Abstract
The parabrachial nuclear complex (PBN) is a nexus for aversion and for the sensory and affective components of pain perception. We have previously shown that during chronic pain PBN neurons in anesthetized rodents have amplified activity. We report a method to record from PBN neurons of behaving, head-restrained mice while applying reproducible noxious stimuli. We find that both spontaneous and evoked activity are higher in awake animals compared with urethane anesthetized mice. Fiber photometry of calcium responses from calcitonin-gene-related peptide–expressing PBN neurons demonstrates that these neurons respond to noxious stimuli. In both males and females with neuropathic or inflammatory pain, responses of PBN neurons remain amplified for at least 5 weeks, in parallel with increased pain metrics. We also show that PBN neurons can be rapidly conditioned to respond to innocuous stimuli after pairing with noxious stimuli. Finally, we demonstrate that changes in PBN neuronal activity are correlated with changes in arousal, measured as changes in pupil area.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The parabrachial complex is a nexus of aversion, including pain. We report a method to record from parabrachial nucleus neurons of behaving mice while applying reproducible noxious stimuli. This allowed us to track parabrachial activity over time in animals with neuropathic or inflammatory pain. It also allowed us to show that the activity of these neurons correlates with arousal states and that these neurons can be conditioned to respond to innocuous stimuli.
Keywords: anesthesia, aversion, CGRP, chronic pain, conditioned response, fiber photometry
Introduction
Pain is a multidimensional experience, composed of unpleasant sensory, affective, and cognitive experiences (Melzack and Casey, 1968; Fields, 1999; Price, 2000; Treede et al., 2000; Auvray et al., 2010). Because of the multidimensionality of the pain experience, chronic pain patients suffer from comorbid emotional and cognitive deficits and exhibit alterations in the function of brain networks linked to these deficits (Borsook, 2012; Bushnell et al., 2013). Therefore, treating pain, and especially chronic pain, requires addressing not only the sensory aspects of pain but also its affective and cognitive components.
Chronic pain affecting the trigeminal system, in particular, is associated with deficits in affective processing, including increases in anxiety and depression (Smith et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2015; Turnes et al., 2022). A key region in the trigeminal system is the parabrachial nuclear complex (PBN). It is a nexus of aversion and of both the nociceptive and affective components of pain processing (Chiang et al., 2019). PBN is involved in other aversive behaviors (Cai et al., 2018), including taste aversion (Carter et al., 2015; Chen et al., 2018), threat memory (Han et al., 2015), and fear conditioning (Campos et al., 2018; Bowen et al., 2020). We and others have found that PBN neurons respond to noxious stimuli under urethane anesthesia (Bernard and Besson, 1990; Uddin et al., 2021) and display amplified activity during chronic pain (Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020).
Most of what is known about the physiology of PBN in the context of pain comes from studies using anesthetized animals. This is problematic because anesthesia may blunt pain-related activity. Further, anesthesia does not allow studies of state-dependent changes in pain perception, changes that are critical to pain behavior. Pain perception depends on both external stimuli and the internal state of the animal. Internal states such as distraction (Buhle and Wager, 2010; Legrain et al., 2011) or hunger (Alhadeff et al., 2018) profoundly alter pain perception. Therefore, it is essential to determine how internal states affect PBN processing in behaving animals.
Recordings of nociceptive responses from PBN in awake animals are complicated by at least two major technical difficulties. First, the location of PBN—at the junction of the midbrain and pons and its relation to major vessels—renders recordings from single units unstable and low signal-to-noise ratios in behaving animals. The second complication is the ability to present to behaving animals reproducible noxious stimuli. We have adapted a head-restrained, awake rodent recording model to address these issues. This allowed us to investigate the physiology of PBN in awake animals in response to both noxious and non-noxious stimuli and the role of internal states in PBN processing.
Materials and Methods
We adhered to accepted standards for rigorous study design and reporting to maximize the reproducibility and translational potential of our findings as described by Landis et al. (2012) and in ARRIVE (Animal Research: Reporting in Vivo Experiments) guidelines. In line with National Institutes of Health recommendations for scientific rigor, we performed an a priori power analysis to estimate required sample sizes (Landis et al., 2012).
Animals
All procedures were conducted according to Animal Welfare Act regulations and Public Health Service guidelines and approved by the University of Maryland School of Medicine Animal Care and Use Committee. We used the following 27 male and female wild-type C57BL/6 mice from The Jackson Laboratory (strain #000644): 9 chronic constriction injury of the infraorbital nerve (CCI-ION) animals, 4 complete Freund's Adjuvant (CFA)–injected animals, and 14 naive animals. We also used eight CGRP (calcitonin-gene-related peptide)-CRE mice, four male and four female, that were bred in house from breeding pairs obtained from The Jackson Laboratory (stock #033168).
Animals were housed in a 12 h dark/light cycle with food and water ad libitum. The animals were group housed before head plate surgery. After surgery they were individually housed.
Head plate implant surgery
Animals were induced with 2–4% isoflurane and placed in a stereotaxic frame. The anesthesia was maintained at 1–2% for the surgery. Animals were given Rimadyl (5 mg/kg) for analgesia. The skin on the top of the skull was removed, and the head plate (model 11, Neurotar) was secured to the skull using a mixture of dental cement and Vetbond (3M). Two screws were implanted anterior to the head plate for grounding. A craniotomy was created to expose the brain above PBN (AP, 4.8–5.3 mm; ML, 0.5–2 mm), and the dura was removed. The area was cleaned using a cortical buffer with antibiotics containing 125 mm NaCl, 5 mm KCl, 10 mm Glucose, 10 mm HEPES, 2 mm 1 m CaCl2, 2 mm 1 m MgCl2, 100 units/ml penicillin, and 0.1 mg/ml streptomycin. The craniotomy was closed using a silicone elastomer (Kwik-Sil, World Precision Instruments). The animal was placed on a heating pad and allowed to recover.
Training
Animals were trained on the Neurotar Mobile HomeCage, where they were head restrained but allowed to locomote freely on a carbon fiber platform supported by a cushion of air. Animals were given 5 d to recover after the implant of the head plate. On day 6 animals were left in their home cage and placed near the Neurotar setup to habituate them to the noise of the air flow for 2 h. On day 7, animals were placed in the Neurotar setup and allowed to move freely for up to 2 h to habituate them to the device. Electrophysiology recordings started on day 8.
Electrophysiology recordings
Awake electrophysiology recordings
We performed recordings in low light conditions. Animals were placed in the Neurotar setup and allowed to acclimate for 5 min. The silicone elastomer was removed, and the craniotomy was cleaned using a cortical buffer with antibiotics. A single platinum-iridium recording electrode (0.3–1 MΩ), produced in our lab, was lowered into PBN. We digitized recorded waveforms using a Plexon system. We isolated units that were responsive to noxious heat (50°C) applied to the anterior maxillary region of the head using a Picasso Lite dental surgical laser (AMD Lasers), positioned 5 mm above the skin and set to 3.5 W for 5 s. The laser was calibrated with a Jenco Electronics microcomputer thermometer to generate 51°C at the end of a 5 s exposure. We waited at least 2 min between consecutive stimuli and inspected the skin after each stimulus for erythema or tissue damage. On finding a neuron that was responsive to noxious heat stimulation, we allowed the cell to return to its baseline firing rate before recording baseline spontaneous activity for 5 min. After that, we recorded responses to noxious heat applied at 2 min intervals to prevent tissue damage for a total of 10–12 trials. After each recording session, the electrode was removed, and the craniotomy was cleaned using a cortical buffer with antibiotics and covered with fresh silicone elastomer.
