ancestry in a familial cohort identifies coding and noncoding autism spectrum disorder variants. Npj Genom Med. 7, 13. 10.1038/s41525-022-00284-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu D., Li R. (2022). Case report: Long-term treatment and follow-up of Kleefstra syndrome-2. Front. Pediatr. 10, 881838. 10.3389/fped.2022.881838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou X., Feliciano P., Shu C., Wang T., Astrovskaya I., Hall J. B., et al. (2022). Integrating de novo and inherited variants in 42,607 autism cases identifies mutations in new moderate-risk genes. Nat. Genet. 54, 1305–1319. 10.1038/s41588-022-01148-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.