Skip to main content
Veterinary World logoLink to Veterinary World
. 2023 May 9;16(5):957–964. doi: 10.14202/vetworld.2023.957-964

Factors associated with pet dog behavior in Thailand

Worakan Boonhoh 1,2,3,4, Tuempong Wongtawan 1,2,4, Prarom Sriphavatsarakom 5, Natalie Waran 6, Chaiwat Boonkaewwan 1,2,4,
PMCID: PMC10420710  PMID: 37576765

Abstract

Background and Aim:

Dog behavior problems pose serious public health and economic and animal welfare concerns. There are many factors influencing dog behavior. This study aims to explore factors associated with pet dog behavior in Thailand using the Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ).

Materials and Methods:

The Thai version of the C-BARQ was validated in 2022. The online C-BARQ survey (Google Forms) was advertised and distributed on social media for 3 months. There were a total of 1827 respondents to the survey. The relationship among 12 subscales and variables was analyzed using Spearman’s correlation.

Results:

“Excitability,” “attachment- and attention-seeking,” and “chasing” were the three most reported behavior problems. “Trainability,” a favored dog behavior, also had a high score in the study. These behaviors were associated with the owner’s age, house type, the dog’s historical background, the number of dogs and the presence of other species in the household, the dog’s training, diet types, and the daily number of meals. The lowest mean score was for “owner-directed aggression,” and it was associated with the dog’s gender and size, the number of dogs and the presence of other species in the household, the dog’s training, and the number of daily meals.

Conclusion:

This is the first empirical study demonstrating factors related to dog behavior in Thailand. It provides an in-depth understanding of the prevalence and factors associated with Thai pet dog behavior and important knowledge for further studies to advocate for dog–human relationships and contribute to a reduction in dog abandonment in Thailand.

Keywords: canine behavioral assessment and research questionnaire, dog behavior, questionnaire, dog-human relationships, Thai dogs

Introduction

Humans have domesticated and selectively bred dogs for different appearances and purposes [1]. Companionship is the primary reason for the adoption of dogs today by humans [2, 3]. Dog behavior problems, which include unacceptable behaviors to the owners, are a serious concern to public health and economic and animal welfare [4]. Aggressive behavior and biting incidences among Thai dogs are often reported in the media, and these behaviors can be found among both pet and stray dogs. In the Bangkok area of Thailand, 5.3% of human injuries recorded in hospital emergency departments are attributed to dog bites [5]. Dog behavior problems can distress owners and disrupt dog–human bonds, which can lead to the contemplation of abandoning or euthanizing pet dogs [6]. Common behavior problems among dogs include aggression, destructiveness, elimination, fearfulness, and separation-related behaviors [7]. Many factors are associated with these behaviors, including the dog’s characteristics and demographic and socioeconomic factors [8].

The Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ), which was developed and first published in English in the United States, is a common questionnaire used to evaluate dog behaviors [9]. The C-BARQ allows dog owners to evaluate their dog’s behavior in various situations and score them for frequency and severity of the behavior using a five-point Likert-type scale. Hsu and Sun [10] conducted the C-BARQ survey in Taiwan and revealed that the age and gender of dogs, the owner’s gender, types of houses, and types of residential areas were all associated with aggressive dog behavior. Dog- and stranger-directed aggression reported in northern Mexico and Brazil have been associated with intrinsic and environmental variables, such as reproductive status, dog age, and dog breed [11, 12]. Moreover, diet has also been linked to aggressive behavior [13]. Separation-related behavior, determined based on behaviors including vocalization, destructiveness, and house soiling, has been associated with the breed and age of dog and the gender of the owner [12, 14, 15]. In addition, first-time dog ownership was associated with excitement and nervousness in dogs [3]. Finally, small breeds were significantly correlated with higher excitement behaviors, such as overexcitement, biting, rushing toward or chasing people, jumping up on people, constant running around, and digging, compared to large breed dogs [3].

The Thai version of the C-BARQ was validated in 2022 by Boonhoh et al. [16]. This survey was shared online, and the data were collected from throughout Thailand using Google Forms (Google LLC, Mountain View, CA, USA). Approximately one-third of households in Thailand have a dog or multiple dogs [17], which the central region, including Bangkok, has the most dogs [16]. A majority of owners in Thailand are females aged 31–40 years. Both male and female pet dogs are roughly equal. The most popular dog breed in Thailand is “mixed breed,” which is categorized as medium size (10–25 kg) [16]. However, data concerning the behavioral problems of pet dogs in Thailand are scarce. This includes sociodemographic data and cultural information that may reveal associations with various pet dog behaviors.

