Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2023 Mar 28;23(4):1095–1112. doi: 10.3758/s13415-023-01076-6

Decoding reappraisal and suppression from neural circuits: A combined supervised and unsupervised machine learning approach

Parisa Ahmadi Ghomroudi 1,, Michele Scaltritti 1, Alessandro Grecucci 1,2
PMCID: PMC10400700  PMID: 36977965

Abstract

Emotion regulation is a core construct of mental health and deficits in emotion regulation abilities lead to psychological disorders. Reappraisal and suppression are two widely studied emotion regulation strategies but, possibly due to methodological limitations in previous studies, a consistent picture of the neural correlates related to the individual differences in their habitual use remains elusive. To address these issues, the present study applied a combination of unsupervised and supervised machine learning algorithms to the structural MRI scans of 128 individuals. First, unsupervised machine learning was used to separate the brain into naturally grouping grey matter circuits. Then, supervised machine learning was applied to predict individual differences in the use of different strategies of emotion regulation. Two predictive models, including structural brain features and psychological ones, were tested. Results showed that a temporo-parahippocampal-orbitofrontal network successfully predicted the individual differences in the use of reappraisal. Differently, insular and fronto-temporo-cerebellar networks successfully predicted suppression. In both predictive models, anxiety, the opposite strategy, and specific emotional intelligence factors played a role in predicting the use of reappraisal and suppression. This work provides new insights regarding the decoding of individual differences from structural features and other psychologically relevant variables while extending previous observations on the neural bases of emotion regulation strategies.

Keywords: Reappraisal, Suppression, Machine learning, ICA, Boosted trees, Grey matter

Introduction

Emotion regulation is now considered as a core construct for mental health. Deficits in emotion regulation may lead to psychological disorders (Ochsner and Gross, 2008; Kring and Sloan, 2009; Sheppes et al., 2015; Frederickson et al., 2018; Grecucci et al., 2020; Messina et al., 2021; Monachesi et al., under review). Evidence suggests that depression and anxiety (Gross and Muñoz, 1995; Campbell-Sills et al., 2006; Grecucci et al., 2020), bipolar disorder (Johnson 2005; Lapomarda et al., 2021a, b), and substance abuse disorder (Sher et al., 2007) may all be the result of severe emotion dysregulation. Difficulties in emotion regulation within social situations are a main feature of borderline personality disorder (Kring and Werner, 2004; Ochsner and Gross, 2008). In addition, aggression (Donahue et al., 2014), anger outbursts and sleep disorders (Gruber et al., 2009) are believed to stem from emotion dysregulation. Due to the pervasive occurrence of emotion regulation problems across psychological disorders, clinicians have started incorporating techniques to regulate emotions in their treatments (Linehan, 1993; Beauregard, 2007; Leahy et al., 2011; Messina et al., 2013; Dadomo et al., 2016, 2018; Frederickson et al., 2018; De Panfilis et al., 2019; Grecucci et al., 2020). Two widely studied emotion regulation strategies are reappraisal and suppression. Reappraisal is an antecedent-focused regulation strategy that modifies emotion before full activation of the emotional response. Reappraisal thus involves a voluntary attempt to reinterpret the meaning of a situation to alter its emotional impact (Gross, 1998). Differently, suppression is focused on the modification of emotions after their full activation and is defined as an attempt to inhibit an ongoing emotion-expressive behavior (Gross & Levenson, 1993). A few experimental studies examining the effect of reappraisal and suppression suggested that reappraisal decreases the behavioral expressions related to negative emotions and does not increase physiological responses compared with when no regulation is implemented (Gross, 2002, 2015; Goldin et al., 2019). In contrast, suppression results in a reduction of positive emotion experience, while leaving negative emotion experience unchanged and yielding a physiological impact (Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997; Gross, 2002; Mauss et al., 2005; Brans et al., 2013).

Interestingly, individuals with more negative affect show a less habitual use of reappraisal and more frequent use of suppression whereas individuals with more positive affect tend to display the opposite pattern (more frequent use of reappraisal and a less habitual use of suppression) (Gross & John, 2003; John & Eng, 2014). Moreover, neuroticism seems to be associated with a reduced use of reappraisal and a more frequent reliance on suppression (John & Gross, 2004).

In the past 20 years, researchers have tried to understand the neural bases of specific emotion regulation strategies. One meta-analysis of 48 task-related fMRI studies focusing on reappraisal (Buhle et al., 2014) reported increased activation in bilateral dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC, vlPFC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), supplemental motor area (SMA), and inferior/superior parietal cortex during both down- and upregulation of emotion. A more recent meta-analysis of 42 fMRI studies on reappraisal and acceptance showed decreased activation of limbic areas, increased activity in dlPFC and left lPFC during reappraisal (Monachesi et al., under review). One study showed increased activity in prefrontal cortex (PFC) and decreased activity in the insula and in the amygdala during reappraisal in response to disgust-eliciting film clips (Goldin et al., 2008). In addition, a MRI study showed the habitual use of reappraisal is associated with higher activity in the fronto-cingulate cortex (Vanderhasselt et al., 2013). Importantly, another study revealed that reduced resting-state functional connectivity between right amygdala and medial PFC and posterior cingulate cortex (which are core components of the default mode network, DMN) predicted success in the use of reappraisal strategies (Uchida et al., 2015). Relatedly, two resting state functional connectivity studies showed that individual differences in reappraisal affect DMN’s functional connectivity (Martins and Mather, 2016; Morawetz et al., 2016). Finally, few studies suggest that the activity of DMN increases during reappraisal when viewing negative stimuli (Sripada et al., 2014; Xie et al., 2016). Previous studies have consistently linked DMN to semantic and autobiographical memory, thought generations, self-reflective processes, and cognitive elaboration of one’s affective state (Raichle et al., 2001; Crosson et al., 2002; Cato et al., 2004; Amodio and Frith, 2006; Olsson and Ochsner, 2008; Binder et al., 2009; Uchida et al., 2015). In line with these observations, we hypothesized that the role of DMN in reappraisal may be related to processes such as self-reflection and semantic elaboration of the meaning of the situation, to tame the affective response.

Concerning suppression, a study showed increased activity in the PFC, insula, and amygdala during suppression in response to disgust-eliciting film clips (Goldin et al., 2008). The habitual use of suppression has been associated with an increase in baseline perfusion of the medial PFC (Abler et al., 2008). Also, this tendency was associated with decreased activation of the orbital medial PFC while expecting to see unpleasant images (Abler et al., 2010). Moreover, a fMRI study showed the habitual use of suppression is associated with higher activity in amygdala (Vanderhasselt et al., 2013). One study on functional connectivity revealed a positive correlation between amygdala and dACC and a negative one between left centromedial amygdala and the SMA when using suppression (Picó-Pérez et al., 2018). Interestingly, one study by Sikka et al. (2022) further suggested that some brain regions involved in suppression (e.g., insula, frontoparietal, and inferior parietal cortex) may overlap with the salience network. In the same vein, Muhtadie et al. (2021) suggested that the salience network, especially the insular part, may be a major hub for emotional salience processing. Indeed, the activity of the insula has been found to be modulated during the down-regulation of unpleasant stimuli (Grecucci et al., 2013a, b). Such modulation has been interpreted as a modulation of the emotive arousal elicited by the stimuli. Indeed, information from different parts of the brain (such as the amygdala, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the hypothalamus) is integrated inside the insula to generate a model of the affective and proprioceptive state of the body. As such, we think the network encoding suppression may show a certain degree of overlap with the salience network. Suppression may act upon the map of the bodily/affective state generated at the level of the insula.

Beside functional task-related studies, researchers also have tried to understand how individual differences in emotion regulation strategies usage are mapped onto structural properties of the brain. Task-related activity provides real time information during cognitive and emotional operations (Poldrack and Gorgolewski, 2017). However, most fMRI studies consist of less than 50 participants, and often tasks vary across studies. Furthermore, for analyzing fMRI data, a large amount of information is required, such as the description of the task and the timings of the events. Therefore, combining task-based studies is more challenging compared to sMRI (Poldrack and Gorgolewski, 2017). Differently, structural brain properties and questionnaire measuring individual differences may represent a valuable and complementary alternative to cognitive tasks (Poldrack et al., 2017; Poldrack and Gorgolewski, 2017). In one sMRI study, a region of interest (ROI) analyses revealed a positive correlation between the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex volume and the use of reappraisal (Giuliani et al., 2011a). In another voxel-based morphometry (VBM) study, a positive correlation between reappraisal and right and left amygdala volume was found (Hermann, Bieber et al., 2013a). More recently, Pappaianni et al. (2020) conducted a study by using an unsupervised machine learning method based on Independent Component Analysis (ICA) known as source-based morphometry (SBM, Xu et al., 2009) to investigate the individual differences in structural brain features as a function of reappraisal usage. In this study, 37 participants were divided to low and high reappraisal groups as a function of their scores on the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003). The results revealed higher concentration of gray matter in a network including the frontal, temporal, and parietal regions, among low reappraisers compared with high reappraisers. However, this study focused only on reappraisal, the sample size was quite small for machine learning analyses, and the comparisons were conducted between two subsamples instead of considering how individual differences are continuously mapped onto the brain.

