Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
. 2023 Aug 12;18(1):nsad040. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsad040

Differential processing of risk and reward in delinquent and non-delinquent youth

Natasha Duell 1,2,, Michael T Perino 3, Ethan M McCormick 4, Eva H Telzer 5
PMCID: PMC10439709  PMID: 37572094

Abstract

The present study examined the behavioral and neural differences in risky decision-making between delinquent (n = 23) and non-delinquent (n = 27) youth ages 13–17 years (M = 16, SD = 0.97) in relation to reward processing. While undergoing functional neuroimaging, participants completed an experimental risk task wherein they received feedback about the riskiness of their behavior in the form of facial expressions that morphed from happy to angry. Behavioral results indicated that delinquent youth took fewer risks and earned fewer rewards on the task than non-delinquent youth. Results from whole-brain analyses indicated no group differences in sensitivity to punishments (i.e. angry faces), but instead showed that delinquent youth evinced greater neural tracking of reward outcomes (i.e. cash-ins) in regions including the ventral striatum and inferior frontal gyrus. While behavioral results show that delinquent youth were more risk-averse, the neural results indicated that delinquent youth were also more reward-driven, potentially suggesting a preference for immediate rewards. Results offer important insights into differential decision-making processes between delinquent and non-delinquent youth.

Keywords: delinquency, adolescents, risk-taking, fMRI, ventral striatum

Introduction

Although some amount of risk-taking during adolescence is normative (Moffitt and Caspi, 2001; Rutter et al., 2006), a small proportion of youth engage in persistent levels of antisocial behavior, hereafter referred to as delinquency (Frick and Viding, 2009; Cook et al., 2015). Delinquent behavior during adolescence is associated with long-term consequences, including poor health, job insecurity and greater risk of lifetime criminality (Bongers et al., 2008; DeLisi et al., 2010; Drury et al., 2019). Despite decades of research examining risk behavior in adolescence, questions remain about what distinguishes youth engaging in normative versus delinquent patterns of risk behavior. Sensitivity to rewards and punishments plays a key role in motivating risk behavior (Bacon et al., 2018). One theory is that delinquent youth are more sensitive to rewards than non-delinquent youth (Byrd et al., 2014). This heightened sensitivity to reward may compel delinquent youth to pursue rewarding stimuli with little regard for potential consequences (Frick et al., 2003), or it may curtail effective self-regulation by motivating an impulsive drive for immediate gratification (Steinberg, 2008; Matthys et al., 2013). To unpack how delinquent and non-delinquent youth process rewards during risk-taking, the present study compared behavioral and neural correlates of risky decision-making.

Risk-taking is consistently associated with heightened reward sensitivity (for a review, see Shulman et al., 2016). During adolescence, subcortical brain regions responsible for processing rewards, such as the ventral striatum (VS), undergo significant changes that render the adolescent brain highly sensitive to rewards (Schreuders et al., 2018). Experimental studies with adolescents have found evidence of heightened activation in reward-sensitive brain regions when adolescents take risks (Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010) and when they receive rewards (Schreuders et al., 2018). This link between reward-related brain activation and risk-taking has been observed among both delinquent (Bjork et al., 2010) and non-delinquent (Barkley-Levenson and Galvan, 2014) youth. However, some research suggests that delinquent youth may be disproportionately sensitive to rewards (Hawes et al., 2021). For example, on experimental gambling tasks, delinquent youth favor gambles with high rewards even though these gambles ultimately lead to large losses (Lane and Cherek, 2001). Furthermore, upon receiving rewards for their decisions, delinquent youth evince greater VS activation than non-delinquent youth (Bjork et al., 2010). Thus, results from studies using experimental tasks and fMRI suggest that delinquent youth demonstrate more exaggerated behavioral and neural sensitivity to rewards than non-delinquent youth.

Exaggerated reward sensitivity among delinquent youth could lead to increased risky behavior through several pathways. Perhaps increased reward responsivity drives increased thrill-seeking behavior. For example, heightened reward sensitivity is associated with an increased propensity to seek out novel and thrilling experiences (Roose et al., 2011). Prior work has shown that delinquent youth demonstrate a higher propensity for thrill-seeking behaviors than their non-delinquent peers (Castellanos-Ryan et al., 2011). From this perspective, thrill-seeking may dampen sensitivity to the potential consequences associated with risky decision-making (Frick et al., 2003). In line with this, the response modulation hypothesis (Gorenstein and Newman, 1980) postulates that delinquent youth possess a dominant, reward-oriented response set, such that rewarded experiences are more salient than punishments, and therefore do not produce avoidance behavior. This is supported by empirical work showing that delinquent youth show performance deficits in the face of competing reward and punishment (Byrd et al., 2014). For example, on an experimental gambling task, boys with oppositional defiant disorder were more likely than their peers to continue choosing risky options, even after those choices yielded more punishments than rewards (Matthys et al., 2004).