Therefore, this study aimed to explore and reveal factors affecting pet dog behavior in Thailand. These findings will provide essential information regarding behavioral problems among dogs, can support the management of clinical behavioral problems, and serve as a foundation for relevant future studies.

Materials and Methods

Ethical approval and Informed consent

The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of Walailak University (WUEC-21-253-01). All respondents read and accepted the consent form through an online document at the beginning of the online questionnaires.

Study period and location

The Thai version of the C-BARQ survey was conducted as an online survey using Google Form (Google LLC, Mountain View, CA, USA) from November 29th and December 31st, 2021. The questionnaire was advertised both online and on-site all over Thailand. For online advertising, we used Facebook, Instagram, Line, and websites that were related to veterinarians and dogs. For onsite advertising, we posted the QR code to many animal hospitals, which allows the respondent to use their mobile phone to scan the code and answer the questionnaire.

Respondents

The participation criteria for the study were as follows: (1) Respondents should be Thai residents with a minimum age of 18 years and (2) they must have owned a dog for more than 1 month, and the dog’s age must be at least 2 months old. Owners with multiple dogs were asked to answer separate questionnaires for each dog. The survey included sociodemographic questionnaires (Table-1) and the Thai C-BARQ [16] (Table-2). A total of 1827 responses were submitted to the survey.

Table-1.

Sociodemographic variables.

Index Detail Index Detail
Owners’ gender 1. Female
2. Male
Home types 1. Home with yard
2. Home with no yard
Dog owner age 1. 18–22 years old
2. 23–30 years old
3. 31–40 years old
4. 41–50 years old
5. 51–60 years old 6. >60 years old
Dog size 1. Toy breed
2. Small breed
3. Medium breed
4. Large breed
5. Giant breed
Dog age 1. 1–5 months old
2. 6–12 months old
3. 13–23 months old
4. 2–6 years old
5. 7–9 years old 6. >9 years old
Number of dogs in the household 1. 1 dog
2. 2 dogs
3. 3 dogs
4. 4 or more dogs
Dog gender 1. Female
2. Male
Dog reproductive status 1. Intact
2. Neutered
Dog training 1. Yes
2. No
Other species of animals in the household 1. None
2. Has other species in the household
Dog walking routine 1. None
2. 1–3 times/week
3. 4–7 times/week
4. >7 times/week
Diet types 1. Conventional diet
2. Homemade diet
3. Biological appropriate raw foods
Duration of dogs was left alone at houses per day 1. None
2. 1–3 h/day
3. 3–6 h/day
4. 6–9 h/day
5. 9–12 h/day 6. >12 h/day
Number of meal per day 1. 1 meal/day
2. 2 meals/day
3. 3 meals/day
4. ≥4 meals/day
5. ad libitum

Table-2.

The formulae used to calculate 12 behavior subscale scores of Thai C-BARQ.

Subscales Formulae
Stranger-directed aggression Questionnaire items (10+11+12+15+16+18+20+21+22+28)/10
Unfamiliar dog/human-directed fear Questionnaire items (36+37+39+40+45+46+52+53)/8
Owner-directed aggression Questionnaire items (13+14+17+19+25+30+31)/7
Dog-directed aggression Questionnaire items (23+24)/2
Excitability Questionnaire items (62+63+64+65+66+67)/6
Dog rivalry Questionnaire items (32+33+34+35)/4
Attachment- and attention-seeking Questionnaire items (68+69+70+71+72+73)/6
Non-social fear and touch sensitivity Questionnaire items (38+41+43+44+47+48+49+50+51)/9
Chasing Questionnaire items (27+74+75+76)/4
Separation-related problems (Elimination) Questionnaire items (88+89)/2
Energy Questionnaire items (91+92+93)/3
Trainability Questionnaire items (2+3)/2

C-BARQ=Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire

Questionnaire

We used the Thai version of the C-BARQ, which was developed and validated from the modified Duffy and Serpell version [18] by Boonhoh et al. [16]. This version of the C-BARQ was chosen because the questionnaire was developed and validated, considering the language and cultural differences [19].

There are 12 subscales of canine behavior in the Thai C-BARQ, including:

  1. Stranger-directed aggression: A dog exhibits aggression toward strangers who approach it, its owner, or other family members, either within or outside the house premise.