Regarding suppression, a study showed a positive correlation between the right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) volume and suppression usage (Kühn et al., 2011). In addition, a study using ROI and VBM analysis showed no relation between suppression use and volume of dACC (Giuliani et al., 2011a). Another study showed a positive correlation between anterior insula volume and use of suppression (Giuliani et al., 2011b). Finally, a VBM study conducted by Hermann et al. (2013a) found a positive correlation between suppression usage and dorsal anterior cingulate/paracingulate cortex and medial PFC grey matter volume.

The studies summarized above do not offer a clear-cut picture of the neural bases of reappraisal and suppression. This may be due to several methodological limitations: low number of participants, a priori defined ROI analyses, and massive univariate approaches. A perhaps more consistent picture can be found when examining the relations between the use of reappraisal vs suppression and some core psychological features. Different studies have suggested that some strategies may be more linked with specific emotional dysregulations (Dadomo et al., 2018; De Panfilis et al., 2019; Grecucci et al., 2020). For example, suppression has been associated with anxiety (Aldao and Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012), and some studies actually revealed that suppression may lead to the development of anxiety (Salters-Pedneault et al., 2006; Werner et al., 2011). By contrast, reappraisal is thought to be negatively associated with anxiety (Martin and Dahlen, 2005; Garland et al., 2011; Desrosiers et al., 2013; Peh et al., 2017). Beside anxiety, emotional intelligence (EI) may also play a role in the use of different regulation strategies. According to Mayer and Salovey (1997) EI is defined as “the ability to perceive accurately, appraise, and express emotion; the ability to access and/or generate feelings when they facilitate thought; the ability to understand emotion and emotional knowledge; and the ability to regulate emotions to promote emotional and intellectual growth.” Few studies have investigated the association between level of EI and use of reappraisal and suppression. One study has reported that a high score in EI was associated with a less frequent use of suppression (Andrei et al., 2016). In addition, other studies reported a positive association between EI level and the use of reappraisal together with a negative association between the use of suppression and EI (Schutte et al., 2009; Cabello et al., 2013; Nozaki, 2018). Consistently, a meta-analysis of 90 studies revealed that the individuals with higher levels of EI are more likely to use reappraisal whereas individuals with lower levels of EI are more prone to use suppression (Peña-Sarrionandia et al., 2015).

Present study

Building on the considerations above, the purpose of the present study was to provide new evidence of how individual differences in the use of reappraisal and suppression can be predicted by structural properties of the brain. Also, the role of emotional intelligence, anxiety, and the use of other strategies will be jointly considered in the same predictive model. By considering these different classes of predictors together, one of our goals is to assess their relative contribution in predicting the use of different strategies of emotion regulation.

Given the fact that reappraisal and suppression are different in their supposed psychological mechanisms (Gross, 2002), we expect different brain regions to be involved. Building on previous studies on this topic (Pappaianni et al., 2020; Giuliani et al., 2011b; Kühn et al., 2011), we hypothesized that the individual differences in reappraisal usage involve a network consisting of cognitive, semantic, and top-down control regions, such as ventral medial PFC, parahippocampus, and temporal regions of the brain. One additional hypothesis is that the network predicting reappraisal may at least partially overlap with the DMN, as the latter has been previously associated with self-referential processes and self-generated thoughts (Andrews-Hanna, 2012; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014), as well as with conceptual processing and perspective-taking. All these processes are thought to be involved in reappraisal. The prediction is that the greater the grey matter concentration inside this network, the greater the reappraisal usage. We also expect EI to have a positive association, and anxiety a negative association with the use of reappraisal.

By contrast, on the basis of previous studies (Giuliani et al., 2011a; Kühn et al., 2011; Hermann et al., 2013), we hypothesize that individual differences in the usage of suppression are related to a network including the insula, for its relationship with arousal and bodily awareness, and parietal-cerebellar regions more linked with response control and monitoring. We also predict this network to overlap with the salience network in which the insula is the main hub. The salience network may be related to the generation and modulation of the affective bodily map generated during emotional experiences. Here, the prediction is that the greater the grey matter concentration in this network, the greater the suppression usage. We also expect EI to have a negative association, and anxiety to have a positive association, in predicting the frequency of usage of suppression. Moreover, together with the psychological features, in the final model we also will include the ERQ score of the other strategy as a predictor (suppression for reappraisal, and reappraisal for suppression). Previous studies (Benson et al., 2019) suggested that the usage of one strategy may be predictive of usage of the other. Building on this, we expect individuals who frequently use one strategy to frequently also use the other.

In the present study, we incorporated these psychological features (together with the structural brain features) in the same predictive models to assess their relative contribution in a cohesive predictive model jointly capturing structural brain measures and psychological features. From a methodological point of view, a combination of unsupervised and supervised machine-learning algorithms was used with a twofold purpose. First, we aimed to decompose the brain into naturally grouping independent grey matter circuits using ICA, an unsupervised machine learning approach. Second, we aimed to predict individual differences in the use of reappraisal and suppression by using the independent circuits of the first analysis by using a regression-based supervised machine learning approach (boosted decision trees) and including EI and anxiety scores as predictors. We believe that this approach may display several advantages or, at least, an important and complementary source of information concerning the issues at stake. ICA is a multivariate method that considers the statistical dependency among voxels (Xu et al., 2009). Accordingly, the brain is separated into independent brain circuits based on regions with covarying grey matter concentration. In other words, the brain is decomposed into naturally grouping networks with lower and consistent dimensionality (Grecucci et al., under review). Such approach also is more coherent with a network perspective in neuroscience (Hamann, 2012).

Additionally, previous studies used correlations or regressions to find association between networks parameters and emotion regulation variables. One limit of such frequentist approach is that results are strictly dependent on the sample and cannot be generalized to new cases. In the current paper, we used a supervised machine learning algorithm to see which structural features of the brain networks better predicts the use of reappraisal or suppression within new cases. In other words, the statistical model derived from the training sample is then tested on a different subset of data to assess generalization to unobserved cases (hold-out method). As such, this model directly tackles the predictive ability of the variables considered.

Method

Participants

Brain scans and questionnaires scores of 135 participants were included in the present study. The data were selected from “Leipzig study for mind-body-emotion interactions” (OpenNeuro database, accession number ds000221, LEMON) and were collected at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences (MPI CBS) in Leipzig (Babayan et al., 2019). The participants were prescreened by telephone interviews. The exclusion criteria for data collection were as follows: no cardiovascular disease, history of psychiatric diseases, history of neurological disorders, history of malignant diseases, intake of the following medications: centrally active medication, beta- and alpha-blocker, cortisol, any chemotherapeutic, or psychopharmacological medication. We extracted a subset of participants with negative drug test and no alcohol use. Seven participants of the original sample of 135 were excluded due to corrupted data. Therefore, the final number of participants was 128 (36 females) with mean age 29.72 ± 12.43 and average 12.73 ± 0.87 years of education. Participants provided written, informed consent, and they agreed to their data being shared anonymously. Participants received compensation for participating in the study after the completion of all assessments.

Image acquisition

Structural images were acquired using a 3 Tesla scanner (MAGNETOM Verio, Siemens Healthcare GmbH, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a 32-channel head coil.

Behavioral data

To address our experimental questions, beside sMRI data, scores from three questionnaire were considered. The German version of the ERQ (Abler and Kessler, 2009) was selected to measure the frequency of usage of reappraisal and suppression. This questionnaire consists of ten questions. Six of the questions measure the tendency to use reappraisal and four questions measure suppression. Each response is on 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The German version (Laux et al., 1981) of State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-G-X2, Spielberger et al., 1970) was used to assess the trait anxiety levels of participants. It consists of 20 questions on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 4 (nearly always). Finally, the German adaptation (Freudenthaler et al., 2008) of Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF, Petrides and Furnham, 2006) was used to assess the EI of the participants, and its subscales Well-Being, Self-Control, Emotionality, and Sociability. This questionnaire consists of 30 items, including two items from each of the 15 facets of the TEIQue.

