Abstract
Mass murder, particularly mass shootings, constitutes a major, growing public health concern. Specific motivations for these acts are not well understood, often overattributed to severe mental illness. Identifying diverse factors motivating mass murders may facilitate prevention.
We examined 1,725 global mass murders from 1900–2019, publicly described in English in print or online. We empirically categorized each into one of ten categories reflecting reported primary motivating factors, which were analyzed across mass murderers generally, as well as between U.S- and non-U.S.-based mass-shooters.
Psychosis or disorganization related to mental illness were infrequently motivational factors (166; 9.6%), and were significantly more associated with mass murder committed using methods other than firearms. The vast majority (998, 57.86%) of incidents were impulsive and emotionally-driven, following adverse life circumstances. Most mass murderers prompted by emotional upset were found to be driven by despair or extreme sadness over life events (161, 16.13% within the category); romantic rejection or loss, or severe jealousy (204, 20.44% within the category); some specific non-romantic grudge (212, 21.24% within the category); or explosive, overwhelming rage following a dispute (266, 26.65% within the category).
Results suggest that policies seeking to prevent mass murder should focus on criminal history, as well as subacute emotional disturbances not associated with severe mental illness in individuals with poor coping skills who have recently experienced negative life events.
Keywords: violence, motivation, mass murder, mass shooting, psychosis
1. Introduction
Mass murders, which we have previously defined as three or more individuals killed in one event in a single setting, without a cooling-off period (Brucato et al., 2021), have garnered great public and academic attention. This has particularly been true for those involving firearms and especially in the United States, where mass shootings have proved increasingly prevalent and deadly in the current millennium (Follman et al., 2020; Peterson & Densley, 2020). With an eye to how such tragic events might be prevented, there has been much focus on the question of what motivates these attacks, often examining the role of mental illness (McGinty et al., 2014; Parks et al., 2019). Sorely-needed research has been limited, however, in two notable respects: first, the proportion of mass-shooting events associated with mental illness varies widely across studies, depending, in part, on how mental illness is defined. Remarkably, Parks and colleagues (2019) noted mental illness rates ranging from 4.7% to 78% across studies of mass-shooters. In a previous report from our group (Brucato et al., 2021), examining mass murderers who employed any means, we provided separate rates of psychotic symptoms (11%), depressive symptoms (10%), personality disorders (5%), neurological conditions (1%), post-traumatic stress symptoms (<1%), bipolar disorder or mania symptoms (<1%), low I.Q. (<1%), autism spectrum disorder symptoms (<1%), anxiety disorder (excluding post-traumatic stress disorder) symptoms (<1%), and other psychiatric symptoms (<1%) among perpetrators. Psychotic symptoms (i.e., delusions, hallucinations and/or disorganized thought), though uncommon, overall, were found to be even less prevalent to a significant extent among mass shooters than among mass murderers who did not employ firearms.
A second limitation involves the challenge of how to optimally categorize the motivating factors precipitating mass murders, moving beyond frank psychiatric illness. Such efforts aim to gain insight into the complex ways in which mental state, personality structure, personal morality, culture, sociocultural factors, and life circumstances intersect among such individuals at the times of their attacks.
While several attempts have been made to delineate between motivations for mass murder, few have been presented in the peer-reviewed literature. A typology developed by Dietz (1986) distinguished between family annihilators, who kill family members, generally in home settings; set-and-run killers, who employ means that allow distance or escape from the murder scene, such as explosives or poisons; and pseudocommandos, who target indiscriminate victims after long planning, driven by feelings of persecution, and who are fascinated with firearms and military techniques.