  2. Unfamiliar dog- or human-directed fear: A dog exhibits fearful behavior when approached by unfamiliar dogs or strangers.

  3. Owner-directed aggression: A dog exhibits aggressive behavior toward its owner or other household members when provoked, groomed, stared at, or approached when eating or playing.

  4. Dog-directed aggression: A dog exhibits aggressive behavior when approached directly by unfamiliar dogs.

  5. Excitability: a dog reacts strongly to exciting or provocative situations, for example, going for a walk, doorbell ringing, the owner arriving home, and car trips and is difficult to calm down.

  6. Dog rivalry: A dog exhibits aggressive behavior toward other familiar dogs in the household.

  7. Attachment- and attention-seeking: A dog exhibits affection toward and seeks attention from the owner or other household members and becomes agitated when the owner pays attention to another person, dog, or animal.

  8. Non-social fear and touch sensitivity: A dog exhibits fearful behavior to sudden or loud noises, traffic, unfamiliar objects, nail clipping, grooming, bathing, and/or examination by a veterinarian.

  9. Chasing: A dog chases cats, birds, squirrels, and/or other small animals when given the opportunity.

  10. Separation-related problems (elimination): A dog eliminates in undesignated areas when left alone at night or during the daytime.

  11. Energy: A dog is active, energetic, playful, puppyish, boisterous, and/or always on the go.

  12. Trainability: A dog obeys simple commands immediately.

Questionnaire scoring method

Each subscale was scored using a Likert-type five-point scale (0–4) that measured the frequency and severity of each behavior. The scores were as follows: 0, Never; 1, seldom; 2, sometimes; 3, usually; and 4, always.

Some questions might have been unanswerable by some respondents; in these cases, the respondents were instructed to select “Not observed/Not applicable” or “N/A,” which was then categorized as a missing value. If more than 25% in each given subscale was “Not observed/Not applicable” or “N/A,” the value for the subscale was treated as missing and excluded from the subscale analysis. The missing values are shown in column 3 of Table-3.

Table-3.

Descriptive data of the Thai C-BARQ 12 subscales.

Thai C-BARQ subscales N Missing values Mean Median SD Min Max 25% 75%
Stranger-directed aggression 1637 190 1.21 1.10 0.96 0.00 4.00 0.40 1.80
Unfamiliar dog/human-directed fear 1563 264 1.02 0.88 0.91 0.00 4.00 0.25 1.50
Owner-directed aggression 1769 58 0.48 0.29 0.63 0.00 3.86 0.00 0.71
Dog-directed aggression 1488 339 1.40 1.00 1.17 0.00 4.00 0.00 2.00
Excitability 1638 189 2.53 2.67 0.88 0.00 4.00 2.00 3.17
Dog rivalry 1149 678 1.18 1.00 1.06 0.00 4.00 0.25 2.00
Attachment- and attention-seeking 1810 17 2.35 2.33 0.88 0.00 4.00 1.83 3.00
Non-social fear and touch sensitivity 1759 68 1.11 1.11 0.59 0.00 3.11 0.67 1.56
Chasing 1759 68 2.09 2.00 1.22 0.00 4.00 1.00 3.00
Separation-related problems (Elimination) 1753 74 0.69 0.00 1.10 0.00 4.00 0.00 1.00
Energy 1784 43 1.34 1.33 0.71 0.00 2.67 0.67 2.00
Trainability 1782 45 2.27 2.50 1.13 0.00 4.00 1.50 3.00

SD=Standard deviation, Min=Minimum, Max=Maximum, 25%=25th percentile, 75%=75th percentile, C-BARQ=Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistical data of the Thai version of the C-BARQ, including population size (N), mean, median, standard deviation, minimum, maximum, 25th percentile, and 75th percentile, were calculated using Jamovi version 2.3 (The Jamovi project, Sydney, Australia). Spearman’s correlation was used to analyze the relationship among subscales and between the subscales and sociodemographic variables (Table-1). Values of coefficients (positive or negative) >0.01 with p ≤ 0.05 were considered statistically significant correlations.