Data analysis

Pre-processing

First, the quality of structural MRI data was assessed to exclude any possible artifacts. Data were then pre-processed using Computational Anatomy Toolbox (CAT12, http://www. neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/), a toolbox for statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12) in MATLAB environment (The Mathworks, Natick, MA). The structural images were manually reoriented to the anterior commissure as the origin. Then, the images were segmented into grey matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) using CAT12. Next, the GM image registration was conducted with Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration using Exponential Lie algebra (DARTEL) tools for SPM12 instead of traditional whole brain registration (Yassa and Stark, 2009; Grecucci et al., 2016; Pappaianni et al., 2018) Finally, the DARTEL images were normalized to MNI-152 standard space and each image were smoothed with a 12-mm, full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel [12, 12, 12].

Unsupervised machine learning to decompose the networks

ICA was applied to the structural MR images of participants to identify independent circuits across the whole brain (Xu et al., 2009; Pappaianni et al., 2018; 2020; Sorella et al., 2019; Saviola et al., 2020; Lapomarda et al., 2021a). ICA, part of Blind Sources Separation methods (Karhunen and Malaroiu, 1999), is an unsupervised machine learning procedure, which can be used to decompose the brain into naturally grouping networks based on covariations in the grey matter concentration. The resulting circuits represent specialized and partially segregated networks. In this study, the GroupICA toolbox (GIFT, http://mialab.mrn.org/software/gift/) was used inside MATLAB environment (The Mathworks, Natick, MA). Following default parameters, 20 independent components were extracted. Infomax algorithm was used to minimize the mutual information of the network outputs (Bell and Sejnowski, 1995; Lee et al., 1999). Then, we selected ICASSO, a GIFT toolbox to investigate the reliability of the ICA algorithm. RandInit and Bootstrap were selected in ICASSO to provide stability of the components (Kubera et al., 2014). As suggested by the authors, ICA was set to run 100 times, and the minimum and maximum cluster sizes were set at 80 and 100 respectively. ICA returned a matrix with the number of participants (row) and a vector of loading coefficients (columns), the columns indicate how each network is expressed in every participant. These loading coefficients were then entered into supervised machine learning to see which circuits correctly predict the use of reappraisal and suppression.

Supervised machine learning to build a predictive model

The loading coefficients found by ICA were entered into supervised machine learning to predict the use of reappraisal and suppression. The MATLAB Statistical and Machine Learning toolbox was used to conduct such analysis. The purpose of this analysis was twofold: to build a model that correctly predicts reappraisal and suppression usage, and to allow generalization of our results to predict new cases. The feature selected to build a predictive model for reappraisal usage were: STAI, TEIQue-SF subscales, ERQ suppression scores, and the ICs loading coefficients. To build a predictive model for suppression usage, the same predictors were selected, except for ERQ suppression scores, which were used instead of the reappraisal ones. Specifically, we used boosted regression tree model, which involves two techniques, namely decision tree algorithms and boosting methods. Decision trees are fit to improve the accuracy of the model and the boosted decision tree trains the model. Boosting algorithm is an adaptive method for combining many trees to improve the predictive model. Specifically, boosted regression trees are additive regression models in which simple trees are fitted in a forward, stepwise fashion. The error in each tree is calculated by a loss function and it is corrected in the next tree. Boosted regression trees have a number of advantages over tree-based methods. First, this model handles different types of predictors and accommodates missing data. In addition, prior data transformation or elimination of outliers are not required. Finally, fitting multiple trees solves the relatively poor predictive performance (Elith et al., 2008).

Further analyses

To check the consistencies of our results and to further explore the direction of the effects (not visible with the boost trees method), we also entered the predictors (ERQ-, STAI-, tEI-scores, loading coefficients of the ICs) in two separate stepwise regressions, one for each strategy to test whether they converge with the previous machine learning results. The regression module of JASP Team (2021). JASP (Version 0.16.0) was used to this aim (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Schematic diagram of the methodology. First the T1 weighted images were preprocessed. Then 20 independent components were extracted using an unsupervised machine learning approach (ICA). Finally, the prediction model for reappraisal and suppression usage was obtained via a supervised machine learning method (boosted decision trees) and stepwise regression.

Results

Unsupervised machine learning

ICA was applied to structural data (Xu et al., 2009; Pappaianni et al., 2018, 2020; Sorella et al., 2019; Saviola et al., 2020; Lapomarda et al., 2021a) and returned a matrix containing 128 rows (number of participants) and 20 columns (number of independent components). The number of components was suggested as default number by GIFT. Only ICs with quality index (Iq) > 0.9 were included (from IC1 to IC17) for further analysis. The other components (IC18,19,20) were excluded (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Independent components from 1 to 17. ICA was able to decompose the brain into 20 covarying grey matter networks. Three ICs were excluded for their reliability (Iq<0.9)

Supervised machine learning

The statistical and machine learning toolbox of MATLAB (The Mathworks, Natick, MA) built a predictive model for reappraisal and suppression usage. In this analysis, the boosted tree algorithm was chosen. The result revealed that suppression score, IC13 (temporo-parahippocampal-orbitofrontal network), STA1 score, and tEI (all subscales) were relevant features in the boosted tree model for reappraisal usage. Moreover, Reappraisal Score, IC7 (insular network), STA1 score, tEI (all subscales) were relevant features in the boosted tree model predicting suppression usage (Fig. 3; Tables 1, 2, and 4).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Top: brain plot reconstruction of C13 predicting reappraisal. Central part: brain plot reconstruction of IC7 predicting suppression. Bottom: brain plot reconstruction of IC8 predicting suppression

Table 1.

Winning models, IC13 for reappraisal and IC7 for suppression usage

Target Features Model R2 RMSE MSE MAE MLS NoL LR
RS (ERQ) SS,STA1S, IC13, Tei SS BT 0.32 0.8489 0.7206 0.6786 8 30 0.1
SS (ERQ) RS,IC7, STA1S, Tei SS BT 0.16 1.0315 0.9522 0.764 8 30 0.1
SS (ERQ) RS, IC8, STA1 S, Tei SS BT 0.12 1.0068 1.0136 0.7931 8 30 0.1

We additionally report the IC8 as the second winning model for suppression as confirmed by stepwise regression.

RS = reappraisal score; SS = suppression score; R2 = coefficient of determination; RMSE = standard deviation of the residuals; MSE = mean squared error; MAE = mean absolute error; MLS = minimum leaf size; NoL = number of learners; LR = learning rate; BT = boosted tree; Tei SS = Tei subscales; STAI S = STAI score.

Table 2.

IC features for reappraisal and suppression, R-squared and RMSE values ordered from best to worse

Target Feature R2 RMSE Target Feature R2 RMSE
Reappraisal (ERQ) Score IC13 0.32 0.8489 Suppression (ERQ) Score IC7 0.16 0.97582
IC8 0.09 0.97268 IC8 0.12 1.0068
IC1 0.08 0.9654 IC4 0.08 1.0199
IC16 0.08 0.97462 IC3 0.07 1.0255
IC4 0.06 0.97561 IC5 0.06 1.0315
IC5 0.06 0.98135 IC13 0.06 1.046
IC10 0.06 0.97761 IC17 0.05 1.0415
IC7 0.05 0.99257 IC15 0.04 1.0487
IC12 0.05 0.98817 IC9 0.02 1.066
IC6 0.04 0.98159 IC14 0.02 1.0566
IC11 0.04 1.0079 IC6 0.01 1.0608
IC2 0.03 0.99897 IC2 0.01 1.0641
IC15 0 1.0109 IC12 -0.01 1.0848
IC3 -0.04 1.0256 IC10 -0.01 1.0679
IC17 -0.04 1.0257 IC16 -0.07 1.1128
IC14 -0.08 1.0621 IC11 -0.08 1.1224
IC9 -0.09 1.0504 IC1 -0.15 1.1426

R2 = coefficient of determination; RMSE = standard deviation of the residuals.

Table 4.

