Keywords: Canis familiaris, dog–human relationship, emotion perception, emotion regulation, social cognition
Abstract
Emotions are critical for humans, not only feeling and expressing them, but also reading the emotional expressions of others. For a long time, this ability was thought to be exclusive to people; however, there is now evidence that other animals also rely on emotion perception to guide their behaviour and to adjust their actions in such way as to guarantee success in their social groups. This is the case for domestic dogs, who have tremendously complex abilities to perceive the emotional expressions not only of their conspecifics but also of human beings. In this paper we discuss dogs’ capacities to read human emotions. More than perception, though, are dogs able to use this emotional information in a functional way? Does reading emotional expressions allow them to live functional social lives? Dogs can respond functionally to emotional expressions and can use the emotional information they obtain from others during problem-solving, that is, acquiring information from faces and body postures allows them to make decisions. Here, we tackle questions related to the abilities of responding to and using emotional information from human expressions in a functional way and discuss how far dogs can go when reading our emotions.
Social media summary: dogs recognise and infer emotional information from humans and use this information to regulate their own behaviour.
1. Introduction
The emergence and the development of the ability to read emotions from individuals bring clear evolutionary advantages. Conversely, the mechanisms behind the ability to read emotions from heterospecifics are less clear. When thinking of domestic dogs, however, being able to recognise the emotional expressions and states of a different species, for instance humans, might be vital for functional social interactions. Living in a complex social world requires sophisticated knowledge about other individuals and this information is what allows animals to predict the behaviour of others (Bugnyar & Henirich, 2005). Social information is critical to adult humans in various ways, including making decisions and defining whether an individual might gain help or resources from others or not (Milinsk, 2016), and the same might be true for dogs.
For Marc Bekoff (2000), emotions can be broadly defined as complex multifaceted psychological phenomena that influence the management and the control of behaviour. Emotions play such a crucial role that their experience, in terms of both quality and intensity, can interfere in how events are registered in our memory (Ades et al., 1990).
Dogs, in their day-to-day lives, are not only passive of their own emotional experience but are also active subjects for expressing their emotions in a communicative way and, further, for recognising the emotions and emotional expressions of others. Dogs are very well adjusted to their multispecies groups, families and life dynamics. However, to what extent does appropriately perceiving the emotions of the people they interact with play a critical role to their success? Does reading emotions allow them to live functional social lives? In this paper, we aim at tackling questions related to dogs’ emotion perception abilities from a functional approach. First, we will bring to light the dog–human relationship, then we will follow with a discussion of the functional perspective on emotion perception and finally we will show evidence that dogs respond to and use implicit information from human emotional expressions.
2. Dogs and humans: More than a bond
A great deal of dogs’ social living occurs in mixed-species groups (Miklósi, 2008). In fact, humans and dogs are more than sympatric species, i.e. there is more than territory overlap in place. In fact, they establish long lasting, dynamic, complex and mutually advantageous relationships (Albuquerque & Ciari, 2013). These two species have co-existed for at least 10,000 years (Pendleton et al., 2018) with genetic evidence suggesting more than 20,000 years of divergence between the ancestor of the modern grey wolf and the ancestor of the domestic dog (Skoglund et al., 2015). During this shared evolutionary history, dogs may have been selected, probably unintentionally, for handling the complexities of heterospecific social relationships, with evidence supporting the hypothesis that they have developed different mechanisms to facilitate interaction with people (e.g. Nagasawa et al., 2015). The interspecific relationship between dogs and humans seems to be unique within the animal kingdom, with no other domestic animal having shared more of their evolutionary history in close contact with humans (Pendleton et al., 2018), and its benefits are of great social, health and economic relevance (e.g. Mills & Hall, 2014; Savalli & Ades, 2015).
Domestic dogs are known to be very good readers of human communicative cues such as pointing and looking (e.g. Ford et al., 2019), even from very young ages (Hare et al., 2002) or with little experience with people (Riedel et al., 2008). Dogs have also been shown to be sensitive to people's attentional state, showing distinct behaviour depending on the person's attention direction and attention availability (e.g. Call et al., 2003; Bräuer et al., 2004; Kaminski et al., 2009; Savalli et al., 2013). In addition, dogs have been found to be sensitive to ostensive directional signals, showing better performance in social tasks when communicative cues are presented in combination with them (Téglas et al., 2012) and not understanding cues as communicative when they are not directed at the subjects (Kaminski et al., 2012). Furthermore, Savalli et al. (2014, 2016) discuss that dogs also produce communicative signals and they do so in a functionally referential and intentional way.
Even though most studies are done with family dogs, i.e. dogs that live with a human family in a household, stray dogs represent more than 80% of the global dog population (Cabral & Savalli, 2020). These animals, so-called free-ranging dogs, are not restricted by human activities and are not under direct human care. They are seen living on the streets as scavengers and are very common in developing countries (Majumder et al., 2014) such as India and Brazil. To date, a few studies have been conducted with these animals and have found evidence for their complex cognitive abilities. For instance, they are capable of assessing the quantity of opponents in intergroup conflicts (Bonanni et al., 2010), assessing the intention (friendly vs. threatening gestures) of humans in a food provisioning task (Bhattacharjee et al., 2018) and using information provided by human pointing distal cues (Bhattacharjee et al., 2020), among others.
Emotional cues, however, are more subtle and their perception can comprise different processes. Despite its recency, the study of emotion perception in animals, especially non-primates, has been growing strong and rapidly. For instance, studies have shown that dogs present cognitive biases when exploring faces and show differential visual processing when presented with human or dog faces (Racca et al., 2012; Somppi et al., 2016). Dogs have been empirically shown to be particularly sensitive to human emotions (Kujala, 2018; Albuquerque, 2017). They discriminate and show differential responses to emotional cues expressed through body postures, facial expressions, vocalisations and odours (Vás et al., 2005; Müller et al., 2015; Albuquerque et al., 2016; Caeiro et al., 2017; D'Aniello et al., 2017), and emotional cues can influence their behaviour (e.g. Merola et al., 2012a; Albuquerque et al., 2021; Bremhorst et al., 2021). Moreover, current research has shown that cultural as well as developmental factors can influence these abilities (Katayama et al., 2019; Amici et al., 2019; Bolló et al., 2020).
3. Functional perspective of emotion perception
A functional approach refers to the history of the behavioural trait as well as to the consequences of possessing such a trait (Keltner & Haidt, 1999). The use of emotional information from conspecifics and, in the case of the dog, heterospecifics, may be seen as an adaptation for handling the complexities of the social environment, regardless of its underlying causes. For instance, affiliative behaviours are more flexible when they involve strategic decision-making, i.e. making choices conditional to the behaviour of the members of one's group (Hall & Brosnan, 2016). Therefore, anticipating someone's future behaviour and being able to respond accordingly is cognitively demanding and highly advantageous.
There is enough evidence to allow conclusions about the expression and the perception of emotions by dogs. Several animal species are known to be sensitive to emotions (e.g. Proops et al., 2018; Nawroth et al., 2018; Albuquerque et al., 2016). However, being able to obtain information from emotional expressions is not necessarily functional. Dogs’ social cognition facilitates the interaction with humans, and the ability to read and respond appropriately to emotional cues may have been – and may still be – key for the establishment of these interspecific bonds. In this context, it becomes crucial to investigate how dogs respond to emotional expressions and whether and how dogs use the emotional information from others in social situations mediated by distinct emotional valences.
From an evolutionary perspective, expressing and perceiving emotions becomes adaptive when the receiver uses the emotional information conveyed by the signaller to solve problems and to guarantee their success over ecologically relevant resources, i.e. one must use the emotional information in a way that increases fitness (e.g. feeding, monopolising food patches, finding mating partners), otherwise expressing and perceiving emotions will not be positively selected. According to van Kleef (2009), emotional expressions may affect an observer by triggering inferential processes and/or affective reactions in them and can benefit the receivers of the information with inputs on their decision-making.
Social life allows individuals to benefit from using public information and learning through socially mediated processes (Kendal et al., 2009; Resende et al., 2021) in various activities, such as feeding, mating, tool-use and cooperating, and these abilities are spread over the animal kingdom. For instance, human infants assess individuals by their behaviour towards others (Hamlim et al., 2007) and can selectively evaluate social interactions (Hamlim et al., 2011); great apes and monkeys can distinguish cooperative from non-cooperative parties (Call et al., 2004; Phillips et al., 2009), allocate differential attention to individuals depending on their role in social contexts (McFarland et al., 2013) and use social information of group members to adjust foraging strategies (Loreto, 2015); and dogs go in a similar direction, having been shown to be capable of using publicly available information (discerning the intention of different people in food-sharing interactions) and discriminating helper individuals from non-helpers (Marshall-Pescini et al., 2011; Chijiwa et al., 2015). Furthermore, a few studies have looked at the predictive use of emotional cues, with data showing that different animal species can use affective cues to direct their own behaviour (Waller et al., 2016; Buttelmann et al., 2009; Morimoto & Fujita, 2012; Buttelmann & Tomasello, 2013).
The relationship between dogs and humans is extremely relevant for the study of the evolution of social cognition in cooperative contexts owing to all its particularities (e.g. Savalli & Albuquerque, 2017). According to Keltner and Haidt (1999), functional explanations at group levels focus on how social information impacts the interaction between individuals who share common goals. Moreover, the functional approach allows the investigation of the function of emotions regardless of the cognitive construction of emotional states (Farb et al., 2013).
The ability to obtain information from faces is considered to be one of the major reasons for humans thriving as social animals (Fantz, 1964). Facial recognition may be divided into different components, including the ability to recognise facial expressions and to access the information within them (Johnson & de Haan, 2015). According to Ferretti and Papaleo (2018), recognising emotions presumes the encoding of multimodal sensory information to provide cues about the emotional states of another individual. Taking from its complex attributions, recognising emotions from faces has long been associated with animals with a large repertoire of facial expressions, such as humans and non-human primates. However, owing to its great adaptive value, especially towards mediating affiliative behaviours, avoiding harmful interactions and, thus, increasing survival chances, this ability seems to be advantageous for many groups of animals (Ferretti & Papaleo, 2018).
In 2018, Albuquerque and colleagues investigated the functional response to emotional expressions. They used dogs’ well-known behaviour ‘mouth-licking’ (i.e. licking one's own mouth) as a model, testing whether dogs reliably respond to emotional expressions. They found that mouth-licking was exhibited significantly more towards human faces showing negative emotional expression compared with happy faces. These findings show functional responses to emotional information by dogs and suggest that dogs have a functional understanding of emotional expressions.
Moreover, a few studies have found that dogs show physiological changes when presented with emotional expressions. For example, Yong and Ruffman (2014) revealed that the cortisol levels of dogs increased after listening to crying human infants and Siniscalchi et al. (2018a) found that domestic dogs show asymmetric engagement of brain hemispheres linked to physiological changes, with the prevalent use of the right hemisphere when processing negative vocalisations and the left hemisphere when processing positive valence.
Heart rate, heart rate variability and other measures such as body temperature have been used to assess the physiological responses of dogs to pleasant and aversive stimuli (e.g. Travain et al., 2016; Riemer et al., 2016). These measures have also been used to investigate dogs’ reactions to more subtle affective stimuli such as facial expressions with different emotional content (e.g. anger, happiness, sadness), and have provided data on differential physiological reactions to emotionally charged stimuli (Siniscalchi et al., 2018b) and that these responses are significantly affected by the subjects’ ontogenetic experiences (Barber et al., 2017). Furthermore, oxytocin levels are an important physiological measure of emotional expression in dogs (Mitsui et al., 2011). They also modulate the way dogs perceive human faces and, therefore, human facial expressions (Kis et al., 2017), and their emotional processing through mechanisms that may facilitate human–dog communication (Somppi et al., 2017).
4. Functional use of emotional information from human expressions
Being able to process emotional displays facilitates group cohesion (Racca et al., 2012) and allows individuals to use other's affective information to cope with events and objects in the environment (Merola et al., 2014). For instance, social referencing is the process of using social cues from another individual when facing novel stimuli and it serves as a source of approaching or avoidance information to guide one's own behaviour regarding a specific event or object (Merola et al., 2012a). A few studies have covered this aspect in domestic dogs, with evidence to support their capability of using human emotional cues via social referencing (e.g. Merola et al., 2012b), from ages as young as 8 weeks (Fugazza et al., 2018). However, while social referencing refers to the use of predictive cues to guide behaviour, it does not imply inferential processes. According to Adolphs (2002), when humans see a ‘scared’ facial expression, for example, we do not only relate it to other facial expressions in terms of its structure, but also acknowledge that the person has probably perceived something ‘scary’ and is likely to scream and run away. This information is not present in the structure of the facial expression itself; it is retrieved from past experiences with the world.
A few studies have pointed out the possibility that dogs may be able to use emotional information in a functional manner. For example, in 2013, Buttelmann and Tomasello exposed dogs to a situation where a person could behave (visually and acoustically) in a happy, disgusted or neutral manner when looking into a box and the subjects could choose one of the boxes to find hidden food. They provided important insights into the use of predictive emotional cues, as the subjects responded appropriately. In another study, Ford et al. (2019) tested dogs with the classic two-choice task, where there were two baited opaque containers that dogs must choose from with no information except the gestural cues of a person in relation to the items. In their experimental design, they presented dogs with conditions where the human directional gestural cues (looking or pointing) were conflicting, i.e. looking at one while pointing at the other. In addition, they included facial expressions of different valences (displayed by the same human signaller) to investigate which way dogs would use the social information available. They found that dogs were more likely to avoid something that had been looked at with a potentially negative face and choose the other item.
The ability to recognise emotional expressions provides individuals with the means for better adjusted interactions in various social contexts and, following Leon-Rodriguez and Sierra-Mejia (2008), the next step is recognising the possible consequences of emotional expressions. In humans, this type of knowledge usually presumes detrimental effects from negative emotions and beneficial influences of positive states. The authors suggest that being able to infer the consequences of emotions requires the establishment of causal and temporal relationships between discrete events (the emotional expressions and the potential subsequent action). Knowledge about the potential behavioural outcomes of others’ emotional experiences is crucial for making appropriate choices, reacting adequately and executing more effective social actions.
Following van Kleef et al. (2010), emotions have a great potential to shape behaviour and this applies not only to the individual level but also to social interactions and decision-making, situations when one's behaviour influences and is influenced by others’. For instance, in order to act in a strategic way, humans consider emotions as a vital source to inform their behaviour, and so will allocate their attention and efforts to it, especially when evaluating when, whether and to what extent to cooperate or to compete. In this sense, individuals use the information provided by others to guide their choices, which helps them to develop an adaptive course of action (van Kleef et al., 2010). For example, in cooperative settings, happiness might trigger approaching behaviours from the observer towards the signaller. On the other hand, according to Marsh et al. (2005), anger expressions facilitate avoidance-related behaviours, meaning that they are indeed perceived as threatening in some way. Furthermore, Farb et al. (2013) suggest an embodied cognition approach to the study of these aspects. They argue that in addition to modulating sensory inputs, emotions can reflect emotional states (cognitive and/or physiological) through observable embodied representations (musculature, posture, behaviour). The functional approach meets the embodied approach from the basis that emotional expressions are not arbitrary, i.e. particular expressions promote particular adaptive functions.
Grounded on this body of literature, Albuquerque et al. (2021) investigated dogs’ capacity to infer emotional states and emotional consequences and make functional use of indirect heterospecific emotional cues. They found that dogs are able to use the emotional information obtained from people in a functional way, i.e. they can infer the potential consequences of the displays and use that information for adjusting their own behaviour. Moreover, dogs were shown to consider the emotional information to a smaller extent when they did not need to use the human to get a desired food.
The findings presented are evidence that dogs are able to functionally use the emotional information displayed by humans when faced with a social problem. They pay attention, obtain information and use this information to adjust their behaviour. Moreover, they are able to use information previously stored in their memory from prior experiences with human emotional expressions to infer the emotional state of people.
5. Future directions
Other behavioural tasks could also contribute to this topic, such as discrimination tasks with the use of incongruent body-face emotional expressions. Usually, the body and face are part of an integrated system that conveys affective information and there is evidence that the presentation of a facial expression in a natural body context to human subjects allows a rapid acquisition of biologically relevant information whilst incongruent stimulus combinations may hamper categorisation (Meeren et al., 2005).
Moreover, investigating whether and how personality, attachment levels and styles, demographic factors and experience act on the motivation–emotion–cognition interface (Rosati & Hare, 2013) would provide further relevant information for the understanding of emotional communication between dogs and humans, of the affective and social lives of these domestic canids and of the evolution of social cognition as a whole.
Further research should also look into species-dependent differential responses and their underlying mechanisms. For instance, there is recent evidence showing that horses and cats are able to integrate multimodal information from facial expressions and vocalisations, i.e. they are able to recognise emotional expressions through image and sound (Nakamura et al., 2018; Quaranta et al., 2020). Thus, it is possible that other animals have the same ability and that these two species can respond to and use emotional information from human expressions in a similar way to dogs. Moreover, cultural differences and different cultural contexts must be taken into consideration if one desires to understand the dog as an entity, as a whole. In addition, looking at more than just dogs that are pets and live within a human or a multispecies family is critical. Most studies are conducted in WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) countries, with WEIRD dogs and people. However, other important research groups around the world are showing other facets of dogs that we did not acknowledge. This is the case for groups such as that of Bhadra (e.g. Majumder et al., 2014; Bhattacharjee et al., 2018; Brubaker et al., 2019), who have been working not only with Indian dogs, but with dogs that live on the streets, in a quite different dynamic than the usually studied subjects. This sort of aspect must be taken into account when a better understanding of the real interspecific phenomena between dogs and humans is sought.
6. Conclusion
There is a body of literature that explores dogs’ emotional lives, in terms of feelings, emotional expressions and emotion perception. However, the function of these abilities is far less understood. Investigating how obtaining information from others’ faces is, for instance, critical for the good adjustment of dogs in human (or multispecies) societies takes a different turn when we start to understand how this information is responded to and how this information is used. Dogs problem-solve all the time and being able to read humans’ emotional expressions, emotional states and emotionally driven behaviour is tremendously advantageous and may be seen as a highly important adaptive feature.
According to Schachter and Singer (1962), the experience of emotions can induce emotional states, which are more long lasting yet less obvious occurrences that are not time constrained to emotional expressions and could be seen as a faculty of physiological arousal and cognitive processing. As is the case for humans, the perception of emotional expressions may allow an observer to infer this subjective information about the producer of the signals (van Kleef, 2009), i.e. from the expression of the emotion, an observer can make certain decisions depending on what they have learned about that experience. Thus, in spite of the potential to expose information that can lead to vulnerability, emotions serve critical functions to organisms (Keltner & Haidt, 1999).
In addition to being able to recognise emotional expressions, dogs are able to access their affective content and respond to them. Moreover, dogs can make functional use of the emotional information they obtain from heterospecific visual emotional displays and utilise this information during decision-making. Recently, researchers worldwide have become more prone to agree that dogs’ social skills are the result of both proximal and ultimate causes and they are better understood according to an interaction prism (Resende & Garcia, 2017; Albuquerque & Savalli, 2017). Therefore, not only must a multimodal approach be taken into consideration when looking at dogs’ abilities to express and perceive emotions, but also an integrated approach must be applied to understanding how far these animals can go when they look at our emotional expressions.
Acknowledgements
First, we thank the Brazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq) and the São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP) for funding this study. We thank Professors Daniel S Mills, Kun Guo and Anna Wilkinson for their countless insights on the topic. We also want to thank Professors Nicolas Châline, Carine Savalli, Patricia Monticelli, Ricardo Dias and Antonio S Souto for all their inputs. Finally, we thank the editor and the reviewers for their important contribution.
Author contribution
NA and BR conceived the study. NA conducted literature review. NA and BR wrote the article.
Financial support
This work has been supported by CNPq (the Brazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development) and FAPESP (the São Paulo Research Foundation n° 2018/25595-0).
Conflicts of interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest
Research transparency and reproducibility
Not applicable.
Data availability
Not applicable.
References
- Ades, C., Botelho, A., Duarte, C. S., Teixeira, M. M., Arruk, M. E., Melo, P. C., & Gazire, P. (1990). Qualidade e intensidade do afeto como determinantes da memória cotidiana. Teoria e Pesquisa, 6(2), 111–123. [Google Scholar]
- Adolphs, R. (2002). Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: Psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1(1), 21–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albuquerque, N., & Ciari, M. (2013). Cães e seres humanos: Uma relação forte, complexa, duradoura e vantajosa. In Chelini M. O. & Otta E. (Orgs.), Terapia Assistida por Animais (pp. 1–22). Manole. [Google Scholar]
- Albuquerque, N., Guo, K., Wilkinson, A., Resende, B., & Mills, D. S. (2018). Mouth-licking by dogs as a response to emotional stimuli. Behavioural Processes, doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albuquerque, N., Guo, K., Wilkinson, A., Savalli, C., Otta, E., & Mills, D. (2016). Dogs recognise dog and human emotion. Biology Letters, doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albuquerque, N., Mills, D. S., Guo, K., Wilkinson, A., & Resende, B. (2021). Dogs can infer implicit information from human emotional expressions. Animal Cognition, doi: 10.1007/s10071-021-01544-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albuquerque, N. S. (2017). Emoções e cães: percepção, reconhecimento e empatia. In Savalli C. & Albuquerque N. S. (Orgs.), Cognição e comportamento de cães: a ciência do nosso melhor amigo (pp.183–210).EDICON. [Google Scholar]
- Albuquerque, N. S., & Savalli, C. (2017). A origem dos cães e de suas habilidades sociocognitivas: teorias e controvérsias. In Savalli C. & Albuquerque N. S. (Orgs.), Cognição e comportamento de cães: a ciência do nosso melhor amigo (pp. 21–42). EDICON. [Google Scholar]
- Amici, F., Waterman, J., Kellermann, C. M., Karimullah, K., & Bräuer, J. (2019). The ability to recognize dog emotions depends on the culturel milieu in which we grow up. Scientific Reports, doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-52938-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barber, A. L. A., Müller, E. M., Randi, D., Müller C. A., & Huber, L. (2017). Heart rate changes in pet and lab dogs as response to human facial expressions. Journal of Animal and Veterinary Sciences, 3(2), 46–55. [Google Scholar]
- Bekoff, M. (2000). Animal emotions: Exploring passionate natures. BioScience, 50(10), 861–870. 24 [Google Scholar]
- Bhattacharjee, D., Mandal, S., Shit, P., Varghese, M. G., Vishnoi, A., & Bhadra, A. (2020). Free-ranging dogs are capable of utilizing complex human pointing cues. Frontiers in Psychology, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhattacharjee, D., Sau, S., & Bhadra, A. (2018). Free-ranging dogs understand human intention and adjust their behavioral responses accordingly. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, doi: 10.3389/fevo.2018.00232. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bolló, H., Kovács, K., Lefter, R., Gombos, F., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., & Kis, A. (2020) REM versus non-REM sleep disturbance specifically affects inter-specific emotion processing in family dogs (Canis familiaris). Scientific Reports, doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-67092-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonanni, R., Natoli, E., Cafazzo, S., & Valsecchi, P. (2010). Free-ranging dogs assess the quantity of opponents in intergroup conflicts. Animal Cognition, doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0348-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 88, 299–317. [Google Scholar]
- Bremhorst, A., Mills, D. S., Würbel, H., & Riemer, S. (2021). Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs. Animal Cognition, doi: 10.1007/s10071-021-01532-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brubaker, L., Bhattacharjee, D., Ghasste, P., Babu, D., Shit, P., Bhadra, A., & Udell, M. A. R. (2019). The effects of human attentional state on canine gazing behaviour: a comparison of free-ranging, shelter, and pet dogs. Animal Cognition, doi: 10.1007/s10071-019-01305-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bugnyar, T., & Heinrich, B. (2005). Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledge and ignorant competitors. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 272, 1641–1646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buttelmann, D., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Do great apes use emotional expressions to infer desires? Developmental Science, 12(5), 688–698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buttelmann, D., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food? Animal Cognition, 16, 137–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabral, F., & Savalli, C. (2020) Concerning the human–dog relationship. Psicologia USP, doi: 10.1590/0103-6564e190109. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Caeiro, C., Guo, K., & Mills, D. (2017). Dogs and humans respond to emotionally competent stimuli by producing different facial actions. Scientific Reports, 7, doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15091-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Call, J., Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 257–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7(4), 488–498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chijiwa, H., Kuroshima, H., Hori, Y., Anderson, J. R., & Fujita, K. (2015). Dogs avoid people who behave negatively to their owner: Third-party affective evaluation. Animal Behaviour, 106, 123–127. [Google Scholar]
- D'Aniello, B., Semin, G. R., Alterisio, A., Aria, M., & Scandurra, A. (2017). Interspecies transmission of emotional information via chemosignals: From humans to dogs (Canis lupus familiaris ). Animal Cognition, 21, doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1139-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science, 146, 668–670. 91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farb, N. A. S., Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Emotions: Form follows function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 393–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferretti, V., & Papaleo, F. (2018). Understanding others: emotion recognition in humans and other animals. Genes, Brain and Behavior, doi: 10.1111/gbb.12544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford, G., Guo, K., & Mills, D. (2019). Human facial expression affects a dog's response to conflicting directional gestural cues. Behavioural Processes, 159, 80–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fugazza, C., Moesta, A., Pogány, A., & Miklósi, A. (2018). Presence and lasting effect of social referencing in dog puppies. Animal Behaviour, 141, 67–75. [Google Scholar]
- Hall, K., & Brosnan, S. F. (2016). Cooperation and deception in primates. Infant Behavior and Development, doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamlim, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamlim, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. PNAS, 108(50), doi: 10.1073/pnas.1110306108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hare, B., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition of dogs. Science, 298, 1634–1636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M. H., & de Haan, M. (2015). Developmental cognitive neuroscience: An introduction (4th ed.). Wiley Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Domestic dogs are sensitive to human's perspective. Behaviour, 1466, 979–998. [Google Scholar]
- Kaminski, J., Schulz, L., & Tomasello, M. (2012). How dogs know when communication is intended for them. Developmental Science, 11, 222–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katayama, M., Kubo, T., Yamakawa, T., Fujiwara, K., Nomoto, K., …, Kikusui, T. (2019). Emotional contagion from humans to dogs is facilitated by duration of ownership. Frontiers in Psychology, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13(5), 505–521. [Google Scholar]
- Kendal, R. L., Kendal, J. R., Hoppit, W., & Laland, K. N. (2009). Identifying social learning in animal populations: a new ‘option-bias’ method. PLoS ONE, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kis, A., Hernádi, A., Miklósi, B., Kanizsár, O., & Topál, J. (2017). The way dogs (Canis familiaris) look at human emotional faces is modulated by oxytocin. An eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kujala, M. V. (2018). Canine emotions as seen through human social cognition. Animal Sentience, 14(1), 1–34. [Google Scholar]
- Leon-Rodriguez, D. A., & Sierra-Mejia, H. (2008). Desarrollo de la comprensións de las consecuencias de las emociones. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicologia, 40(1), 35–45. [Google Scholar]
- Loreto, A. (2015). Estratégias de forrageio e uso de informação por macacos-prego (Sapajus sp.) semi-livres (Masters Dissertation, Institute of Psychology, University of São Paulo). www.teses.usp.br.
- Majumder, S. S., Bhadra, A., Ghosh, A., Mitra, S., Bhattacharjee, D., Chatterjee, J., … Bhadra, A. (2014). To be or not to be social: Foraging associations of free-ranging dogs in an urban ecosystem. Acta Ethologica, doi: 10.1007/s10211-013-0158-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2005) The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion, 5(1), 119–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall-Pescini, S., Passalacqua, C., Ferrario, A., Valsecchi, P., & Prato-Previde, E. (2011). Social eavesdropping in the domestic dog. Animal Behaviour, 81(6), 1177–1183. [Google Scholar]
- McFarland, R., Roebuck, H., Yan, Y., Majolo, B., Li, W., & Guo, K. (2013). Social interactions through the eyes of macaques and humans. PLoS ONE, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meeren, H. K. M., van Heijnsbergen, C. C. R. J., & de Gelder, B. (2005) Rapid perceptual integration of facial expressions and emotional body language. PNAS, 102(45), doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507650102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., Lazzaroni, M., Marshall-Pescini, S. (2014). Dogs’ comprehension of referential emotional expressions: familiar people and familiar emotions are easier. Animal Cognition, doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0668-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2012a) Social referencing in dog–owner dyads? Animal Cognition, doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0443-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2012b). Dogs’ social referencing towards owners and strangers. PLoS ONE, 7(10), doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047653 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miklósi, A. (2008). Dog Behaviour, evolution and cognition. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Milinsk, M. (2016). Reputation, a universal currency for human social interactions. Philosophical Transactions Royal Society B, 371, doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mills, D., & Hall, S. (2014). Animal-assisted interventions: making better use of the human-animal bond. Veterinary Record, 174, 269–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitsui, S., Yamamoto, M., Nagasawa, M., Mogi, K., Kikusui, T., Ohtani, N., & Ohta, M. (2011). Urinary oxytocin as a noninvasive biomarker of positive emotion in dogs. Hormones and Behavior, 60, 239–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morimoto, Y., & Fujita, K. (2012). Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) modify their own behaviors according to a conspecific's emotional expressions. Primates, 52, 279–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller, C. A., Schmitt, K., Barber, A. L. A., & Huber, L. (2015). Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces. Current Biology, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., Ohtani, N., Ohta, M., Sakuma, Y., … Kikusi, T. (2015). Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human–dog bonds. Science, 348, 333–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura, K., Takimoto-Inose, A., & Hasegawa, T. (2018). Cross-modal perception of human emotion in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Scientific Reports, doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26892-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nawroth, C., Albuquerque, N., Savalli, C., Single, M-S., & McElligott, A. (2018). Goats prefer positive human emotional facial expressions. Royal Society Open Science, doi: 10.1098/rsos.180491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pendleton, A., Shen, F., Taravella, A. M., Emery, S., Veeramah, K. R., Boyko, A. R., & Kidd, J. M. (2018). Comparison of village dog and wolf genomes highlights the role of the neural crest in dog domestication. BMC Biology, 16, doi: 10.1186/s12915-018-0535-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips, W., Barnes, J. L., Mahajan, N., Yamaguchi, M., & Santos, L. R. (2009). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Capuchin monkeys’ (Cebus paella) understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 12(6), 938–945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proops, L., Grounds, K., Smith, A. V., & McComb K. (2018). Animals remember previous facial expressions that specific human have exhibited. Current Biology, 28, 1–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quaranta, A., d'Ingeo, S., Amoruso, R., & Siniscalchi, M. (2020). Emotion recognition in cats. Animals, 10(7), doi: 10.3390/ani10071107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Racca, A., Guo, K., Meints, K., & Mills, D. S. (2012). Reading faces: Differential lateral gaze bias in processing canine and human facial expressions in dogs and 4-year-old children. PLoS ONE, 7(4), doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resende, B., Ballesteros-Ardilla, A., Fragaszy, D., Visalberghi, E., Izar, P. (2021). Revisiting the fourth dimension of tool use: How objects become tool for capuchin monkeys. Evolutionary Human Sciences, doi: 10.1017/ehs.2021.16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resende, B., & Garcia, M. (2017). Influências sociais no comportamento do cão. In Savalli C. & Albuquerque N. S. (Orgs.), Cognição e comportamento de cães: A ciência do nosso melhor amigo (pp. 105–132). EDICON. 29 [Google Scholar]
- Riedel, J., Schumann, K., Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The early ontogeny of human–dog communication. Animal Behaviour, 75, 1003–1014. [Google Scholar]
- Riemer, S., Assis, L., Pike, T. W., & Mills, D. S. (2016). Dynamic changes in ear temperature in relation to separation distress in dogs. Physiology and Behavior, doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosati, A. G., & Hare, B. (2013). Chimpanzees and bonobos exhibit emotional responses to decision outcomes. PLoS ONE, 8(5), doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savalli, C., & Ades, C. (2015). Benefícios que o convívio com um animal de estimação pode promover para saúde e bem-estar do ser humano. In Chelini M. O. & Otta E. (Orgs.), Terapia Assistida por Animais. Manole. [Google Scholar]
- Savalli, C., Ades, C., & Gaunet, F. (2014). Are dogs able to communicate with their owners about a desirable food in a referential and intentional way? PLoS ONE, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savalli, C., & Albuquerque, N. (2017). Cognição e comportamento de cães: A ciência do nosso melhor amigo. EDICON. [Google Scholar]
- Savalli, C., Resende, B., & Gaunet, F. (2016) Eye contact is crucial for referential communication in pet dogs. PLoS ONE, 11(9), doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0162161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savalli, C., Resende, B. D., & Ades, C. (2013). Are dogs sensitive to the human's visual perspective and signs of attention when using a keyboard with arbitrary symbols to communicate? Revista de Etologia, 12, 29–38. [Google Scholar]
- Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional states. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siniscalchi, M., d'Ingeo S., Fornelli, S., & Quaranta, A. (2018a). Lateralized behavior and cardiac activity of dogs in response to human emotional vocalizations. Scientific Reports, 8, doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18417-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siniscalchi, M., d'Ingeo, S., & Quaranta, A. (2018b). Orienting asymmetries and physiological reactivity in dogs’ response to human emotional faces. Learning and Behavior, doi: 10.3758/s13420-018-0325-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skoglund, P., Ersmark, E., Palkopoulou, E., & Dalén, L. (2015). Ancient wolf genome reveals an early divergence of domestic dog ancestors and admisture into high-latitude breeds. Current Biology, 25, 1515–1519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somppi, S., Törnqvist, H., Kujala, M. V., Hännien, L., Krause, M. C., & Vainio, O. (2016). Dogs evaluate threatening facial expressions by their biological validity – Evidence from gazing patterns. PLoS ONE, 11(1), doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somppi, S., Törnqvist, H., Topál, J., Koskela, A., Hänninen, L., Krause, C. M., & Vainio, O. (2017). Nasal oxytocin treatment biases dogs’ visual attention and emotional response toward positive human facial expressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Téglas, E., Gergely, A., Kupán, K., Miklósi, A., & Topál, J. (2012). Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Current Biology, 22, 1–4. 30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Travain, T., Colombo, E. S., Grandi, L. C., Heinzl, E., Pelosi, A., Prato-Previde, E., & Valsecchi, P. (2016). How good is this food? A study on dogs’ emotional responses to a potentially pleasant event using infrared thermography. Physiology and Behavior, doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.019.94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 184–188. [Google Scholar]
- Van Kleef, G. A., de Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2010). An interpersonal approach to emotion in social decision Making: the emotions as social information model. In Zanna M. P. (Ed.), Advances in experimental psychology (pp. 45–96). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Vás, J., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, A., & Csányi, V. (2005). A friend or an enemy? Dogs’ reaction to an unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and friendliness at different times. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 94, 99–115. [Google Scholar]
- Waller, B. M., Whitehouse, J., & Micheletta, J. (2016). Macaques can predict social outcomes from facial expressions. Animal Cognition, 19, 1031–1036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yong, M. H., & Ruffman, T. (2014) Emotional contagion: dogs and humans show a similar physiological response to human infant crying. Behavioural Processes, 108, 155–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.