Results

Thai C-BARQ 12 subscale data

The most complete subscale was “attachment and attention-seeking,” which had only 17 missing values. In contrast, “dog rivalry” contained the highest number of missing values, with 678. Considering each subscale score, “excitability” was found to have the highest mean score of 2.53, while “owner-directed aggression” had the lowest mean score of 0.48. The median for “separation-related problems (elimination)” had the lowest score (0.00), and the median for “excitability” was the highest (2.67). The minimum score of all subscales was 0, while the maximum score (4.00) was recorded in nine out of 12 subscales (Table-3). The lowest value of the maximum scores was for “energy” (2.67). At the 25th percentile, “owner-directed aggression,” “dog-directed aggression,” and “separation-related problems (elimination)” scored 0; at the 75th percentile, the highest score was for “excitability” (3.17) (Table-3).

Relationships among Thai C-BARQ subscales

Regarding the relationship among subscales, significant negative correlations were detected between “unfamiliar dog–/human-directed fear” and “energy” (r = −0.053; p = 0.035), and between “unfamiliar dog–/human-directed fear” and “trainability” (r = −0.062; p = 0.014). The subscale “attachment- and attention-seeking” was positively correlated with “excitability” (r = 0.056; p = 0.023), “non-social fear and touch sensitivity” (r = 0.052; p = 0.029), and “chasing” (r = 0.074; p = 0.002). A positive association was also observed between “owner-directed aggression” and “energy” (r = 0.052; p = 0.029) (Table-4).

Table-4.

Spearman’s correlations between 12 subscales of Thai C-BARQ.

i) ii) iii) iv) v) vi) vii) viii) ix) x) xi) xii)
i) Stranger-directed aggression
ii) Unfamiliar dog/human-directed fear 0.017
iii) Owner-directed aggression −0.034 −0.004
iv) Excitability −0.003 0.014 0.008
v) Dog-directed aggression 0.010 0.025 −0.010 0.020
vi) Dog rivalry 0.017 0.008 0.015 0.034 −0.035
vii) Attachment- and attention-seeking −0.018 0.004 −0.021 −0.056* −0.033 0.045
viii) Non-social fear and touch sensitivity 0.041 0.029 0.007 −0.023 −0.010 0.029 0.052*
ix) Chasing 0.038 −0.025 0.017 0.028 0.033 0.029 0.074** -0.040
x) Separation-related problems (elimination) 0.031 −0.038 −0.002 −0.025 0.018 −0.011 0.009 0.005 0.010
xi) Energy −0.027 −0.053* 0.052* 0.012 −0.001 0.021 0.041 0.024 0.020 −0.003
xii) Trainability −0.008 −0.062* −0.037 −0.001 −0.051 0.032 0.036 0.007 0.000 −0.016 0.019
*

p<0.05,

**

p<0.01, ***p<0.001 (2 tailed). C-BARQ=Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire

Factors associated with Thai C-BARQ subscales

Stranger-directed aggression and unfamiliar dog–/human-directed fear

We observed significant associations between subscales and variables or factors in this study. A score for stranger-directed aggression was negatively correlated with dog size (p < 0.001) and the frequency of walks (p < 0.001), while it was positively correlated with the number of dogs in the household (p < 0.001) and diet type. Biologically Appropriate Raw Food (BARF), homemade, and conventional diets were associated with the highest to lowest stranger-directed aggression, respectively (p = 0.005). The unfamiliar dog–/human-directed fear score was positively correlated with the owner’s age (p = 0.037), dog’s age (p = 0.044), and number of meals per day (p = 0.019); however, it was negatively associated with body size (p < 0.001) and dog’s gender (p = 0.006). Receiving no training (p = 0.028) and consuming BARF (p = 0.044) were associated with high levels of fearfulness toward unfamiliar dogs and humans (Table-5).

Table-5.

Spearman’s correlations between Thai C-BARQ subscales and factors.