Independent Component 13, 7, 8

Network Area Brodmann area Volume
(cc)
Random effects: max value (x, y, z)
Reappraisal (IC13) Inferior temporal gyrus 20, 37 0.5/2.1 5.1 (−40, −12, −27)/7.1 (53, −29, −17)
Sub-gyral 20 0.4/2.3 4.7 (−40, −16, −21)/6.8 (50, −31, −16)
Fusiform gyrus 20, 37 0.4/2.3 5.2 (−40, −14, −25)/6.6 (40, −11, −26)
(IC13) Middle temporal gyrus 20, 21 0.8/1.3 4.2 (-58, -51, 0)/6.5 (39, −6, −31)
Uncus 20, 34 0.2/0.4 4.2 (−37, −14, −27)/5.9 (36, −7, −29)
Cerebellar tonsil * 0.0/1.4 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/5.1 (25, −56, −38)
Parahippocampal gyrus 20, 28, 34, 36 0.0/0.6 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/4.9 (13, −5, −17)
Superior temporal gyrus 22 0.0/0.6 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/4.5 (52, −29, 6)
Lentiform nucleus * 0.0/0.3 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/4.3 (28, −9, 2)
Extra-nuclear * 0.0/0.2 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/3.9 (31, −6, 0)
Rectal gyrus 11 0.2/0.0 3.6 (−9, 32, −26)/−999.0 (0, 0, 0)
Thalamus * 0.1/0.0 3.5 (−18, −19, 8)/−999.0 (0, 0, 0)
Suppression (IC7) Insula 13, 40, 41 6.0/6.0 8.1 (−40, −28, 18)/7.1 (45, −25, 19)
Superior temporal gyrus 13, 22, 41, 42 1.4/0.7 7.3 (−43, −28, 15)/6.0 (48, −27, 17)
Sub-gyral * 0.4/0.4 6.6 (−40, −31, 22)/5.5 (42, −28, 24)
(IC7) Transverse temporal gyrus 41 1.2/0.4 6.6 (−40, −29, 12)/5.2 (45, −20, 12)
Inferior parietal lobule 40 2.4/1.6 6.4 (−43, −28, 22)/6.4 (46, −28, 22)
Postcentral gyrus 2, 40, 43 0.3/1.7 5.0 (−49, −25, 18)/6.2 (52, −24, 18)
Extra-nuclear * 0.1/0.4 4.1 (−34, −28, 24)/5.3 (40, −21, 22)
Claustrum * 0.0/0.1 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/4.6 (37, −5, 7)
Middle frontal gyrus 8, 46 0.0/0.4 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/4.2 (24, 22, 42)
Supramarginal gyrus 40 0.2/0.0 4.0 (−49, −39, 30)/−999.0 (0, 0, 0)
Precentral gyrus 6, 13, 44 0.1/0.4 3.6 (−45, −8, 6)/3.9 (49, −7, 7)
Middle temporal gyrus 19, 39 0.0/0.2 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/3.9 (45, −63, 18)
Superior parietal lobule 7 0.0/0.1 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/3.7 (28, −50, 43)
Medial frontal gyrus 25 0.1/0.2 3.6 (−1, 27, −13)/3.6 (4, 25, −18)
Superior frontal gyrus 8 0.0/0.1 −999.0 (0, 0, 0)/3.6 (21, 25, 43)
Suppression (IC8) Cerebellar tonsil * 2.8/2.4 11.5 (−3, −56, −38)/11.2 (3, −56, -38)
Inferior semilunar lobule * 2.4/1.7 11.3 (−3, −59, −40)/11.0 (3, −59, −40)
Fourth ventricle * 0.1/0.3 4.8 (−3, −52, −25)/9.9 (0, −53, −33)
Nodule * 1.2/1.2 8.4 (−3, −52, −30)/8.5 (0, −55, −30)
Uvula of vermis * 0.3/0.2 7.4 (−3, −61, −32)/7.0 (3, −61, −32)
Uvula * 0.8/0.4 5.3 (−6, −67, −34)/6.4 (0, −61, −30)
Culmen * 3.2/0.4 5.0 (−30, −52, −23)/3.8 (28, −52, −21)
Declive * 0.4/0.0 4.3 (−27, −59, −21)/−999.0 (0, 0, 0)
Pyramis of Vermis * 0.1/0.0 3.5 (−3, −70, −29)/−999.0 (0, 0, 0)

Talairach labels of regions of interest, Brodmann area, volume (expressed in cc) and max values coordinates are shown

Further analysis

To ensure the goodness of the boosted regression trees and to find a possible convergence across different methods, we also used regression to predict reappraisal and suppression scores. This method allows to estimate the weight of each factor in predicting the variables of interest. Multiple linear regression using stepwise data entry showed that STAI, ERQ-suppression, tEI (wellbeing subscale) and IC13 (temporo-parahippocampal-orbitofrontal network) significantly predicted reappraisal usage F (4,123) = 11.404, p < 0.001 (Bonferroni corrected threshold), following the equation: reappraisal usage = 3.758 − (0.038 * STAI) + (0.277 * IC13) + (0.250 * ERQ (suppression score) + (0.230 * Tei_wellbeing). Multiple linear regression using stepwise data entry showed that STAI, ERQ-reappraisal scores, tEI (emotionality subscale), and IC8 (frontopariatal and cerebellar network) significantly predicted suppression scores F (4,123) = 6.926, p < 0.001 (Bonferroni corrected threshold), following the equation: suppression usage = 3.405 + (0.024 * STAI) − (0.235 * IC8) + (0.264 * ERQ reappraisal score) − (0.304 * tEI_emotionality). Moreover, the result depicted that reappraisal and suppression strategies covary (Fig. 3; Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3.

Result from stepwise regression analysis with winning models IC13 for reappraisal and IC8 for suppression usage

Variable β SE t p 95% CI
STAI_Trait_Anxiety −0.038 0.011 −3.525 <0.001 [−0.059, −0.017]
ICA 13 0.277 0.078 3.54 <0.001 [0.122, 0.432]
ERQ_suppression 0.25 0.075 3.347 0.001 [0.122, 0.432]
TeiQueSF_well_being 0.23 0.104 2.201 0.03 [0.122, 0.432]
TeiQueSF_emotionality −0.364 0.11 −3.318 0.001 [−0.581, −0147]
ICA 8 −0.235 0.087 −2.692 0.008 [−0.408, −0.062]
ERQ_reappraisal 0.264 0.093 2.852 0.005 [0.081, 0.448]
STAI_Trait_Anxiety 0.024 0.012 2.103 0.037 [0.001, 0.047]

SE = standard error.

Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to provide new evidence of how individual differences in the use of two different strategies of emotion regulation, reappraisal vs. suppression, can be predicted by stable structural features of the brain and by relevant psychological features, such as anxiety and emotional intelligence. We first applied an unsupervised machine learning algorithm to sMRI scans of 128 healthy individuals to decompose the brain into naturally grouping independent grey matter circuits (ICs). The unsupervised machine learning algorithm returned 17 distinct brain networks. Among these, IC13 captured a higher grey matter concentration within a temporo-parahippocampal-orbitofrontal network and was predictive of the use of reappraisal. Differently, IC7 reflected a higher concentration of gray matter within an insular network and was predictive of suppression. Notably, the subsequent stepwise regression analysis confirmed the role of IC13 (together with the other psychological variables considered) for reappraisal, whereas IC8, capturing the network of higher gray matter concentration within fronto-temporo-cerebellar regions (together with the other psychological variables considered), appeared to predict the use of suppression. Although the IC8 was not the component highlighted in the winning model (i.e., the model with higher R and lower RMSE; Table 2) of the boosted trees, it was the second component in order of importance just after the IC7. So, we consider both good candidates for the neural bases of suppression. In the following sections, we discuss our results in detail.

Temporo-Parahippocampal-orbitofrontal network for reappraisal

The supervised machine learning approach and the stepwise regression both provide converging evidence that IC13 predicts individual differences in the use of reappraisal. IC13 is mainly composed of temporal, parahippocampal, and orbitofrontal regions. Concerning temporal regions, evidence from various neuroimaging studies converges on the importance of these areas in semantic and linguistic processes, particularly with reference to the lateral and ventral temporal cortex (middle temporal, inferior temporal, fusiform, and parahippocampal gyri) in reappraisal implementation (Ochsner & Gross, 20052007; Ochsner et al., 2004, 2012; Buhle et al., 2014; Forseth et al., 2018).

Considering the role of the frontal regions, a greater success with the use of reappraisal has been shown to be positively correlated with resting state functional connectivity between the right amygdala and the left ventrolateral PFC (Morawetz et al., 2016) and negatively correlated with the functional connectivity between the right amygdala and the medial PFC (Uchida et al., 2015). Concerning the involvement of this frontotemporal network, it nicely dovetails with a well-established psychological model of emotion regulation (Ochsner and Gross, 2008), suggesting that prefrontal control regions may intervene to regulate and adjust semantic and perceptual representations of the stimuli in lateral-temporal regions during reappraisal (Ochsner & Gross, 2005, 2007; Ochsner et al., 2012; Messina et al., 2015, 2016). Another frontal region that is considered pivotal for successful reappraisal is the orbitofrontal cortex (Wager et al., 2008). In fact, a few studies suggest it plays a role in cognitive control functions that are critical for reappraisal, such as inhibition (Ochsner et al., 2004; Banks et al., 2007; Kanske et al., 2011). It is further interesting to note that recent evidence highlights the relevance of the connectivity between the OFC and the amygdala—a critical region for emotion processing—in the context of reappraisal. For example, Gao et al. (2021) have showed that the functional coupling between orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala is associated with use of reappraisal. Also, Kanske and colleagues (2021) showed decrease activity in the amygdala and increased activity in the orbitofrontal cortex during reappraisal. Possibly, these links between OFC and amygdala are an important neural underpinning of emotional regulations strategies and reappraisal in particular.