Alternatively, heightened reward sensitivity in delinquent adolescents may be associated with a preference for immediate gratification. This preference for immediate rewards may be heightened among delinquent youth due to impairments in the ability to delay gratification (Piquero et al., 2018), which may reflect a strategy of delinquent youth pursuing smaller, more immediate rewards, even if larger rewards could be possible with more effort or time. Decisions involving immediately available rewards are associated with heightened activation in subcortical brain regions implicated in reward processing, including the VS (McClure et al., 2004). Neural systems responsible for processing and responding to rewards may interact with motor systems to increase the likelihood of quick, rather than thoughtful and calculated, responses (Luna, 2012). From this vantage point, risky, reward-oriented decision-making among delinquent youth may be more about the desire to pursue immediate rewards than about insensitivity to punishments.

Examining the neural mechanisms associated with risk-taking in the context of reward may shed light on the psychological processes that drive behavioral differences between delinquent and non-delinquent youth. To this end, the present study had two key aims: (i) to identify behavioral differences in risk-taking between delinquent and non-delinquent youth and (ii) to identify differences in neural sensitivity when taking risks and receiving rewards. To answer these questions, delinquent and non-delinquent youth completed the Social Analog Risk Task (SART), which is modified from the well-validated Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002). During the task, participants took greater risks to obtain larger rewards. Each reward brought the participant closer to the end of the trial. If the trial ended before the participant decided to cash in their earnings, they lost all rewards for that trial. Participants received dynamic feedback on the amount of risk they incurred via facial expressions that morphed from happy (no risk) to angry (high risk), where higher risk indicated being closer to the end of the trial. Thus, the task allowed us to examine neural sensitivity both when making risky decisions and when cashing out to obtain rewards. Behaviorally, we anticipated that delinquent youth would take more risks than non-delinquent youth because they were more sensitive to rewards than the amount of risk they incurred (as indicated by facial expressions). At the neural level, we explored whether delinquent youth demonstrated differential neural sensitivity to increasing risks and increasing rewards relative to non-delinquent youth. Examining the neural correlates of risky decision-making will help clarify the neuropsychological processes supporting differences in risk behavior between delinquent and non-delinquent youth.

Method

Participants

Participants included 50 youth age 13–17 years (M = 16, SD = 0.97) from Eastern Illinois recruited for participation in a study comparing decision-making and behavioral outcomes between delinquent and non-delinquent youth. Approximately half of the sample (n = 23) was recruited for having engaged in antisocial behaviors, resulting in contact with the justice system such as theft, fighting or drug use. These youth, hereon referred to as ‘delinquent youth’, were recruited from one of three locations: an alternative school for students expelled or suspended for gross misconduct, the local juvenile detention center or the local parole and probation office. Although the original delinquent sample included n = 25 participants, two participants were excluded from the final sample due to excessive motion in the fMRI scanner. The other half of the sample (n = 27) was recruited from mainstream schools in the same geographic area. Participants were compensated US $50 for participation. Informed consent and assent were obtained in accordance with the university’s institutional review board.

Demographic information for the two groups is presented in Table 1. Non-delinquent and delinquent youth did not significantly differ in age or gender, but there were significantly more White participants in the non-delinquent group and significantly more Black participants in the delinquent group. Rates of self-reported risk-taking and externalizing symptoms between the two groups as well as rates of delinquency and institutional discipline experienced by youth in the delinquent sample are reported in Table 2. As expected, delinquent youth reported higher externalizing and risk behaviors than non-delinquent youth.

Table 1.

Demographic information for non-delinquent and delinquent groups

Group Test
Demographic Non-delinquent (n = 27) Delinquent (n = 23) (Non-delinquent–delinquent)
Age (years) M = 15.85, SD = 0.64 M = 16.19, SD = 1.24 t(48) = −1.262, P = 0.213
Female 52% (n = 14) 52% (n = 12) t(48) = −0.022, P = 0.982
White 78% (n = 21) 48% (n = 11) χ 2(1) = 4.836, P = 0.028
Black 11% (n = 3) 52% (n = 12) χ2(1) = 9.972, P = 0.002
Others 11% (n = 3) 0% (n = 0)

Table 2.

Self-reported externalizing behaviors and lifetime disciplinary history in delinquent and non-delinquent groups

Behavior Group Minimum Maximum Mean SD t(48)
Risk-taking Non-delinquent 1 2.17 1.309 0.355
Delinquent 1.42 3.67 2.247 0.49 7.825a
Externalizing Non-delinquent 1 2.69 1.456 0.427
Delinquent 1 4.38 2.551 1.114 4.726a
Number of suspensionsb Delinquent 0 8 4.708 2.985
Number of expulsionsb Delinquent 0 3 0.792 0.833
Number of arrestsb Delinquent 0 7 1.333 2.18
a

Means of non-delinquent and delinquent samples are significantly different at P < 0.001.

b

Data not collected for non-delinquent youth.