Thai C-BARQ subscales Owner gender Owner age Home type Dog size Dog age Dog gender Dog reproductive status Number of dogs in the household Other species in the household Training Walking the dog/week Feeding types Meals/day Left alone (h/day)
Stranger- directed aggression −0.015 0.037 −0.048 −0.168*** 0.028 0.018 0.041 0.128*** −0.028 0.029 −0.103*** 0.070** 0.032 −0.010
Unfamiliar dog/human- directed fear −0.004 0.053* −0.021 −0.136*** 0.051* −0.070** 0.016 0.035 0.016 0.055* 0.024 0.051* 0.059* 0.022
Owner- directed aggression 0.029 −0.023 0 −0.166*** 0.043 0.095*** −0.024 −0.050* 0.166*** 0.076** 0.019 0.030 0.061* 0.010
Excitability 0.026 −0.042 0.086*** 0.016 −0.080** 0.028 −0.105*** −0.030 −0.020 −0.009 0.012 −0.059* 0.023 0.02
Dog-directed aggression 0.029 0.057* −0.068** −0.041 0.104*** 0.079** 0.065* 0.133*** −0.014 0.037 −0.048 0.070* −0.035 0.015
Dog rivalry −0.022 0.00 −0.095** −0.067* 0.083** 0 0.039 0.263*** −0.018 0.062* −0.108*** 0.125*** −0.076** 0.028
Attachment- and attention- seeking 0.015 −0.049* 0.045 −0.044 −0.116*** −0.017 −0.128*** 0.040 0.019 0.067** −0.015 −0.010 0.096*** −0.016
Non-social fear and touch sensitivity −0.034 −0.022 −0.034 −0.063** 0.103*** −0.071** 0.005 0.050* −0.050* 0.078** -0.072** 0.010 0.012 0.057*
Chasing −0.031 0.017 −0.112*** 0.103*** −0.031 −0.033 0.024 0.060* −0.156*** 0.021 -0.011 0.069** −0.013 0.003
Separation- related problems (elimination) 0.070** −0.069** 0.076** −0.122*** −0.053* 0.044 −0.120*** −0.022 −0.034 0.015 0.005 −0.045 0.024 0.002
Energy 0.002 −0.097*** −0.026 0.097*** −0.337*** 0.054* −0.224*** −0.040 0.028 −0.028 0.066** −0.035 0.071** −0.013
Trainability −0.001 0.046* 0.022 0.135*** −0.071** 0.001 0.016 −0.061* −0.019 −0.163*** 0.212*** −0.054* −0.071** −0.029
*

p<0.05,

**

p<0.01,

***

p<0.001 (2 tailed). C-BARQ=Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire

Owner-directed aggression and excitability

The owner-directed aggression score was positively associated with male dogs (p < 0.001), the presence of other species in the household (p < 0.001), no training (p = 0.001), and the number of meals per day (p = 0.01); however, it was negatively correlated with dog body size (p < 0.001) and the number of dogs in the household (p = 0.035). Excitability was observed to be dependent on several variables. Younger dogs (p = 0.001), neutered dogs (p < 0.001), dogs fed with BARF diet (p = 0.016), and those that lived in homes without yards (p < 0.001) (e.g., apartments and condominiums) were associated with a high excitability score (Table-5).

Dog-directed aggression and dog rivalry

Older dog owners (p = 0.028), older dogs (p < 0.001), male dogs (p = 0.002), neutered dogs (p = 0.013), multi-dog households (p < 0.001), feeding type (p = 0.007), and homes with yards (e.g., detached houses) (p = 0.008) were linked to an increased dog-directed aggression score. Similarly, some of these factors were found to be associated with dog rivalry scores (residential type, p = 0.001; older dogs, p = 0.005; greater number of dogs in the household, p < 0.001; and diet type, p < 0.001). In addition, smaller body size (p < 0.001), untrained dogs (p = 0.036), lower dog walking times per week (p < 0.001), and fewer daily meals (p = 0.01) were also factors associated with dog rivalry behavior levels (Table-5).

Attachment- and attention-seeking, and non-social fear and touch sensitivity behaviors

The attachment- and attention-seeking score was found to be correlated with younger dog owners (p = 0.036), younger dogs (p < 0.001), intact dogs (p < 0.001), untrained dogs (p = 0.004), and several daily meals (p < 0.001). The non-social fear and touch sensitivity score were related to smaller size dogs, older dog ages, female dogs, a greater number of dogs in the household, the absence of other animals in the household, untrained dogs, routines with less dog walking time, and longer periods of dogs being left alone in the home (Table-5).

Chasing and separation-related problems (elimination)

The chasing score correlated with more space in the home (p < 0.001), larger dog size (p < 0.001), a greater number of dogs in the home (p = 0.012), absence of other species in the home (p < 0.001), and diet type (p = 0.004). The separation-related problem (elimination) score was correlated with male dog owners (p = 0.003), younger dog owners (p = 0.004), homes with no yard (p = 0.001), smaller dog sizes (p < 0.001), younger dogs (p = 0.026), and intact dogs (p < 0.001) (Table-5).