Concerning the parahippocampal gyrus, evidence suggests that this area plays a key role in memory processes, including coding and retrieval, as well as in emotional processes (Hamann, 2001; Gosselin et al., 2006; Van den Stock et al., 2012; Frank et al., 2014). In particular, according to Deak et al. (2017), increase activity in the parahippocampal area during reappraisal may be due to the semantic processes recruited when shaping a different interpretation of the same context. Consistently, a few studies have shown a link between parahippocampus and amygdala, in terms of functional connectivity. This link possibly mediates the connection between semantic/contextual processing and emotion (Aminoff et al., 2013; LaBar & Cabeza, 2006).

As expected, this temporo-parahippocampal-orbitofrontal network (IC13) partially overlaps with the DMN. The DMN includes brain regions that are active when individuals are not engaged in a specific task or during self-referential processes and self-generated thoughts (Andrews-Hanna, 2012; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014). Core regions of the DMN indeed consist of posterior cingulate cortex, parts of the precuneus, medial PFC, bilateral inferior parietal lobule, and parts of the posterior temporal areas. In addition, hippocampus, medial temporal lobe, lateral temporal cortex and temporal pole also are often believed to be part of the DMN (Buckner et al., 2008; Broyd et al., 2009; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Power et al., 2011). Interestingly, the dorsal medial PFC, the angular gyrus, the middle temporal gyrus, and the anterior temporal region that are part of DMN are involved in semantic processing (Binder et al., 2009; Wirth et al., 2011). The DMN may be involved in conceptual processing, perspective-taking, and reasoning; all these processes are crucial in reappraisal (Buhle et al., 2014). In conclusion, the greater the grey matter concentration inside this network, the greater these underlying abilities and, thus, the larger the reappraisal usage.

Insular network and Fronto-parietal-cerebellar network predict suppression

The supervised machine learning approach returned the IC7 to be the best neural predictor of suppression usage. This network involves the insula and other key regions. The insula receives and combines inputs from various limbic and cortical regions, such as the amygdala the anterior cingulate cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex. The integration of these regions produces a coherent model of self that consist of bodily states (Craig, 2002; 2009; 2010), which are essential elements when asked to suppress our emotions. Notably, a previous experiment on the regulation of emotions elicited during interpersonal situations reported significant modulation of the insula during emotion regulation (Grecucci et al., 2013a; b). These authors hypothesized that the modulation of activity at the level of the insula may represent the regulation of the emotion-driven physiological arousal.

For what entails the frontal regions included in IC7, Hayes et al. (2010) highlighted the association between the activation of the middle frontal gyrus and the ability to suppress facial expressions. Hence, the increased grey matter concentration in this area for frequent users of suppression strategies might stem from an enhanced awareness of facial expression. In addition, this network included superior temporal regions and the middle temporal gyrus. These areas may be involved in mentalizing, especially with reference to the awareness of others’ intentions through the decoding of facial expressions or head and body motion (Frith and Frith, 2003; Dörfel et al., 2014). Remarkably, this network greatly overlap with the so-called salience network, which indeed involves the insula, but also portions of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, the amygdala, the ventral striatum, and the substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area (Seeley et al., 2007; Menon and Uddin, 2010). Suppression relies on the integration of interoceptive awareness, proprioceptive awareness, social awareness, and personal salience (Muhtadie et al., 2021). The salience network is active during salient emotional stimuli, social behavior, and self-awareness (Craig, 2009; Menon and Uddin, 2010; Gogolla et al., 2014). For these reasons, the salience network may play a role in the use of suppression. A representation of the state of the body as encoded in the insula (Muhtadie et al., 2021) may be necessary for suppression to modulate the bodily affective state (Grecucci et al., 2013a, b). Again, the greater the grey-matter concentration inside this network, the greater these abilities, thus the enhanced use of suppression.

Stepwise regression revealed IC8 to play a major role in predicting the use of suppression, although with a negative relation. This circuit also was highlighted by the supervised machine-learning analysis (just after the IC7 for importance). IC8 consists of a large frontopariatal and cerebellar network. Frontal regions of the brain are considered to be very important for cognitive strategies of emotion regulation (Buhle et al., 2014; Kohn et al., 2014) but also in coordinating motor areas and in general cognitive monitoring of other functions (Aron et al., 2014; DePue et al., 2016). Moreover, frontopariatel regions have been shown to support attentional control functions (Cole et al., 2013, 2014; Dodds et al., 2011; Power et al., 2011; Scolari et al., 2015). The negative relation between such cognitive control/attentional regions (IC8) and suppression may be interpreted as indicating that suppression may rely less on top-down control mechanisms, which are instead pivotal when deploying cognitive control and attentional strategies. Both attention and cognitive control would thus not be involved in suppression. Building on this finding, one can conclude that the lesser the grey matter concentration inside this network, the lesser cognitive control abilities displayed, the larger the usage of suppression.

Emotional intelligence and anxiety in predicting reappraisal and suppression

Our analyses (both boosted trees and stepwise regression) confirmed the additional role of anxiety and EI in predicting the use of reappraisal. Reappraisal has been generally considered an adaptive strategy, associated with healthiness and personal satisfaction (Aldao et al., 2010), that successfully modulates the affective state (Webb et al., 2012). Our analyses confirmed a positive relationship between EI and reappraisal (Hertel et al., 2009; Fernández-Berrocal and Extremera, 2016). Also, reappraisal has been long considered a protective factor over anxiety disorders (Hofmann et al., 2009). Our results confirm the negative relationship between anxiety and reappraisal. Importantly, the analyses also confirmed the role of anxiety and EI in suppression, although the direction, as expected, was the opposite compared with reappraisal. For what concerns EI (emotionality subscale), we have found that it is negatively correlated with the use of suppression. This further confirms previous observations according to which suppression comes with several costs in terms of physiological, cognitive, and emotional functioning (Nezlek and Kuppens, 2008; Brans, et al., 2013). Indeed, suppression has been associated with decreased emotional well-being and difficulties in the recognition and expression of emotions (Petrides, 2009). Thus, our study confirms previous investigations and suggests a relationship between low EI and maladaptive emotion regulation processes (Peña-Sarrionandia et al., 2015).

Reappraisal and suppression are complementary strategies

Finally, our results suggest that reappraisal and suppression, respectively considered as adaptive and maladaptive strategies, are not mutually exclusive. In our model, the usage of one predicts the other, and they covary in a positive way. This means that individuals using one strategy also may be prone to use the other (Ferschmann et al., 2021), suggesting that an adaptive regulation may be built upon a wide range of strategies to be selected according to the context (Sahdra et al., 2020).

It is worth mentioning that previous studies separately assessed the association between one strategy and factors, such as anxiety, EI, and the use of other strategies. In our study, instead, we included all these variables in one unique model, thus enabling the evaluation of their relative influence. This model thus provides information concerning the joint role of these factors in predicting reappraisal or suppression. From our results, it is clear that EI and the use of the other strategy both outperform anxiety in their ability to predict the use of specific strategies (see the equations in the Results section).

Conclusions and limitations

The purpose of this study was to decode individual differences in the use of reappraisal and suppression from structural brain networks, EI, and anxiety scores by using supervised and unsupervised machine-learning techniques. The result revealed a temporo-parahippocampal-orbitofrontal network predicting the habitual use of reappraisal. Differently, an insular network and a fronto-parietal-cerebellar network significantly predicted the use of suppression. In addition, the results reveal that EI and anxiety are both significant predictors of the use of reappraisal and suppression, although in opposite directions. Finally, the results suggest that reappraisal and suppression are complementary strategies, and the use each strategy is positively associated with the use of the other one. From the results of this study, we can conclude that independent neural circuits, EI, and anxiety jointly predict individual differences in the use of two important strategies of emotion regulation.