Anesthetized electrophysiology recordings
Animals were anesthetized via intraperitoneal injections of urethane (10% w/v). Following anesthesia, the mice were placed in a stereotaxic frame with a heating pad, and a craniotomy was made over the recording site to target PBN (AP, 4.8–5.3 mm; ML, 0.5–2 mm). The recording and stimulation protocol was identical to the awake mice protocol.
Electrophysiology data analysis
Recordings were sorted using Offline Sorter software (Plexon) using dual thresholds and principal component analysis. Responses to thermal stimuli were analyzed with custom MATLAB scripts. Significant responses were defined as firing activity exceeding the 95th confidence intervals (CIs) of the average prestimulus firing rate. Thus, evoked responses are expressed as significant responses above baseline firing levels, thereby correcting for changes to baseline (spontaneous) firing rates. Peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) were generated to analyze responses to repeated stimuli.
We defined afterdischarges—periods of sustained activity that outlast a stimulus presentation (Okubo et al., 2013)—as PSTH bins in which activity exceeded the 95th confidence intervals for a period lasting at least 5 s after stimulus offset.
CCI-ION
We used a rodent model of neuropathic pain that was induced by CCI-ION (Bennett and Xie, 1988; Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020). Animals were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine (100/10 mg/kg, i.p.). We made intraoral incisions along the roof of the mouth, beginning distal to the first molar. The ION was freed from the surrounding connective tissue and loosely tied with a silk thread (4–0) 1–2 mm from the nerves emerging from the infraorbital foramen. Animals were monitored for 2 d in their home cage as they recovered. Each animal served as its own control as recordings were compared before and after CCI in each animal.
CFA
We used CFA to induce persistent inflammatory pain. Animals were induced with 2–4% isoflurane and injected with 15 µl of CFA (catalog #F5881, Sigma-Aldrich) subcutaneously in the snout, ipsilateral to the recording site. Animals were monitored for 24 h in their home cage as they recovered before recording. Each animal served as its own control, as above.
Behavior
Facial grimace
To assess ongoing pain, we analyzed facial grimace behaviors in animals placed in the Neurotar Mobile HomeCage, and images of the face were taken every 60 s for 15 min. Animals were scored with defined action units (AU) that examined orbital tightening, nose bulging, whisker positioning, and cheek bulging to derive a mouse grimace score (MGS; Langford et al., 2010; Akintola et al., 2017). We calculated the MGS as the average score of all the AUs.
Mechanical sensitivity
To assess mechanical sensitivity, animals were placed in the Neurotar Mobile HomeCage, and von Frey filaments (North Coast Medical) were applied to the anterior maxillary region, ipsilateral to CCI or CFA injections. A response was defined by an active swipe of the filament with the forepaws. We used the up-down method to determine response thresholds, as described previously (Dixon, 1965; Chaplan et al., 1994; Akintola et al., 2017; Raver et al., 2020). For each mouse, baseline response thresholds were determined before CCI-ION/CFA injections, and thresholds were reassessed on each day of recordings from PBN. Hyperalgesia was defined as mechanical withdrawal threshold ≥20% below baseline.
Viral construct injection
We anesthetized the animals with isoflurane and placed them in a stereotaxic frame. Either left or right PBN (−5.2 mm AP, ±1.5 mm ML, −2.2 to −2.9 mm DV) was targeted via a small craniotomy. We injected 0.5 µl of adeno-associated virus (AAV)9-syn.Flex.GCaMP6f.WPRE. SV40 (catalog #100833, Addgene) at a rate of 0.1 µl/min using glass pipettes (40–60 µm tip diameter), coupled to a Hamilton syringe controlled by a motorized pump. The pipette was left in place for 10 min before being slowly retracted over 5–10 min. Head plates were implanted immediately after the virus injections.
Because AAV9 can be transported retrogradely (Castle et al., 2014), recorded signals might reflect activity in axons projecting to PBN. We carefully considered this possibility by analyzing histologic sections through structures that project to PBN. We did not detect retrogradely labeled cells in any of these structures.
Fiber photometry
Recording
We anesthetized animals using 2% isoflurane and placed them in a stereotaxic frame. Mice were implanted with a fiber optic probe (400 μm diameter, 0.39 NA; RWD Life Science) in the PBN (−5.2 mm AP, +1.5 mm ML, −2.2 to −2.5 mm DV); during the same surgery they were injected with the viral construct, and the head plate was implanted. The mice were given 3 weeks to recover and to allow for viral expression in their home cage.
For recordings, the fiber optic probe was connected to an RZX10 LUX fiber photometry processor running Synapse software (Tucker-Davis Technologies) through a Doric Mini Cube (Doric Lenses). LEDs at 465 nm and 405 nm were used for GCaMP excitation and isosbestic control, respectively. LED power was calibrated weekly using a digital optical power meter (Thorlabs).
Analysis
We used pMat software (Bruno et al., 2021) to quantify photometry signals as changes in GCaMP fluorescence. These were converted to z-scores that were time locked using a time window starting 5 s before the onset of the noxious stimulus and lasting until 30 s poststimulus for each trial. A significant response was defined as one exceeding the 95% confidence interval of the baseline across all trials. The duration and AUC were calculated using the significant response of the z-score averaged across trials.
Conditioning
Recordings
Mice were head restrained in the Neurotar Mobile HomeCage to enable recordings of single units that are responsive to noxious heat (as above). When a responsive unit was identified, we presented a tone (1 s, 100 kHz, 88 dB) 10 times, with 2 min intervals, without applying noxious heat, while recording neuronal responses and pupil size. We then administered a conditioning paradigm, where a 1 s tone [conditioned stimulus (CS)] was paired with a 5 s noxious heat stimulus, where the tone was presented 3 s after the onset of the noxious heat, to align with the laser reaching noxious temperature. This pairing was repeated 15 times at 1 min intervals. The CS was presented until extinction with 2 min between presentations of the CS. We then administered the conditioning paradigm a second time. The CS was again presented until extinction.
The firing rates of conditioned trials were binned at 0.1 s before being split into trials time locked at the stimuli onset. A significant response was defined as exceeding the 95% confidence interval of the prestimulus baseline across all trials. Group data were quantified using average firing rate and pupil size, calculated using binned data (0.1 s) during the stimulation duration (1 s) across trials.
Pupillometry
We videotaped pupils in head-restrained mice, under low light conditions using a webcam (Logitech Brio). Recordings were acquired at 30 Hz using custom MATLAB scripts and the Image Acquisition Toolbox (MathWorks). Noxious heat stimulation time stamps were recorded by a microcontroller (Arduino Uno R3) using a custom script in Python (https://www.python.org/). These time stamps aligned the video with the electrophysiological data. Pupil area was measured from the videos off-line using Facemap software (Stringer et al., 2019). We excluded times when Facemap detected that the animal blinked. Pupil area was normalized between [0,1] using the minimum and maximum value within each recording session. Normalized pupil areas of conditioned trials were binned at 0.1 s before being split into trials time locked at the stimuli onset. A significant response was defined as exceeding the 95% confidence interval of the baseline across all trials.
We defined spontaneous activity as activity at least 3 min before the onset of the first stimulus. Firing rates and normalized pupil areas during this time were binned using a width of 1 s. The normalized pupil areas were downsampled to accommodate the bins of the spike rate by calculating the median within each bin. We applied a minimum cutoff of 10 Hz to the firing rates. To investigate the relationship between spontaneous activity and normalized pupil area we quantified it as the Pearson correlation coefficient.
To determine the relative time dynamics between firing rates and normalized pupil area after heat stimuli trials, we calculated a cross-correlation to produce lag values. This was done by binning the firing rates and normalized pupil areas using a width of 0.1 s and splitting them into trials, time locked around the stimulus onsets. The lag that maximized the magnitude of correlation was selected for further analysis. The firing rates and normalized pupil area used for cross-correlation were then shifted using this lag before taking the Pearson correlation coefficient.
Statistical analysis
We analyzed group data using GraphPad Prism version 9 for Mac computers (GraphPad) and custom scripts in MATLAB. Unless otherwise noted, data are presented as median ± 95% CI. Unless otherwise indicated, we used nonparametric Mann–Whitney U tests because data were not normally distributed. Individual statistical tests were run for each animal, as shown in the figures. We then averaged data from all animals for each condition to calculate group comparisons, and Cohen's d was calculated for effect size. Correlations are Pearson correlations.
Results
Anesthesia suppresses spontaneous and evoked PBN responses
PBN neurons recorded in urethane anesthetized animals respond robustly to nociceptive inputs (Bernard and Besson, 1990; Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020; Uddin et al., 2021). We developed an approach to apply reproducible noxious stimuli to awake, head-restrained mice while recording well-isolated single units from the PBN nuclear complex (Figs. 1, 2). Noxious stimuli were generated by a laser-generated light spot (3 mm diameter) applied to the snout. This generated a heat stimulus that increased—during the 5 s stimulus period—from body temperature to 50°C (Fig. 1A,B, red bar). Figure 1 compares examples of perievent histograms recorded from individual PBN neurons in an anesthetized mouse (Fig. 1A) and in an awake mouse (Fig. 1B). In the neuron from the awake animal, spontaneous activity, shown before the onset of the noxious heat, is markedly higher than that in the anesthetized animals. The amplitude of the response is also larger in the neuron from the awake animal.
Group data comparisons quantify the differences in response properties in anesthetized and awake mice (Fig. 1C–E). Spontaneous firing rates were, on average, >5 times higher in awake mice (Fig. 1C; Cohen's d, 1.09; p < 10−4; Table 1). As detailed (see above, Materials and Methods), we considered significant responses spiking activity that exceeded the 95th percent confidence interval of the average prestimulus firing rate. We computed response magnitude and duration of these significant response epochs. Response magnitudes (computed as spikes per stimulus per bin) were, on average, >5 times higher in awake mice (Fig. 1D; Cohen's d, 1.49; p = 0.003; Table 1) compared with those in anesthetized mice. Response durations were similar in neurons from awake and anesthetized mice (Fig. 1E; p > 0.99; Table 1).
Table 1.
Figure | Animals | Metric | Comparison | Test statistic | Median | Sample size | p Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 C | N/A | Spontaneous activity | Anesthetized vs awake | Mann–Whitney U = 109.5 | Anesthetized= 7.94, awake = 32.80 | Anesthetized= 23 cells, awake = 39 cells | <10−4 |
1 D | N/A | Response magnitude | Anesthetized vs awake | Mann–Whitney U = 13 | Anesthetized= 10.15, awake = 44.35 | Anesthetized= 23 cells, awake = 28 cells | <10−4 |
1 E | N/A | Response duration | Anesthetized vs awake | Mann–Whitney U = 112 | Anesthetized= 4.20, awake = 4.25 | Anesthetized= 8 cells, awake = 28 cells | >0.99 |
4 E | F6 | Spontaneous activity | Pre- vs post-CCI | Mann–Whitney U = 18 | Pre = 20.50, post = 32.80 | Pre = 8 cells, post = 7 cells | 0.28 |
M9 | Mann–Whitney U = 14 | Pre = 7.47, post = 23.25 | Pre = 11 cells, post = 6 cells | 0.058 | |||
F7 | Mann–Whitney U = 41 | Pre = 35.35, post = 25.15 | Pre = 8 cells, post = 12 cells | 0.624 | |||
M12 | Mann–Whitney U = 56 | Pre = 43.90, post = 35.00 | Pre = 8 cells, post = 12 cells | 0.691 | |||
4 F | F6 | Response duration | Pre- vs post-CCI | Mann–Whitney U = 3.5 | Pre = 3.20, post = 9.40 | Pre = 6 cells, post = 7 cells | 0.009 |
M9 | Mann–Whitney U = 0 | Pre = 4.25, post = 9.50 | Pre = 6 cells, post = 5 cells | 0.0016 | |||
F7 | Mann–Whitney U = 3 | Pre = 4.40, post = 11.90 | Pre = 8 cells, post = 11 cells | 0.0002 | |||
M12 | Mann–Whitney U = 0 | Pre = 4.70, post = 10.70 | Pre = 7 cells, post = 18 cells | <10−4 | |||
4 G | F6 | Response magnitude | Pre- vs post-CCI | Mann–Whitney U = 4 | Pre = 24.62, post = 50.35 | Pre = 6 cells, post = 7 cells | 0.1143 |
M9 | Mann–Whitney U = 12 | Pre = 23.70, post = 98.65 | Pre = 6 cells, post = 5 cells | 0.0353 | |||
F7 | Mann–Whitney U = 17 | Pre = 58.90, post = 307.00 | Pre = 8 cells, post = 11 cells | 0.0125 | |||
M12 | Mann–Whitney U = 2 | Pre = 64.60, post = 374 | Pre = 7 cells, post = 18 cells | <10−4 | |||
6 A | N/A | Spontaneous activity | Anesthetized vs awake post-CCI | Mann–Whitney U = 69.50 | Anesthetized= 11.76, awake = 32.80 | Anesthetized= 10 cells, awake = 39 cells | 0.0012 |
6 B | N/A | Response duration | Anesthetized vs awake post-CCI | Mann–Whitney U = 115.5 | Anesthetized= 16.70, awake = 11.15 | Anesthetized= 10 cells, awake = 38 cells | 0.0586 |
6 C | N/A | Response magnitude | Anesthetized vs awake post-CCI | Mann–Whitney U = 85 | Anesthetized= 65.70, awake = 282.00 | Anesthetized= 10 cells, awake = 41 cells | 0.0034 |
7 D | M10 | Spontaneous activity | Pre- vs post-CFA | Mann–Whitney U = 1 | Pre = 2.95, post = 46.70 | Pre = 5 cells, post = 7 cells | 0.0051 |
F14 | Mann–Whitney U = 25 | Pre = 29.80, post = 70.20 | Pre = 15 cells, post = 12 cells | 0.0009 | |||
F15 | Mann–Whitney U = 34 | Pre = 28.05, post = 46.70 | Pre = 14 cells, post = 10 cells | 0.0358 | |||
M16 | Mann–Whitney U = 14 | Pre = 26.00, post = 52.00 | Pre = 8 cells, post = 13 cells | 0.0045 | |||
7 E | M10 | Response duration | Pre- vs post-CFA | Mann–Whitney U = 0 | Pre = 3.50, post = 12.50 | Pre = 5 cells, post = 7 cells | 0.0043 |
F14 | Mann–Whitney U = 3 | Pre = 3.90, post = 9.10 | Pre = 16 cells, post = 12 cells | <10−4 | |||
F15 | Mann–Whitney U = 2 | Pre = 2.10, post = 9.18 | Pre = 15 cells, post = 10 cells | <10−4 | |||
M16 | Mann–Whitney U = 0 | Pre = 3.78, post = 9.60 | Pre = 8 cells, post = 13 cells | <10−4 | |||
7 F | M10 | Response magnitude | Pre- vs post-CFA | Mann–Whitney U = 0 | Pre = 11.92, post = 136.00 | Pre = 5 cells, post = 7 cells | 0.0025 |
F14 | Mann–Whitney U = 2 | Pre = 17.20, post = 354.30 | Pre = 13 cells, post = 12 cells | <10−4 | |||
F15 | Mann–Whitney U = 0 | Pre = 36.60, post = 317.50 | Pre = 11 cells, post = 8 cells | <10−4 | |||
M16 | Mann–Whitney U = 1 | Pre= 67.60, post = 701.50 | Pre = 7 cells, post = 10 cells | 0.0002 | |||
N/A | N/A | Spontaneous activity | Group data, awake post-CFA males vs females | Mann- Whitney U = 198 | Males = 51.7, females = 61.9 | Males = 20 cells, females = 22 cells | 0.59 |
N/A | N/A | Response duration | Group data, awake post-CFA males vs females | Mann- Whitney U = 150.5 | Males = 12.3, females = 10.7 | Males = 20 cells, females = 22 cells | 0.1286 |
N/A | N/A | Response magnitude | Group data, awake post-CFA males vs females | Mann- Whitney U = 166 | Males = 432.4, females = 415 | Males = 20 cells, females = 22 cells | 0.916 |
8 B | N/A | N/A | Firing rate vs pupil size correlation | Spearman's r = 0.542 | N/A | N = 301 correlated values | 2.36e−24 |
No sex differences in PBN neuronal activity
We investigated sex differences by pooling 90 neurons recorded from eight animals (four of each sex). In naive animals, there were no sex differences in spontaneous activity [p = 0.4, median (males) = 24.65, CI (males) = 20.51,42.72; median (females) = 29.95, CI (females) = 24.38,38.66], response duration [p = 0.84, median (males) = 4.8, CI (males) = 7.767, 20.94; median (females) = 5.3, CI (females) = 7.971, 17.63] or response magnitude [p = 0.9, median (males) = 37.95, CI (males) = 53.29, 166.6; median (females) = 35.05, CI (females) = 41.97, 76.39]. Similarly, there were no sex differences after CCI in spontaneous activity [p = 0.34, median (males) = 32.45, CI (males) = 28.57, 77.52; median (females) = 25.9, CI (females) = 22.15, 62.45], response duration [p = 0.97, median (males) = 10.2, CI (males) = 10.09, 15.25; median (females) = 10.85, CI (females) = 9.53, 19.89], or response magnitude [p = 0.07, median (males) = 371.5, CI (males) = 292, 564.8; median (females) = 154, CI (females) = 108.5, 422.4].
PBCGRP neurons are activated in response to acute pain
PBN neurons that respond to noxious stimuli, and whose activity is related to chronic pain conditions, are in the lateral portion of the PBN (Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020). Many neurons in this region express CGRP, a peptide implicated in aversive behaviors, including pain (Campos et al., 2018; Palmiter, 2018; Chiang et al., 2020). To test whether PBN neurons that express CGRP (PBCGRP neurons) respond to noxious stimuli, we used in vivo calcium imaging with fiber photometry to record population activity of PBCGRP neurons.
Figure 3, A–C, depicts GCaMP expression in PBCGRP neurons of a male and female (Fig. 3B,C) mouse, and the location of the optical fiber (Fig. 3A). An example of a heat-evoked CGaMP response is shown in Figure 3D. The portions of the response that exceeds the 95% confidence interval above the mean baseline levels, indicated by the dotted horizontal line, was defined as significant and is labeled in green. The color bar shows the increase in heat, from room temperature to 50°C, which is just above the heat pain threshold for these animals. This threshold was determined based on published data and on the known activation threshold of relevant nociceptors (Yeomans and Proudfit, 1996; Deuis et al., 2017).
We averaged responses recorded over 4 d from each animal. The duration and area under the curve (AUC) of these responses from eight animals (four of each sex) are depicted in Figure 3E. (Data from the animal shown in Fig. 3D are marked in green circles in Fig. 3E.) This experiment was not powered to test for sex differences. These findings demonstrate that PBCGRP neurons respond robustly to noxious heat stimuli applied to the trigeminal region.
Neuropathic pain is associated with hyperactivity of PBN neurons in awake animals
We, and others, have shown in anesthetized rodents that the activity of PBN neurons is amplified in models of chronic pain (Matsumoto et al., 1996; Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020). We tested whether similar changes occur also in awake animals. As in our previous studies (Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020), we used the CCI-ION model, developed by Bennett and Xie (1988), to induce persistent pain-like behaviors. We tested mechanical hypersensitivity of the face in head-restrained mice using a modified version of the up-down method (Chaplan et al., 1994; see above, Materials and Methods). In each of the four animals with CCI, mechanical withdrawal thresholds were lower than in pre-CCI levels (Fig. 4A), indicating that the mice experienced increased tactile hypersensitivity. These changes were recorded between 8 and 56 d after CCI surgery.
We used mechanical hypersensitivity as a behavioral readout because the thermal hypersensitivity tests of the face were unreliable, as even 0.1 W power differences in the calibrated laser output—the finest differences in power output the laser could produce—resulted in fluctuating behavioral responses. This may be because of the high sensitivity of the face to the thermal stimulation. Mechanical sensitivity tests, on the other hand, produced consistent behavioral responses at each filament level.
To assess ongoing pain, we used the MGS (Langford et al., 2010; Akintola et al., 2017). MGS scores were higher in mice after CCI, relative to pre-CCI levels, and remained higher for as long as they were recorded (up to 8 weeks; Fig. 4B), suggesting that CCI mice were exhibiting persistent, ongoing pain.
We compared recordings of well-isolated single units from PBN in the same mice before and after CCI. There were distinct changes in the response magnitude and duration, but not the spontaneous activity, of PBN neurons after CCI (Fig. 4E–G). Figure 4 shows an example of perievent rasters and perievent histograms recorded from individual PBN neurons in the same animal before (Fig. 4C) and 7 d after CCI (Fig. 4D). The neuron recorded after CCI showed an increase in both the magnitude and the duration of responses to heat stimuli applied to the face. In this neuron, spontaneous activity is also higher after CCI; however, when averaged across animals, spontaneous activity did not change after CCI (see below). The neurons recorded before CCI began responding at ∼50°C, presumably the noxious threshold. In contrast, the neuron recorded after CCI began to fire at temperatures well below noxious threshold, at ∼30°C (Fig. 4C,D). Before CCI, all neurons from all animals responded only to noxious temperatures; after CCI, all neurons also responded to innocuous temperatures.
We quantified the differences in response properties before and after CCI for each of the mice (Fig. 4E–G). We included, as post-CCI data, neurons recorded from 2 to 8 weeks after CCI, a period in which all animals displayed tactile hypersensitivity and increased MGS scores. There was no difference in spontaneous firing rates after CCI in any of the animals (Fig. 4E), but there was an increase in the magnitude of the response (Fig. 4G; Table 1) and response duration (Fig. 4F; Table 1). The magnitude of the responses was three times larger after CCI (Fig. 4G; effect size, 0.5; p < 10−4), and response durations were 3.25 times longer (Fig. 4F; effect size, 0.5; p < 10−4). Median response duration after CCI was 10.5 s, far outlasting the duration of the stimulus. These prolonged responses are defined as afterdischarges (Matsumoto et al., 1996; Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020). The increase in response duration is consistent with our previous findings in urethane anesthetized rodents, demonstrating that chronic pain is associated with an increase in the incidence and duration of afterdischarges in PBN neurons (Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020).
Time course of changes in PBN activity
By recording from awake animals, we were able to investigate the time course of the PBN activity before and after the induction of nerve injury and to determine the relationship between the activity of PBN neurons and the pain behaviors in individual animals. Figure 5 plots the duration (Fig. 5A) and magnitude (Fig. 5B) of responses in each of the neurons, pooled from the four animals, across time. Response duration and magnitude remained elevated relative to pre-CCI levels for at least 5 weeks post-CCI period, a period in which the animals experienced hyperalgesia (Fig. 5A). We recorded a small number of neurons from animals 6–8 weeks post-CCI; although some of these neurons had elevated activity (Fig. 5A,B), the small sample size precludes conclusions about activity levels at these time points.
Urethane anesthesia suppresses the effects of CCI on PBN activity
We showed above that urethane anesthesia suppresses the responses of PBN neurons to acute nociceptive inputs (Fig. 1). We also showed that CCI results in amplification of PBN responses recorded from awake rodents (Fig. 4). Here, we tested whether this amplification was affected by urethane anesthesia. That is, we directly compared PBN activity after CCI, in anesthetized versus awake mice.
Group data comparisons quantified the differences in responses to noxious heat stimuli in anesthetized and awake mice after CCI (Fig. 6). Spontaneous firing rates were >3 times higher in awake mice (Fig. 6A; effect size, 4.5; p = 0.03). Response magnitudes were >5 times higher in awake mice (Fig. 6C; effect size, 5.5; p = 0.003). Response durations were similar in neurons from awake and anesthetized mice (Fig. 6B; p > 0.99). These findings indicate that spontaneous activity and responses to noxious stimuli in PBN neurons of CCI mice are larger in the awake mice compared with urethane anesthetized animals but that the duration of afterdischarges is similar in these conditions.
Inflammatory pain is associated with hyperactivity of PBN neurons in awake animals
To determine whether similar changes in PBN activity occur also in other models of pain, we induced in a separate group of animals inflammatory pain by injection of CFA in the snout. We assessed tactile hypersensitivity levels at the snout daily on each of the 3 d before CFA injections and on days 2–6 after injections. After CFA, mechanical thresholds decreased (Fig. 7A) indicating that the mice were experiencing increased tactile hypersensitivity.
Between 2 and 6 d after CFA application, there were changes in the spontaneous and evoked activity of PBN neurons. Figure 7 depicts an example of perievent rasters and perievent histograms recorded from individual PBN neurons in the same animal before (Fig. 7B) and after CFA (Fig. 7C). After CFA, there were increases in spontaneous activity, response duration, and response magnitude in all animals tested (Fig. 7D–F).
We quantified the differences in response properties before and 6 d after CFA in each of the awake mice (Fig. 7D–F). Unlike in CCI animals, there was an increase in spontaneous firing rates after CFA (Table 1). Neurons from all animals were pooled into pre- and post-CFA groups, revealing an average 3.3-fold increase in spontaneous activity (Fig. 7D; effect size, 0.49; p < 10−4). The increase in spontaneous activity may reflect the persistent inflammatory nature of CFA pain, which may not be as prominent in animals with CCI. Just as with the CCI animals, response durations were 2.7 times longer (Fig. 7E; effect size, 0.49; p < 10−4) and response magnitudes were 2.7 times larger after CFA (Fig. 7F; effect size, 0.52; p < 10−4) in all animals tested, indicating the presence of amplified afterdischarges. Together with the CCI data, these data indicate that mouse models of both neuropathic pain (CCI) and inflammatory pain (CFA) are associated with hyperactivity of PBN neurons in awake mice.
We investigated sex differences by pooling 42 neurons recorded in all four animals (two males and two females). In animals recorded after CFA injections, there were no differences in spontaneous activity [p = 0.59, median (males) = 51.7, CI (males) = 43.94, 100.7; median (females) = 61.9, CI (females) = 51.64, 98.86], response duration [p = 0.13, median (males) = 12.3, CI (males) = 10.54, 16.21; median (females) = 10.7, CI (females) = 10.04, 11.94], or response magnitude [p = 0.92, median (males) = 432.4, CI (males) = 307.8, 662.3; median (females) = 340, 566.9].
PBN activity is correlated with arousal
A benefit of recording from awake animals is the ability to test the hypothesis that PBN activity can be modulated by the behavioral state of an animal. One approach to assess behavioral states is to measure pupil size as a proxy of the arousal state (de Gee et al., 2020).
Spontaneous activity rates were correlated with pupil area. Figure 8A depicts a representative recording session, plotting both pupil area (blue) and the spontaneous firing rate of a PBN neuron (red) for each 100 ms bin of a 5 min recording epoch. Note that increases in the neuronal firing rate appear to immediately precede pupil dilations. Figure 8B quantifies this relationship by plotting instantaneous neuronal firing rates (1 s bins) against the pupil area, demonstrating a positive correlation between these metrics (r = 0.54). A significant positive correlation was seen in 14 of 17 cells across all 7 animals tested (four males, three females), with r values ranging from 0.12 to 0.55. These findings suggest that arousal states—approximated by pupil area—correlate with changes in PBN activity.
To determine whether arousal states affect evoked responses of PBN neurons, we constructed and overlayed PSTHs (0.1 s bin size) of neuronal activity and pupil area (Fig. 8C), averaged over multiple recording epochs. The PSTHs were aligned to the onset of the 5 s heat stimuli applied to the snout. Pupil dilation and PBN activity after the onset of the noxious heat were highly correlated (Fig. 8C,D; r = 0.83, p = 1.75 × 10−13). A significant positive correlation was seen in 13 neurons from seven animals tested, with r values ranging from 0.5 to 0.9. Four cells had a negative correlation (Fig. 8D).
To determine whether changes in firing rates preceded or followed pupil dilation, we used cross-correlation analysis to obtain time-lagged Pearson correlation coefficients (Box et al., 2015). By incrementally shifting the binned firing rate time series relative to the pupil area, finding the correlation, and identifying the temporal shift (lag) that results in maximal correlation, it is possible to determine a leader–follower relationship. This analysis revealed that there was a positive lag in most cells (15 of 17), indicating that PBN activity preceded pupil area (Fig. 8E).
Auditory conditioning of PBN activity
Pairing activation of PBN neurons with sucrose produces robust and long-lasting conditioned taste aversion in mice, whereas suppressing PBN activity suppresses the acquisition and expression of this conditioned aversion (Carter et al., 2015; Chen et al., 2018). These findings suggest that the activity of PBN neurons can be conditioned to respond to innocuous stimuli. To test this, we conditioned head-restrained mice by pairing an auditory tone (1 s, 100 kHz, 80 dB) with a noxious heat stimulus applied to the snout. We did this while recording from nociceptive neurons in the PBN nucleus. Figure 9 depicts an example of one of these recording sessions.
Before conditioning, this PBN neuron responded to the heat stimulus (Fig. 9A) but not to the auditory stimulus (Fig. 9B). We then delivered a conditioning paradigm using 15 pairings of auditory tone (1 s) and heat (5 s) stimuli. We presented the auditory stimulus 3 s after the onset of the noxious heat stimulus so that the auditory stimulus was presented just before the heat reaches noxious temperatures. After the pairing session, the neuron responded robustly to the auditory tone and continued to do so for a median of 13 presentations of the tone (delivered every 120 s; Fig. 9B; range, 16–10 stimuli). After the tone responses extinguished, we delivered a second session of 15 pairings of the auditory tone and heat stimuli, after which the neuron again began responding to the auditory stimulus and did so for the next 14 stimuli until this auditory response extinguished (Fig. 9D; median, 13.5 stimuli; range, 20–9 stimuli). Sham pairings, where the auditory tone was presented without the noxious heat stimulus, had no effect on PBN activity (Fig. 9, gray traces).
We conditioned nociceptive PBN neurons to auditory stimuli in 24 neurons from six animals (three of each sex), using the conditioning paradigm described above while measuring pupil area. None of these neurons responded to auditory stimuli before conditioning but responded to the auditory stimuli after conditioning. Responses to auditory stimuli were evoked for at least 6 and up to 22 stimuli presentations (median, 13 stimuli) after the conditioning, with a median response magnitude of 89.79 Hz (Fig. 9C). All neurons could be conditioned again after extinction of the auditory stimuli (Fig. 9E; median, 64.97 Hz). Pupil area increased during the auditory tone (median, 0.41) after conditioning in 10 of 14 trials recorded across six animals (Fig. 9F), and pupil area increased again after the second condoning (Fig. 9I; median, 0.41). These data suggest that PBN neurons that respond to noxious heat stimuli can be conditioned to respond to an innocuous auditory tone.
Discussion
Urethane anesthesia alters properties of PBN neurons
Much of what we know about the neurophysiology of nociception and pain originates from studies using anesthetics that may alter neuronal responses to noxious stimuli. For example, both propofol and isoflurane suppress responses of PBN neurons (Luo et al., 2018). We and others have found that PBN neurons do respond to noxious stimuli under urethane anesthesia (Bernard and Besson, 1990; Uddin et al., 2018; Raver et al., 2020; Uddin et al., 2021). We chose urethane in those studies because it produces—in cortical networks—less disruption of synaptic transmission and neuronal activity compared with isoflurane (Sceniak and Maciver, 2006; Shumkova et al., 2021) and because urethane provides improved systemic physiologic conditions for brainstem recordings compared with ketamine (Lee and Jones, 2018).
In the present study we show that responses recorded under urethane are different from the ones recorded in awake animals. In awake animals, spontaneous PBN neuronal activity is, on average, three times higher, and PBN responses to noxious stimuli are, on average, five times larger in amplitude than those in urethane anesthetized animals. In contrast, response duration was indistinguishable in awake and urethane anesthetized animals.
PBCGRP neurons respond to noxious stimuli
PBN neurons expressing CGRP have been implicated in driving pain behaviors. Inhibition of PBCGRP neurons attenuates pain behaviors (Han et al., 2015). PBCGRP neurons are also involved in acute pain perception as they respond to acute noxious stimuli (Campos et al., 2018; Kang et al., 2022). Our findings corroborate this conclusion, demonstrating that populations of PBCGRP neurons respond robustly to thermal noxious stimuli.
One study reported that CGRP application in PBN can produce analgesia and that this effect is blocked by subnanomolar concentrations of a CGRP antagonist (Wang et al., 2021). To our knowledge, this is the only report suggesting that CGRP might be antinociceptive. In contrast, we and others have found that PBCGRP neurons robustly increase their activity in response to noxious stimuli.
PBN activity correlates with internal states
Recordings from awake animals are required to determine how internal states might affect PBN activity and pain perception. Internal states directed toward distractions, such as cognitively demanding tasks, can affect pain perception. Detection of a noxious stimulus is compromised while performing a working-memory-intensive task (Buhle and Wager, 2010). Inflammatory pain is attenuated in states of hunger through a process involving neuropeptide Y (NPY) signaling (Alhadeff et al., 2018); manipulating NPY in PBN can mimic hunger states, suggesting a role of PBN in this state-dependent pain modulation. Similarly, suppressing PBN activity inhibits arousal (Kaur et al., 2017), whereas activating PBN neurons increases arousal (Kaur and Saper, 2019).
We found a direct correlation between PBN activity and arousal, as measured by pupil area, during presentation of a noxious stimulus. As increases in PBN activity preceded pupil dilation, PBN activity may be influencing internal states. A potential concern is that thermal stimulation may directly lead to pupil dilation, independent of central mechanisms. This is because thermal stimulation of the snout activates the V1 branch of the trigeminal nerve (Kim et al., 2014), potentially causing pupil dilation via the ciliary nerve, which contains fibers from the V1 branch of the trigeminal nerve (Joo et al., 2014; McDougal and Gamlin, 2015). However, our finding that pupil size and PBN activity are correlated in the absence of heat stimuli argues against this possibility.
Nociceptive PBN neurons can be conditioned to respond to an auditory tone
In addition to its role in nociception and arousal, the PBN is also involved in aversive behaviors. For example, stimulating the PBN induces conditioned taste aversion, whereas silencing it attenuates conditioned taste aversion (Carter et al., 2015; Chen et al., 2018). Optogenetic stimulation of the PBN nucleus induces context-dependent freezing, demonstrating its involvement with threat memory and fear conditioning (Han et al., 2015; Campos et al., 2018; Bowen et al., 2020). Although these studies show that PBN neurons are necessary and sufficient for fear conditioning, they do not elucidate the underlying function of specific PBN neurons. We found that the same PBN neurons that respond to noxious stimuli can be conditioned to respond to non-noxious stimuli. Increases in pupil size mirrored this conditioned behavior. These findings suggest that conditioned aversion may involve plasticity in the response properties of PBN neurons, consistent with evidence for a causal role of the PBN in conditioned aversive behaviors (Carter et al., 2015; Campos et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2018; Bowen et al., 2020).
The auditory-noxious stimulus conditioning paradigm in our study evoked short-lasting conditioning of both pupil dilation responses and PBN responses to the conditioned auditory stimuli (Fig. 9). In contrast, conditioned aversion induced by pairing direct stimulation of the PBN with tastants, or pairing tastants with appetite-suppressing substances, produced aversive behaviors that can last days (Carter et al., 2015; Campos et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2018; Bowen et al., 2020). A causal role for the PBN in this long-lasting aversion is supported by finding that suppressing PBN activity attenuates the conditioned behavior (Carter et al., 2015; Campos et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2018; Bowen et al., 2020).
Our findings (Fig. 9) confirm that individual PBN neurons can respond to multiple modalities (Sammons et al., 2016; Campos et al., 2018; Bowen et al., 2020), including noxious stimuli (Kang et al., 2022). Individual PBN neurons can respond during the conditioning phase, where a noxious stimulus is paired with a non-noxious tone, but they do not respond when presented with the conditioned tone 24 h later (Kang et al., 2022). Our findings show that the same PBN neurons that respond to noxious heat can be rapidly conditioned to respond to an auditory tone, confirming that PBN neurons may be responsible for aversive memory formation. However, we also show that this conditioned response is short lived, lasting only up to 1 h after conditioning, suggesting that PBN neurons may be involved in short-term aversive memory retrieval but not in long-term aversive memory retrieval.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health–National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grants R01NS099245 and R01NS069568.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Akintola T, Raver C, Studlack P, Uddin O, Masri R, Keller A (2017) The grimace scale reliably assesses chronic pain in a rodent model of trigeminal neuropathic pain. Neurobiol Pain 2:13–17. 10.1016/j.ynpai.2017.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alhadeff AL, Su Z, Hernandez E, Klima ML, Phillips SZ, Holland RA, Guo C, Hantman AW, De Jonghe BC, Betley JN (2018) A neural circuit for the suppression of pain by a competing need state. Cell 173:140–152.e15. 10.1016/j.cell.2018.02.057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auvray M, Myin E, Spence C (2010) The sensory-discriminative and affective-motivational aspects of pain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:214–223. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett GJ, Xie YK (1988) A peripheral mononeuropathy in rat that produces disorders of pain sensation like those seen in man. Pain 33:87–107. 10.1016/0304-3959(88)90209-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard JF, Besson JM (1990) The spino(trigemino)pontoamygdaloid pathway: electrophysiological evidence for an involvement in pain processes. J Neurophysiol 63:473–490. 10.1152/jn.1990.63.3.473 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsook D (2012) Neurological diseases and pain. Brain 135:320–344. 10.1093/brain/awr271 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowen AJ, Chen JY, Huang YW, Baertsch NA, Park S, Palmiter RD (2020) Dissociable control of unconditioned responses and associative fear learning by parabrachial CGRP neurons. Elife 9:e59799. 10.7554/eLife.59799 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Box GEP, Jenkins GM, Reinsel GC, Ljung GM (2015) Time series analysis: forecasting and control. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Bruno CA, O'Brien C, Bryant S, Mejaes JI, Estrin DJ, Pizzano C, Barker DJ (2021) pMAT: an open-source software suite for the analysis of fiber photometry data. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 201:173093. 10.1016/j.pbb.2020.173093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buhle J, Wager TD (2010) Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task. Pain 149:19–26. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bushnell MC, Ceko M, Low LA (2013) Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:502–511. 10.1038/nrn3516 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cai YQ, Wang W, Paulucci-Holthauzen A, Pan ZZ (2018) Brain circuits mediating opposing effects on emotion and pain. J Neurosci 38:6340–6349. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2780-17.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campos CA, Bowen AJ, Roman CW, Palmiter RD (2018) Encoding of danger by parabrachial CGRP neurons. Nature 555:617–622. 10.1038/nature25511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter ME, Han S, Palmiter RD (2015) Parabrachial calcitonin gene-related peptide neurons mediate conditioned taste aversion. J Neurosci 35:4582–4586. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3729-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castle MJ, Gershenson ZT, Giles AR, Holzbaur EL, Wolfe JH (2014) Adeno-associated virus serotypes 1, 8, and 9 share conserved mechanisms for anterograde and retrograde axonal transport. Hum Gene Ther 25:705–720. 10.1089/hum.2013.189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaplan SR, Bach FW, Pogrel JW, Chung JM, Yaksh TL (1994) Quantitative assessment of tactile allodynia in the rat paw. J Neurosci Methods 53:55–63. 10.1016/0165-0270(94)90144-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen JY, Campos CA, Jarvie BC, Palmiter RD (2018) Parabrachial CGRP neurons establish and sustain aversive taste memories. Neuron 100:891–899.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiang MC, Bowen A, Schier LA, Tupone D, Uddin O, Heinricher MM (2019) Parabrachial complex: a hub for pain and aversion. J Neurosci 39:8225–8230. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1162-19.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiang MC, Nguyen EK, Canto-Bustos M, Papale AE, Oswald AM, Ross SE (2020) Divergent neural pathways emanating from the lateral parabrachial nucleus mediate distinct components of the pain response. Neuron 106:927–939.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Gee JW, Tsetsos K, Schwabe L, Urai AE, McCormick D, McGinley MJ, Donner TH (2020) Pupil-linked phasic arousal predicts a reduction of choice bias across species and decision domains. Elife 9:e54014. 10.7554/eLife.54014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deuis JR, Dvorakova LS, Vetter I (2017) Methods used to evaluate pain behaviors in rodents. Front Mol Neurosci 10:284. 10.3389/fnmol.2017.00284 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dixon WJ (1965) The up-and-down method for small samples. J Am StatAssoc 60:967–978. 10.1080/01621459.1965.10480843 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fields HL (1999) Pain: an unpleasant topic. Pain (Suppl 6):S61–S69. 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00139-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han S, Soleiman MT, Soden ME, Zweifel LS, Palmiter RD (2015) Elucidating an affective pain circuit that creates a threat memory. Cell 162:363–374. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.05.057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joo W, Yoshioka F, Funaki T, Mizokami K, Rhoton AL (2014) Microsurgical anatomy of the trigeminal nerve. Clin Anat 27:61–88. 10.1002/ca.22330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang SJ, Liu S, Ye M, Kim DI, Pao GM, Copits BA, Roberts BZ, Lee KF, Bruchas MR, Han S (2022) A central alarm system that gates multi-sensory innate threat cues to the amygdala. Cell Rep 40:111222. 10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaur S, Saper CB (2019) Neural circuitry underlying waking up to hypercapnia. Front Neurosci 13:401. 10.3389/fnins.2019.00401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaur S, Wang JL, Ferrari L, Thankachan S, Kroeger D, Venner A, Lazarus M, Wellman A, Arrigoni E, Fuller PM, Saper CB (2017) A genetically defined circuit for arousal from sleep during hypercapnia. Neuron 96:1153–1167.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim YS, Chu Y, Han L, Li M, Li Z, Lavinka PC, Sun S, Tang Z, Park K, Caterina MJ, Ren K, Dubner R, Wei F, Dong X (2014) Central terminal sensitization of TRPV1 by descending serotonergic facilitation modulates chronic pain. Neuron 81:873–887. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landis SC, et al. (2012) A call for transparent reporting to optimize the predictive value of preclinical research. Nature 490:187–191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langford DJ, Bailey AL, Chanda ML, Clarke SE, Drummond TE, Echols S, Glick S, Ingrao J, Klassen-Ross T, Lacroix-Fralish ML, Matsumiya L, Sorge RE, Sotocinal SG, Tabaka JM, Wong D, van den Maagdenberg AM, Ferrari MD, Craig KD, Mogil JS (2010) Coding of facial expressions of pain in the laboratory mouse. Nat Methods 7:447–449. 10.1038/nmeth.1455 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee C, Jones TA (2018) Effects of ketamine compared with urethane anesthesia on vestibular sensory evoked potentials and systemic physiology in mice. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 57:268–277. 10.30802/AALAS-JAALAS-17-000131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legrain V, Crombez G, Mouraux A (2011) Controlling attention to nociceptive stimuli with working memory. PLoS One 6:e20926. 10.1371/journal.pone.0020926 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo T, Yu S, Cai S, Zhang Y, Jiao Y, Yu T, Yu W (2018) Parabrachial neurons promote behavior and electroencephalographic arousal from general anesthesia. Front Mol Neurosci 11:420. 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00420 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto N, Bester H, Menendez L, Besson JM, Bernard JF (1996) Changes in the responsiveness of parabrachial neurons in the arthritic rat: an electrophysiological study. J Neurophysiol 76:4113–4126. 10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.4113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDougal DH, Gamlin PD (2015) Autonomic control of the eye. Compr Physiol 5:439–473. 10.1002/cphy.c140014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melzack R, Casey KL (1968) Sensory, motivational and central control determinants of pain. In: The skin senses (Kenshalo DR, ed), pp 423–439. Springfield, IL: Thomas. [Google Scholar]
- Okubo M, Castro A, Guo W, Zou S, Ren K, Wei F, Keller A, Dubner R (2013) Transition to persistent orofacial pain after nerve injury involves supraspinal serotonin mechanisms. J Neurosci 33:5152–5161. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3390-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmiter RD (2018) The parabrachial nucleus: CGRP neurons function as a general alarm. Trends Neurosci 41:280–293. 10.1016/j.tins.2018.03.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ (2019) Paxinos and Franklin's the mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates, p 376. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Price DD (2000) Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science 288:1769–1772. 10.1126/science.288.5472.1769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raver C, Uddin O, Ji Y, Li Y, Cramer N, Jenne C, Morales M, Masri R, Keller A (2020) An amygdalo-parabrachial pathway regulates pain perception and chronic pain. J Neurosci 40:3424–3442. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0075-20.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sammons JD, Weiss MS, Victor JD, Di Lorenzo PM (2016) Taste coding of complex naturalistic taste stimuli and traditional taste stimuli in the parabrachial pons of the awake, freely licking rat. J Neurophysiol 116:171–182. 10.1152/jn.01119.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sceniak MP, Maciver MB (2006) Cellular actions of urethane on rat visual cortical neurons in vitro. J Neurophysiol 95:3865–3874. 10.1152/jn.01196.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shumkova V, Sitdikova V, Rechapov I, Leukhin A, Minlebaev M (2021) Effects of urethane and isoflurane on the sensory evoked response and local blood flow in the early postnatal rat somatosensory cortex. Sci Rep 11:9567. 10.1038/s41598-021-88461-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith JG, Elias LA, Yilmaz Z, Barker S, Shah K, Shah S, Renton T (2013) The psychosocial and affective burden of posttraumatic neuropathy following injuries to the trigeminal nerve. J Orofac Pain 27:293–303. 10.11607/jop.1056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stringer C, Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, Reddy CB, Carandini M, Harris KD (2019) Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science 364:255. 10.1126/science.aav7893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treede RD, Apkarian AV, Bromm B, Greenspan JD, Lenz FA (2000) Cortical representation of pain: functional characterization of nociceptive areas near the lateral sulcus. Pain 87:113–119. 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00350-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turnes JM, Araya EI, Barroso AR, Baggio DF, Koren LO, Zanoveli JM, Chichorro JG (2022) Blockade of kappa opioid receptors reduces mechanical hyperalgesia and anxiety-like behavior in a rat model of trigeminal neuropathic pain. Behav Brain Res 417:113595. 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113595 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin O, Studlack P, Akintola T, Raver C, Castro A, Masri R, Keller A (2018) Amplified parabrachial nucleus activity in a rat model of trigeminal neuropathic pain. Neurobiol Pain 3:22–30. 10.1016/j.ynpai.2018.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin O, Anderson M, Smith J, Masri R, Keller A (2021) Parabrachial complex processes dura inputs through a direct trigeminal ganglion-to-parabrachial connection. Neurobiol Pain 9:100060. 10.1016/j.ynpai.2021.100060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang LL, Wang HB, Fu FH, Yu LC (2021) Role of calcitonin gene-related peptide in pain regulation in the parabrachial nucleus of naive rats and rats with neuropathic pain. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 414:115428. 10.1016/j.taap.2021.115428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu TH, Hu LY, Lu T, Chen PM, Chen HJ, Shen CC, Wen CH (2015) Risk of psychiatric disorders following trigeminal neuralgia: a nationwide population-based retrospective cohort study. J Headache Pain 16:64. 10.1186/s10194-015-0548-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeomans DC, Proudfit HK (1996) Nociceptive responses to high and low rates of noxious cutaneous heating are mediated by different nociceptors in the rat: electrophysiological evidence. Pain 68:141–150. 10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03177-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]