Petee, Padgett, and York (1997) collected data on 106 mass murders occurring from 1965–1995, involving three or more fatalities, from newspapers in 11 major American cities. Motivations for murder and target selection were analyzed, yielding eight subtypes: In the anger/revenge – specific person(s) target category, a killer seeks revenge against specific, familiar individuals, for some perceived wrong, typically avoiding uninvolved parties unless they interfere. Those of the anger/revenge – specific place target type select a specific location because it is symbolic of or associated with a grievance. In the anger/revenge – diffuse target category, mass murderers either express rage toward specific groups or categories of people, or target whomever is available in a given area. The domestic/romantic category involves either family members in conflict or under duress, generally in the home, but sometimes in public settings, or a mass murderer who retaliates following a romantic rejection in a non-cohabitation situation. Motives for direct interpersonal conflict mass murders do not stem from long-standing anger or conflict, but are more immediate in nature, involving a volatile person who erupts over a minor dispute. A felony-related mass murder is secondary to some other felonious act, typically theft, and victims are sometimes witnesses to the felony. Gang-motivated mass murder involves an offender in a dispute who has a gang affiliation and often kills alongside confederates. Political cases are generally associated with ideological or political terrorism. Finally, non-specific motive cases cannot be readily categorized, which, the authors suggest, may be due to a history of mental illness.
We sought to categorize our sample of 1,725 mass murders with the aim of better understanding the roles of severe mental illness, involving psychotic symptoms; criminal objectives; and other motivational factors (e.g., acute, severe emotional upset following adverse life events).
2. Methods
We examined 1,725 worldwide cases of mass murder, which occurred between 1900–2019, included in the Columbia Mass Murder Database (CMMD), based on extensive review of English-language databases of murderers and mass murderers publicly available in print and online. We used the Congressional definition of mass murder (i.e., three or more fatalities) (Douglas et al., 2012; Krouse & Richardson, 2015). We exclusively included personal-cause cases, excluding those associated with war, state- or group-sponsored terrorism, gang activity, or organized crime. We collected perpetrator names and incidents from academic, governmental/public (e.g. Everytown for Gun Safety), and lay/popular (e.g., Amok Fandom list) sources, though all information in our database was obtained from primary sources and reports. The methods involved in the creation of the database, including search terms, the databases and sources used, and how all data were coded, have previously been described in detail (Brucato et al., 2021). Note that the sample included 409 perpetrators who might be better categorized as spree killers, due to having killed two (three for CMMD analyses) or more people in more than one location or at more than one point in time, with no cooling-off period (which was defined as killing across one week or less) between murders. The database excluded 1,571 additional perpetrators who were considered for possible inclusion, but who committed two episodes of murder with any victim count more than seven days apart, irrespective of any connections between murders. Persons best termed serial killers, due to a cooling-off period between killings, which we defined as a week or more, would fall into this exclusionary category.
To identify motivating factors, the apparent motivations in the first 300 cases of mass murder prepared for the database were examined, based on two or more English-language media primary sources. The first step in this process utilized the motivating factors identified by two of the authors of the present article (G.B., M.H.S.) and their colleagues, in an unrelated study (Petreca, Burgess, Stone, & Brucato, 2020). Their categories included practical/secondary gain, excess emotionality, revenge, sadism, group-cause, sexual, and mental health-related/psychotic. These motivational categories served as a basic framework for the current study. We modified and developed the categories empirically as cases were reviewed, and clear, overarching patterns emerged. The resulting categories we employed, which have been fully described and distinguished, with examples, in the Supplement, were: (1) Self-Survival Objectives; (2) Criminal Objectives; (3) Personal Religious or Political Beliefs; (4) Psychosis or Other Severe Psychiatric Disturbance; (5) Severe Emotional Upset, divided into the nine subcategories of: despair or extreme sadness over life event(s); disgruntled employee; disgruntled student; family/marital/custody dispute; overwhelming and impulsive anger; revenge for bullying; romantic rejection or loss/severe jealousy; social/peer rejection; or a specific non-romantic grudge; (6) Psychopathic or Sadistic Traits; (7) Narcissistic Traits; (8) Schizoid-Autistic Detachment; (9) Disorganization-Motivated, subdivided into cases involving psychosis, intellectual or cognitive impairment, or severe psychiatric disturbance, and those associated with drug or alcohol intoxication; and (10) Unknown/No Determinable Motive.