There are some limitations to note. Self-report questionnaires were used, and biases in these types of assessments due to the lack of awareness might have affected the results. Also, whereas the sample size of our study is in line with the recent literature focusing on similar issues in terms of structural neuroimaging data (Picó-Pérez et al., 2019; Baltruschat et al., 2021), it is smaller compared to investigations focusing on behavioural and psychological measures and predictors (Martin and Dahlen, 2005; Andrei et al., 2016; Nozaki, 2018). This warrants some caution when interpreting the individual differences at the level of psychological dimensions.

Furthermore, we only used structural data limited to grey matter. Future studies may want to extend these results to with matter and functional data. Finally, we used boosted regression trees to predict strategies usage. Other machine learning approaches are available and could have been used to predict the variable of interest. Because we do not know yet which algorithm works better for what kind of data, the present study may pave the way for additional methodological research on the comparison between different algorithms.

Funding

Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Trento within the CRUI-CARE Agreement. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration

Competing interest

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Abler, B., Hofer, C., & Viviani, R. (2008). Habitual emotion regulation strategies and baseline brain perfusion. www.cfn.upenn.edu/perfusion/software.htm [DOI] [PubMed]
  2. Abler B, Hofer C, Walter H, Erk S, Hoffmann H, Traue HC, Kessler H. Habitual emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms in healthy subjects predict fMRI brain activation patterns related to major depression. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging. 2010;183(2):105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Abler B, Kessler H. Emotion regulation questionnaire—Eine deutsche Version des ERQ von Gross & John [A German version of ERQ] Diagnostica. 2009;55:144–152. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.55.3.144. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aldao, A., & ’Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). When are adaptive strategies most predictive of psychopathology? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 276–281. [DOI] [PubMed]
  5. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(2):217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aminoff EM, Kveraga K, Bar M. The role of the parahippocampal cortex in cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17(8):379–390. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7(4):268–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Andrei F, Smith MM, Surcinelli P, Baldaro B, Saklofske DH. The trait emotional intelligence questionnaire: Internal structure, convergent, criterion, and incremental validity in an Italian sample. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development. 2016;49(1):34–45. doi: 10.1177/0748175615596786. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Andrews-Hanna JR. The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist. 2012;18(3):251–270. doi: 10.1177/1073858411403316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the Brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010;65(4):550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2014;1316(1):29–52. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Trends in cognitive sciences. Elsevier Ltd.; 2014. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on; pp. 177–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Babayan, A., Erbey, M., Kumral, D., Reinelt, J. D., Reiter, A. M. F., Röbbig, J., Lina Schaare, H., Uhlig, M., Anwander, A., Bazin, P. L., Horstmann, A., Lampe, L., Nikulin, V., & , Okon-Singer, H., Preusser, S., Pampel, A., Rohr, C. S., Sacher, J., Thöne-Otto, A., … Villringer, A. (2019). Data descriptor: A mind-brain-body dataset of MRI, EEG, cognition, emotion, and peripheral physiology in young and old adults. Scientific Data, 6. 10.1038/sdata.2018.308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  14. Baltruschat, S., Cándido, A., Maldonado, A., Verdejo-Lucas, C., Catena-Verdejo, E., & Catena, A. (2021). There is more to mindfulness than emotion regulation: A study on brain structural networks. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659403 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  15. Banks SJ, Eddy KT, Angstadt M, Nathan PJ, Luan Phan K. Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2007;2(4):303–312. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Beauregard M. Mind does really matter: Evidence from neuroimaging studies of emotional self-regulation, psychotherapy, and placebo effect. Progress in Neurobiology. 2007;81(4):218–236. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bell, A. J., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1995). An information-maximisation approach t o blind separation and blind deconvolution. [DOI] [PubMed]
  18. Benson L, English T, Conroy DE, Pincus AL, Gerstorf D, Ram N. Age differences in emotion regulation strategy use, variability, and flexibility: An experience sampling approach. Developmental Psychology. 2019;55(9):1951–1964. doi: 10.1037/dev0000727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19(12):2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Brans K, Koval P, Verduyn P, Lim YL, Kuppens P. The regulation of negative and positive affect in daily life. Emotion. 2013;13(5):926–939. doi: 10.1037/a0032400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Broyd SJ, Demanuele C, Debener S, Helps SK, James CJ, Sonuga-Barke EJS. Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33(3):279–296. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38. 10.1196/annals.1440.011 [DOI] [PubMed]
  23. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wage TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, Webe J, Ochsner KN. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24(11):2981–2990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cabello R, Salguero JM, Fernández-Berrocal P, Gross JJ. A Spanish adaptation of the emotion regulation questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2013;29(4):234–240. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000150. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH, Brown TA, Hofmann SG. Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006;44(9):1251–1263. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cato MA, Crosson B, Gökçay D, Soltysik D, Wierenga C, Gopinath K, Himes N, Belanger H, Bauer RM, Fischler IS, Gonzalez-Rothi L, Briggs RW. Processing words with emotional connotation: An fMRI study of time course and laterality in rostral frontal and Retrosplenial cortices. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16(2):167–177. doi: 10.1162/089892904322984481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cole MW, Bassett DS, Power JD, Braver TS, Petersen SE. Intrinsic and task-evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron. 2014;83(1):238–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16(9):1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature reviews neuroscience volume 3. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2002;3:655–656. doi: 10.1038/nrn894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Craig AD. Emotional moments across time: A possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2009;364(1525):1933–1942. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Craig AD. Once an island, now the focus of attention. Brain Structure & Function. 2010;214(5–6):395–396. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0270-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Crosson B, Cato MA, Sadek JR, Gökçay D, Bauer RM, Fischler IS, Maron L, Gopinath K, Auerbach EJ, Browd SR, Briggs RW. Semantic monitoring of words with emotional connotation during fMRI: Contribution of anterior left frontal cortex. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8(5):607–622. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702801394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dadomo H, Grecucci A, Giardini I, Ugolini E, Carmelita A, Panzeri M. Schema therapy for emotional dysregulation: Theoretical implication and clinical applications. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7:1987. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dadomo H, Panzeri M, Caponcello D, Carmelita A, Grecucci A. Current opinion in psychiatry. Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2018. Schema therapy for emotional dysregulation in personality disorders: A review; pp. 43–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Deak A, Bodrogi B, Biro B, Perlaki G, Orsi G, Bereczkei T. Machiavellian emotion regulation in a cognitive reappraisal task: An fMRI study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2017;17:528–541. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0495-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. De Panfilis C, Schito G, Generali I, Gozzi L, Ossola P, Marchesi C, Grecucci A. Emotions at the border: Increased punishment behavior during fair interpersonal exchanges in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2019;128(2):162–172. doi: 10.1037/abn0000404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. DePue MK, Lambie GW, Liu R, Gonzalez J. Investigating supervisory relationships and therapeutic alliances using structural equation modeling. Counselor Education and Supervision. 2016;55(4):263–277. doi: 10.1002/ceas.12053. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Desrosiers A, Vine V, Klemanski DH, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Mindfulness and emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: Common and distinct mechanisms of action. Depression and Anxiety. 2013;30(7):654–661. doi: 10.1002/da.22124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dodds CM, Morein-Zamir S, Robbins TW. Dissociating inhibition, attention, and response control in the frontoparietal network using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21(5):1155–1165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Donahue JJ, Goranson AC, McClure KS, van Male LM. Emotion dysregulation, negative affect, and aggression: A moderated, multiple mediator analysis. Personality and Individual Differences. 2014;70:23–28. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dörfel D, Lamke JP, Hummel F, Wagner U, Erk S, Walter H. Common and differential neural networks of emotion regulation by detachment, reinterpretation, distraction, and expressive suppression: A comparative fMRI investigation. NeuroImage. 2014;101:298–309. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Elith J, Leathwick JR, Hastie T. A working guide to boosted regression trees. In Journal of Animal Ecology. 2008;77(4):802–813. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01390.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Fernández-Berrocal P, Extremera N. Ability emotional intelligence, depression, and well-being. Emotion Review. 2016;8(4):311–315. doi: 10.1177/1754073916650494. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Forseth, K. J., Kadipasaoglu, C. M., Conner, C. R., Hickok, G., Knight, R. T., & Tandon, N. (2018). A lexical semantic hub for heteromodal naming in middle fusiform gyrus. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 141(7), 2112–2126. 10.1093/brain/awy120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  45. Ferschmann L, Vijayakumar N, Grydeland H, Overbye K, Mills KL, Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Pfeifer JH, Tamnes CK. Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression relate differentially to longitudinal structural brain development across adolescence. Cortex. 2021;136:109–123. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Frank, D. W., Dewitt, M., Hudgens-Haney, M., Schaeffer, D. J., Ball, B. H., Schwarz, N. F., Hussein, A. A., Smart, L. M., & Sabatinelli, D. (2014). Emotion regulation: Quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. In neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews, Vol. 45, pp. 202–211. Elsevier ltd. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed]