Energy and trainability

The rating for energy was associated with younger dog owners (p < 0.001), larger dogs (p < 0.001), younger dogs (p < 0.001), male dogs (p = 0.022), and intact dogs (p < 0.001), greater dog walking time routines (p < 0.001), and a greater number of daily meals (p = 0.003). The trainability score was dependent on dog owner age (p = 0.05), with older owners having correspondingly higher scores. Trainability was also associated with larger dogs (p < 0.001), young dogs (p = 0.003), lower number of dogs in the household (p = 0.01), trained dogs (p < 0.001), dog walking routine times (p < 0.001), lower number of daily meals (p = 0.003), and conventional diet (p = 0.022) (Table-5).

Discussion

In the previous studies, the most frequent dog behavior problem reported by owners were excitability [3]; separation-related behavior [20]; and aggression [7, 21], particularly human-directed aggression [21]. The present study revealed that based on the high scores in the Thai C-BARQ online questionnaire, “excitability,” “attachment and attention-seeking,” and “chasing” were the top three dog behavior problems in Thailand. Excitability had the highest mean score in the present study, which was associated with homes with no yard, young dogs, and intact dogs. Our results are similar to the findings of Kobelt et al. [3], in addition to having children and toys in the household and first-time dog owners. Attachment- and attention-seeking behavior was associated with younger owners and dogs, which may be the result of first-time dog ownership and age-influenced dog behavior. Chasing was correlated with homes with yards and larger dogs because of the activity of the bigger dogs and available home area.

Gender associated with dog behaviors

A previous study showed that male dogs develop behavior problems more frequently than female dogs [7]. Intact dogs had notably more behavior problems – including excitability, attachment- and attention-seeking, inappropriate elimination, and energy – compared with those of neutered dogs. Neutered dogs posed lesser problems in terms of aggression than intact dogs, while they commonly caused other behavior problems, including obsessive behavior, attention-seeking behavior, and house soiling [7]. In addition, in this study, male dogs showed markedly higher scores for owner-directed aggression, dog-directed aggression, and high energy, but female dogs showed more fear of strangers and unfamiliar dogs.

We found that younger male dog owners who lived in homes with no yard (e.g., apartments or condominiums) were correlated with separation-related behavior. This finding may result from young male owners having fewer management skills and experience with owning pet dogs. A limitation of this study is its lack of data related to previous dog ownership experience. This problem is not only present among owners but is also found among intact, young, smaller dogs. Savalli et al. [12] reported that female dog owners had a higher risk of separation-related behavior among their dogs, which differed from the results of the present study.

Breed and age associated with dog behavior

Large breeds and younger dogs were reported to be easier to train in our study. These findings are similar to those of Hart and Hart [22], who reported that dog breeds influenced dog training [22]. Border collies and retrievers scored high on trainability [23], which is influenced by behavioral phenotypes in their genes [24]. Pet lifespans have been increasing, and research on dog aging has been increasing [25, 26]. The previous studies have reported that dog behavior and personality change throughout their lifetimes [25, 26]. Activity-independence educed quickly from puppyhood into adolescence and gradually reduced during later age periods [25]. Senior dogs made more mistakes than young dogs in a memory test and displayed higher avoidance of objects associated with new subjects [26]. These factors could be the reason for the lower trainability score observed in older dogs in our study. In addition, older dogs had higher unfamiliar dog-/human-directed fear scores similar to the results of Kutsumi et al. [27], who found that puppies responded more positively toward strangers than adult dogs. This could be attributed to the degeneration of their sensory organs.

Factors associated with aggression behaviors

Male and small breed dogs, having other species in the homeand fewer daily meals were associated with a high risk of owner-directed aggression in our study. Smaller-sized dogs and the high number of dogs in the home were associated with stranger-directed aggression, while dog-directed aggression problems were found to be associated with older neutered dogs, homes with no yard, and having many dogs in the home. Hsu and Sun [10] reported owner-directed aggression associated with male and older dogs, neutered dogs, and female owners, while dog- and stranger-directed aggression associated with intrinsic and environmental variables, including homes with yard space, rural areas [10], older dogs [11, 28], and mixed breed dogs [12]. These problems have many factors involved, including socialization deficits, hormones, and genetic and neurophysiological components [29].

Types of food associated with dog behaviors

We found that the BARF diet was associated with high risks of dog-directed aggression, stranger-directed aggression, unfamiliar dog–/human-directed fear, dog-directed aggression, dog rivalry and chasing, while a conventional kibble diet was associated with excitability and trainability. Dietary nutrition, particularly protein, is reported to have the potential to manipulate dog behavior [30]. Dodman et al. [30] reported a decrease in aggressive behavior in dogs fed with low-to-medium protein diets, while those fed a high-protein diet showed marked aggression. The BARF diet is categorized as a protein-rich diet and therefore exerts similar effects. Moreover, feeding dogs with tryptophan supplements and a low-protein diet might help decrease aggression [13] as tryptophan is converted into the neurotransmitter serotonin in the brain. However, animals need protein for maintaining health, wound healing, immune functions, and enzyme activities. In addition, low-protein diets or protein restriction can result in impaired body functions and muscle breakdown, leading to weakness, low energy, and altered behavior among dogs.

Conventional diets were associated with high excitability and trainability scores in the present study. Kibble food may contain a higher proportion of carbohydrates compared to that of BARF diets. In addition, high-calorific diets may influence excitability behaviors. Thus, Kibble food can be used as a treat for positive reinforcement of trained behaviors, such as sitting or staying [31].

Conclusion

The present study illustrated the significant variables correlated to common behaviors among pet dogs in Thailand for the first time. These findings provide insight into dog behavioral issues experienced by their owners. The most frequently reported dog behavior problems were “excitability,” “attachment- and attention-seeking,” and “chasing.” While the “trainability” subscale also had a high mean score, it was not a problematic behavior of the dogs but rather was an admired behavior. Finally, owner-directed aggression had the lowest mean score in this study. These results provide veterinary practitioners with insights into factors related to common behavioral problems in dogs. These findings also serve as an important informational basis for further studies to promote dog–human relationships and reduce dog abandonment in Thailand.

Authors’ Contributions

TW, CB, PS, and WB: Designed the study, prepared the questionnaire, and revised the manuscript. NW: Commented and advised on the experiment and revised the manuscript. WB: Collected and analyzed the data and drafted, revised, and edited the manuscript. All authors have read, reviewed, and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgments

This research work is financially supported by Walailak University Graduate Research Fund (Grant no. CGS-RF-2022/02). We thank all the volunteers and respondents who patiently responded and answered the questionnaire.

Footnotes

This research work is financially supported by Walailak University Graduate Research Fund (Grant no. CGS-RF-2022/02). We thank all the volunteers and respondents who patiently responded and answered the questionnaire.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Veterinary World remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published institutional affiliation.

References

  • 1.Wayne R.K, Leonard J.A, Vilà C. Genetic analysis of dog domestication. In: Zeder M.A, Bradley D, Emswhiller E, Smith D, editors. Documenting Domestication:New Genetic and Archaeological Paradigms. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press; 2006. pp. 279–293. Available from: https://books.google.co.th/books?hl=th&lr=&id=EaVTxjrbIFQC&oi=fnd&pg=PA279&ots=vzfeZVe5CJ&sig=pq1IhlxIs6lqFlFSVhNFfjql49c&redir_esc=y#v=onepage&q&f=false . Retrieved on 02-11-2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Jagoe A, Serpell J. Owner characteristics and interactions and the prevalence of canine behaviour problems. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1996;47(1–2):31–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kobelt A.J, Hemsworth P.H, Barnett J.L, Coleman G.J. A survey of dog ownership in suburban Australia-conditions and behaviour problems. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003;82(2):137–148. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Amat M, Manteca X, Mariotti V.M, de la Torre J.L.R, Fatjó J. Aggressive behavior in the English cocker spaniel. J. Vet. Behav. 2009;4(3):111–117. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bhanganada K, Wilde H, Sakolsataydorn P, Oonsombat P. Dog-bite injuries at a Bangkok teaching hospital. Acta Trop. 1993;55(4):249–255. doi: 10.1016/0001-706x(93)90082-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kurachi T, Irimajiri M, Mizuta Y, Satoh T. Dogs predisposed to anxiety disorders and related factors in Japan. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017;196:69–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Col R, Day C, Phillips C.J.C. An epidemiological analysis of dog behavior problems presented to an Australian behavior clinic, with associated risk factors. J. Vet. Behav. 2016;15:1–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Holland K.E. Acquiring a pet dog:A review of factors affecting the decision-making of prospective dog owners. Animals (Basel) 2019;9(4):124. doi: 10.3390/ani9040124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hsu Y, Serpell J.A. Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2003;223(9):1293–1300. doi: 10.2460/javma.2003.223.1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hsu Y, Sun L. Factors associated with aggressive responses in pet dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010;123(3–4):108–123. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.González-Ramírez M.T, Quezada-Berumen L, Landero-Hernández R. Assessment of canine behaviors using C-BARQ in a sample from Northern Mexico. J. Vet. Behav. 2017;20:52–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Savalli C, Albuquerque N, Vasconcellos A.S, Ramos D, de Mello F.T, Serpell J.A. Characteristics associated with behavior problems in Brazilian dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021;234:105213. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.DeNapoli J.S, Dodman N.H, Shuster L, Rand W.M, Gross K.L. Effect of dietary protein content and tryptophan supplementation on dominance aggression, territorial aggression, and hyperactivity in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2000;217(4):504–508. doi: 10.2460/javma.2000.217.504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Marx A, Lenkei R, Fraga P.P, Wallis L, Kubinyi E, Faragó T. Age-dependent changes in dogs'(Canis familiaris) separation-related behaviours in a longitudinal study. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021;242(4):105422. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pongrácz P, Gómez S.A, Lenkei R. Separation-related behaviour indicates the effect of functional breed selection in dogs (Canis familiaris) Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020;222:104884. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Boonhoh W, Sriphavatsarakom P, Waren N, Wongtawan T, Boonkaewwan C. The Validation of Thai Version of Canine Behavioural Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ) and the Exploration of Dog Ownership in Thailand. 2022. Available from: https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=421∳ . Retrieved on 22-09-2022.
  • 17.Department of Livestock Development. Pet register. 2022. Available from: https://164.115.40.46/petregister . Retrieved on 01-11-2022.
  • 18.Duffy D.L, Serpell J.A. Predictive validity of a method for evaluating temperament in young guide and service dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012;138(1–2):99–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Beaton D.E, Bombardier C, Guillemin F, Ferraz M.B. Guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 2000;25(24):3186–3191. doi: 10.1097/00007632-200012150-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Voith V.L, Wright J.C, Danneman P.J. Is there a relationship between canine behavior problems and spoiling activities, anthropomorphism, and obedience training? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1992;34(3):263–272. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bamberger M, Houpt K.A. Signalment factors, comorbidity, and trends in behavior diagnoses in dogs:1,644 cases (1991–2001) J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2006;229(10):1591–1601. doi: 10.2460/javma.229.10.1591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hart B.L, Hart L.A. Selecting pet dogs on the basis of cluster analysis of breed behavior profiles and gender. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 1985;186(11):1181–1185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wilson B, Serpell J, Herzog H, McGreevy P. Prevailing clusters of canine behavioural traits in historical US demand for dog breeds (1926–2005) Animals (Basel) 2018;8(11):197. doi: 10.3390/ani8110197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Takeuchi Y, Hashizume C, Arata S, Inoue-Murayama M, Maki T, Hart B.L, Mori Y. An approach to canine behavioural genetics employing guide dogs for the blind. Anim. Genet. 2009;40(2):217–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2052.2008.01823.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Turcsán B, Wallis L, Berczik J, Range F, Kubinyi E, Virányi Z. Individual and group level personality change across the lifespan in dogs. Sci. Rep. 2020;10(1):17276. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-74310-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Piotti P, Piseddu A, Aguzzoli E, Sommese A, Kubinyi E. Two valid and reliable tests for monitoring age-related memory performance and neophobia differences in dogs. Sci. Rep. 2022;12(1):16175. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-19918-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kutsumi A, Nagasawa M, Ohta M, Ohtani N. Importance of puppy training for future behavior of the dog. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 2013;75(2):141–149. doi: 10.1292/jvms.12-0008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Casey R.A, Loftus B, Bolster C, Richards G.J, Blackwell E.J. Human directed aggression in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris):Occurrence in different contexts and risk factors. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014;152:52–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Haug L.I. Canine aggression toward unfamiliar people and dogs. Vet. Clin. North Am. Small Anim. 2008;38(5):1023–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2008.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Dodman N.H, Reisner I, Shuster L, Rand W, Luescher U.A, Robinson I, Houpt K.A. Effect of dietary protein content on behavior in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 1996;208(3):376–379. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Writz A. Can Kibble be Used as Treats? (Dog Trainer Answers) - HoundGames. 2021. Available from: https://www.houndgames.com/kibble-used-as-treats . Retrieved on 14-11-2022.

Articles from Veterinary World are provided here courtesy of Veterinary World

RESOURCES