Based on this typology, as the full list of incidents included in the CMMD was amassed, the first (G.B.) and senior (R.G.) authors, a clinical psychologist and psychiatrist, respectively, divided the cases and reviewed available English-language sources for information regarding motivating factors. Each rater then reviewed the other’s designations. Wherever there was disparity between the categories selected by the raters, cases were reviewed and discussed, until there was agreement regarding the most appropriate designations. Furthermore, for the following variables, a code of “Yes” was only applied in cases wherein at least one primary source confirmed the presence of the variable (i.e., all others would be coded “no”): completed suicide at the scene; perpetrator injured or killed at the scene; history of psychotic symptoms; history of non-psychotic psychiatric/neurologic symptoms; recreational drug use/alcohol misuse; and criminal history. See the Supplement for further details.
2.1. Analyses
We tallied descriptive statistics for all 1,725 offenders, including age, gender, and race. The frequencies of each motivational subtype were tabulated, as well as the various subcategories of emotionally-driven killings. We separately examined perpetrators who used firearms (i.e., mass-shooters) and those who did not (i.e., mass murderers of other types), and U.S.-based mass-shooters versus non-U.S.-based mass-shooters. Differences in demographics and clinical features between groups were examined using Pearson χ2 tests for categorical variables (e.g., history of psychotic symptoms, gender), and two-sample t-tests for continuous variables. Statistical significance was evaluated at an alpha level of 0.05; as this is an exploratory analysis, alpha level was not adjusted for multiple comparisons. As such, p-values should be interpreted with caution. Data analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4 (Cary, NC, 2013).
3. Results
The raters’ designations were in full accord for 1,699 (98.49%) of cases. The remaining 26 cases were assigned designations following review and consensus between the raters. Demographic features are as previously reported (Brucato et al., 2021, Table 1).
Table 1.
Variable | N | Frequency | % |
---|---|---|---|
Gender | 1725 | ||
Male | 1611 | 93.39 | |
Female | 106 | 6.14 | |
Transgender | 3 | 0.17 | |
Unknown | 5 | 0.29 | |
Race | 1725 | ||
Caucasian | 1038 | 60.17 | |
African American | 310 | 17.97 | |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 290 | 16.81 | |
Native American/First Nations | 11 | 0.64 | |
Mixed/More Than One Race | 24 | 1.39 | |
Other | 4 | 0.23 | |
Unknown | 48 | 2.78 | |
Ethnicity | 1725 | ||
Hispanic | 152 | 8.81 | |
Non-Hispanic | 1496 | 86.72 | |
Unknown | 77 | 4.46 | |
Age Category | 1609 | ||
9–19 | 158 | 9.82 | |
20–29 | 564 | 35.05 | |
30–39 | 466 | 28.96 | |
40–49 | 276 | 17.15 | |
50–59 | 108 | 6.71 | |
60–75 | 37 | 2.30 | |
Location | 1725 | ||
U.S. | 1061 | 61.51 | |
Canada | 39 | 2.26 | |
Mexico/Central America | 12 | 0.70 | |
Africa | 47 | 2.72 | |
Asia | 259 | 15.01 | |
Australia or New Zealand | 45 | 2.61 | |
Europe | 214 | 12.41 | |
South America | 29 | 1.68 | |
Mid-Travel between Regions | 19 | 1.10 | |
Fatalities | 1725 | ||
3 or 4 | 800 | 46.38 | |
5 or 6 | 458 | 26.55 | |
7–10 | 283 | 16.41 | |
11–25 | 121 | 7.01 | |
25–100 | 49 | 2.84 | |
Over 100 | 14 | 0.81 | |
Completed Suicide at Scene | 1725 | ||
No | 1256 | 72.81 | |
Yes | 469 | 27.19 | |
Perpetrator Injured or Killed at scene | 1725 | ||
No | 1555 | 90.14 | |
Non-Fatally Injured (by Others) | 49 | 2.84 | |
Accidentally Non-Fatally Injured (by Self) | 17 | 0.99 | |
Killed (by Others) | 104 | 6.03 | |
History of Psychotic Symptoms | 1725 | ||
No | 1496 | 86.72 | |
Yes | 229 | 13.28 | |
Non-Psychotic Psychiatric/Neurologic Symptoms | 1725 | ||
No | 1417 | 82.14 | |
Yes | 308 | 17.86 | |
Recreational Drug Use/Alcohol Misuse | 1725 | ||
No | 1405 | 81.45 | |
Yes | 320 | 18.55 | |
Criminal History | 1725 | ||
No | 1372 | 79.54 | |
Yes | 353 | 20.46 | |
Weapon | 1725 | ||
Non-Firearm | 540 | 31.30 | |
Firearm | 1185 | 68.70 | |
Categories of Motivating Factors | 1725 | ||
Self-Survival | 16 | 0.93 | |
Criminal Objectives | 324 | 18.78 | |
Religious or Political Beliefs | 101 | 5.86 | |
Psychosis/Other Severe Psychiatric Disturbance | 98 | 5.68 | |
Emotional Upset | 998 | 57.86 | |
Related to Psychopathic or Sadistic Traits | 46 | 2.67 | |
Related to Narcissistic Traits | 10 | 0.58 | |
Related to Schizoid/Autistic Detachment | 10 | 0.58 | |
Disorganization Related to Severe Psychiatric Disturbance | 68 | 3.94 | |
Disorganization Related to Drug or Alcohol Intoxication | 25 | 1.45 | |
No Motivation/Category Determined | 29 | 1.68 | |
Sub Types of “Emotional Upset” | 1725 | ||
Type 1: Despair or extreme sadness over life event(s) | 161 | 9.33 | |
Type 2: Disgruntled employee | 51 | 2.96 | |
Type 3: Disgruntled student | 8 | 0.46 | |
Type 4: Family/marital/custody dispute (non-impulsive in nature) | 57 | 3.30 | |
Type 5: Overwhelming, impulsive anger | 266 | 15.42 | |
Type 6: Revenge for bullying | 22 | 1.28 | |
Type 7: Romantic rejection or loss/severe jealousy | 204 | 11.83 | |
Type 8: Social/peer rejection (e.g., feeling isolated or unwanted, not necessarily involving bullying) | 17 | 0.99 | |
Type 9: Specific non-romantic grudge | 212 | 12.29 | |
Type 9: Specific grudge | 212 | 21.24 |
Raters were able to classify all but 29 (1.68%) mass murder incidents into one of the categories of motivating factors (see Table 1 and the Supplement for a full description of each category). Criminal objectives were identified in 324 (18.78%) events. Slightly more than half (998, 57.86%) of incidents were motivated by impulses related to severe affective upset, associated with sudden or pent-up emotionality related to adverse life events (i.e., “severe emotional upset”). Most mass murderers assigned to this category were found to be driven by despair or extreme sadness over life events (161, 9.33% of overall events, 16.13% within the category); romantic rejection or loss, or severe jealousy (204, 11.83% of overall events, 20.44% within the category); some specific non-romantic grudge (212, 12.29% of overall events, 21.24% within the category); or explosive, overwhelming rage following some dispute (266, 15.42% of overall events, 26.65 % within the category). Several motivational factors emerged as highly uncommon, including self-survival (16, 0.93%), religious or political beliefs (101, 5.86%), psychosis/other severe psychiatric disturbance (98, 5.68%), disorganization associated with severe psychiatric disturbance (68, 3.94%) or drug or alcohol intoxication (25, 1.45%), psychopathic or sadistic traits (46, 2.67%), narcissistic traits (10, 0.58%), and schizoid/autistic detachment (10, 0.58%).
When comparing categories of mass murders that involved firearms versus those that did not (Table 2), perpetrators were significantly more likely to use firearms when motivated by criminal objectives or extreme emotional upset (p=0.007, p=0.010, respectively). Mass murderers motivated by psychosis or other severe psychiatric disturbance (p=0.001), psychopathic or sadistic traits (p=0.014), and disorganization related to severe psychiatric disturbance (p<0.001) were significantly more likely to employ methods other than firearms. Additionally, mass murderers were less likely to use firearms when driven by severe emotional upset, associated with despair or extreme sadness over life events (p=0.003).
Table 2.
Non-Firearm (n=540) | Firearm (n=1185) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Motivating Factor | n | % | n | % | p* |
Self-Survival | 5 | 0.93 | 11 | 0.93 | 0.996 |
Criminal Objectives | 81 | 15.00 | 243 | 20.51 | 0.007 |
Religious or Political Beliefs | 30 | 5.56 | 71 | 5.99 | 0.721 |
Psychosis/Other Severe Psychiatric Disturbance | 45 | 8.33 | 53 | 4.47 | 0.001 |
Emotional Upset | 288 | 53.33 | 710 | 59.92 | 0.010 |
Sub Types of “Emotional Upset” | |||||
Type 1: Despair or extreme sadness over life event(s) | 62 | 21.53 | 99 | 13.94 | 0.003 |
Type 2: Disgruntled employee | 9 | 3.13 | 42 | 5.92 | 0.070 |
Type 3: Disgruntled student | 2 | 0.69 | 6 | 0.85 | 1.000 |
Type 4: Family/marital/custody dispute (non-impulsive in nature) | 15 | 5.21 | 42 | 5.92 | 0.663 |
Type 5: Overwhelming, impulsive anger | 75 | 26.04 | 191 | 26.90 | 0.781 |
Type 6: Revenge for bullying | 3 | 1.04 | 19 | 2.68 | 0.152 |
Type 7: Romantic rejection or loss/severe jealousy | 62 | 21.53 | 142 | 20.00 | 0.588 |
Type 8: Social/peer rejection (e.g., feeling isolated or unwanted, not necessarily involving bullying) | 3 | 1.04 | 14 | 1.97 | 0.422 |
Type 9: Specific non-romantic grudge | 57 | 19.79 | 155 | 21.83 | 0.475 |
Related to Psychopathic or Sadistic Traits | 22 | 4.07 | 24 | 2.03 | 0.014 |
Related to Narcissistic Traits | 4 | 0.74 | 6 | 0.51 | 0.515 |
Related to Schizoid/Autistic Detachment | 5 | 0.93 | 5 | 0.42 | 0.201 |
Disorganization Related to Severe Psychiatric Disturbance | 41 | 7.59 | 27 | 2.28 | <.001 |
Disorganization Related to Drug or Alcohol Intoxication | 10 | 1.85 | 15 | 1.27 | 0.345 |
Unknown/No Determinable Motive | 9 | 1.67 | 20 | 1.69 | 0.975 |
P-values are derived from chi-square tests comparing each row with the rest of the sample. For comparisons with any cell values<5, Fishers Exact Test is used.
We separately examined mass-shooters which were carried out within and without the U.S. (see Table 3). Criminal objectives were common among U.S.-based mass shooters (213, 25.51%) but not among those from other geographic areas (30, 8.57%, p<0.001). Conversely, religious or political beliefs were more commonly associated with non-U.S.-based mass shootings (p<0.001). Again, extreme emotionality in the face of adverse life events emerged as the most common motivation for mass shootings irrespective of geographic location (482, 57.72% U.S.-based; 228, 65.14% non-U.S.-based). In terms of specific emotional grievances, despair over life events (p=0.023) and family disputes (p<0.001) were more common in the U.S. than elsewhere, while revenge for bullying was more common outside of the U.S. (p<0.001), as was a specific non-romantic grudge (p=0.017). We again found the primary drivers to be despair or extreme sadness over life events (77, 15.98% U.S.-based; 22, 9.65% non-U.S.-based); romantic rejection or loss, or severe jealousy (102, 21.16%; 40, 17.54% non-U.S.-based); some specific non-romantic grudge (93, 19.29% U.S.-based; 62, 27.19% non-U.S.-based); or explosive, overwhelming rage following a dispute (122, 25.31% U.S.-based; 69, 30.26% non-U.S.-based).
Table 3.
U.S.-Based (n=835) | Non-U.S.-Based (n=350) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Motivating Factor | n | % | n | % | p |
Self-Survival | 7 | 0.84 | 4 | 1.14 | 0.740 |
Criminal Objectives | 213 | 25.51 | 30 | 8.57 | <.001 |
Religious or Political Beliefs | 32 | 3.83 | 39 | 11.14 | <.001 |
Psychosis/Other Severe Psychiatric Disturbance | 40 | 4.79 | 13 | 3.71 | 0.414 |
Emotional Upset | 482 | 57.72 | 228 | 65.14 | 0.017 |
Sub Types of “Emotional Upset” | |||||
Type 1: Despair or extreme sadness over life event(s) | 77 | 15.98 | 22 | 9.65 | 0.023 |
Type 2: Disgruntled employee | 32 | 6.64 | 10 | 4.39 | 0.235 |
Type 3: Disgruntled student | 4 | 0.83 | 2 | 0.88 | 1.000 |
Type 4: Family/marital/custody dispute (non-impulsive in nature) | 38 | 7.88 | 4 | 1.75 | <.001 |
Type 5: Overwhelming, impulsive anger | 122 | 25.31 | 69 | 30.26 | 0.165 |
Type 6: Revenge for bullying | 4 | 0.83 | 15 | 6.58 | <.001 |
Type 7: Romantic rejection or loss/severe jealousy | 102 | 21.16 | 40 | 17.54 | 0.260 |
Type 8: Social/peer rejection (e.g., feeling isolated or unwanted, not necessarily involving bullying) | 10 | 2.07 | 4 | 1.75 | 1.000 |
Type 9: Specific non-romantic grudge | 93 | 19.29 | 62 | 27.19 | 0.017 |
Related to Psychopathic or Sadistic Traits | 17 | 2.04 | 7 | 2.00 | 0.968 |
Related to Narcissistic Traits | 4 | 0.48 | 2 | 0.57 | 1.000 |
Related to Schizoid/Autistic Detachment | 3 | 0.36 | 2 | 0.57 | 0.635 |
Disorganization Related to Severe Psychiatric Disturbance | 16 | 1.92 | 11 | 3.14 | 0.197 |
Disorganization Related to Drug or Alcohol Intoxication | 8 | 0.96 | 7 | 2.00 | 0.143 |
Unknown/No Determinable Motive | 13 | 1.56 | 7 | 2.00 | 0.589 |
P-values are derived from chi-square tests comparing each row with the rest of the sample. For comparisons with any cell values<5, Fishers Exact Test is used.
Differences between motives involved in non-spree versus spree-type mass-shooters can be found in Supplementary Table 1.
4. Discussion
In the present study, we sought to examine motivating factors of mass murder with the goal of expanding on previously proposed systems in a number of critical ways. To increase the generalizability of findings, we examined a large dataset of 1,725 mass murder incidents and included worldwide cases spanning 1900–2019. We examined cases not only involving firearms, but also those carried out by any methods, permitting exploration of possibly distinct motivating factors between groups.
Motivational categories employed in the study were developed based upon previous work (Petreca, Burgess, Stone, & Brucato, 2020) in conjunction with an examination of a subsample of 300 mass murderers, allowing data to define subtypes, rather than envisioning motivating factors in advance and attempting to fit all mass murders into them. Emphasis was placed upon whether offenders were seeking to obtain some specific secondary gain, including criminal objectives, with forethought; acting out of impulsive, overwhelming emotion in the wake of challenging life events and circumstances; acting as a part of longstanding personality drivers that were independent of circumstances; or whether there was simply no logic to a killer’s actions, reflecting sheer disorganization. Finally, we distinguished between cases of serious mental illness, involving psychosis, in which the offender had a specific external aim which was idiosyncratically logical, but based upon a delusional premise, such as responding to what one believes is the voice of God, and cases in which there was only disorganized thought or behavior, sometimes associated with illicit substance use or alcohol misuse. The utility of the motivating factors we examined is supported by the excellent agreement between independent raters, who selected the same designations in 98.49% of cases and were able to arrive at consensus on the remaining 26 cases with discussion. Discordant designations almost uniformly arose in the context of cases in which more than one motivating factor seemed possible, and the system required selecting the one which predominated. Raters were able to identify motivations in all but 29 (1.68%) of mass murder incidents. Our hope is that the motivational categories we have presented will also allow for easy adaptation for future research, including further examinations of mass murder, but also studies of possible differences in motives between mass murderers and murderers who kill once or in a serial manner over time.
A lack of insight into the diverse situational and psychological factors which might precipitate mass shootings and other mass murder incidents have hampered efforts to develop policies and improved screening methods aimed at prevention. To date, efforts to understand specific drivers for these tragic events have involved establishing categories that might not encompass the full spectrum of motivating factors for perpetrating mass murder (Dietz, 1986; Arluke et al., 2018; Petee, Padgett and York, 1997; Rocque and Duwe, 2018).
Among the general public and in media reports following mass murder events, the killings are sometimes misrepresented as uniformly involving severe psychopathology, characterized by psychotic symptoms and/or disorganized in nature, or as being perpetrated by individuals taking prescribed psychotropic medications (Hall et al., 2019; Knoll IV & Dietz, 2023). Empirical evidence suggests that, in reality, while severe mental illness is somewhat overrepresented among those who perpetrate mass shootings—the most common type of mass murder—it plays a significant role in only a small fraction of cases. As noted in our introduction, the impact of mental illness reported in a given study of mass shooters is very much dependent upon how that term is defined (Parks et al., 2019). In a previous report from the CMMD (Brucato et al., 2021), we observed lifetime psychotic symptoms among 11% of mass murderers, consistent with previous reports, including 18% who did not use firearms and 8% who did. Findings suggested that mental illness may, in many cases, be incidental to the motivations for the murders. In addition to an overemphasis on severe psychopathology among those who carry out such acts, the specific triggers that precipitate mass murder have been stereotyped in the public eye. It is commonly believed, for instance, that mass murders are often triggered by bullying; work- or school-related grievances; a desire for attention; or in the context of poor social connections.
Our findings regarding motivating factors were highly consistent in terms of frequencies, irrespective of whether we examined mass murderers of all types, mass-shooters versus those who employed other methods, or mass-shooters based within versus outside of the U.S., and were generally consistent with the literature (Petee, Padgett & York, 1997; Dietz, 1986). In particular, criminal objectives, such as theft, killing individuals who might have served as witnesses to a crime or eliminating persons who impede some desired end, such as obtaining an inheritance, are common, especially in the U.S., as also reported by Petee, Padgett and York (1997). Most mass murder incidents, however, are impulsive in nature— a point that has implications for those seeking to prevent future mass murder by screening for persons potentially planning such attacks (Petee, Padgett & York, 1997). These attacks are largely associated with either sudden emotionality or some “last straw” event that precipitates expression of long pent-up affect, occurring in the wake of difficult life events or circumstances.
We found the most common triggers for such emotional reactions to be despair or extreme sadness over life events; romantic rejection or loss, or severe jealousy; some specific grudge; or explosive, overwhelming rage following a dispute. Inconsistent with popular stereotypes of mass murder triggers, we found mass murder driven by disorganized psychotic illness, attention-seeking, and upset specifically associated with workplace or school grievances to be uncommon. Our distinction between psychotic symptoms related to the commands of voices or firmly-held delusional beliefs versus disorganized psychosis helps to clarify that, when psychotic illness is a driver for mass murder, we would not expect such an individual to be openly bizarre or obviously impaired. Such an individual might be expected to be organized enough to conceal such delusional beliefs, for instance, during a psychiatric evaluation or when attempting to acquire a firearm, and to recognize the impact and meaning of murderous actions.
We fully recognize that one of the chief limitations of this study, and such work in general, is the uncertain validity of motivating factors drawn from media reports and court/police records – an unavoidable limitation of this type of work. We further acknowledge an underrepresentation of mass murders in non-English-speaking countries, which may have resulted in systematic overrepresentation of primarily English-speaking countries (see, e.g., the Early Warning Project’s analysis of statistical risk for mass killings ranked by country).
The fact that the CMMD dataset spans more than a century introduces further potential limitations, such as possible changes in the prevalence of certain motivating factors or the attention given to some motivating factors over others in media reports. Additionally, we conducted multiple statistical tests to compare motivations between perpetrators who used firearms and those who did not, as well as those perpetrated within the U.S. and outside the U.S. Because of the exploratory nature of these analyses, we elected to report all results without correcting for multiple comparisons. We therefore recommend further investigation of observed associations.
Despite these potential limitations, the motivating factors described herein were empirically developed and categorized based on a dataset of 1,725 mass murders. There is presently no universally-accepted system for classifying motivating factors of mass murder. The key implication arising from our findings is that, rather than focusing on the role of mental illness in mass shootings, emphasis should be placed on criminal history and objectives, as well as a lack of emotional coping skills for dealing with life’s challenges, associated with psychological challenges, but not severe mental illness. This information can be considered by law enforcement and policymakers as they seek to develop and improve measures aimed at preventing mass murders, such as those involving availability of firearms.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This work would not have been possible without the support and diligence of all contributing authors.
Sources of Funding
The authors were supported by R01 MH113861 (RRG); New York State Office of Mental Hygiene; and the Elizabeth K. Dollard Charitable Trust.
Disclosure Statement
R. Girgis acknowledges research support from Otsuka, Allergan/Forest, BioAvantex and Genentech as well as royalties and/or advances from books on mental health published by Wipf and Stock and Routledge/Taylor and Francis. He has consulted for Noble Insights and consults for law firms. Dr. Brucato receives royalties and/or advances from the books The New Evil: Understanding the Emergence of Modern Violent Crime from Rowman & Littlefield and Understanding and Caring for People with Schizophrenia: Fifteen Clinical Cases from Routledge/Taylor and Francis, as well as from a Boston College Connell School of Nursing Innovation Grant. No other authors have disclosures.
The sources of funding had no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication. The corresponding author had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication. We were not paid to write this article by a pharmaceutical company or other agency.
References
- Arluke A, Lankford A, Madfis E (2018). Harming animals and massacring humans: Characteristics of public mass and active shooters who abused animals. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 36: 739–751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brucato G, Appelbaum PS, Hesson H, Shea EA, Dishy G, Lee K, Pia T, Syed F, Villalobos A, Wall MM, Lieberman JA, Girgis RR (2021). Psychotic symptoms in mass shootings vs. mass murders not involving firearms: findings from the Columbia Mass Murder Database. Psychological Medicine, 17:1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dietz PE (1986). Mass, serial, and sensational homicides. Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine, 62:477–491. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglas JE, Burgess AW, Burgess AG, & Ressler RK (2012). Crime classification manual: Astandard system for investigating and classifying violent crime (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Early Warning Project. (2023). “Ranking All Countries: 2022–2023 Statistical Risk Assessment for Mass Killing.” Retrieved from https://earlywarningproject.ushmm.org/ranking-of-all-countries.
- Everytown for Gun Safety. (2020). “Mass Shootings in America: 2009–2020.” Retrieved from https://everytown.org
- Follman M, Aronsen G & Pan D U.S. Mass Shootings, 1982–2020: Data from Mother Jones’ Investigation. https://www.motherjones.com/politics/2012/12/mass-shootings-mother-jones-full-data/ [updated January 26, 2020].
- Hall RCW, Friedman SH, Sorrentino R, Lapchenko M, Marcus A, & Ellis R (2019). The myth of school shooters and psychotropic medications. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 37(5):540–558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knoll J IV & Dietz P (2023). Mass violence and the complex spectrum of mental illness and mental functioning. JAMA Psychiatry, 80(2):186–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krouse WJ, & Richardson DJ (2015). Mass murder with firearms: Incidentsand victims, 1999–2013 (Vol. 2020). Washington, DC: Congressional Research Service. [Google Scholar]
- McGinty EE, Webster DW, & Barry CL (2014). Gun policy and serious mental illness: priorities for future research and policy. Psychiatric Services, 65:50–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parks J, Bechtold D, Shelp F, Lieberman J & Coffey S (2019). Mass Violence in America: Causes, Impacts and Solutions. https://www.thenationalcouncil.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/08/Mass-Violence-in-America_8-6-19.pdf?daf=375ateTbd56. National Council for Behavioral Health.
- Petee TA, Padgett KG & York TS (1997). Debunking the stereotype: An examination of mass murder in public places. Homicide Studies, 1:317–337. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson J & Densley J (2020). The Violence Project Database of Mass Shootings in the United States, 1966–2020. https://www.theviolenceproject.org/.
- Petreca VG, Burgess AW, Stone MH, Brucato G (2020). Dismemberment and mutilation: a data-driven exploration of patterns, motives, and styles. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 65(3):888–896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocque M and Duwe G (2018). Rampage shootings: an historical, empirical, and theoretical overview. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19:28–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.