  47. Frederickson, J. J., Messina, I., & Grecucci, A. (2018). Dysregulated anxiety and dysregulating defenses: Toward an emotion regulation informed dynamic psychotherapy. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  48. Freudenthaler HH, Neubauer AC, Gabler P, Scherl WG, Rindermann H. Testing and validating the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue) in a German-speaking sample. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45(7):673–678. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. In Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological sciences, Vol. 358, Issue 1431, pp. 459–473. Royal Society. 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  50. Gao, W., Biswal, B., Chen, S. D., Wu, X. R., & Yuan, J. J. (2021). Functional coupling of the orbitofrontal cortex and the basolateral amygdala mediates the association between spontaneous reappraisal and emotional response. NeuroImage, 232. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117918 [DOI] [PubMed]
  51. Garland EL, Gaylord SA, Fredrickson BL. Positive reappraisal mediates the stress-reductive effects of mindfulness: An upward spiral process. Mindfulness. 2011;2(1):59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0043-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Giuliani NR, Drabant EM, Bhatnagar R, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and brain plasticity: Expressive suppression use predicts anterior insula volume. NeuroImage. 2011;58(1):10–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Giuliani NR, Drabant EM, Gross JJ. Anterior cingulate cortex volume and emotion regulation: Is bigger better? Biological Psychology. 2011;86(3):379–382. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gogolla N, Takesian AE, Feng G, Fagiolini M, Hensch TK. Sensory integration in mouse insular cortex reflects GABA circuit maturation. Neuron. 2014;83(4):894–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Goldin PR, McRae K, Ramel W, Gross JJ. The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;63(6):577–586. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Goldin PR, Moodie CA, Gross JJ. Acceptance versus reappraisal: Behavioral, autonomic, and neural effects. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;19(4):927–944. doi: 10.3758/s13415-019-00690-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gosselin N, Samson S, Adolphs R, Noulhiane M, Roy M, Hasboun D, Baulac M, Peretz I. Emotional responses to unpleasant music correlates with damage to the parahippocampal cortex. Brain. 2006;129(10):2585–2592. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Grecucci A, Giorgetta C, Van’t Wout, M., Bonini, N., and Sanfey, A. G. Reappraising the ultimatum: An fMRI study of emotion regulation and decision making. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:399–410. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Grecucci A, Giorgetta C, Bonini N, Sanfey AG. Living emotions, avoiding emotions: Behavioral investigation of the regulation of socially driven emotions. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;3:616. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Grecucci A, Messina I, Amodeo L, Lapomarda G, Crescentini C, Dadomo H, Panzeri M, Theuninck A, Frederickson J. A dual route model for regulating emotions: Comparing models, techniques and biological mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Grecucci, A., Rubicondo, D., Siugzdaite, R., Surian, L., & Job, R. (2016). Uncovering the social deficits in the autistic brain. A source-based morphometric study. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10(AUG). 10.3389/fnins.2016.00388 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  62. Gross J. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology. 1998;2:271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Gross, J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3). [DOI] [PubMed]
  64. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry. 2015;26(1):1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348–362. 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 [DOI] [PubMed]
  66. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(6), 970–986. 10.1037//0022-3514.64.6.970 [DOI] [PubMed]
  67. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106(1):95–103. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Gross J, ’Muñoz R. Emotion regulation and mental health. Clinical Psychology. 1995;2(2):151–164. [Google Scholar]
  69. Gruber J, Harvey AG, Wang PW, Brooks JO, Thase ME, Sachs GS, Ketter TA. Sleep functioning in relation to mood, function, and quality of life at entry to the systematic treatment enhancement program for bipolar disorder (STEP-BD) Journal of Affective Disorders. 2009;114(1–3):41–49. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Hamann, s. Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5(9):394–400. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01707-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hamann S. Trends in cognitive sciences. Elsevier Ltd.; 2012. Mapping discrete and dimensional emotions onto the brain: Controversies and consensus; pp. 458–466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hayes, J. P., Morey, R. A., Petty, C. M., Seth, S., Smoski, M. J., McCarthy, G., & LaBar, K. S. (2010). Staying cool when things get hot: Emotion regulation modulates neural mechanisms of memory encoding. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4. 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00230 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  73. Hermann A, Bieber A, Keck T, Vaitl D, Stark R. Brain structural basis of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013;9(9):1435–1442. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Hermann A, Leutgeb V, Scharmüller W, Vaitl D, Schienle A, Stark R. Individual differences in cognitive reappraisal usage modulate the time course of brain activation during symptom provocation in specific phobia. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders. 2013;3(1):16. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-3-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hertel J, Schütz A, Lammers CH. Emotional intelligence and mental disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2009;65(9):942–954. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Hofmann SG, Heering S, Sawyer AT, Asnaani A. How to handle anxiety: The effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2009;47(5):389–394. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. JASP Team. (2021). JASP (Ver. 0.16.0) [computer software]. https://jasp-stats.org/
  78. John OP, Gross JJ. Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:1301–1334. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. John OP, Eng J. Three approaches to individual differences in affect regulation: Conceptualizations, measures, and findings. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. 2. Guilford; 2014. pp. 321–345. [Google Scholar]
  80. Johnson SL. Life events in bipolar disorder: Towards more specific models. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25(8):1008–1027. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kanske P, Heissler J, Schönfelder S, Bongers A, Wessa M. How to regulate emotion? Neural networks for reappraisal and distraction. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21(6):1379–1388. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Karhunen, J., & Malaroiu, S. (1999). Local independent component analysis using clustering. [DOI] [PubMed]
  83. Kohn N, Eickhoff SB, Scheller M, Laird AR, Fox PT, Habel U. Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation - an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. NeuroImage. 2014;87:345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. M. (2009). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. Guliford.
  85. Kring AM, Werner KH. Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. pp. 359–385. [Google Scholar]
  86. Kubera KM, Sambataro F, Vasic N, Wolf ND, Frasch K, Hirjak D, Thomann PA, Wolf RC. Source-based morphometry of gray matter volume in patients with schizophrenia who have persistent auditory verbal hallucinations. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2014;50:102–109. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Kühn, S., Gallinat, J., & Brass, M. (2011). “Keep calm and carry on”: Structural correlates of expressive suppression of emotions. PLoS One, 6(1). 10.1371/journal.pone.0016569 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  88. LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7(1):54–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn1825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lapomarda, G., Grecucci, A., Messina, I., Pappaianni, E., & Dadomo, H. (2021a). Common and different gray and white matter alterations in bipolar and borderline personality disorder: A source-based morphometry study. Brain Research, 1762, 147401. 10.1016/j.brainres.2021.147401 [DOI] [PubMed]
  90. Lapomarda, G., Pappaianni, E., Siugzdaite, R., Sanfey, A. G., Rumiati, R. I., & Grecucci, A. (2021b). Out of control: An altered parieto-occipital-cerebellar network for impulsivity in bipolar disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 406, 113228. 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113228 [DOI] [PubMed]
  91. Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C. (1981). Das state-trait-Angstinventar. Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisung. Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH.
  92. Leahy RL, Tirch D, Napolitano LA. Emotion regulation in psychotherapy: A practitioner’s guide. 2011. [Google Scholar]
  93. Lee TW, Girolami M, Sejnowski TJ. Independent component analysis using an extended infomax algorithm for mixed subgaussian and supergaussian sources. Neural Computation. 1999;11(2):417–441. doi: 10.1162/089976699300016719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Linehan, M. (1993). Skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder. Guilford Press.
  95. Martin RC, Dahlen ER. Cognitive emotion regulation in the prediction of depression, anxiety, stress, and anger. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;39(7):1249–1260. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. Martins B, Mather M. Default mode network and later-life emotion regulation: Linking functional connectivity patterns and emotional outcomes. In: Ong AD, Löckenhoff CE, editors. Emotion, aging, and health. American Psychological Association; 2016. pp. 9–29. [Google Scholar]
  97. Mauss IB, Levenson RW, McCarter L, Wilhelm FH, Gross JJ. The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion. 2005;5(2):175–190. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Mayer, J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (Salovey P & D. Sluyter, Eds.). Educational implications.
