Skip to main content
Schizophrenia Bulletin Open logoLink to Schizophrenia Bulletin Open
. 2023 Aug 10;4(1):sgad021. doi: 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgad021

Using Smartphones to Identify Momentary Characteristics of Persecutory Ideation Associated With Functional Disability

Benjamin Buck 1,, Mary Wingerson 2, Justin S Tauscher 3, Matthew Enkema 4, Weichen Wang 5, Andrew T Campbell 6, Dror Ben-Zeev 7
PMCID: PMC10439515  PMID: 37601285

Abstract

Objectives

Though often a feature of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, persecutory ideation (PI) is also common in other psychiatric disorders as well as among individuals who are otherwise healthy. Emerging technologies allow for a more thorough understanding of the momentary phenomenological characteristics that determine whether PI leads to significant distress and dysfunction. This study aims to identify the momentary phenomenological features of PI associated with distress, dysfunction, and need for clinical care.

Methods

A total of 231 individuals with at least moderate PI from 43 US states participated in a study involving 30 days of data collection using a smartphone data collection system combining ecological momentary assessment and passive sensors, wherein they reported on occurrence of PI as well as related appraisals, responses, and cooccurring states. Most (N = 120, 51.9%) participants reported never having received treatment for their PI, while 50 participants had received inpatient treatment (21.6%), and 60 (26.4%) had received outpatient care only.

Results

Individuals with greater functional disability did not differ in PI frequency but were more likely at the moment to describe threats as important to them, to ruminate about those threats, to experience distress related to them, and to change their behavior in response. Groups based on treatment-seeking patterns largely did not differ in baseline measures or momentary phenomenology of PI as assessed by self-report or passive sensors.

Conclusions

Smartphone data collection allows for granular assessment of PI-related phenomena. Functional disability is associated with differences in appraisals of and responses to PI at the moment.

Keywords: schizophrenia, mobile health (mHealth), sensing, ecological momentary assessment, mobile phones, psychosis

Introduction

Persecutory ideation (PI)—persistent dysfunctional thought related to threats of intentional harm—is reported in up to 15% of the population.1 While often associated with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, PI is common in a range of presentations, including mood,2,3 anxiety,4 personality,5 and neurodegenerative6 disorders, as well as among individuals who are otherwise considered healthy. A growing body of evidence supports a continuum of PI,7 ranging from more normative thoughts about potential danger to persistent, firmly held, disruptive beliefs based on little evidence8 associated with impairments in work and social functioning.9,10 Given the heterogeneous outcomes among individuals who experience PI, a growing emphasis of research has been to identify characteristics that distinguish more common PI from its more disruptive or disabling forms, including persecutory delusions.

Recent developments—for example, the NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC11) initiative and Hierarchical Taxonomy of Pathology (HiTOP12) model—have increased attention to dimensional factors underlying symptoms as opposed to categorical diagnoses. These approaches aim to identify psychological or biological dimensions or constructs that vary in the general population and are linked with functional outcomes, and use assessment of these dimensions to tailor interventions more precisely. Central to a symptom-specific approach is identifying the factors that contribute to symptom’s emergence, maintenance, and downstream negative impacts. Indeed, a robust body of literature has identified several such factors. Individuals with more severe PI are more likely to report high levels of worry or rumination,13 negative beliefs about the self,14 poor sleep,15 and safety-seeking behaviors.16,17 These findings have helped contribute to the development of interventions that appear more effective than more generic or diagnosis-specific therapies.18,19

While extant work has identified traits of individuals who experience more severe PI, questions remain about the real-time, real-place responses to PI that determine the extent to which it causes dysfunction and disability. Most studies have assessed PI using retrospective measures, including self-report scales, surveys, or clinical interviews.20 These approaches require respondents to summarize, estimate, or aggregate their experiences over long periods of time, away from the environmental influences that lead to their emergence.21,22 In addition to being susceptible to memory inaccuracies, demand characteristics, and errors emerging from poor insight,23–25 these tools are best suited to identify broader trends and do not assess momentary phenomenology. Identifying the momentary characteristics associated with dysfunction in PI can provide targets for emerging approaches that provide interventions directly to individuals at the moments they need them, for example, digital or just-in-time adaptive interventions. Such interventions are acceptable to individuals with serious mental illnesses26 and achieve similar effectiveness to clinic-based care.27

Digital technologies provide new tools to address each of these limitations. A number of technologies—pagers (with accompanying journals28–31), palm pilots or PDAs,32 and more recently mobile phones33,34 have all been used to assess PI phenomenology with ecological momentary assessment (EMA) or passive mobile sensors—eg, light, sound, accelerometer, and GPS sensors— built into standard smartphones. These studies reduce reliance on retrospective recall, or—in the case of passive sensors—on self-report altogether. Studies using digital technologies have suggested several factors associated with dysfunction: cognitive appraisals (e.g. conviction around certain persecutory beliefs32), affective experiences (eg, increased fear, depression or defeat in response to PI35,36), and behavioral responses (eg, staying home and avoiding contact with others to seek safety37). Few studies using EMA have included individuals along the full PI continuum or examined relationships of momentary PI phenomenology to psychiatric disability. None to the best of our knowledge have done so with passive behavioral sensing strategies. Taken together, these technologies provide remarkable opportunities to identify momentary factors that contribute to and maintain the distress and dysfunction associated with PI.

Our team previously used a multimodal smartphone data collection system to better understand the real-time, real-place characteristics associated with dysfunction resulting from another cross-diagnostic symptom indicative of psychosis risk: Auditory verbal hallucinations, or voices.38 To identify cross-diagnostic factors associated with distress and dysfunction, we compared real-time experiences across 3 groups representing clinical severity: (1) those who had received inpatient services for their voices, (2) those who had received outpatient services for their voices only, and (3) those who had never received care. We found that individuals with a help-seeking history described their voices at the moment as louder and more powerful than those who had never received care. They also spent significantly more time at home and less near other people. These findings provided support for a dimensional approach to understanding PI, and provided evidence of real-time affective and behavioral differences predicted from existing models of AVH.

This study builds on that initial work by deploying EMA and passive sensors in an exploratory approach to identifying the cross-diagnostic cognitive, affective, and behavioral characteristics of PI associated with functional disability. We took an overall exploratory approach. First, we aimed to characterize overall frequency of various aspects of momentary PI-related phenomenology – including cognitive appraisals, affective experiences, and behavioral responses. Second, we examined the relationship of these factors to levels of clinical severity as defined by levels of treatment received. Consistent with our team’s previous work focused on AVH, we expected that individuals with PI that had received treatment for it would on average present with a profile consistent with greater clinical severity, including reporting more problematic appraisals of PI, more frequent impacts of PI on one’s functioning, greater levels of general distress, and behavior patterns consistent with increased social withdrawal (eg, less time spent around others, less phone activity). Third, we sought to examine whether individuals with greater functional disability presented with this same general profile. These analyses aim to extend the literature on PI by providing more granular information on the phenomenology of and responses to PI that determine whether it causes distress and dysfunction. Identifying these attributes can help symptom-specific intervention approaches to assessment and intervention.

Methods

Participants

Two hundred and thirty-one individuals with PI completed data collection. Inclusion criteria included being (1) at or over the age of 18 years; (2) an English speaker; (3) an Android smartphone user (to enable deployment of the study’s specialty software) with an active data plan, and (4) providing responses consistent with moderate PI (ie, ≥ 11) on the ideas of persecution subscale of the Revised Green Paranoid Thoughts Scale.39 The R-GPTS is 10-item scale of persecutory ideation derived from the full-length Green Paranoid Thoughts Scale; previous psychometric work validating this instrument provided guidance for this clinical cutoff for inclusion in the study. Exclusion criteria included (1) living outside of the United States, (2) lack of availability for the 30-day timeframe of data collection, or (3) previous study participation.

Procedures

Study procedures were based on our team’s previous study studying the antecedents and consequents of auditory verbal hallucinations (or “voices”), described elsewhere.38,40 All participants were recruited remotely using Google Ads. These ads were designed to appear in prospective participants’ online search results when they used terms were consistent with medical (eg, psychiatric help, bipolar disorder), or non-medical descriptions of illness (eg, spied on, conspiracy) terms. Ads also appeared for users whose search terms matched one of the additional keywords generated automatically by Google Ads “broad match” algorithm. Participants who clicked on a Google Ad were taken to the study website, which provided detailed information about the study, including a video, text, and infographic describing details of study procedures and the smartphone data collection system. Interested participants followed a link to questions verifying their phone and email address, the study consent form, as well as a screening questionnaire (including the R-GPTS) to confirm their understanding of study details. Once participants completed identity verification and the consent questionnaires, they could provide informed consent, complete all baseline study questionnaires, and download the study mobile app. Participants were asked to keep their smartphones with them for the 30-day data collection period and respond to prompts to complete brief questionnaires. While engaging in data collection, participants could reach out to a research coordinator for technical support or troubleshooting by phone or email. A member of the research team would reach out to participants to address technological issues when participants’ devices did not provide information for 3 days. When participants completed data collection, the application no longer collected information from their device, they were instructed to uninstall the app, and they were provided $125 as compensation for participating ($75 for participation, and $50 to defray any additional data processing or transmission costs).

Baseline Measures

Self-Report

Functional disability was assessed with the Sheehan Disability Scale (SDS41), a brief self-report measure of symptom-related impairments in work/school activities, family relationships, and social functioning. Depressive and anxiety symptoms were assessed with the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-942) and Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire (GAD-743), respectively, 2 commonly used questionnaires each summed for an overall score. Insomnia was assessed with the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI44), a 7-item assessment of participants’ satisfaction with their sleep and degree of difficulty with sleep patterns. Three subscales of the endorsed and anticipated stigma inventory—including the Beliefs about Mental Illness, Beliefs about Mental Health Treatment, and Beliefs About Help-Seeking subscales—were administered to assess participants’ stigmatizing attitudes related to mental health. Each subscale comprises 8 items, rated on a 5-point Likert scale, and summed to generate a total.

Participants also provided data on their current and lifetime experiences with treatment across a range of formal and informal care settings (eg, outpatient care, partial program, and online interventions). Based on their responses, participants were grouped based on treatment intensity in one of 3 groups: (1) inpatient hospitalization (N = 50, 21.6%), (2) outpatient treatment (N = 61, 26.4%), and (3) no-treatment history (N = 120, 51.9%).

Mobile data collection

Ecological Momentary Assessment

Data were collected via a smartphone application deployed in our team’s previous studies.36–38 Participants agreed to carry their smartphone device with them and complete brief questionnaires—ie, EMAs—in response to prompts sent 4 times semi-randomly between the hours of 9 AM and 9 PM. Participants could also self-initiate an EMA entry; however, given the potential for reporting biases (eg, self-initiating data collection because one is experiencing an increase in PI), we only assessed prompted responses in this study. Questionnaires had 12 items, all assessed on a 4-point Likert scale (0 = not at all, 3 = very much). The first items pertained to PI occurrence, or whether participants were currently experiencing thoughts related to others wanting to harm them (“Are you currently feeling that someone or something wants to harm you?”). Participants who endorsed this item with any response greater than 0 (“not at all”) were provided 3 items related to cognitive appraisals or reactions and 2 items related to affective and behavioral impact. Cognitive response items included (1) conviction (“How certain are you that this is true?”), (2) importance (“How much does the threat matter to you?”) and (3) rumination (“How much are you thinking about the threat?”). Impact items focused on (1) distress (i.e. “How much is this distressing you?”) and (2) behavior change (ie, “How much are you changing your behavior right now because of the threat?”). At every EMA (ie, regardless of whether they endorsed PI), participants also completed items related to (1) anxiety (“How anxious do you feel right now?), (2) self-esteem (“How self-confident do you feel right now?”), (3) sadness (“How sad do you feel right now?”), (4) sociality (“How much do you want to be around people right now?”), (5) energy (“How well-rested do you feel right now?”), and (6) presence of others (“Are you alone?”; rated dichotomously as “yes” or “no”).

Passive Sensing

The study application simultaneously collected data passively by recording data from existing sensors on the device, including accelerometers, light and audio sensors, GPS, and logs of call and SMS activity. Detailed information related to development and optimization of the smartphone data collection system can be found in previous published work.45–47 In the present analysis, we examined 5 variables, including (1) time spent in primary one’s primary location (ie, time spent at home), (2) distance traveled, (3) time spent around speech, (4) time sitting still (ie, inactivity), (5) SMS messages sent and received, and (5) outgoing and incoming phone call duration. To derive geospatial activity data, the study application recorded estimated locations every 10 minutes through GPS, WiFi, and cellular tower location services. Time spent around speech was derived through samples of ambient sound collected every 3 minutes and run through a speech detection algorithm. To protect participant privacy, the app collected no raw audio, but simply categorized whether the algorithm detected speech. Time sitting still was recorded using Google Activity Recognition API. This system uses an algorithm to approximate physical activity based on physical movement of the device.

Data Analytic Plan

First, we aimed to characterize the sample according to their momentary experiences of PI. We qualitatively examined the frequencies of responses to each of the EMA items, as well as mean scores on baseline questionnaires. Second, we examined whether treatment intensity groups differed on baseline, EMA, and passively sensed variables. Following our group’s previous approach to examining auditory verbal hallucinations,38 we created contrast variables for each participant to isolate 2 comparisons. These contrast variables compared (1) the inpatient group to the no-treatment group, as well as the (2) outpatient group to the no-treatment group. For baseline variables, linear regression models were conducted using a single contrast variable as a predictor. For longitudinal EMA variables, mixed models were used. In these models, observations were nested within individuals, and intercepts were allowed to vary. The group contrast variables were entered as fixed effects, and appropriate distributions were selected based on dispersion of responses. Finally, to examine the relationship between momentary PI characteristics and functional disability, we conducted mixed-effects models using participants’ total scores on the SDS provided at baseline as predictors. These models—which also allowed intercepts to vary at random—examined the extent to which levels of psychiatric disability reported at baseline were associated with differences in momentary PI-related cognitive responses or distress. For analyses examining passively sensed variables, analyses are based on measures aggregated by the day of data collection. These analyses were also conducted using generalized mixed-effects models, with appropriate distribution family and link function chosen based on each variable’s characteristics. For variables with high numbers of zeroes (eg, SMS messages and phone calls), we conducted 2 models, one predicting the occurrence of an SMS or phone call that day (ie, binary model) and another predicting the count or scale values on days when each occurred (ie, continuous model).

Results

After removing duplicate and test entries, N = 1701 participants verified their phone number on the study landing page. Of those, N = 522 (30.7%) did not continue through the informed consent pipeline, and N = 600 (35.3%) were screened for ineligibility. Of the remaining participants who were sent links to download the study app (N = 579, 34.0%), 181 did not install the app (31.3%), 41 dropped from the study or were lost to follow-up (28.2%), and 4 (0.7%) were excluded for being discovered to be ineligible after completing automated screening procedures. This left N = 231 study completers.

Participants’ average age was 38.08, (SD = 11.60), and most participants identified as white (N = 163, 70.9%), non-Hispanic (N = 213, 92.2%), and cisgender women (N = 164, 71.0%). Participants resided in 43 US states and the District of Columbia. Full participant demographics can be found in table 1. With regard to EMA engagement, 2 participants who began the study period did not provide any prompted EMA entries (these participants provided self-initiated entries, but these are excluded in the present analysis). Of those that provided responses to prompts, participants typically provided responses to about half of approximately 120 prompt opportunities (M = 63.53, SD = 32.20).

Table 1.

Sample Descriptive Statistics

No Lifetime Treatment
(n = 120)
Outpatient Treatment
(n = 61)
Inpatient Treatment
(n = 50)
M or N (SD or %) M or N (SD or %) M or N (SD or %) F or χ2 P
Age 38.48 12.40 37.51 11.04 37.80 10.39 0.16 .85
Race 10.85 .37
White/ Caucasian 80 66.7% 41 68.3% 42 84.0%
Black/ African American 25 20.8% 10 16.7% 4 8.0%
Pacific Islander 0 0.0% 1 1.7% 0 0.0%
American Indian/ Alaskan Native 2 1.7% 0 0.0% 0 0.0%
Asian 3 2.5% 1 1.7% 1 2.0%
More than one race 10 8.3% 7 11.7% 3 6.0%
Ethnicity 0.57 .75
Non-Hispanic/ Non-Latino 112 93.3% 55 90.2% 46 92.0%
Hispanic/ Latino 8 6.7% 6 9.8% 4 8.0%
Gender 5.62 .69
Female 87 72.5% 44 72.1% 33 66.0%
Male 27 22.5% 14 23.0% 15 30.0%
Transgender man 3 2.5% 0 0.0% 0 0.0%
Transgender woman 1 0.8% 1 1.6% 0 0.0%
Other/ Non-binary 2 1.7% 2 3.3% 2 4.0%
Diagnoses (self-report)^
Alzheimer’s or Parkinson’s 1 0.8% 0 0.0% 0 0.0% 0.95 .62
Bipolar disorder 39 33.1% 31 50.8% 26 52.0% 7.88 .02*
Depressive disorder 69 58.5% 45 73.8% 40 80.0% 8.99 .01*
Borderline PD 19 16.1% 8 13.1% 11 22.0% 1.61 .45
Paranoid PD 4 3.4% 7 11.5% 7 14.0% 6.96 .03*
Schizoaffective Disorder 7 5.8% 10 16.4% 18 36.0% 24.60 <.001***
Schizophrenia 11 9.3% 11 18.0% 10 20.0% 4.47 .11
Post-traumatic stress disorder 48 40.7% 34 55.7% 36 72.0% 14.38 <.001***
Substance use disorder 24 20.3% 18 29.5% 18 36.0% 4.93 .09^
Schizotypal PD 2 1.7% 0 0.0% 2 4.0% 2.56 2.77
Anxiety disorder 57 48.3% 48 78.7% 44 88.0% 31.13 <.001***
None of the above 26 22.0% 0 0.0% 1 2.0% 24.67 <.001***
Living status 8.50 .20
Independent 63 52.5% 35 57.4% 25 50.0%
Living with family 44 36.7% 20 32.8% 19 38.0%
Assisted/supported housing 2 1.7% 5 8.2% 3 6.0%
Homeless 11 9.2% 1 1.6% 3 6.0%
Employment status 3.56 0.47
Unemployed 81 68.6% 34 55.7% 35 70.0%
Working part-time 20 16.9% 15 24.6% 8 16.0%
Working full-time 17 14.4% 12 19.7% 7 14.0%
Education 5.43 .71
Less than high school diploma 15 12.5% 3 4.9% 4 8.0%
High school diploma/ GED 71 59.2% 35 57.4% 30 60.0%
Associate’s degree 20 16.7% 14 23.0% 8 16.0%
Bachelor’s degree 10 8.3% 6 9.8% 4 8.0%
Graduate degree 4 3.3% 3 4.9% 4 8.0%

*P <.05, **P <.01, ***P <.001, ^P <.10;

Note: Percentages and totals based on valid, non-missing responses. ^Non-mutually exclusive codes so total > 231, one participant that described their diagnosis as “Other” and their provider’s “diagnostic impression of schizoaffective” was coded as such.

Baseline Measures

Treatment intensity groups did not significantly differ on baseline measures. Pairwise comparisons revealed that only on one baseline measure were there significant differences between any 2 of the 3 groups. Participants with a history of inpatient treatment had higher scores on the GPTS-R, t(168) = 2.25, P = .03, relative to the participants with no history of treatment. None of the other comparisons between treatment and no-treatment groups were significant (see omnibus models and means in table 2).

Table 2.

Mean Values of Baseline Assessments, and Results of One-Way ANOVAs Examining Omnibus Group Differences Between all 3 Groups

No Lifetime Treatment (N = 120) Outpatient Treatment
(N = 61)
Inpatient Treatment
(N = 50)
F df P
R-GPTS (total) 30.86 (6.90) 31.59 (6.83) 33.44 (6.63) 2.53 2, 228 .08^
PHQ-9 (total) 21.16 (4.66) 20.39 (5.22) 20.38 (4.62) 0.74 2, 228 .48
GAD-7 (total) 16.12 (4.50) 15.73 (4.39) 17.10 (3.61) 1.46 2, 227 .24
ISI (mean) 2.54 (0.81) 2.55 (0.93) 2.61 (0.85) 0.12 2, 228 .89
SDS-3 (total) 24.97 (5.98) 26.05 (5.45) 26.16 (5.12) 1.13 2, 221 .33
EASI-MI (total) 18.03 (6.37) 16.36 (5.87) 16.51 (6.08) 1.94 2, 228 .15
EASI-TR (total) 21.80 (6.72) 20.41 (6.75) 21.45 (6.31) 0.90 2, 228 .41
EASI-TS (total) 20.68 (7.18) 20.08 (7.05) 19.42 (7.35) 0.56 2, 228 .57
EASI-3 (mean) 20.17 (5.36) 18.95 (5.36) 19.13 (5.51) 1.29 2, 228 .27

*P <.05, **P <.01, ***P <.001, ^P <.10;

Note: For measures where scores are typically reported as sum totals, mean imputation was used to account for missing values.

R-GPTS, Revised Green Paranoid Thoughts Scale; PHQ-9, Patient Health Questionnaire; GAD-7, Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire; ISI, Insomnia Severity Index; SDS-3, Sheehan Disability Scale, 3-item total; EASI-MI, Endorsed, and Anticipated Stigma Inventory, Mental Illness Scale; EASI-TR, EASI, Treatment subscale; EASI-TS, EASI, Treatment-Seeking Subscale; EASI-3, Mean of the 3 administered EASI subscales.

The only significant paired comparison (ie, comparing each treatment condition with no treatment) was between those with no lifetime PI treatment and those with inpatient PI treatment, which significantly differed t(168) = 2.25, P =.03. No other head-to-head comparisons were significant.

Response Frequencies

First, we aimed to describe typical patterns in the experience of PI. For simplicity, we focus reporting here on the proportion of responses where participants reported each phenomenon at a moderate or extreme level; however, full-item response frequencies can be found in table 3 and correlations between PI occurrence and cognitive response variables can be found in supplementary material. Of 14 549 EMA responses, most (N = 7748, 53.3%) instances involved participants reporting that they were not currently experiencing PI, and only approximately a quarter (N = 4259, 29.3%) of responses involved an experience of moderate or extreme PI. This differed from other symptoms assessed, for example, sadness and anxiety, where participants endorsed some level of each in most EMA questionnaires. At the same time, most participants that provided EMA responses did—at some point during the study period—provide a response consistent with at least moderate (N = 220, 91.3%) or extreme PI (N = 168, 73.4%). Participants also reported some variability regarding cognitive responses to PI. In nearly 80% of all responses where the participant reported some level of PI, the participant also reported feeling that the threat involved in their PI was either moderately (N = 1795, 27.8%) or extremely (N = 3436, 53.3%) important to them. Most respondents rated their current level of rumination as moderate (N = 1924, 29.7%) or extreme (N = 2894, 44.6%), and most reported feeling moderate (N = 1968, 30.6%) or extreme (N = 2993, 46.5%) levels of conviction. A similar number reported feeling moderate (N = 1779, 27.5%) or extreme (N = 2978, 46.0%) levels of distress. Responses were more divided with regard to behavior change in response to the persecutory threat, as a little over half of the sample (N = 3627, 56.0%) reported moderate or extreme changes to behavior, and slightly less than half reported not at all (N = 940, 14.5%) or a little behavior change (N = 1917, 29.6%). With regard to other factors associated with PI, participants most commonly reported low levels of self-confidence, desire to socialize, and low levels of energy (see table 3).

Table 3.

Frequencies of Persecutory Ideation EMA Items

Not at All A Little Moderately Extremely Total Responses^
PI occurrence
Persecutory ideation:
Are you currently feeling that someone or something wants to harm you?
7748 2542 1847 2412 14 549
(53.3%) (17.5%) (12.7%) (16.6%)
PI cognitive responses
PI conviction:
How certain are you that this is true?
360 1119 1968 2993 6440
(5.6%) (17.4%) (30.6%) (46.5%)
PI importance:
How much does the threat matter to you?
169 1050 1795 3436 6450
(2.6%) (16.3%) (27.8%) (53.3%)
PI rumination:
How much are you thinking about it?
208 1457 1924 2894 6483
(3.2%) (22.5%) (29.7%) (44.6%)
PI impact
PI distress:
How much is this distressing you?
268 1448 1779 2978 6473
(4.1%) (22.4%) (27.5%) (46.0%)
PI behavior change:
How much are you changing your behavior right now because of the threat?
940 1917 1555 2072 6484
(14.5%) (29.6%) (24.0%) (32.0%)
Affect / Environment
Anxiety:
How anxious do you feel right now?
3250 4077 3342 3219 13 888
(23.4%) (29.4%) (24.1%) (23.2%)
Self-esteem:
How self-confident do you feel right now?
4489 4400 3440 1549 13 878
(32.3%) (31.7%) (24.8%) (11.2%)
Sadness:
How sad do you feel right now?
4164 4575 2691 2459 13 889
(30.0%) (32.9%) (19.4%) (17.7%)
Sociality:
How much do you want to be around people right now?
6409 3962 2360 1157 13 888
(46.1%) (28.5%) (17.0%) (8.3%)
Energy:
How well-rested do you feel right now?
6409 3962 2360 1157 13 888
(46.1%) (28.5%) (17.0%) (8.3%)
Alone:
Are you alone?
7329 (No) 6648 (Yes) 13 977
(52.4%) (47.6%)

Note: ^Responses excluded if participants chose to “SKIP” the item or if they did not complete the questionnaire.

Ecological Momentary Assessment

EMA-assessed ratings of PI occurrence, cognitive responses, or impact were not found to differ significantly between the treated and untreated groups. In only one comparison pertaining to affect, individuals from the outpatient group were less likely to report feeling sad than those from the no-treatment group, β = −0.23, SE = 0.11, P = .04.

A second set of models examined the relationship between functional disability and momentary characteristics of PI. Increased disability was associated with an increased likelihood of appraising threats as important, β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, P = .02, ruminating about those threats, β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, P = .005, changing one’s behavior in response to the threat, β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, P = .003, experiencing distress related to the threat, β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, P = .007. Disability was also associated with levels of anxiety (β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, P = .035) and sadness (β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, P = .048) experienced during the 30-day data collection period. Notably, while these phenomenological features were associated with disability levels, frequency of PI was not, β = 0.01, SE = 0.01, P = .25. Significant models are reported in table 4.

Table 4.

Mixed Models Demonstrating Significant Relationships of Psychiatric Disability at Baseline to Momentary Persecutory Ideation Factors

Importance
How Much Does the Threat Matter to You?
Rumination
How Much are You Thinking About the Threat?
Behavior
Are You Changing Your ­Behavior in Response to the Threat?
Est SE t Est SE t Est SE t
Fixed effects
Intercept 1.68 0.19 8.93*** 1.45 0.19 7.74*** 0.89 0.21 4.23***
Psychiatric Disability (SDS) 0.02 0.01 2.40* 0.02 0.01 2.86** 0.02 0.01 2.96**
Random effects
σ2 0.42 0.45 0.61
τ00 id 0.34 0.33 0.41
ICC 0.45 0.42 0.40
Nid 219 219 219
Observations 6234 6267 6269
AIC 12 868.34 13 451.14 15 278.55
Distress
How much is this distressing
you?
Sadness
How sad do you feel right now?
Anxiety
How anxious do you feel right now?
Est SE t Est SE t Est SE t
Fixed effects
Intercept 1.44 0.19 7.55*** 0.89 0.21 4.22*** 1.08 0.21 5.05***
Psychiatric Disability (SDS) 0.02 0.01 2.71** 0.02 0.01 1.99* 0.02 0.01 2.12*
Random effects
σ2 0.49 0.67 0.66
τ00 id 0.34 0.45 0.46
ICC 0.41 0.40 0.41
Nid 219 222 222
Observations 6258 13 360 13 358
AIC 13 896.66 33 341.02 33 198.70

*P < .05; *P < .01;

Sensing and Device Use

Across all participants, average days involved around 4 hours in settings that devices logged as near speech (M = 3.78, SD = 3.50), and about 18 hours per day at the participant’s primary location (M = 18.27, SD = 6.16). Participants traveled on average around 29 miles per day on average (M = 41 424.01 meters, SD = 529 144.16), sent and received around 30 text messages (M = 27.46, SD = 44.99), and were on the phone for about 17 minutes (M = 0.29 hours, SD = 0.50). With regard to primary analyses, treatment groups did not differ with regard to passively sensed variables, with one exception. On days that a text message was either sent or received, individuals who had received outpatient services exchanged fewer messages than those who had not received treatment, β = −0.31, SE = −0.15, P = .04. With regard to baseline psychiatric disability, similarly only one model was significant, as higher levels of psychiatric disability were associated with higher number of text message exchanges on days texts were exchanged at all, β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, P = .03.

Discussion

This study provides granular insights into the momentary characteristics that are associated with poorer functional outcomes. Individuals with greater functional disability are more likely to describe threats as important to them, to ruminate about those threats, to experience distress related to them, and to change their behavior in response. Functional disability was not associated, however, with the frequency with which individuals experienced PI. Clinical and nonclinical groups with PI largely did not differ with one another in regard to either momentary or baseline characteristics. In our team’s previous work examining characteristics associated with treatment intensity among individuals with AVH, we found that individuals who had received inpatient or outpatient treatment differed in predictable ways from those who had never received care. The results of the present study suggest that PI may have a more complex relationship to help-seeking behaviors. Last, a general lack of significant differences in passively sensed variables deviates from our team’s prior work examining AVH, and suggests that passively sensed data may be better suited to identify changes occurring within individuals rather than distinguish across groups.

Descriptive results provide a snapshot of typical cross-diagnostic PI phenomenology. Results were consistent with previous findings29 that PI and its negative momentary effects appear episodic. This contradicts traditional views of persecutory delusions, wherein individuals with PI are thought to hold consistent and inflexible beliefs about threats. Though all participants enrolling in the study screened positive for moderate PI using a retrospective measure, at most data collection time points, participants reported not experiencing PI. When experiencing PI; however, they were likely to report moderate or severe appraisals of the PI as important, certain, and causing them to ruminate. This suggests that disruptions caused by PI may most often result from momentary exacerbations. Identifying and reducing individual risk factors—eg, poor sleep, stress—likely reduces the frequency of these episodes, as has been suggested in studies using retrospective measures.19

Several factors were identified as associated with functional disability in PI, including perceived importance of the threat, rumination, associated distress, and changing one’s behavior in response. The use of dimensional factors to better predict PI-related outcomes aligns with models that acknowledge continuum of psychiatric phenomena. There exist identifiable factors that determine the functional impact of PI, and these factors are not specific to individuals who have received services. These results are also broadly consistent with cognitive models of PI, according to which an individual’s appraisals and behavioral responses determine the extent to which psychiatric phenomena result in distress or dysfunction.48 This was particularly clear in light of findings that PI frequency on its own was not associated with functional disability. In addition to reinforcing the logic of the cognitive model, this finding also cautions against relying entirely on frequency of PI as a metric of symptom severity. These findings also align with acceptance and commitment models recently applied to psychosis,49 according to which it is not unwanted thoughts that result in impairment, but rather responses to unwanted thoughts that increases their perceived importance (eg, changing one’s behavior because of a thought, ruminating).

Interestingly, neither real-time or retrospective measures of PI differed across groups defined by their level of treatment intensity. This was the case both with baseline measures and in momentary data collection. This pattern of results differed from our team’s previous work examining differences between groups by treatment intensity among people who experience auditory verbal hallucinations (or voices), where individuals with higher levels of care intensity reported higher levels of distress and dysfunction related to their voices. While the precise cause of this is unclear, several factors could be relevant. There are numerous factors that determine whether an individual is going to receive mental health services, including distance from clinics,50 available resources,51 as well as attitudes toward help-seeking,52 although importantly, controlling for stigma did not change overall patterns in findings related to help-seeking groups. Further, it is possible that PI severity has a complex relationship with help-seeking. Its associated distress might both make help-seeking more likely, while at the same time, associated fear of disclosure might discourage it.53 Given the fact that PI may itself constitute a barrier to accessing care, these findings indicate intervention delivery strategies that circumvent these barriers. including telemedicine, or even guided self-help.

Baseline functional disability did not appear to predict passive sensor values consistent with social withdrawal. This overall trend differs some from our team’s previous work examining AVH. In that study, individuals with clinical AVH appeared to engage in a number of behaviors consistent with social withdrawal: Spending more time at home and less time around other people. The reduced magnitude of effects in a population of individuals with PI could be attributed to several factors. First, the present study involved a sample that was about 60% the size of that of our prior work focused on AVH; greater statistical power could be needed to detect true population differences. Second, it might be the case that the behavioral disruptions caused by PI are less observable. While participants with greater functional disability were more likely to report changing their behavior in response to threats, these changes may have been more covert or internal. Third, our findings might support different and more personalized uses of passively sensed data. These data may be most useful to detect changes within participants rather than between them, as has been suggested by our team’s work examining PI in schizophrenia,37 as well as others using these tools alongside more sophisticated personalized estimates to better understand depression,54 anxiety,55,56 or other psychotic symptoms.57

Our study is not without limitations. First, the study was conducted entirely remotely and online. The population from which our sample was drawn might overrepresent attributes specific to individuals more likely to participate in online studies, including access to digital technologies or technological literacy. For example, in a manner consistent with other online research, our sample overrepresented females. Second, measures of disability were not assessed longitudinally. We cannot conclude then whether real-time attributes of PI are the result of disability, a cause of it, or concurrently associated with a shared underlying variable. The SDS also is a brief measure that provides a rough overall estimate of self-perceived functional disability; a more thorough assessment may have revealed slightly different results. Third, we did not engage in purposive sampling to represent individuals across PI severity levels. While application of our inclusion criteria resulted in a reasonable distribution of severity levels and varied diagnoses, it is possible that certain forms of PI were overrepresented relative to others. It is particularly notable that a small subset of the sample reported a history of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. It is unclear whether this reflects the fact that most people who experience PI present with other diagnoses or that our study recruitment insufficiently engaged individuals with this diagnostic history. There is a dearth of epidemiological studies to help answer this question. Fourth, interpretation of passive sensor data is challenging without grounding in individual patterns. In other words, it might be the case that an elevation on a particular sensor could indicate dysfunction for one individual and adaptive resilience for another. Our study provides an important first step to understand broad patterns in differences in activity detected by these sensors, but future studies should take such a person-centered approach, or seek to validate how passive sensors might be used nomothetically.58,59 Fifth, the exploratory approach of our study prevents more targeted assessment of the synergistic interactive effects that result from the co-occurrence of PI and other psychotic and non-psychotic symptoms.

These results provide support for the importance of real-time, real-place responses to PI in determining functional disability across the continuum of PI. PI is episodic, and varies according to important phenomenological attributes. Further, individuals who have not received treatment for these experiences do not appear overall to experience fewer functional disruptions from these symptoms. Intervention approaches that transcend barriers to individuals with PI, and provide them support in real-time appear better suited to reduce their negative impact and may reduce the likelihood that they evolve into disabling psychiatric conditions.

Supplementary Material

sgad021_suppl_Supplementary_Table

Contributor Information

Benjamin Buck, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA.

Mary Wingerson, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA.

Justin S Tauscher, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA.

Matthew Enkema, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA.

Weichen Wang, Department of Computer Science, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH.

Andrew T Campbell, Department of Computer Science, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH.

Dror Ben-Zeev, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA.

Funding

Research reported in this manuscript was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) under award numbers R01MH112641 as well a NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (Grant #27917). Dr. Buck is also supported by a Mentored Patient-Oriented Research Career Development Award from NIMH (K23MH122504). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. The organizations supporting this research had no role in the design, collection, analysis, interpretation or writing of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

Dr. Ben-Zeev has financial interests in FOCUS technology, CORE technology, and Merlin LLC. He has an intervention content licensing agreement with Pear Therapeutics and has provided consultation services to Trusst Health, K Health, Boehringer Ingelheim, eQuility, Deep Valley Labs, and Otsuka Pharmaceuticals. The other authors have no conflicts to disclose.

References

  • 1.Freeman D, McManus S, Brugha T, Meltzer H, Jenkins R, Bebbington P.. Concomitants of paranoia in the general population. Psychol Med. 2011;41(05):923–936. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ohayon MM, Schatzberg AF.. Prevalence of depressive episodes with psychotic features in the general population. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(11):1855–1861. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.11.1855 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Azorin JM, Akiskal H, Hantouche E.. The mood-instability hypothesis in the origin of mood-congruent versus mood-incongruent psychotic distinction in mania: validation in a French National Study of 1090 patients. J Affect Disord. 2006;96(3):215–223. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.08.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Tibbo P, Swainson J, Chue P, LeMelledo JM.. Prevalence and relationship to delusions and hallucinations of anxiety disorders in schizophrenia. Depress Anxiety. 2003;17(2):65–72. doi: 10.1002/da.10083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Pearse LJ, Dibben C, Ziauddeen H, Denman C, McKenna PJ.. A study of psychotic symptoms in borderline personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2014;202(5):368–371. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Zhao QF, Tan L, Wang HF, et al. The prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2016;190:264–271. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Shevlin M, McElroy E, Bentall RP, Reininghaus U, Murphy J.. The psychosis continuum: testing a bifactor model of psychosis in a general population sample. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(1):133–141. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Freeman D. Suspicious minds: The psychology of persecutory delusions. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27(4):425–457. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Yung AR, Buckby JA, Cotton SM, et al. Psychotic-like experiences in nonpsychotic help-seekers: associations with distress, depression, and disability. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(2):352–359. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Olfson M, Lewis-Fernández R, Weissman MM, et al. Psychotic symptoms in an urban general medicine practice. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(8):1412–1419. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cuthbert BN. The RDoC framework: Facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry. 2014;13:28–35. doi: 10.1002/wps.20087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kotov R, Waszczuk MA, Krueger RF, et al. The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. J Abnorm Psychol. 2017;126(4):454–477. doi: 10.1037/abn0000258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Freeman D, Stahl D, McManus S, et al. Insomnia, worry, anxiety and depression as predictors of the occurrence and persistence of paranoid thinking. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47(8):1195–1203. doi: 10.1007/s00127-011-0433-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Murphy P, Bentall RP, Freeman D, O’Rourke S, Hutton P.. The paranoia as defence model of persecutory delusions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(11):913–929. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30339-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Freeman D, Brugha T, Meltzer H, Jenkins R, Stahl D, Bebbington P.. Persecutory ideation and insomnia: findings from the second British National Survey Of Psychiatric Morbidity. J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44(15):1021–1026. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Freeman D. Persecutory delusions: a cognitive perspective on understanding and treatment. Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(7):685–692. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00066-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Freeman D, Garety P.. Advances in understanding and treating persecutory delusions: a review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(8):1179–1189. doi: 10.1007/s00127-014-0928-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lincoln TM, Peters E.. A systematic review and discussion of symptom specific cognitive behavioural approaches to delusions and hallucinations. Schizophr Res. 2019;203:66–79. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Freeman D, Emsley R, Diamond R, et al. ; Oxford Cognitive Approaches to Psychosis Trial Study Group. Comparison of a theoretically driven cognitive therapy (the Feeling Safe Programme) with befriending for the treatment of persistent persecutory delusions: a parallel, single-blind, randomised controlled trial. Lancet Psychiatry. 2021;8(8):696–707. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00158-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bell IH, Lim MH, Rossell SL, Thomas N.. Ecological momentary assessment and intervention in the treatment of psychotic disorders: a systematic review. Psychiatr Serv. 2017;68(11):1172–1181. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201600523 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Statham V, Emerson LM, Rowse G.. A systematic review of self-report measures of paranoia. Psychol Assess. 2019;31(2):139–158. doi: 10.1037/pas0000645 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Haddock G, McCarron J, Tarrier N, Faragher EB.. Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS). Psychol Med. 1999;29(4):879–889. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799008661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sabbag S, Twamley EM, Vella L, Heaton RK, Patterson TL, Harvey PD.. Assessing everyday functioning in schizophrenia: not all informants seem equally informative. Schizophr Res. 2011;131(1–3):250–255. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.05.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Starr-McCluer M, Schwarz N, Sudman S.. Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports. South Econ J. 2006;61:1259. doi: 10.2307/1060776. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Schwarz N. Retrospective and concurrent self-reports: the rationale for real-time data capture. In: Stone AA, Shiffman SS, Atienza A, Nebeling L, eds.. The Science of Real-Time Data Capture: Self-Reports in Health Research. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ben-Zeev D, Kaiser SM, Brenner CJ, Begale M, Duffecy J, Mohr DC.. Development and usability testing of FOCUS: a smartphone system for self-management of schizophrenia. Psychiatr Rehabil J. 2013;36(4):289–296. doi: 10.1037/prj0000019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ben-Zeev D, Brian RM, Jonathan G, et al. Mobile health (mHealth) versus clinic-based group intervention for people with serious mental illness: a randomized controlled Trial. Psychiatr Serv. 2018;69(9):978–985. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201800063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lataster T, Valmaggia L, Lardinois M, van Os J, Myin-Germeys I.. Increased stress reactivity: a mechanism specifically associated with the positive symptoms of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med. 2013;43(7):1389–1400. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Thewissen V, Bentall RP, Lecomte T, van Os J, Myin-Germeys I.. Fluctuations in self-esteem and paranoia in the context of daily life. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008;117(1):143–153. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Thewissen V, Bentall RP, Oorschot M, et al. Emotions, self-esteem, and paranoid episodes: an experience sampling study. Br J Clin Psychol Soc. 2011;50(2):178–195. doi: 10.1348/014466510X508677 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Peters E, Lataster T, Greenwood K, et al. Appraisals, psychotic symptoms and affect in daily life. Psychol Med. 2012;42(5):1013–1023. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711001802 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ben-Zeev D, Ellington K, Swendsen J, Granholm E.. Examining a cognitive model of persecutory ideation in the daily life of people with schizophrenia: a computerized experience sampling study. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(6):1248–1256. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Depp CA, Mausbach B, Granholm E, et al. Mobile interventions for severe mental illness. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2010;198(10):715–721. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181f49ea3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Alvarez-Jimenez M, Alcazar-Corcoles MA, González-Blanch C, Bendall S, McGorry PD, Gleeson JF.. Online, social media and mobile technologies for psychosis treatment: a systematic review on novel user-led interventions. Schizophr Res. 2014;156(1):96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.03.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Nowak U, Krkovic K, Kammerer MK, Lincoln TM.. Pinpointing affective disturbances in psychosis: a comparison of temporal affect dynamics in individuals with psychotic disorders, individuals with attenuated psychotic symptoms, and clinical and healthy controls. J Psychiatr Res. 2022;153:260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.06.047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nowak U, Lincoln TM.. An experience-sampling study on the relevance of affect dynamics to paranoid ideation. Emotion. 2021;23:111–123. doi: 10.1037/emo0000807 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Buck B, Hallgren K, Scherer EA, et al. Capturing behavioral indicators of persecutory ideation using mobile technology. J Psychiatr Res. 2019;116:112–117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Ben-Zeev D, Buck B, Chander A, et al. Mobile RDoC: using smartphones to understand the relationship between auditory verbal hallucinations and need for care. Schizophr Bull Open. 2020;1:sgaa060. doi: 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgaa060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Freeman D, Loe BS, Kingdon D, et al. ; The revised Green et al. Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS): psychometric properties, severity ranges, and clinical cut-offs. Psychol Med. 2021;51(2):244–253. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719003155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Buck B, Chander A, Brian RM, Wang W, Campbell AT, Ben-Zeev D.. Expanding the reach of research: quantitative evaluation of a web-based approach for remote recruitment of people who hear voices. JMIR Form Res. 2021;5(6):e23118. doi: 10.2196/23118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sheehan DV. The anxiety disease. New York, NY: Scribner; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL.. The PHQ-9: a new depression diagnostic and severity Measure. Psychiatr Ann. 2002;32(9):509–515. doi: 10.3928/0048-5713-20020901-06 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, Löwe B.. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:1092. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bastien CH, Vallières A, Morin CM.. Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2001;2(4):297–307. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00065-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wang R, Chen F, Chen Z, et al. StudentLife: assessing mental health, academic performance and behavioral trends of college students using smartphones. Proceedings of the 2014 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing - UbiComp’14 Adjunct. 2014:3–14. doi: 10.1145/2632048.2632054 [DOI]
  • 46.Wang R, Aung MSH, Abdullah S, et al. CrossCheck: Towards passive sensing and detection of mental health changes in people with schizophrenia. Proceedings of the International Conference on Ubiquitous Computing (UbiComp), Heidelberg Germany, 12–16 September 2016. doi: 10.1145/2971648.2971740. [DOI]
  • 47.Ben-Zeev D, Brian R, Wang R, et al. CrossCheck: integrating self-report, behavioral sensing, and smartphone use to identify digital indicators of psychotic relapse. Psychiatr Rehabil J. 2017;40(3):266–275. doi: 10.1037/prj0000243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Freeman D. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for psychotic disorders. Psychiatric Times. 2013;30(12):16. https://www.psychiatrictimes.com/view/cognitive-behavioral-therapy-psychotic-disorders [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Louise S, Fitzpatrick M, Strauss C, Rossell SL, Thomas N.. Mindfulness- and acceptance-based interventions for psychosis: our current understanding and a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2018;192:57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.05.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Morales DA, Barksdale CL, Beckel-Mitchener AC.. A call to action to address rural mental health disparities. J Clin Transl Sci. 2020;4(5):463–467. doi: 10.1017/cts.2020.42 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Fox JC, Blank M, Rovnyak VG, Barnett RY.. Barriers to help seeking for mental disorders in a rural impoverished population. Community Ment Health J. 2001;37(5):421–436. doi: 10.1023/A:1017580013197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Gronholm PC, Thornicroft G, Laurens KR, Evans-Lacko S.. Mental health-related stigma and pathways to care for people at risk of psychotic disorders or experiencing first-episode psychosis: a systematic review. Psychol Med. 2017;47:1867–1879. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717000344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Clement S, Schauman O, Graham T, et al. What is the impact of mental health-related stigma on help-seeking? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative studies. Psychol Med. 2015;45(1):11–27. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.de Angel V, Lewis S, White K, et al. Digital health tools for the passive monitoring of depression: a systematic review of methods. NPJ Digit Med. 2022;5(1):1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41746-021-00548-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Jacobson NC, Bhattacharya S.. Digital biomarkers of anxiety disorder symptom changes: personalized deep learning models using smartphone sensors accurately predict anxiety symptoms from ecological momentary assessments. Behav Res Ther. 2022;149:104013. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2021.104013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Jacobson NC, Summers B, Wilhelm S.. Digital biomarkers of social anxiety severity: digital phenotyping using passive smartphone sensors. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(5):e16875. doi: 10.2196/16875 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Benoit J, Onyeaka H, Keshavan M, Torous J.. Systematic review of digital phenotyping and machine learning in psychosis spectrum illnesses. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2020;28(5):296–304. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Torous J, Staples P, Barnett I, Onnela JP, Keshavan M.. A crossroad for validating digital tools in schizophrenia and mental health. npj Schizophr. 2018;4(1):1–2. doi: 10.1038/s41537-018-0048-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Strauss GP, Raugh IM, Zhang L, et al. Validation of accelerometry as a digital phenotyping measure of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia. 2022;8(1):37. doi: 10.1038/s41537-022-00241-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

sgad021_suppl_Supplementary_Table

Articles from Schizophrenia Bulletin Open are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press on behalf of the University of Maryland's School of Medicine, Maryland Psychiatric Research Center

RESOURCES