SART

Participants completed the SART (adapted from Humphreys et al., 2016) based on the well-validated BART (Lejuez et al., 2002). The SART was developed to index risk-taking as a function of sensitivity to social feedback. Participants were told that they would be playing a ‘trick-or-treat’ game in which they would approach 24 people at their house (Figure 1). Each house corresponded with one trial. At each house, participants could knock on the door by making a button press (accompanied by a knocking sound) to earn points. The screen displayed the resident’s face and always started with a 100% happy expression. With each knock, the face morphed from happy to neutral to angry, at which point the door would slam (accompanied by a loud slamming sound) and participants would lose all points earned in that trial. The slam point, or number of knocks leading to a door slam, varied between 3 and 10 knocks (M = 6.5, SD = 1.29). There were three trials per slam point (e.g. 3 out of 24 houses had a slam point of 10). Participants could ‘cash in’ (accompanied by a rising note sound) the points earned on each door at any point before the door slam and move on to the next trial. A running total of points were presented as a points meter on the left side of the screen.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

(A) Illustration of the ‘trick-or-treat’ task. Each decision was self-paced with a 500–4000 ms jitter between each event. If participants knocked the door until the resident’s face reached 50% anger, the door would slam (top half of figure). Participants could also choose to cash in any time (bottom half of figure). (B). Illustration of variability in slam points. In the top half of the figure, the resident’s expression changes quickly, resulting in a slam after only four knocks. In the bottom half of the figure, the resident’s expression changes slower, resulting in a slam after seven knocks.

Participants saw 12 individual faces (4 White, 4 Black and 4 Asian; all faces were female) twice each during the task. Each unique face appeared at least once before any face reappeared. The task was self-paced and did not advance unless participants decided to either knock or cash in. A random jitter (500–4000 ms) followed each event (i.e. (i) a knock, (ii) a new face after the decision to cash in and (iii) a new face following a slam). Faces were presented in a fixed order. Prior to the scan session, research assistants trained participants on the task. To incentivize performance, participants were shown a box of age-appropriate prizes (e.g. snacks and movies) and were told that they could select prizes based on the points earned during the game. In reality, all participants were allowed to choose three prizes.

Whereas the BART offers no information about the balloon’s explosion point, the SART offers dynamic feedback (i.e. morphing face) about each door’s slam point, allowing participants to modify their behavior based on the anger level displayed in the face. This unique feature of the SART allowed us to measure participants’ sensitivity to risk. Behaviorally, we examined how knock decisions (i.e. total number of knocks on each trial) varied as a function of risk (i.e. the trial-specific slam point). At the neural level, we had two conditions of interest: (i) neural sensitivity to risk (i.e. when deciding to knock, does brain activation change as the face gets angrier?) and (II) neural sensitivity to reward (i.e. when deciding to cash in, does brain activation change as reward value increases?).

fMRI data acquisition and analysis

A 3 Tesla Siemens Trio MRI scanner was used to collect imaging data. Structural scans consisted of a T1* magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE; Repitition Time (TR) = 1.9 s; Time-to-Echo (TE) = 2.3 ms; Field-of-View (FOV) = 230; matrix = 256 × 256; sagittal plane; slice thickness = 1 mm; 192 slices) and a T2*-weighted, matched-bandwidth (MBW), high-resolution anatomical scan (TR = 4 s; TE = 64 ms; FOV = 230; matrix = 192 × 192; slice thickness = 3 mm; 38 slices). During the trick-or-treat task, T2*-weighted echoplanar images (EPI; slice thickness = 3 mm; 38 slices; TR = 2 s; TE = 25 ms; matrix = 92 × 92; FOV = 230 mm; voxel size 2.5 × 2.5 × 3 mm3) were acquired. MBW and EPI scans were obtained using an oblique axial orientation in order to maximize brain coverage.

The Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK) statistical package was used for data preprocessing and analysis. Preprocessing involved correcting for head motion using spatial realignment. We censored TRs with 2 mm or more of absolute motion in any direction. There were no significant group differences in movement between the delinquent and non-delinquent groups (t(48) = 0.974, P = 0.335). Next, all images were coregistered to the high-resolution T1* MPRAGE structural scan and segmented into gray matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid. MWB and EPI images were warped into the standard stereotactic space defined by the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) and the International Consortium for Brian Mapping by applying the transformation matrices used in MPRAGE segmentation. To increase the signal-to-noise ratio in the functional images, EPI images were smoothed using an 8 mm Gaussian kernel, full-width-at-half maximum. Each trial was then convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function using the general linear model in SPM8. Finally, a high-pass temporal filter (cutoff = 128 s) was applied to the time series in order to remove low-frequency drift and a restricted maximum likelihood algorithm with an autoregressive model order of 1 was used to estimate serial autocorrelations.

The SART is an event-related design. We included general linear models for each condition of interest (i.e. knock decisions, cash-in decisions and slam events) in the fixed effect models. Knock decisions on doors ending in cash-ins were modeled separately from knock decisions on doors ending in slams. As in previous research (Lejuez et al., 2002; Telzer et al., 2015), analyses only included knock decisions made during doors ending in cash-ins because slams artificially curtail participants’ knocking behavior. Each trial was modeled from the onset of the event to the point at which participants made their decision. The jittered intertrial periods between knock and cash-in decisions were not modeled and served as an implicit baseline.

To examine how neural activation changed as a function of risks (i.e. increasing knocks) and rewards (i.e. increasing receipt of points), a parametric modulator (PM) was included for our two conditions of interest: knock decisions and cash-in decisions. For knock decisions, the PM values represented the cumulative number of knocks within a trial (i.e. house). The PM values for each knock were centered within person around the average number of cumulative knocks within the trial. This PM allowed us to examine how the brain tracked increasing risk. For cash-in decisions, PM values represented the total number of knocks for each trial (i.e. the total risk that was taken for each house). The PM values for each cash-in trial were centered within person around the average number of knocks for the entire task. This PM allowed us to examine how the brain tracked increasing reward receipt, as more knocks equated to more points earned. This analytic approach was used to be consistent with prior studies using the SART task (e.g. McCormick et al., 2018; Van Hoorn et al., 2018).

Random-effects group-level analyses included the individual-level contrast images. Group-level analyses were conducted using GLMFlex, which removes outliers and sudden activation changes, partitions error terms, analyzes all voxels containing data and corrects for variance–covariance inequality (http://mrtools.mgh.harvard.edu/index.php/GLM_Flex). Our primary analyses included two-sample t-tests comparing the delinquent to non-delinquent groups on our contrasts of interest. Because slam events occurred too infrequently, this condition was not included in the analyses. We corrected all analyses for multiple comparisons using Monte Carlo simulations through 3DClustSim (updated version November 2016) in the software package AFNI (Ward, 2000) and accounted for the intrinsic smoothness of the data with the -acf function within the 3dFWHMx command. We used a voxel-wise threshold of P <0.005, corresponding to P <0.05, family-wise error cluster-corrected. All reported results are available on NeuroVault (Gorgolewski et al., 2015; see https://neurovault.org/collections/12312/).

Results

We conducted all behavioral analyses using Mplus v.8. To compare general performance on the SART between delinquent and non-delinquent youth, we first conducted independent samples t-tests for the total number of knocks (i.e. overall risk-taking). No significant differences in risk-taking (i.e. knocking) emerged between delinquent and non-delinquent youth, t(48) = 1.797, d = 0.51, P = 0.079.

To examine the behavioral sensitivity to risk, we estimated a model with risk-taking—indexed as the number of knocks for each trial—as the dependent variable, trial-specific slam point as a within-person independent variable (higher values indicate the face gets angry slower) and group (delinquent = 1) as a between-person independent variable. Including trial-specific slam point in the model allowed us to explore how risk behavior changed as a function of anger rate. Additionally, we examined the cross-level slam point × group interaction to explore whether sensitivity to risk differed between delinquent and non-delinquent youth. To control for learning effects (e.g. Van Hoorn et al., 2018), we entered trial number, previous trial outcome (1 = slam) and current trial outcome (1 = slam) as within-person covariates in the model.

Results from the multilevel model indicated that as slam point increased, risk-taking increased (Table 3). In other words, on trials with a slower transition to an angry face, participants knocked more frequently than on trials with a faster transition to an angry face. Furthermore, group moderated the effect of slam point on knock decisions. To explore this interaction, we examined the association between the slam point and the number of knocks in the delinquent and non-delinquent groups separately. Results indicated that both delinquent (B = 0.503, SE = 0.028, β = 0.655, P < 0.001) and non-delinquent (B = 0.588, SE = 0.024, β = 0.726, P < 0.001) youth knocked less frequently as slam point decreased (i.e. faces became angry faster). To further unpack the interaction, we explored group differences in risk-taking between trials with slow vs fast slam points. To do this, we tested for group differences in the number of knocks when the slam point was fastest (3 knocks to slam; face gets angry quickly) and when the slam point was slowest (10 knocks to slam; face gets angry slowly). Results indicated that delinquent youth evinced less risk-taking (i.e. cashed out sooner) than non-delinquent youth on slow trials (B = −0.959, SE = 0.362, β = −0.211, P = 0.008), but that there were no differences in risk-taking on fast trials (B = −0.094, SE = 0.066, β = −0.115, ns). Thus, whereas delinquent and non-delinquent youth made comparably risky decisions on trials with a fast slam point, they were less risky on trials with a slow slam point (Figure 2).

Table 3.

Results from the multilevel model examining differences in sensitivity to social feedback between delinquent and non-delinquent youth on social risk-taking task

Outcome: number of knocks B SE (B) β
Within Trial 0.013 0.009 0.055
Previous outcome −0.452*** 0.112 −0.079
Current outcome 0.27* 0.124 0.046
Slam point 0.525*** 0.032 0.752
Between Group −0.506* 0.248 −0.294
Cross-level Slam point × group −0.101* 0.042 a

Note. Trial number, previous outcome (1 = Slam), and current outcome (1 = Slam) were included as covariates to control for learning effects on decision-making. The slam point represents how quickly the face morphed into an angry expression; higher values indicate that participants could knock more before reaching the slam point. Group represents the delinquent (1) and non-delinquent (0) youth. Coefficients for the main effects are from a model excluding the interaction term. ***P < 0.001; * P < 0.05; a Standardized coefficients are not available for cross-level interactions in Mplus.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Illustration of the cross-level interaction between the slam point and the group. The rate of anger represents the various slam points across trials (slow = 10 knocks; fast = 3 knocks). On slow trials, non-delinquent youth knocked more frequently than delinquent youth. There were no group differences in knocks on fast trials.

Neural sensitivity to risk and reward

At the whole-brain level, we examined neural tracking of risks and rewards by measuring changes in neural activation as a function of changes in risk (i.e. angry faces) and rewards (i.e. points earned). We started by exploring the main effects of risk and reward within the full sample (both delinquent and non-delinquent youth). In the first analysis, we modeled neural tracking of increasing risk by examining activation during knock decisions with the cumulative number of knocks within trials included as the PM. Results indicated increased neural tracking of risk (i.e. brain activation increased as faces became angrier) in regions such as the VS and insula. Additionally, we observed decreased neural tracking of risk (i.e. brain activation decreased as faces became angrier) in regions such as the fusiform gyrus, orbitofrontal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (see Table 4 for the full list of regions). Next, we modeled neural tracking of increasing reward by examining activation during cash-in decisions with the total number of knocks within trials included as the PM. Results showed increased neural tracking of reward (i.e. brain activation increased as earnings increased) in the VS, insula, fusiform gyrus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (see Table 5 for the full list of regions).

Table 4.

Regions showing increased and decreased neural tracking of risk (angry faces) during knock decisions

MNI coordinates
Region k a t-value x y z
Increased tracking Right anterior insula 368 6.960 36 23 10
Right ventral striatum 183 5.333 12 8 −5
dorsal anterior cingulate cortex 502 4.989 9 26 34
Left anterior insula 272 5.506 −30 20 −8
Right precentral gyrus 280 5.134 39 −13 64
Decreased tracking Right fusiform gyrus 32884a 7.411 45 −64 −17
Left fusiform gyrus a 6.436 −45 −58 −17
Orbitofrontal cortex/ventromedial prefrontal cortex a 7.264 −9 62 −5
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 266 5.824 48 47 7

Note. The map was thresholded at P < 0.005. Monte Carlo Simulation yielded a minimum cluster size of 139 for whole-brain analysis. a Regions part of the same cluster share a superscript.

Table 5.

Regions showing increased neural tracking of rewards during cash-in decisions

MNI coordinates
Region k t-value x y z
Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex 493 4.358 −6 35 7
Left anterior insula 182 4.301 −36 14 −2
Left putamen/caudate 144 3.93 −21 −4 19
Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 471 4.554 36 35 37
Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 615 4.348 −48 26 28
Right fusiform gyrus 2465 3.974 42 −55 −11
Left fusiform gyrus 624 3.77 −45 −55 −14

Note. The map was thresholded at P < 0.005. Monte Carlo Simulation yielded a minimum cluster size of 135 for whole-brain analysis.

Finally, we examined group differences in neural tracking of risk and reward by conducting an independent sample t-test on the analyses for knock decisions and cash-ins (including number of knocks as the PM). Results indicated no significant group differences in neural tracking of risk. However, we found group differences in neural tracking of reward (i.e. cash-ins). Compared to non-delinquent youth, delinquent youth evinced greater neural tracking of rewards (i.e. brain activation increased as earnings increased) in regions such as the VS, inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and fusiform gyrus (Table 6 for the full list of regions). For descriptive purposes, we extracted parameter estimates of signal intensity from the VS and plotted activation across levels of reward for the delinquent and non-delinquent groups separately. As shown in Figure 3, delinquent youth evinced increasing sensitivity to obtaining higher rewards (i.e. points earned) in the VS, whereas non-delinquent youth showed relatively low VS activation across reward value. Due to an insufficient number of trials, we did not have enough statistical power to parallel the behavioral results and examine the neural findings separately for ‘fast’ and ‘slow’ trials.

Table 6.

Regions showing greater neural tracking during reward receipt (cash-ins) in delinquent than non-delinquent youth

MNI coordinates
Region k t-value x y z
Left VS 472 3.146 −12 17 −2
Left IFG 576 3.980 −33 38 7
Right IPL 301 3.855 39 −52 43
Right fusiform gyrus 284 3.005 30 −61 −5
Left fusiform gyrus 300 2.691 −30 −70 −11

Note. The map was thresholded at P < 0.005. Monte Carlo Simulation yielded a minimum cluster size of 140 for whole-brain analysis.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Differences in neural tracking of reward (cash-ins) between non-delinquent and delinquent youth in the VS.

Discussion

The present study compared risk behavior and neural sensitivity to risks and rewards between delinquent and non-delinquent youth. Using an experimental socioemotional risk task that provided real-time feedback on the amount of risk incurred, we explored dynamic changes in brain activity during decision-making. Whereas risk-taking was similar between delinquent and non-delinquent youth on high-risk trials with a fast slam point (i.e. fewer opportunities to earn rewards), delinquent youth were less risky on trials with a slow slam point (i.e. more opportunities to earn rewards). At the neural level, delinquent youth demonstrated greater VS tracking of rewards than non-delinquent youth. Together, these findings suggest that delinquent youth do not ubiquitously take more risks than their non-delinquent peers. Furthermore, delinquent youth demonstrate heightened neural sensitivity to rewards than non-delinquent youth.

Since prior work suggests that delinquent youth are less risk-averse (Frick et al., 2003) and more sensitive to rewards (Hawes et al., 2021) than non-delinquent youth, we expected that delinquent youth would take advantage of the opportunity to acquire as many rewards as possible and therefore take more risks than non-delinquent youth. This hypothesis was not supported. Instead, delinquent youth took the same amount of risks as non-delinquent youth on trials with a fast slam point and took fewer risks on trials with a slow slam point. Differences in delinquent youth’s risk behavior between fast and slow trials demonstrate that delinquent youth integrated feedback into their decision-making and used that information to cash-in sooner.

There are two potential explanations for less risk-taking among delinquent youth on the slow trials: either delinquent youth were more sensitive to risk (i.e. heightened sensitivity to angry faces, which provided information about when the door was going to slam), or they were more sensitive to reward (i.e. they made a conscious decision to cash-in earlier to obtain an immediate reward). Indeed, some research suggests that delinquent youth are more averse to negative socioemotional stimuli than non-delinquent youth (Eftekhari et al., 2004) and avoid situations that induce negative feelings (Richter et al., 2002). However, other work suggests that delinquent youth have an overall blunted response to socioemotional cues (Gonzalez-Gadea et al., 2014). We speculate that if delinquent youth were highly sensitive to the angry faces, they would have been more risk-averse on the fast trials, when the faces became angry sooner, rather than on slow trials. For this reason, we suspect that delinquent youth cashed in earlier with the intention of obtaining more immediate rewards, consistent with prior work suggesting delinquent youth struggle to delay gratification (Matthys et al., 2013). Nevertheless, future research will need to replicate these findings. One important consideration is the confounder between risk (impending door slam) and the emotional stimulus of an angry face. Thus, future work should compare delinquent youth’s sensitivity to risks and rewards on tasks with and without socioemotional feedback.

Results from the fMRI analyses indicated that delinquent youth evinced linear increases in VS activation to rewards when cashing in their points. These results suggest that delinquent youth may have been more sensitive than non-delinquent youth to rewards, consistent with prior research (Hawes et al., 2021). The VS is involved in tracking the subjective value of stimuli and processing the presence or expectation of rewards (Knutson et al., 2001; Eldar et al., 2016). Prior work has implicated VS activity in antisocial behavior (Holz et al., 2017). For example, heightened VS activity during risk-taking predicts earlier binge drinking among youth, over and above behavioral assessments of risk (Morales et al., 2018). Hyperactivity of the VS is also linked to preferences for smaller, immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards (Hariri et al., 2006). These findings offer useful contextual information about the potential neuropsychological processes associated with decision-making. Thus, our findings present an alternative to theories purporting that delinquent behavior is due to dampened risk or threat processing (Du, 2019) and suggest that activity in reward-processing brain regions like the VS may be a key neurocognitive mechanism contributing to increased risk behavior among delinquent youth.

In addition to the VS, delinquent youth showed heightened neural tracking of rewards during cash-in decisions in the fusiform gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and IPL. The fusiform gyrus is largely involved in facial (Iidaka, 2013) and emotional information processing (Frank et al., 2019; Graham et al., 2021). Thus, greater neural tracking of reward in this region among delinquent youth may suggest greater encoding of angry faces as the reward value increased. Relatedly, the IFG is involved in monitoring and cognitive control (Dosenbach et al., 2008) as well as emotional processing (Rosen et al., 2017). Coupled with greater activation in the IPL, which has been previously linked to executive control (Dosenbach et al., 2008) and monitoring feedback (Vickery and Jiang, 2009), greater neural tracking of reward in these regions may indicate regulation of affective responses to angry faces, perhaps in order to cash in. These findings show that delinquent youth are not insensitive to risk or to socioemotional feedback but do indeed integrate this information into their decision-making. In future research, it will be important to directly test whether delinquent youth process information about risk differently when that information is social versus non-social.

Somewhat surprisingly, delinquent and non-delinquent youth showed no significant group differences in neural tracking of risks (i.e. faces becoming angrier as participants got closer to losing all their points). This finding is in line with theories suggesting that delinquent youth are not averse to social threat (Frick et al., 2003). However, this is not to say that delinquent youth completely disregarded social information on the task, as indicated by the observed activation in the fusiform gyrus during cash-in decisions. Furthermore, it is important to remember that the behavioral results suggested differences in risk-taking between delinquent and non-delinquent youth on ‘slow’ trials only. Therefore, it is possible that group differences in neural tracking of risk would have emerged had we been able to compare the neural findings between ‘slow’ and ‘fast’ trials.

The findings from this study offer an important contribution to the neurodevelopmental literature, which currently lacks a clear understanding of how risky decision-making processes differ between youth engaging in delinquent vs normative patterns of risk behavior. Nevertheless, there are a couple of limitations in this study worth considering when interpreting the results. In addition to the small sample size, it is important to acknowledge the over-representation of Black adolescents in our sample of delinquent youth. Black adolescents’ experiences with authority figures are subjectively and quantitatively different from their White peers. With a larger sample size, future work may be able to disentangle whether there are racial and ethnic differences in neural processing of risk and reward and be able to frame those differences in key contextual factors such as experiences with discrimination. Additionally, socioeconomic data were not available for the subjects. Prior work has shown that socioeconomic factors like lower parent education (Brieant et al., 2020) and worse neighborhood quality (Gonzalez et al., 2016) are associated with greater risk-taking and heightened neural sensitivity in mesolimbic reward areas during adolescence. Thus, future work should consider how socioeconomic factors interact with adolescents’ sensitivity to risks and rewards and their subsequent decision-making.