  99. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure & Function. 2010;214(5–6):655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Messina I, Grecucci A, Viviani R. Neurobiological models of emotion regulation: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of acceptance as an emotion regulation strategy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2021;16(3):257–267. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Messina I, Bianco S, Sambin M, Viviani R. Executive and semantic processes in reappraisal of negative stimuli: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:956. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Messina, I., Sambin, M., Palmieri, A., & Viviani, R. (2013). Neural correlates of psychotherapy in anxiety and depression: A meta-analysis. PLoS One, 8(9). 10.1371/journal.pone.0074657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  103. Messina I, Sambin M, Beschoner P, Viviani R. Changing views of emotion regulation and neurobiological models of the mechanism of action of psychotherapy. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2016;16(4):571–587. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0440-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Monachesi, B., Grecucci, A., Ahmadi Ghomroudi, P., & Messina, I. (under review). Understanding the neural architecture of emotion regulation by comparing two opposite strategies: A meta-analytic approach. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  105. Morawetz C, Bode S, Baudewig J, Kirilina E, Heekeren HR. Changes in effective connectivity between dorsal and ventral prefrontal regions moderate emotion regulation. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26(5):1923–1937. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Muhtadie L, Haase CM, Verstaen A, Sturm VE, Miller BL, Levenson RW. Neuroanatomy of expressive suppression: The role of the insula. Emotion. 2021;21(2):405–418. doi: 10.1037/emo0000710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Nezlek JB, Kuppens P. Regulating positive and negative emotions in daily life. Journal of Personality. 2008;76(3):561–580. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00496.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Nozaki Y. Cross-cultural comparison of the association between trait emotional intelligence and emotion regulation in European-American and Japanese populations. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018;130:150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  109. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(5):242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The neural architecture of emotion regulation. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. The Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 87–109. [Google Scholar]
  111. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. Cognitive emotion regulation: Insights from social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17(2):153–158. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00566.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, Robertson ER, Chopra S, Gabrieli JDE, Gross JJ. For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage. 2004;23(2):483–499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, Buhle JT. Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2012;1251:E1–E24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Olsson, A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2008). The role of social cognition in emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences (Vol. 12, issue 2, pp. 65–71). 10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed]
  115. Pappaianni E, de Pisapia N, Siugzdaite R, Crescentini C, Calcagnì A, Job R, Grecucci A. Less is more: Morphometric and psychological differences between low and high reappraisers. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2020;20(1):128–140. doi: 10.3758/s13415-019-00757-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Pappaianni E, Siugzdaite R, Vettori S, Venuti P, Job R, Grecucci A. Three shades of grey: Detecting brain abnormalities in children with autism using source-, voxel- and surface-based morphometry. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;47(6):690–700. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Peh, C. X., Liu, J., Bishop, G. D., Chan, H. Y., Chua, S. M., Kua, E. H., & Mahendran, R. (2017). Emotion regulation and emotional distress: The mediating role of hope on reappraisal and anxiety/depression in newly diagnosed cancer patients. Psychooncology, 1191–1197. [DOI] [PubMed]
  118. Peña-Sarrionandia, A., Mikolajczak, M., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Integrating emotion regulation and emotional intelligence traditions: A meta-analysis. In Frontiers in psychology, Vol. 6, Issue FEB. Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  119. Petrides KV. Psychometric properties of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue) In: Parker J, Saklofske D, Stough C, editors. Assessing emotional intelligence. The Springer series on human exceptionality. Springer; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  120. Petrides KV, Furnham A. TEIQue-SF: Trait emotional intelligence questionnaire-short form. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2006;36:552–569. doi: 10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00019.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  121. Picó-Pérez M, Alonso P, Contreras-Rodríguez O, Martínez-Zalacaín I, López-Solà C, Jiménez-Murcia S, Verdejo-García A, Menchón JM, Soriano-Mas C. Dispositional use of emotion regulation strategies and resting-state cortico-limbic functional connectivity. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2018;12(4):1022–1031. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9762-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Picó-Pérez M, Ipser J, Taylor P, Alonso P, López-Solà C, Real E, Segalàs C, Roos A, Menchón JM, Stein DJ, Soriano-Mas C. Intrinsic functional and structural connectivity of emotion regulation networks in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety. 2019;36(2):110–120. doi: 10.1002/da.22845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Poldrack RA, Baker CI, Durnez J, Gorgolewski KJ, Matthews PM, Munafò MR, Nichols TE, Poline JB, Vul E, Yarkoni T. Scanning the horizon: Towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2017;18(2):115–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Poldrack RA, Gorgolewski KJ. OpenfMRI: Open sharing of task fMRI data. NeuroImage. 2017;144:259–261. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, Vogel AC, Laumann TO, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Functional network Organization of the Human Brain. Neuron. 2011;72(4):665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2001;98(2):676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Sahdra, B. K., Brockman, R., Hayes, S. C., Hofmann, S., Kashdan, T., & Ciarrochi, J. (2020). Individual differences in the co-use of reappraisal and emotion suppression in daily life: Implications for daily positive and negative affect.
  128. Salters-Pedneault K, Roemer L, Tull MT, Rucker L, Mennin DS. Evidence of broad deficits in emotion regulation associated with chronic worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2006;30(4):469–480. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9055-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  129. Saviola, F., Pappaianni, E., Monti, A., Grecucci, A., Jovicich, J., & de Pisapia, N. (2020). Trait and state anxiety are mapped differently in the human brain. Scientific Reports, 10(1). 10.1038/s41598-020-68008-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  130. Schutte NS, Manes RR, Malouff JM. Antecedent-focused emotion regulation, response modulation and well-being. Current Psychology. 2009;28(1):21–31. doi: 10.1007/s12144-009-9044-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  131. Scolari M, Seidl-Rathkopf KN, Kastner S. Current opinion in behavioral sciences. Elsevier Ltd.; 2015. Functions of the human frontoparietal attention network: Evidence from neuroimaging; pp. 32–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(9):2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Sheppes G, Suri G, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2015;11:379–405. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Sher KJ, Bartholow BD, Peuser K, Erickson DJ, Wood MD. Stress-response-dampening effects of alcohol: attention as a mediator and moderator. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116(2):362–377. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Sikka P, Stenberg J, Vorobyev V, Gross JJ. The neural bases of expressive suppression: A systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2022;138:104708. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Sorella, S., Lapomarda, G., Messina, I., Frederickson, J. J., Siugzdaite, R., Job, R., & Grecucci, A. (2019). Testing the expanded continuum hypothesis of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neural and psychological evidence for shared and distinct mechanisms. NeuroImage: Clinical, 23. 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  137. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. STAI manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; 1970. [Google Scholar]
  138. Sripada C, Angstadt M, Kessler D, et al. Volitional regulation of emotions produces distributed alterations in connectivity between visual, attention control, and default networks. NeuroImage. 2014;89:110–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Uchida, M., Biederman, J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Micco, J., de Los Angeles, C., Brown, A., Kenworthy, T., Kagan, E., & Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2015). Emotion regulation ability varies in relation to intrinsic functional brain architecture. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(12), 1738–1748. 10.1093/scan/nsv059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  140. van den Stock J, de Gelder B. Emotional information in body and background hampers recognition memory for faces. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2012;97(3):321–325. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Vanderhasselt MA, Baeken C, Van Schuerbeek P, Luypaert R, De Raedt R. Inter-individual differences in the habitual use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression are associated with variations in prefrontal cognitive control for emotional information: An event related fMRI study. Biological Psychology. 2013;92(3):433–439. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008;59(6):1037–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Webb TL, Miles E, Sheeran P. Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin. 2012;138(4):775–808. doi: 10.1037/a0027600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Werner KH, Goldin PR, Ball TM, Heimberg RG, Gross JJ. Assessing emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder: The emotion regulation interview. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2011;33(3):346–354. doi: 10.1007/s10862-011-9225-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  145. Wirth M, Jann K, Dierks T, Federspiel A, Wiest R, Horn H. Semantic memory involvement in the default mode network: A functional neuroimaging study using independent component analysis. NeuroImage. 2011;54(4):3057–3066. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Xie X, Mulej Bratec S, Schmid G, Meng C, Doll A, Wohlschläger A, Finke K, Förstl H, Zimmer C, Pekrun R, Schilbach L, Riedl V, Sorg C. How do you make me feel better? Social cognitive emotion regulation and the default mode network. NeuroImage. 2016;134:270–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Xu L, Groth KM, Pearlson G, Schretlen DJ, Calhoun VD. Source-based morphometry: The use of independent component analysis to identify gray matter differences with application to schizophrenia. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30(3):711–724. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Yassa MA, Stark CEL. A quantitative evaluation of cross-participant registration techniques for MRI studies of the medial temporal lobe. NeuroImage. 2009;44(2):319–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES