Abstract
α-Synuclein aggregates constitute the pathology of Lewy body (LB) disease. Little is known about the effects of LB pathology in preclinical (presymptomatic) individuals, either as isolated pathology or coexisting with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology (β-amyloid (Aβ) and tau). We examined the effects of LB pathology using a cerebrospinal fluid α-synuclein-seed amplification assay in 1,182 cognitively and neurologically unimpaired participants from the BioFINDER study: 8% were LB positive, 26% Aβ positive (13% of those were LB positive) and 16% tau positive. LB positivity occurred more often in the presence of Aβ positivity but not tau positivity. LB pathology had independently negative effects on cross-sectional and longitudinal global cognition and memory and on longitudinal attention/executive function. Tau had cognitive effects of a similar magnitude, but these were less pronounced for Aβ. Participants with both LB and AD (Aβ and tau) pathology exhibited faster cognitive decline than those with only LB or AD pathology. LB, but not AD, pathology was associated with reduced sense of smell. Only LB-positive participants progressed to clinical LB disease over 10 years. These results are important for individualized prognosis, recruitment and choice of outcome measures in preclinical LB disease trials, but also for the design of early AD trials because >10% of individuals with preclinical AD have coexisting LB pathology.
Subject terms: Neurodegeneration, Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease
A longitudinal study of clinically unimpaired individuals reveals that Lewy body pathology measured in vivo is associated with worse smell and cognitive functions and predicted subsequent cognitive decline and progression to Parkinson’s disease or dementia with Lewy bodies.
Main
Lewy body pathology, the primary pathology of Lewy body disease (LBD), comprises the intraneuronal accumulation of aggregates of misfolded alpha-synuclein (α-syn) as LB and neurites1,2. The term LBD is used to describe mainly Parkinson’s disease (PD) and dementia with LB (DLB)1,2. Importantly, LB pathology is also often found as a copathology in AD3. According to studies using postmortem immunohistochemistry, LB pathology is present in approximately 8–12% of neurologically and cognitively unimpaired elderly individuals (‘incidental LBD’) with mean ages between 70 and 75 years4–6. Because of the previous lack of accurate biomarkers for misfolded α-syn, little is known about the effects of LB pathology in this preclinical (also known as ‘presymptomatic’) phase of LBD. However, recent advances have shown that misfolded α-syn can be detected in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of patients with LB pathology using in vitro seed amplification assay (SAA) of α-syn7. This novel method has shown very high sensitivity and specificity in the detection of neuropathologically verified LBD, especially in individuals with limbic and cortical LBs8–10 and for clinically diagnosed PD11. There are no studies using this method to examine the effects of LB pathology on cross-sectional and longitudinal changes in clinical symptoms in neurologically and cognitively unimpaired individuals, neither as an isolated pathology nor coexisting with AD pathology. Knowing these effects is of great importance in the design of preclinical LBD trials, specifically to plan the screening procedure and to choose sensitive outcome measures. The coexistence of LB and AD pathologies is also important considering the many ongoing preclinical AD trials enrolling cognitively unimpaired participants with AD pathology. If LB pathology has cognitive effects similar to AD pathology, trials that account for the presence of LB pathology could better detect the effects of anti-Aβ treatments. As a copathology to AD, accounting for the presence of LB pathology may also be relevant when examining the cognitive trajectories of preclinical individuals in observational studies according to the AD framework that characterizes the presence of Aβ (A), tau (T) and non-AD specific neurodegeneration (N) for diagnosis12 and prognosis13.
In this study we aimed to examine the earliest effects of LB pathology on cognitive and other clinical outcomes and compare them to the effects of Aβ and tau pathologies. The cognitive outcomes were examined cross-sectionally and longitudinally up to 10 years in 1,182 cognitively and neurologically unimpaired participants from the BioFINDER-1 (NCT01208675) and BioFINDER-2 (NCT03174938) studies. In addition, a survival analysis was performed examining subsequent progression to DLB or PD based on the presence of LB pathology at baseline.
Results
Participants and prevalence of AD and LB pathologies
Participant characteristics are shown in Table 1. AD pathology was defined as being Aβ and tau positive using CSF or positron emission tomography (PET) methods according to the National Institute of Aging–Alzheimer’s Association criteria12. LB pathology was defined as being α-syn SAA+ using a real-time, quaking-induced conversion assay (RT-QuIC)8. Note that the α-syn SAA we applied detects only the LB pathology seen in PD and DLB but not the presence of α-syn in the rare α-syn disorder multiple systemic atrophy10. Biomarker status was determined at study baseline. The prevalence of Aβ, tau and LB pathologies is shown in Fig. 1a–c. We found that 94 (8%) were LB+, 304 (26%) Aβ+ and 195 (16%) tau+, and 38 were both Aβ+ and LB+ (13% of Aβ positives). When defining AD pathology as being both Aβ+ and tau+ (ref. 12), we found that 941 (80%) participants had no AD nor LB pathology (AD–/LB–), 74 (6%) only LB pathology (AD–/LB+), 147 (12%) only AD pathology (AD+/LB–) and 20 (2%) both AD and LB pathology (AD+/LB+). All pathologies were more common with increasing age (see Fig. 1d–f and odds ratio (OR) in the figure legend). Adjusted for age, males were more than two times more likely to be LB+ compared with females (OR 2.56, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.66–3.99). No significant association between sex and Aβ or tau was found.
Table 1.
Variable | AD–/LB– (n = 941) | AD–/LB+ (n = 74) | AD+/LB– (n = 147) | AD+/LB+ (n = 20) | Total (n = 1,182) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age, years | 69 (9.1) | 73 (7.4) | 73 (5.7) | 75 (4.4) | 70 (8.8) |
Education, years | 12 (3.4) | 12 (3.2) | 12 (3.6) | 13 (4.7) | 12 (3.5) |
Sex, n female | 572 (60.8%) | 30 (40.5%) | 89 (60.5%) | 7 (35.0%) | 698 (59.1%) |
MMSE, points | 29 (1.1) | 29 (1.3) | 29 (1.2) | 28 (1.6) | 29 (1.1) |
Global cognition (z-score) | 0.056 (0.75) | −0.470 (0.80) | −0.630 (0.83) | −0.940 (0.79) | −0.070 (0.81) |
Memory (z-score) | −0.014 (0.99) | −0.560 (1.2) | −0.630 (1.3) | −1.000 (0.87) | −0.140 (1.1) |
Attention/executive (z-score) | −0.056 (0.91) | −0.390 (0.86) | −0.440 (0.86) | −0.600 (0.96) | −0.130 (0.91) |
Smell function (z-score) | 0.000 (0.96) | −1.300 (1.3) | −0.045 (0.77) | −0.990 (0.53) | −0.090 (1.0) |
Motor function (CIMP-QUEST, z-score) | 0.010 (0.91) | −0.650 (1.4) | −0.380 (1.2) | −0.054 (0.60) | −0.082 (1.0) |
Motor function (UPDRS-III, z-score) | 0.066 (0.97) | −0.110 (1.2) | −0.220 (1.0) | −0.730 (1.0) | −0 (1.0) |
Signs of REM sleep disorder, na | 24 (2.6%)a | 7 (9.5%)a | 2 (1.4%)a | 0 (0%)a | 33 (2.8%)a |
A/T/LB, n | |||||
A–/T–/LB– | 795 (84.5%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 795 (67.3%) |
A–/T+/LB– | 27 (2.9%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 27 (2.3%) |
A+/T–/LB– | 119 (12.6%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 119 (10.1%) |
A–/T–/LB+ | 0 (0%) | 55 (74.3%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 55 (4.7%) |
A–/T+/LB+ | 0 (0%) | 1 (1.4%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (0.1%) |
A+/T–/LB+ | 0 (0%) | 18 (24.3%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 18 (1.5%) |
A+/T+/LB– | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 147 (100%) | 0 (0%) | 147 (12.4%) |
A+/T+/LB+ | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 20 (100%) | 20 (1.7%) |
CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 | 0.095 (0.022) | 0.087 (0.024) | 0.043 (0.0097) | 0.039 (0.0081) | 0.087 (0.028) |
Aβ-PET (SUVR)b | 0.50 (0.093) | 0.54 (0.11) | 0.81 (0.11) | 0.83 (0.061) | 0.52 (0.12) |
CSF P-tau217 (pg ml–1) | 5.9 (2.7) | 7.1 (3.9) | 26 (16) | 43 (35) | 10 (12) |
Tau-PET (SUVR)c | 1.1 (0.092) | 1.1 (0.097) | 1.6 (0.400) | 2.1 (0.630) | 1.2 (0.190) |
CSF α-syn SAA positivity (LB pathology), n | 0 (0%) | 74 (100%) | 0 (0%) | 20 (100%) | 94 (8.0%) |
Data are shown as mean (s.d.) unless otherwise specified. Note that tau-PET and Aβ-PET data are from BioFINDER-2 only, and CSF P-tau217 and CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 are from BioFINDER-1.
aBased on whether the participant had been told that he/she seems to ‘act out his/her dreams’48. Data available only for BioFINDER-2.
bMeasured in a composite neocortical ROI using [18F]flutemetamol with pons as reference region.
cMeasured in a temporal meta-ROI using [18F]RO948 with inferior cerebellar cortex as reference region46.
Cross-sectional associations with clinical outcomes
When comparing cross-sectional group differences at baseline (Fig. 2a–e), participants that were AD–/LB+ or AD+/LB–performed significantly worse in baseline global cognition (measured with a preclinical Alzheimer cognitive composite (PACC)) and memory (measured with a ten-word delayed recall test) compared with those who were AD–/LB–. Moreover, AD+/LB+ participants had significantly worse global cognition compared with AD–/LB+. When examining the three pathologies as independent predictors in the same model, LB and tau pathologies had similar significant effects on lower global cognition and memory performance (Fig. 2f–h). On the other hand, Aβ pathology was the only pathology independently associated with worse attention/executive function but had no independent effect on memory function (Fig. 2g–h). The effect on memory was further examined in a recognition task, where only tau was significantly associated with worse performance (Supplementary Table 1). Reduced sense of smell was seen in participants that were AD–/LB+ or AD+/LB+ (Fig. 2d) and was specific to LB pathology (Fig. 2i). Its accuracy for LB pathology was further examined in receiving operating characteristic (ROC) analysis using LB status as outcome. The smell test predicted LB pathology with an area under the curve (AUC) of 0.80 (95% CI 0.71–0.89, overall accuracy, 85%; Extended Data Fig. 1).
Worse motor function, measured using an informant-based questionnaire, was seen in those participants that were AD–/LB+ or AD+/LB– compared with those that were AD–/LB– but, when examining the independent effects of LB, Aβ and tau (in the same model) on motor function, none of the pathologies was significant at this preclinical disease stage (Fig. 2e,j). The absence of effect on motor function was also confirmed when using the unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale part III (UPDRS-III) as a measure of motor function (Extended Data Fig. 2).
The cross-sectional effect sizes of LB pathology on cognitive, smell and motor functions were similar regardless of whether the models were adjusted for Aβ and/or tau or not (Extended Data Table 1).
Extended Data Table 1.
Linear regression models using cognition, smell or motor function as outcome. Basic models included LB status, age, sex and years of education (education was not included in models with motor function) as predictors. At the second step, Aβ was added to the model and at the third step tau. The third model is thus the same as shown in Fig. 2F-J. Estimates in bold indicate a significant worse effect on the outcome. n/a, not applicable (not included in model)
Cross-sectional associations with pathologies and atrophy
There was a significant association between LB and Aβ status in that LB positivity was more likely to occur in the presence of Aβ positivity (P = 0.001 from the χ2 test), and also when adjusting for age (OR for LB, 1.72; P = 0.017). There was no significant association between LB and tau status (P = 0.15, P = 0.41 adjusted for age). Finally, LB status was not associated with magnetic resonance imaging measures of gray matter integrity in this preclinical population (see Methods for analyses).
Longitudinal associations with cognitive outcomes
The effect of the different AD/LB groups and the independent effects of all three pathologies (that is, LB, Aβ and tau) on longitudinal cognitive function were examined in linear mixed-effects (LME) models focusing on the interaction of time × AD/LB group (Fig. 3a–c) and time × pathology (absent/present; Fig. 3d–f). Group comparisons showed that those participants that were AD–/LB+, AD+/LB– or AD+/LB+ progressed faster in all cognitive measures than those who were AD–/LB– (Fig. 3a–c). Further, participants who were positive for both AD and LB pathologies (AD+/LB+) progressed significantly faster in memory and attention/executive function than those with only AD or LB pathology (AD+/LB– or AD–/LB+) (Fig. 3b–c), and in global cognition compared with those with AD–/LB+ (Fig. 3a). To demonstrate that differences in trajectories were not caused by baseline group differences, we performed a sensitivity analysis using change in cognition as outcome and adjusted the model for baseline cognitive test score (Extended Data Table 2). This confirmed the significant differences shown in Fig. 3a–c. When studying the independent effects of LB, Aβ and tau pathologies on cognitive progression we found similar effects of LB and tau pathologies on all cognitive outcomes, but these were less pronounced for Aβ (Fig. 3d–f). The longitudinal LME models with/without adjusting for Aβ and tau are shown in Extended Data Table 3 (showing that the effect sizes of LB pathology were similar regardless of whether adjusting for AD pathology or not).
Extended Data Table 2.
Linear mixed-effects (LME) models examining the interaction between AD/LB group and time (years) using change in cognition (global, memory or attention/executive) since baseline as outcome. The models were adjusted for baseline cognition (global, memory or attention/executive z-score), age, sex, and years of education, and included a random slope and intercept. The sensitivity analyses were performed in all instances where a baseline group difference was found (Fig. 2a–c).
Extended Data Table 3.
Linear mixed-effects (LME) models examining the interaction between pathology and time (years) using cognition as outcome. Basic models included the interaction between time and LB pathology, age, sex and years of education, as well as a random slope and intercept. At the second step, Aβ was added to the model and at the third step tau. The third model is thus the same as shown in Fig. 3d–f.
LB pathology and progression to PD or DLB
During a mean (s.d.) follow-up time of 4.46 (2.68) years of all study participants, 70 progressed to AD, 31 to vascular disease/dementia, 16 to DLB and six to PD irrespective of biomarker status at baseline (Extended Data Table 4). The survival analysis of progression to a clinical diagnosis of DLB or PD stratified on LB status at baseline is shown in Fig. 4. Only LB+ participants subsequently progressed to DLB or PD. The mean follow-up time of LB+ participants (n = 94) to the last visit or progression to DLB/PD was 3.92 (2.61) years, during which 23.4% progressed to either DLB or PD. Of those LB+ participants who did not progress to DLB/PD (n = 72), 14 (19.4%) developed LB pathology-related signs such as orthostatic hypotension (n = 9), signs of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder (n = 3) or signs of parkinsonism (n = 2) without fulfilling the clinical criteria for DLB or PD. In comparison, the smell test was not associated with a clinical follow-up diagnosis of DLB or PD, either as a binary predictor using survival analysis (P = 0.69) or as a continuous predictor in ROC analysis (AUC 0.64, 95% CI 0.45–0.83).
Extended Data Table 4.
Mean follow-up time: 4.46 (SD 2.68) years. For AD, VaD, DLB and FTD, event to clinical progression was determined as progression to at least MCI (that is, not requiring progression to dementia) together with the other clinical criteria. PD was diagnosed according to Gelb et al., but all also fulfilled the MDS criteria for probable PD at conversion. Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer’s disease; DLB, dementia with Lewy bodies; FTD, frontotemporal dementia; MDS, Movement Disorder Society; MSA, multiple system atrophy; NPH, normal pressure hydrocephalus; PD, Parkinson’s disease; PSP, progressive supranuclear palsy; VaD, vascular disease/dementia
Discussion
This study examines and compares the independent clinical effects of LB, Aβ and tau pathologies in a large population of cognitively and neurologically unimpaired individuals. Overall we found that LB pathology, detected by α-syn SAA in CSF, was present in 8% of the population (Fig. 1). It had similar effects on cross-sectional (Fig. 2) and longitudinal (Fig. 3) cognitive outcomes as AD pathology. Reduced smell function was specifically associated with LB pathology and a smell test could predict LB status with an AUC of 0.80 (Extended Data Fig. 1). Finally, survival analysis showed that only participants who were LB+ at baseline progressed to a clinical diagnosis of DLB or PD (Fig. 4).
The effect of LB pathology on memory function had a similar magnitude to that of tau pathology, both cross-sectionally (Fig. 2g) and longitudinally (Fig. 3e). As shown previously, the effect of tau on memory function in cognitively unimpaired individuals is related to early tau accumulation in the medial temporal lobe14–16. For LB pathology it could be related more to retrieval of memory than encoding, as indicated in our supplementary analysis showing that tau but not LB pathology was related to worse memory recognition (Supplementary Table 1). Although this result is preliminary due to the lower sample size, it is supported by previous results17. Other potential mechanisms could be the early involvement of LB pathology in the medial temporal regions18 or that LB pathology has a negative effect on dopamine19, which is important for memory encoding20.
The modest, isolated, cross-sectional effect of Aβ on attention/executive function agrees very well with recent results in cognitively unimpaired individuals15. This effect may reflect the early Aβ accumulation in regions important for attention and executive function that belong to the default mode and central executive networks21. The effect of LB pathology on global cognition measured with PACC (Figs. 2f and 3d), which is a measure used as a primary outcome in several preclinical AD trials22, is important to note. Not testing for LB positivity when including participants in such AD trials could hamper the ability to detect effects of anti-Aβ or anti-tau treatments on cognitive decline and could potentially be one of several causes for continuous cognitive decline despite complete removal of Aβ from the brain in AD participants23,24. Overall, we found that similar cognitive measures that are sensitive to changes in preclinical AD are also sensitive to LB pathology (Fig. 3) and could be suitable for preclinical LBD trials.
In the present study we found that 13% of Aβ+ participants were also LB+ (Table 1), and that LB positivity was significantly more likely to occur in the presence of Aβ positivity (OR 1.72) but not tau positivity. This is in agreement with neuropathology-based studies showing a clear overrepresented co-occurrence of AD and LB pathology in sporadic AD25,26.
Potential mechanisms for this co-occurrence could be a previously suggested seeding effect of Aβ on α-syn27 and indications that the APOE ε4 allele not only drives AD pathology but has also been associated with more severe LB pathology in AD28,29. Further, in autosomal dominant AD and Down syndrome, both of which have an overproduction of Aβ, LB pathology occurs more frequently than merely by chance (LB pathology prevalence of 50% in individuals with Down syndrome30 and 60% in autosomal dominant AD31 defined as LB pathology presence in at least the amygdala). This co-occurrence could be an issue when interpreting the effects of anti-AD treatments in Aβ+ cognitively unimpaired individuals, especially if LB+ participants are unequally distributed between treatment and placebo arms.
Regarding recruitment to future trials of preclinical LBD, detection of LB positivity will be crucial in identifying a relevant study population (for example for anti-LB therapy32). Our study shows that there is a relatively low prevalence of LB positivity (8%) in healthy middle-aged and elderly individuals, similar to that shown in recent smaller studies using α-syn SAA (3–10%) (refs. 33,34).
At the same time we found that progression to a clinical diagnosis of DLB or PD occurred only among participants who were LB+ at baseline, supporting the use of an α-syn SAA to identify preclinical LBD for early disease-modifying therapy trials in analogy to how Aβ and tau biomarkers are used today for preclinical AD trials. However, the low prevalence of LB pathology in unimpaired populations suggests that large screenings are needed to recruit adequately large populations, making CSF analyses a feasible, but not optimal, tool. The screening process would instead benefit from the use of blood or skin biopsies for α-syn SAA testing35–37, if they have comparable performance to CSF methods. Related to such a screening procedure, we found that the smell test of the present study had an AUC of 0.80 for discriminating LB+ versus LB– participants (Extended Data Fig. 1). Although this is not sufficiently accurate for identification of LB pathology on its own, it indicates that smell tests, potentially combined with other measures, might be used in a prescreening procedure to identify suitable individuals for more expensive and invasive α-syn SAA testing. Our finding of an association between a smell test and LB pathology is supported by neuropathological studies showing that incidental (preclinical) LBD is associated with impaired sense of smell38. This is also found for DLB/PD dementia cases as well as AD with, but not without, significant LB pathology39. Over 50 studies have found that impaired sense of smell is a common feature in clinically diagnosed AD40, but our results suggest that this symptom is entirely caused by concomitant LB pathology (Fig. 2i).
Regarding motor function, no convincing effect of LB pathology was observed using either an informant-based questionnaire (Fig. 1j) or a motor function scale (UPDRS-III; Extended Data Fig. 2b). Although LB pathology has been linked to motor function in symptomatic individuals41, motor function may be affected at later stages in some cases with LBD as shown in prodromal LBD (that is, DLB at the mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stage and isolated REM sleep behavior disorder), where only about one-half of these patients have reduced dopamine transporter uptake in basal ganglia as demonstrated by single-photon emission computed tomography or PET42. Moreover, neither the informant-based motor questionnaire nor the UPDRS-III score may be sufficiently sensitive to capture the earliest signs of motor dysfunction. Future studies may benefit from the inclusion of digital assessments (for example, wearable sensors) and utilization of a dual-task paradigm for detection of subtle motor impairments.
Aβ and tau positivity have previously been shown to be strongly linked to near-term cognitive progression in cognitively unimpaired individuals13. The present study shows that detection of LB pathology in clinically unimpaired individuals also has prognostic implications, both in terms of progression to clinical DLB or PD (Fig. 4) and cognitive decline (Fig. 3). Cognitive decline is also supported by retrospective analyses based on neuropathological findings, highlighting for example the rapid progression in attention/executive function of AD+/LB+ individuals versus AD or LB pathology only43,44, similar to the present study where we show the additive effect of having both pathologies (Fig. 3a–c). Overall, this indicates that it could be equally important to measure the presence of LB pathology as measuring Aβ and tau, perhaps in an A/T/LB framework. Using this updated biomarker framework could further increase the understanding of the heterogeneity in clinical progression observed in participants classified according to the traditional framework using Aβ and tau pathologies only45.
This study has some limitations. Although the population was large (n = 1,182), few individuals were AD+/LB+ (n = 20), resulting in low statistical power to detect group differences compared with this group. Therefore, a lack of statistically significant difference versus this group should be interpreted with caution. This does not, however, affect the results of the independent effects of the three pathologies (n = 94 LB+, n = 304 Aβ+ and n = 195 tau+; Table 1), which overall confirmed the group analyses (Figs. 2f–j and 3d–f). Further, this study had missing data. That is, analyses were performed on all eligible participants and not on a restricted sample with complete data for all cognitive and noncognitive measures. The rationale for this was to not introduce a selection bias, but this resulted in missing data (detailed in Supplementary Tables 2 and 3). According to the study design, assessments of motor and smell function were included in only parts of the BioFINDER study. Despite this, significant associations were found between LB pathology and smell function (Fig. 2 and Extended Data Fig. 1). To examine whether the lack of an association with motor function could be due to lower statistical power (type II error) we performed a power analysis showing that, with the current sample (n = 660 with UPDRS data), we were powered to detect even a small effect size (Extended Data Fig. 2). Regarding the associations with cognition, it should be noted that the underlying cognitive tests used to measure these cognitive domains do not capture all aspects of that domain. Significant associations are thus dependent on the type of underlying tests used to measure a specific cognitive domain. Due to the study designs, baseline Aβ- and tau-PET were available only in BioFINDER-2 and thus CSF P-tau217 and CSF Aβ42/40 were instead used in BioFINDER-1 as biomarkers of Aβ and tau. These CSF biomarkers have, however, shown very high agreement with their PET counterparts46,47.
In summary, this study shows that LB pathology measured using α-syn SAA is associated with early changes in cognitive performance and smell function in cognitively and neurologically unimpaired individuals, and indicates an increased risk of near-term progression to DLB or PD. Further, 13% of clinically unimpaired Aβ+ individuals are also LB+ and the coexistence of both LB and AD pathologies has additional detrimental effects on cognition compared with either pathology alone. Therefore, these findings may have implications for the design of preclinical AD drug trials, where one could consider excluding LB+ individuals or ascertaining that the distribution of LB positivity is equal between treatment and placebo arms. Finally, the results can inform about the design of future preclinical LBD trials evaluating drugs targeting, for example, misfolded α-syn.
Methods
Participants
All participants were part of either the BioFINDER-1 (NCT01208675; n = 754) or BioFINDER-2 (NCT03174938; n = 428) study described previously46,51,52. Only participants with a complete dataset of Aβ, tau and α-syn data and who were cognitively and neurologically unimpaired at baseline were included. The classification of cognitively unimpaired was defined according to the National Institute of Aging—Alzheimer’s Association criteria as not fulfilling the criteria for MCI or dementia12. Cognitively unimpaired participants consisted of cognitively healthy controls (n = 853) and participants with subjective cognitive decline (n = 329) who performed within normal ranges on a large cognitive test battery (that is, did not have MCI). The detailed study criteria have been published previously46,52,53. Briefly, the participants were aged 40–100 years, performed ≥24 points on the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) and spoke and understood Swedish to the extent that an interpreter was not necessary. None of the included participants fulfilled the clinical criteria for PD54 or prodromal DLB42 at baseline. All patients were enrolled and underwent baseline examination either from 2007 to 2015 (BioFINDER-1) or from 2017 to 2021 (BioFINDER-2). The exclusion criterion of MCI was defined as performing worse than −1.5 s.d. in at least one of the cognitive domains memory, attention/executive, verbal or visuospatial function. In BioFINDER-1 this was assessed by a senior neuropsychologist after a thorough neuropsychological battery, as described in detail previously55. In BioFINDER-2, MCI classification was operationalized as performing worse than −1.5 z-scores in any cognitive domain according to a regression-based norm accounting for age and education in Aβ-negative controls56 (see refs. 57,58 for a description on regression-based z-scores). Cognitive domain z-scores were derived by calculating the mean z-score of the tests in each of the following domains: attention/executive function (trail-making test A, trail-making test B and symbol digit modalities test (SDMT)), verbal ability (verbal fluency animals and the 15-word short version of the Boston naming test), memory (ten-word delayed recall from the Alzheimer’s disease assessment scale (ADAS)) and visuospatial (incomplete letters and cube analysis from the visual object and space perception battery) functions. Participants with subjective cognitive decline had subtle cognitive symptoms (perceived by participant or informant) but did not fulfill the criteria of MCI.
All participants provided written informed consent. Ethical approval was given by the Regional Ethical Committee in Lund, Sweden.
Clinical outcomes
All clinical outcomes were z-scored according to the distribution of Aβ– cognitively unimpaired participants in BioFINDER-1 and -2 except for the smell test, which was normalized using LB– cognitively unimpaired participants in BioFINDER-1 and -2. The modified preclinical Alzheimer cognitive composite-5 (mPACC5, also referred to as PACC) was used as a measure of global cognition, containing tests of memory, executive, attention and verbal function59. It was calculated based on the previously described PACC5 using MMSE, SDMT and animal fluency59. Because the memory tests logical memory and the free and cued selective reminding tests were not available in BioFINDER, the ten-word delayed recall task from ADAS–cognition (ADAScog)60 was used (weighted twice), as previously applied in several studies61,62. The mPACC5 was thus calculated using z-scores based on the distribution in Aβ– cognitively unimpaired in the following way: (MMSE + (ADAScog delayed recall × 2) + SDMT + animal fluency)/5.
Memory was measured using the ten-word delayed recall task from ADAScog60. Attention/executive function was measured using SDMT63 and, if that was not available, the serial 7s task of MMSE was used (see Supplementary Tables 2 and 3 for missingness)64.
Smell function was assessed in BioFINDER-1 using the brief smell identification test (Sensonics International)65 and in BioFINDER-2 using the very similar ODOFIN Burghart sniffin sticks (MediSense)66. To account for differences in maximum score (16 and 12, respectively), results were z-scored based on the distribution of LB– participants in each cohort, separately.
Motor function was measured using the total score from the motor section of the informant-based cognitive impairment questionnaire (CIMP-QUEST)67, which assesses for example bradykinesia, changed way of walking, poorer balance, clumsier hands, tremor, changed facial expressions and dysarthria. In a supplementary analysis (Extended Data Fig. 2), motor function was measured using UPDRS-III (ref. 68). UPDRS-III was performed and rated by three certified physiotherapists.
Biomarker of Aβ
Aβ positivity was determined as having abnormal [18 F]-flutemetamol PET using a predefined cutoff of 0.53 standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) measured in a neocortical composite region using pons as a reference region, as previously described46. Due to the study design, Aβ-PET was not included at baseline in BioFINDER-1 and, here, abnormality was defined using a predefined CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio cutoff of <0.066, as previously described53. Aβ42 and Aβ40 were analyzed on a Cobas E 601 analyzer using the Roche NeuroToolKit.
Both CSF and PET cutoff have previously been established using mixture modeling statistics69, and the two modalities have shown a very high concordance70.
Biomarker of tau
Tau positivity was defined as either abnormal CSF P-tau217 (BioFINDER-1) or abnormal tau-PET (BioFINDER-2). CSF P-tau217 was measured using the Meso Scale Discovery platform using an assay developed by Eli Lilly, and tau-PET was performed using RO948 labeled with radioactive fluorine [18 F] as previously described46. SUVR was measured in a temporal metaregion of interest (ROI) using the inferior cerebellar cortex as reference region46. Cutoffs were established at mean + 2 s.d. in Aβ-negative controls as previously described; the cutoff for CSF P-tau217 was >11.42 pg ml–1 and for tau-PET >1.32 SUVR (ref. 46).
Preparation of recombinant α-synuclein (LB pathology)
Purification of recombinant wild-type α-syn was performed as previously reported10, with minor modifications. Briefly, transformed Escherichia coli BL21 (DE3) bacteria (New England Biolabs) from a glycerol stock were streaked on a selective plate containing kanamycin (Kan+, 50 µg ml–1, Sigma) and incubated at 37 °C overnight. A single colony was selected and inoculated into 5 ml of Luria broth (LB, Sigma) with kanamycin and allowed to grow for 4–5 h at 37 °C with continuous agitation at 250 rpm. This starter culture was then added to 1 l of LB containing kanamycin and the overnight express autoinduction system (Merk-Millipore, no. 71300-4) in a fully baffled flask. Cells were grown in a shaking incubator at 37 °C, 200 rpm overnight. The following day the culture was split into four 250 ml flasks and centrifuged at 3,200g for 10 min at 4 °C. The pellet was gently resuspended in 25 ml of osmotic shock buffer containing 40% sucrose (Sigma), 2 mM EDTA (Sigma) and 30 mM Tris (Bio-Rad) at pH 7.2 using a serological pipette, and incubated for 10 min at room temperature under mild agitation on a rotator mixer. The solution was then centrifuged at 9,000g, 20 min at 20 s and 20 µl of saturated MgCl2 (Sigma) added. After 3 min incubation under mild rocking on ice the suspension was centrifuged at 9,000g for 30 min at 4 °C and the supernatant collected into a 100 ml glass beaker. pH was reduced to 3.5 by the addition of 400–600 µl HCl 1 M (PanReac AppliChem) and incubated under stirring for 10 min at room temperature. After a second centrifugation at 9,000g for 30 min at 4 °C, the supernatant was collected into a clean 100 ml glass beaker. pH was adjusted to 7.5 by the addition of 400–600 µl of NaOH 1 M (Sigma). The protein extract was filtered through a 0.22 µm filter (Merk-Millipore), loaded into a Ni–NTA column (Cytiva, no. 17525501) on an NGC chromatography system (Bio-Rad) and washed with 20 mM Tris pH 7.5 at room temperature. The column was further washed with 50 mM imidazole (Sigma) in Tris 20 mM pH 7.5, generating a peak that was not collected. A linear gradient up to 500 mM imidazole in 20 mM Tris pH 7.5 was performed, and the peak collected between 30 and 75% of imidazole buffer (150 and 375 mM, respectively). This peak was loaded onto a Q-HP anion exchange column (Cytiva, no. 17115401) and washed in Tris 20 mM pH 7.5, followed by another washing in 100 mM NaCl in Tris 20 mM pH 7.5. Again, a linear gradient up to 500 mM of NaCl in Tris 20 mM pH 7.5 was carried out to collect the peak between 300 and 350 mM NaCl. The fractions were pooled, filtered through a 0.22 µm filter and dialyzed against Milli-Q water overnight at 4 °C using a 3.5 kDa MWCO dialysis membrane (Thermo-Scientific). The following day, the protein was moved into fresh Milli-Q water and dialyzed for a further 4 h. Protein concentration was measured by spectrophotometry using a theoretical extinction coefficient at 280 nm of 0.36 (mg ml–1)–1 cm–1. Finally, the protein was lyophilized for 6 h and stored in aliquots at a final concentration of 1 mg ml–1 after resuspension into 500 µl of phosphate buffer (PB, 40 mM, pH 8.0, Sigma). Lyophilized aliquots were stored at −80 °C until usage.
α-Syn RT-QuIC analyses
α-Syn RT-QuIC analyses were performed blinded to clinical status and diagnosis of the participant and according to an established protocol9,10,71, with minor modifications. Briefly, six 0.8 mm silica beads (OPS Diagnostics) per well were preloaded into black, clear-bottom, 96-well plates (Nalgene Nunc International). CSF samples were thawed and vortexed 10 s before use. Fifteen microliters of CSF was added to 85 μl of a reaction mix composed of 40 mM PB pH 8.0, 170 mM NaCl, 10 mM thioflavin-T (Sigma), 0.0015% SDS (Bio-Rad) and 0.1 g l–1 filtered recombinant α-syn (100 kDa Amicon centrifugal filters, Merck Millipore). Plates were closed with a plate sealer film (Nalgene Nunc International) and incubated into a Fluostar Omega plate reader (BMG Labtech) at 42 °C with intermittent double-orbital shaking at 400 rpm for 1 min, followed by 1 min rest. Fluorescence was measured every 45 min with 450 nm excitation and 480 nm emission filters during the 30 h test run. Samples and controls were run in quadruplicate and considered positive after the first run when at least three out of four replicates reached a threshold arbitrarily set at 30% of the median of Imax values reached by positive control replicates. To keep the risk of false positive results to a minimum, we repeated three times the analysis of samples showing seeding activity in only one or two out of four replicates in the first run. We considered a positive result only when at least four of the 12 total replicates reached threshold. We used 30 different batches of α-syn recombinant protein throughout the study, each undergoing a quality control test before use. We ran at least one positive and one negative control on each plate. Positive controls were chosen from patients with probable or definite DLB or PD whose CSF samples yielded four out of four positive replicates during screening. In each validated experiment (plate) included in the final analysis, the positive control/s showed at least three out of four positive replicates.
Magnetic resonance imaging
Participants were examined using a Siemens 3 T Trio scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions) in BioFINDER-1 and a Siemens 3 T MAGNETOM Prisma scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions) in BioFINDER-2, as previously described21,46. T1 images underwent volumetric segmentation and parcellation using FreeSurfer (v.6.0, https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). The following regions were used in group comparisons (AD/LB) and independent effect of Aβ, tau and LB pathology: amygdala (left and right volumes); hippocampus (left and right volumes); medial temporal (left and right entorhinal, fusiform and parahippocampal cortical thicknesses); lateral temporal (left and right bank of superior temporal sulcus, inferior temporal, middle temporal, superior temporal, temporal pole and transverse temporal cortical thicknesses); medial parietal (paracentral, isthmus cingulate, posterior cingulate and precuneus cortical thicknesses); lateral parietal (postcentral, inferior parietal, superior parietal and supramarginal cortical thicknesses); occipital (cuneus, lateral occipital, lingual and pericalcarine cortical thicknesses); and frontal (remaining cortical FreeSurfer regions) areas. All but amygdala and hippocampus were surface weighted when constructing the composite region. Analyses were adjusted for age, sex and magnetic resonance camera (and also, in the case of hippocampus and amygdala volumes, total intracranial volume).
Moreover, all T1-weighted images were preprocessed using voxel-based morphometry running under the Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). First, images were segmented into gray matter, white matter and CSF. We then used the diffeomorphic nonlinear image registration tool46 to create a study-specific template based on the gray and white matter tissues of the whole sample. Once the template was created, gray matter tissues were warped into Montreal Neurological Institute space using individual flow fields resulting from registration, and voxel values were modulated for volumetric changes introduced by normalization. Finally the images were smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with 12 mm full-width at half-maximum. To account for differences in head size in statistical analyses, we calculated the total intracranial volumes of each subject as the sum of the gray matter, white matter and CSF volumes. In addition to analyses using the FreeSurfer-based ROIs described above, we also performed voxel-wise comparisons using analysis of variance on the smoothed gray matter images with group as a factor at four levels (AD–LB–, AD–LB+, AD+/LB–, AD+LB+) while controlling for age, sex, intracranial volume and magnetic resonance camera. All results were adjusted for multiple comparisons using a family-wise error rate correction set at P < 0.05.
Statistical analyses
In cross-sectional analyses, AD/LB group, age, sex (assigned, not self-reported) and, for cognitive test outcomes, years of education, were used as independent variables in general linear regression models. When UPDRS-III was used as outcome the models were also adjusted for UPDRS-III rater (three raters in total). Dependent variables were either cognitive, smell or motor function. Next, binarized Aβ, tau and LB pathologies (to facilitate easier comparison of estimates) were used instead of AD/LB group. All three pathologies were used in the same model together with age, sex and years of education (education was not included in models of motor function). In longitudinal analyses, LME models were used (R packages lme4 and lmerTest). Cognitive function was used as outcome, and significant results are presented for the interactions AD/LB group × time and pathology × time. Models also included age, sex, years of education and random slopes and intercepts. For models including pathology × time, the interaction between time and all covariates was also included. In a sensitivity analysis, the LME models using AD/LB group × time were also adjusted for baseline cognitive test result and had the change from baseline in cognitive test result as outcome (for comparisons where there was a baseline group difference in cognition). All available data were used in statistical analyses. Missing data and number of participants at each visit are described in Supplementary Tables 2 and 3. A two-sided P < 0.05 was considered to indicate statistical significance. Multiple comparison corrections were performed using the false discovery rate method at α = 0.05, applying correction per outcome (that is, six comparisons for AD/LB group comparisons and three for the independent effects of LB, Aβ and tau pathology). Power calculations for linear regression models were performed using the R package WebPower (80% power, α = 0.05). Statistical analyses were performed using R v.4.1.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Online content
Any methods, additional references, Nature Portfolio reporting summaries, source data, extended data, supplementary information, acknowledgements, peer review information; details of author contributions and competing interests; and statements of data and code availability are available at 10.1038/s41591-023-02450-0.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
The study was supported by the Alzheimer’s Association (no. SG-23-1061717), the Swedish Research Council (nos. 2022-00775, 2021-00905, 2018-02052 and 2017-01541), ERA PerMed (no. ERAPERMED2021-184), the Knut and Alice Wallenberg foundation (no. 2017-0383), the Strategic Research Area MultiPark (Multidisciplinary Research in Parkinson’s disease) at Lund University, the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation (nos. AF-981132, AF-980907 and AF-968598), the Swedish Brain Foundation (nos. FO2021-0293 and FO2022-0204), The Parkinson Foundation of Sweden (no. 1280/20), the Cure Alzheimer’s fund, the Konung Gustaf V:s and Drottning Victorias Frimurarestiftelse, the Skåne University Hospital Foundation (no. 2020-O000028), Regionalt Forskningsstöd (nos. 2022-1259 and 2022-1346) and the Swedish federal government under the ALF agreement (nos. 2022-Projekt0080, 2022-Projekt0085 and 2018-Projekt0279), Regione Emilia Romagna (no. FIN-RER 2020, C.U.P. E45J19000810002), the Carisbo Foundation (no. 2022.0093) and the Italian Ministry of Health (Ricerca Corrente, no. 2021-22). The precursor of 18F-flutemetamol was provided by GE Healthcare and the precursor of 18F-RO948 by Roche. Figure 1 was created with support from L. L. Raket.
Extended data
Author contributions
O.H. and P.P. conceived the project. S.P. and M.R. contributed equally as first authors. O.H. and P.P. supervised the project and contributed equally as senior authors. C.Q., M.R. and S.D. produced recombinant α-syn for the RT-QuIC assay and performed substrate quality control. C.Q., M.R., A.M. and S.D. executed RT-QuIC experiments. M.R. and S.B. quality checked data of the RT-QuIC assay. O.H. was principal investigator of the study and E.S. was responsible for its logistics. S.P., E.S., S.H., P.T., M.H.N. and O.H. were responsible for clinical assessment of study participants. S.J. performed AD biomarker measurements. S.P. performed statistical analyses. N.M-C. provided statistical support. S.P. wrote the initial draft of the paper with direct input from P.P. and O.H. All authors contributed substantially to the subsequent version of the manuscript and approved the final version for submission.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Medicine thanks T. Montine, C. Moussa, J. Levin and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editor: Jerome Staal, in collaboration with the Nature Medicine team.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Lund University.
Data availability
Anonymized data will be shared by request from a qualified academic investigator for the sole purpose of replicating procedures and results presented in the article and providing data transfer is in agreement with EU legislation on the general data protection regulation and decisions by the Ethical Review Board of Sweden and Region Skåne, which should be regulated in a material transfer agreement.
Code availability
No custom code or mathematical algorithm that was central to conclusions was used in this study.
Competing interests
None of the authors have any financial interest in the α-syn SAA test. S.P. has acquired research support (for the institution) from ki elements/Alzheimer Drug Discoveries Foundation. In the past 2 years he has received consultancy/speaker fees from Bioartic, Biogen, Eli Lilly and Roche. O.H. has acquired research support (for the institution) from ADx, AVID Radiopharmaceuticals, Biogen, Eli Lilly, Eisai, Fujirebio, GE Healthcare, Pfizer and Roche. In the past 2 years he has received consultancy/speaker fees from AC Immune, Amylyx, Alzpath, BioArctic, Biogen, Cerveau, Eisai, Eli Lilly, Fujirebio, Genentech, Novartis, Novo Nordisk, Roche and Siemens. The remaining authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Sebastian Palmqvist, Marcello Rossi, Piero Parchi, Oskar Hansson.
Contributor Information
Piero Parchi, Email: piero.parchi@unibo.it.
Oskar Hansson, Email: oskar.hansson@med.lu.se.
Extended data
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41591-023-02450-0.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41591-023-02450-0.
References
- 1.Taylor JP, et al. New evidence on the management of Lewy body dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19:157–169. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30153-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hansson O. Biomarkers for neurodegenerative diseases. Nat. Med. 2021;27:954–963. doi: 10.1038/s41591-021-01382-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Horvath J, Herrmann FR, Burkhard PR, Bouras C, Kovari E. Neuropathology of dementia in a large cohort of patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2013;19:864–868. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2013.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gibb WR, Lees AJ. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1988;51:745–752. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.51.6.745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Klos KJ, et al. Alpha-synuclein pathology in the spinal cords of neurologically asymptomatic aged individuals. Neurology. 2006;66:1100–1102. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000204179.88955.fa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Parkkinen L, Soininen H, Laakso M, Alafuzoff I. Alpha-synuclein pathology is highly dependent on the case selection. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 2001;27:314–325. doi: 10.1046/j.0305-1846.2001.00342.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bellomo G, et al. alpha-Synuclein seed amplification assays for diagnosing synucleinopathies: the way forward. Neurology. 2022;99:195–205. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000200878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hall S, et al. Performance of alphaSynuclein RT-QuIC in relation to neuropathological staging of Lewy body disease. Acta Neuropathol. Commun. 2022;10:90. doi: 10.1186/s40478-022-01388-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Groveman BR, et al. Rapid and ultra-sensitive quantitation of disease-associated alpha-synuclein seeds in brain and cerebrospinal fluid by alphaSyn RT-QuIC. Acta Neuropathol. Commun. 2018;6:7. doi: 10.1186/s40478-018-0508-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rossi M, et al. Ultrasensitive RT-QuIC assay with high sensitivity and specificity for Lewy body-associated synucleinopathies. Acta Neuropathol. 2020;140:49–62. doi: 10.1007/s00401-020-02160-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Siderowf A, et al. Assessment of heterogeneity among participants in the Parkinson’s Progression Markers Initiative cohort using alpha-synuclein seed amplification: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. 2023;22:407–417. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(23)00109-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jack CR, Jr., et al. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ossenkoppele R, et al. Amyloid and tau PET-positive cognitively unimpaired individuals are at high risk for future cognitive decline. Nat. Med. 2022;28:2381–2387. doi: 10.1038/s41591-022-02049-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Berron D, et al. Early stages of tau pathology and its associations with functional connectivity, atrophy and memory. Brain. 2021;144:2771–2783. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Tideman P, et al. Association of beta-amyloid accumulation with executive function in adults with unimpaired cognition. Neurology. 2022;98:e1525–e1533. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000013299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Vogel JW, et al. Four distinct trajectories of tau deposition identified in Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Med. 2021;27:871–881. doi: 10.1038/s41591-021-01309-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Economou A, Routsis C, Papageorgiou SG. Episodic memory in Alzheimer disease, frontotemporal dementia, and dementia with Lewy bodies/Parkinson disease dementia: disentangling retrieval from consolidation. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2016;30:47–52. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Adler CH, et al. Unified staging system for Lewy body disorders: clinicopathologic correlations and comparison to Braak staging. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 2019;78:891–899. doi: 10.1093/jnen/nlz080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Xu J, et al. Dopamine-dependent neurotoxicity of alpha-synuclein: a mechanism for selective neurodegeneration in Parkinson disease. Nat. Med. 2002;8:600–606. doi: 10.1038/nm0602-600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lee JY, et al. Dopamine facilitates associative memory encoding in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2021;598:321–326. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03948-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Palmqvist S, et al. Earliest accumulation of beta-amyloid occurs within the default-mode network and concurrently affects brain connectivity. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:1214. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01150-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hung A, Schneider M, Lopez MH, McClellan M. Preclinical Alzheimer disease drug development: early considerations based on Phase 3 clinical trials. J. Manag. Care Spec. Pharm. 2020;26:888–900. doi: 10.18553/jmcp.2020.26.7.888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mintun MA, et al. Donanemab in early Alzheimer’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 2021;384:1691–1704. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2100708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Swanson CJ, et al. A randomized, double-blind, phase 2b proof-of-concept clinical trial in early Alzheimer’s disease with lecanemab, an anti-Abeta protofibril antibody. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2021;13:80. doi: 10.1186/s13195-021-00813-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hamilton RL. Lewy bodies in Alzheimer’s disease: a neuropathological review of 145 cases using alpha-synuclein immunohistochemistry. Brain Pathol. 2000;10:378–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2000.tb00269.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Mikolaenko I, et al. Alpha-synuclein lesions in normal aging, Parkinson disease, and Alzheimer disease: evidence from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 2005;64:156–162. doi: 10.1093/jnen/64.2.156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bassil F, et al. Amyloid-beta (Abeta) plaques promote seeding and spreading of alpha-synuclein and tau in a mouse model of Lewy body disorders with Abeta pathology. Neuron. 2020;105:260–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Dickson DW, et al. APOE epsilon4 is associated with severity of Lewy body pathology independent of Alzheimer pathology. Neurology. 2018;91:e1182–e1195. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tsuang DW, et al. Genetic association between the APOE*4 allele and Lewy bodies in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2005;64:509–513. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000150892.81839.D1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lippa CF, Schmidt ML, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ. Antibodies to alpha-synuclein detect Lewy bodies in many Down’s syndrome brains with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 1999;45:353–357. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199903)45:3<353::aid-ana11>3.0.co;2-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lippa CF, et al. Lewy bodies contain altered alpha-synuclein in brains of many familial Alzheimer’s disease patients with mutations in presenilin and amyloid precursor protein genes. Am. J. Pathol. 1998;153:1365–1370. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9440(10)65722-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Upcott M, Chaprov KD, Buchman VL. Toward a disease-modifying therapy of alpha-synucleinopathies: new molecules and new approaches came into the limelight. Molecules. 2021;26:7351. doi: 10.3390/molecules26237351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Concha-Marambio L, et al. Accurate detection of alpha-synuclein seeds in cerebrospinal fluid from isolated rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder and patients with Parkinson’s disease in the DeNovo Parkinson (DeNoPa) cohort. Mov. Disord. 2023;38:567–578. doi: 10.1002/mds.29329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Russo MJ, et al. High diagnostic performance of independent alpha-synuclein seed amplification assays for detection of early Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. Commun. 2021;9:179. doi: 10.1186/s40478-021-01282-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kluge A, et al. Detection of neuron-derived pathological alpha-synuclein in blood. Brain. 2022;145:3058–3071. doi: 10.1093/brain/awac115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mammana A, et al. RT-QuIC detection of pathological alpha-synuclein in skin punches of patients with Lewy body disease. Mov. Disord. 2021;36:2173–2177. doi: 10.1002/mds.28651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wang Z, et al. Skin alpha-synuclein aggregation seeding activity as a novel biomarker for Parkinson disease. JAMA Neurol. 2020;78:1–11. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.3311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Adler CH, et al. Incidental Lewy body disease: clinical comparison to a control cohort. Mov. Disord. 2010;25:642–646. doi: 10.1002/mds.22971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Beach TG, et al. Severe hyposmia distinguishes neuropathologically confirmed dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer’s disease dementia. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0231720. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Silva MME, Mercer PBS, Witt MCZ, Pessoa RR. Olfactory dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease Systematic review and meta-analysis. Dement. Neuropsychol. 2018;12:123–132. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-020004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wong YC, Krainc D. Alpha-synuclein toxicity in neurodegeneration: mechanism and therapeutic strategies. Nat. Med. 2017;23:1–13. doi: 10.1038/nm.4269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.McKeith IG, et al. Research criteria for the diagnosis of prodromal dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology. 2020;94:743–755. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000009323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Brenowitz WD, et al. Mixed neuropathologies and associations with domain-specific cognitive decline. Neurology. 2017;89:1773–1781. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ryman SG, et al. Cognition at each stage of Lewy body disease with co-occurring Alzheimer’s disease pathology. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2021;80:1243–1256. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Dubois B, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations of the International Working Group. Lancet Neurol. 2021;20:484–496. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(21)00066-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Palmqvist S, et al. Discriminative accuracy of plasma phospho-tau217 for Alzheimer disease vs other neurodegenerative disorders. JAMA. 2020;324:772–781. doi: 10.1001/jama.2020.12134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Janelidze S, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid p-tau217 performs better than p-tau181 as a biomarker of Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Commun. 2020;11:1683. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-15436-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Postuma RB, et al. A single-question screen for rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder: a multicenter validation study. Mov. Disord. 2012;27:913–916. doi: 10.1002/mds.25037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gelb DJ, Oliver E, Gilman S. Diagnostic criteria for Parkinson disease. Arch. Neurol. 1999;56:33–39. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.McKeith IG, et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2005;65:1863–1872. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000187889.17253.b1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Palmqvist S, et al. Performance of fully automated plasma assays as screening tests for Alzheimer disease-related beta-amyloid status. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:1060–1069. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.1632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Palmqvist S, et al. Prediction of future Alzheimer’s disease dementia using plasma phospho-tau combined with other accessible measures. Nat. Med. 2021;27:1034–1042. doi: 10.1038/s41591-021-01348-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Palmqvist S, et al. An accurate fully automated panel of plasma biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;19:1204–1215. doi: 10.1002/alz.12751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Postuma RB, et al. MDS clinical diagnostic criteria for Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 2015;30:1591–1601. doi: 10.1002/mds.26424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Petrazzuoli F, et al. Brief cognitive tests used in primary care cannot accurately differentiate mild cognitive impairment from subjective cognitive decline. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2020;75:1191–1201. doi: 10.3233/JAD-191191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Borland E, Stomrud E, van Westen D, Hansson O, Palmqvist S. The age-related effect on cognitive performance in cognitively healthy elderly is mainly caused by underlying AD pathology or cerebrovascular lesions: implications for cutoffs regarding cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2020;12:30. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00592-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Borland E, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment: normative data from a large Swedish population-based cohort. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59:893–901. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Shirk SD, et al. A web-based normative calculator for the uniform data set (UDS) neuropsychological test battery. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2011;3:32. doi: 10.1186/alzrt94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Papp KV, Rentz DM, Orlovsky I, Sperling RA, Mormino EC. Optimizing the preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite with semantic processing: the PACC5. Alzheimers Dement. (N.Y.) 2017;3:668–677. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2017.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1984;141:1356–1364. doi: 10.1176/ajp.141.11.1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Insel PS, et al. Determining clinically meaningful decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2019;93:e322–e333. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Pichet Binette A, et al. Combining plasma phospho-tau and accessible measures to evaluate progression to Alzheimer’s dementia in mild cognitive impairment patients. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2022;14:46. doi: 10.1186/s13195-022-00990-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Smith, A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test (Western Psychological Services, 1991).
- 64.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Doty RL, Marcus A, Lee WW. Development of the 12-item Cross-Cultural Smell Identification Test (CC-SIT) Laryngoscope. 1996;106:353–356. doi: 10.1097/00005537-199603000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Cantone E, Maione N, Di Rubbo V, Esposito F, Iengo M. Olfactory performance after crenotherapy in chronic rhinosinusitis in the elderly. Laryngoscope. 2015;125:1529–1534. doi: 10.1002/lary.25173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Åstrand, R., Rolstad, S. & Wallin, A. Cognitive Impairment Questionnaire (CIMP-QUEST): reported topographic symptoms in MCI and dementia. Acta Neurol. Scand.121, 384–391 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 68.Fahn S. in Recent Developments in Parkinson’s Disease (eds Marsden, C. D. et al.) 153–163 (Macmillan Healthcare Information, 1987).
- 69.Benaglia T, Chauveau D, Hunter DR, Young D. S. mixtools: an R Package for analyzing finite mixture models. J. Stat. Softw. 2009;32:1–29. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Janelidze S, et al. Concordance between different amyloid immunoassays and visual amyloid positron emission tomographic assessment. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74:1492–1501. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.2814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Rossi M, et al. Diagnostic value of the CSF alpha-synuclein real-time quaking-induced conversion assay at the prodromal MCI stage of dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology. 2021;97:e930–e940. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Anonymized data will be shared by request from a qualified academic investigator for the sole purpose of replicating procedures and results presented in the article and providing data transfer is in agreement with EU legislation on the general data protection regulation and decisions by the Ethical Review Board of Sweden and Region Skåne, which should be regulated in a material transfer agreement.
No custom code or mathematical algorithm that was central to conclusions was used in this study.