Furthermore, the behavioral analyses in this study do not directly parallel the neural analysis. Specifically, the behavioral data suggested that risk behavior differed between slow and fast trials. However, we were underpowered at the neural level to break down our analyses into slow and fast trials. Thus, although we did not find group differences in neural tracking of risk (i.e. changing emotional expressions) overall, it is possible that group differences would have emerged when splitting the data into fast and slow trials. In spite of this limitation, the neural findings in this study offer unique information about the neuropsychological processes associated with risk behavior between delinquent and non-delinquent youth that is currently limited in the neurodevelopmental literature.

The present study increases our understanding of the neuropsychological pathways that may distinguish youth who engage in normative versus non-normative and persistent patterns of risk behavior. Differentiating increases in normal tolerance of risk from increased propensity to commit delinquent acts is paramount for improving adolescent outcomes and requires assessment of both behavioral preferences and neural responses to risk and reward systems. By examining these processes with an ecologically valid paradigm, we found that delinquent youth did not show greater preference for risk nor did they show reduced neural tracking of risk than their non-delinquent counterparts, but did show greater neural tracking of reward. This suggests that reward processing may be one key neural process associated with delinquent youths’ decision-making in risky contexts. Examining how these neuropsychological mechanisms interact in delinquent behavior could help make treatments more efficacious and improve our ability to minimize delinquency among youth.

Contributor Information

Natasha Duell, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27514, United States; Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27514, United States.

Michael T Perino, Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis, Missouri 63110, United States.

Ethan M McCormick, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden 2333 AK, The Netherlands.

Eva H Telzer, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27514, United States.

Data Availability

The data underlying this article will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Funding

This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (T32-HD07376 to N.D. and K99HD105002 to M.T.P.) and partially supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (NSF SES 1459719 to E.H.T.), the National Institute of Drug Abuse (R01DA039923 to E.H.T.) and generous funds from the Department of Psychology at the University of Illinois to E.H.T.

Conflict of interest

The authors declared that they had no conflict of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

References

  1. Bacon A.M., Corr P.J., Satchell L.P. (2018). A reinforcement sensitivity theory explanation of antisocial behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 123, 87–93. [Google Scholar]
  2. Barkley-Levenson E., Galvan A. (2014). Neural representation of expected value in the adolescent brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 1646–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bjork J.M., Chen G., Smith A.R., Hommer D.W. (2010). Incentive-elicited mesolimbic activation and externalizing symptomatology in adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 827–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bongers I.L., Koot H.M., van der Ende J., Verhulst F.C. (2008). Predicting young adult social functioning from developmental trajectories of externalizing behavior. Psychological Medicine, 38, 989–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brieant A., Peviani K.M., Lee J.E., King-Casas B., Kim-Spoon J. (2020). Socioeconomic risk for adolescent cognitive control and emerging risk taking behaviors. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 31, 71–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Byrd A.L., Loeber R., Pardini D.A. (2014). Antisocial behavior, psychopathic features and abnormalities in reward and punishment processing. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 17, 125–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Castellanos-Ryan N., Rubia K., Conrod P.J. (2011). Response inhibition and reward response bias mediate the predictive relationship between impulsivity and sensation seeking and common and unique variance in conduct disorder and substance misuse. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 35, 140–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cook E.C., Pflieger J.C., Connell A.M., Connell C.M. (2015). Do specific transitional patterns of antisocial behavior during adolescence increase risk for problems in young adulthood? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 95–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. DeLisi M., Kosloski A., Sween M., Hachmeister E., Moore M., Drury A.J. (2010). Murder by numbers: monetary costs imposed by a sample of homicide offenders. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 21, 501–13. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dosenbach N.U., Fair D.A., Cohen A.L., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 99–105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Drury A.J., DeLisi M., Elbert M.J. (2019). What becomes of chronic juvenile delinquents? Multifinality at midlife. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 18, 119–34. [Google Scholar]
  12. Du Y. (2019). Developing an integrated biosocial theory to understand juvenile delinquency: from social, cognitive, affective, and moral (SCAM) perspectives. International Journal of Contemporary Pediatrics, 6, 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  13. Eftekhari A., Turner A.P., Larimer M.E. (2004). Anger expression, coping, and substance use in adolescent offenders. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 1001–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Eldar E., Rutledge R.B., Dolan R.J., Niv Y. (2016). Mood as representation of momentum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 15–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Frank D.W., Costa V.D., Averbeck B.B., Sabatinelli D. (2019). Directional interconnectivity of the human amygdala, fusiform gyrus, and orbitofrontal cortex in emotional scene perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 122, 1530–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Frick P.J., Cornell A.H., Bodin S.D., Dane H.E., Barry C.T., Loney B.R. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and developmental pathways to severe conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 246–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Frick P.J., Viding E. (2009). Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 1111–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gonzalez M.Z., Allen J.P., Coan J.A. (2016). Lower neighborhood quality in adolescence predicts higher mesolimbic sensitivity to reward anticipation in adulthood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 48–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gonzalez-Gadea M.L., Herrera E., Parra M., et al. (2014). Emotion recognition and cognitive empathy deficits in adolescent offenders revealed by context-sensitive tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gorenstein E.E., Newman J.P. (1980). Disinhibitory psychopathology: a new perspective and model for research. Psychological Review, 87, 301–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gorgolewski K.J., Varoquaux G., Rivera G., et al. (2015). NeuroVault.org: a web-based repository for collecting and sharing unthresholded statistical maps of the human brain. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 9, 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Graham A.M., Marr M., Buss C., Sullivan E.L., Fair D.A. (2021). Understanding vulnerability and adaptation in early brain development using network neuroscience. Trends in Neurosciences, 44, 276–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hariri A.R., Brown S.M., Williamson D.E., Flory J.D., de Wit H., Manuck S.B. (2006). Preference for immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 13213–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hawes S.W., Waller R., Byrd A.L., et al. (2021). Reward processing in children with disruptive behavior disorders and callous-unemotional traits in the ABCD study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 333–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Holz N.E., Boecker-Schlier R., Buchmann A.F., et al. (2017). Ventral striatum and amygdala activity as convergence sites for early adversity and conduct disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 261–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Humphreys K.L., Galan C.A., Tottenham N., Lee S.S. (2016). Impaired social decision-making mediates the association between ADHD and social problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Iidaka T. (2013). Role of the fusiform gyrus and superior temporal sulcus in face perception and recognition: an empirical review. Japanese Psychological Research, 56, 33–45. [Google Scholar]
  28. Knutson B., Adams C.M., Fong G.W., Hommer D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21, RC159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lane S.D., Cherek D.R. (2001). Risk taking by adolescents with maladaptive behavior histories. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 9, 74–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lejuez C.W., Read J.P., Kahler C.W., et al. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 8, 75–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Luna B. (2012). The relevance of immaturities in the juvenile brain to culpability and rehabilitation. Hastings Law Journal, 63, 1469–86. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Matthys W., Vanderschuren L.J.M.J., Schutter D.J.L.G. (2013). The neurobiology of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: altered functioning in three mental domains. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 193–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Matthys W., Van Goozen S.H, Snoek H., Van Engeland H. (2004). Response perseveration and sensitivity to reward and punishment in boys with oppositional defiant disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 362–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McClure S.M., York M.K., Montague P.R. (2004). The neural substrates of reward processing in humans: the modern role of fMRI. The Neuroscientist, 10, 260–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McCormick E.M., Perino M.T., Telzer E.H. (2018). Not just social sensitivity: adolescent neural suppression of social feedback during risk taking. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 134–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Moffitt T.E., Caspi A. (2001). Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 355–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Morales A.M., Jones S.A., Ehlers A., Lavine J.B., Nagel B.J. (2018). Ventral striatal response during decision making involving risk and reward is associated with future binge drinking in adolescents. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43, 1884–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Piquero A.R., Farrington D.P., Jennings W.G. (2018). Money now, money later: linking time discounting and criminal convictions in the Cambridge study in delinquent development. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 62, 1131–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Richter J., Krecklow B., Eisermann M. (2002). Intercorrelations between temperament, character and parental rearing among delinquent adolescents: a cross-validation. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43, 210–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Roose A., Bijttebier P., Claes L., Lilienfeld S.O. (2011). Psychopathic traits in adolescence: associations with the revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory systems. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 201–5. [Google Scholar]
  41. Rosen M.L., Sheridan M.A., Sambrook K.A., et al. (2017). Salience network response to changes in emotional expressions of others is heightened during early adolescence: relevance for social functioning. Developmental Science, 21, e12571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rutter M., Kim-Cohen J., Maughan B. (2006). Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 276–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schreuders E., Braams B.R., Blankenstein N.E., Peper J.S., Güroglu B., Crone E.A. (2018). Contributions of reward sensitivity to ventral striatum activity across adolescence and early adulthood. Child Development, 89, 797–810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Shulman E.P., Smith A.R., Silva K., Icenogle G., Duell N., Chein J, Steinberg L. (2016). The dual systems model: Review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 103–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Steinberg L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Telzer E.H., Fuligni A.J., Lieberman M.D., Miernicki M.E., Galvan A. (2015). The quality of adolescents’ peer relationships modulates neural sensitivity to risk taking. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 389–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Van Hoorn J., McCormick E.M., Telzer E.H. (2018). Moderate social sensitivity in a risky context supports adaptive decision making in adolescence: evidence from brain and behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13, 546–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Van Leijenhorst L., Moor B.G., Op de Macks Z.A., Rombouts S.A.R.B., Westenberg P.M., Crone E.A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision-making: neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. NeuroImage, 51, 345–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Vickery T.J., Jiang Y.V. (2009). Inferior parietal lobule supports decision making under uncertainty in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 916–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ward B.D. (2000). Simultaneous inference for fMRI data. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data underlying this article will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author.


Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES