Summary
Working memory requires maintenance of and executive control over task-relevant information on a timescale of seconds. Spatial working memory depends on interactions between hippocampus, for the representation of space, and prefrontal cortex, for executive control. A monosynaptic hippocampal projection to the prefrontal cortex has been proposed to serve this interaction. However, connectivity and inactivation experiments indicate a critical role of the nucleus reuniens in hippocampal-prefrontal communication. We have investigated the dynamics of oscillatory coherence throughout the prefrontal-hippocampal-reuniens network in a touchscreen-based working memory task. We found that coherence at distinct frequencies evolved depending on phase and difficulty of the task. During choice, the reuniens did not participate in enhanced prefrontal-hippocampal theta but in gamma coherence. Strikingly, the reuniens was strongly embedded in performance-related increases in beta coherence, suggesting the execution of top-down control. In addition, we show that during working memory maintenance the prefrontal-hippocampal-reuniens network displays performance-related delay activity.
Subject areas: Neuroscience, Behavioral neuroscience, Cognitive neuroscience
Graphical abstract
Highlights
-
•
A touchscreen-based working memory task allows temporal separation of memory phases
-
•
Oscillatory synchronization of mPFC, nRe, and CA1 changes with phase and difficulty
-
•
During choice, the nRe participates in beta and gamma but not theta synchronization
-
•
All structures engage in performance-related delay activity during memory maintenance
Neuroscience; Behavioral neuroscience; Cognitive neuroscience
Introduction
Working memory (WM) can be defined as a short-lasting online memory buffer which holds information that is relevant to an ongoing task and is thought to rely on two parallel processes: online maintenance of information and its executive control.1,2 WM is critical not only for high-performance intellectual abilities but essential for accomplishing everyday life. Accordingly, impairment of WM as occurs with chronic stress or as core cognitive symptom in psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia or depression represents a major debilitating factor.3,4,5,6,7 In mammals WM critically depends on the prefrontal cortex (PFC). However, this brain region does not act alone but rather relies on functional connectivity with other brain regions for the execution of WM.3 In spatial WM - one of the best studied forms of WM - inactivation studies have shown that both the PFC and the hippocampus (HC) and ultimately the functional interaction between these brain regions are required.8,9,10,11,12 Consistent with these findings, recordings of neuronal activity during WM tasks have revealed enhanced oscillatory coherence of the HC and medial PFC (mPFC) at different frequency bands, which in turn correlates with task performance.13,14,15,16,17,18,19 Along these lines it has been shown that diseases and interventions that compromise hippocampal-prefrontal connectivity and coherence also impair WM performance.17,18,19,20,21
Regarding the anatomical substrates of HC-PFC interactions, presumably the best studied connection is a direct, mono-synaptic projection from the ventral hippocampal CA1 region (vCA1) to the medial PFC.22 The direction of this projection is in agreement with the direction of information flow during WM-related increases in theta coherence between these brain regions as computed from neuronal recordings13,14,15,16,17 (but see18). However, in addition to this mono-synaptic projection, the HC and PFC are also connected via poly-synaptic pathways.23,24 The most prominent of these is relayed through the nucleus reuniens of the thalamus - the largest of the midline thalamic nuclei (nRE; for review see23,24,25). Here, projection neurons in the deep layers of mPFC directly synapse onto nRE neurons which project to stratum lacunosum-moleculare of the vCA1 region to synapse onto distal dendrites of excitatory and inhibitory neurons providing a strong thalamic input to the HC.26,27,28,29,30,31,32 However, the nRE also receives dense afferents from vCA1/Subiculum and sends strong efferents to superficial and deep layers of the pre- and infralimbic cortices of the mPFC.28,33,34 The nRE has thus been suggested to serve as a central hub in bidirectional HC-mPFC communication, a notion that is supported by functional studies that showed reduced mPFC-CA1 synchrony after nRE inactivation and behavioral studies which showed that nRE inactivation affects especially those behaviors, which depend on prefrontal-hippocampal communication including WM.35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44
In the current study we have investigated the dynamics of interregional communication in the mPFC-nRE-vCA1 network by analyzing the coherence of local field potentials (LFPs) across the three nodes at different stages of a touch-screen based WM task. We found that communication within the network occurs at distinct frequencies depending on the immediate cognitive demand at hand and its difficulty. In particular we report performance related increases in mPFC-nRE-vCA1 beta coherence during decision periods as well as performance-related oscillatory delay activity during WM maintenance.
Results
Implementation of a touch-screen-based working memory task
WM is commonly analyzed in delayed non-match to sample (DNMTS) paradigms, where a subject after a delay period has to choose the stimulus that has not previously been presented.45 In rodents such DNMTS tasks are predominantly implemented in T-mazes, which require little training and achieve high performance levels even with prolonged delay intervals. Alternatively, more demanding two-lever DNMTS boxes have been used.46 However, in both behavioral paradigms the animal only has a left and a right option in the choice run and could thus generate an action plan already in the sample run, making it difficult to gauge the impact and temporal structure of WM, in particular as confounding motor strategies (e.g., positioning head or body toward the opposite side after the sample run) may be applied (see also18,47,48). To overcome these difficulties, we have implemented a DNMTS protocol in automated mouse touch screen chambers47 (Figure 1A), which is more similar to WM tests in primates and puts particular strain on hippocampal-prefrontal communication.49,50 In this task mice have to respond by touch to an illuminated sample at a random (five options) screen location during a sample phase (encoding). This sample touch is rewarded at the opposite side of the conditioning chamber. After a delay interval of initially 2 s (maintenance), the sample and a novel random test location are illuminated and the animal needs to respond to the novel location (choice), which is rewarded (Figure 1A). As the animals have to visit the opposite side of the chamber after the sample touch and the test location can then be illuminated on either side of the sample location, the risk of post-sample action planning and confounding motor strategies is reduced. Accordingly, the correct choice has to be made based on WM of the sample location, which in turn permits a more reliable temporal separation of WM phases. Training the animals with a maintenance period of 2 s with one training session of 30 min per day we found that 76% of mice were capable of learning the task to criterion (>70% accuracy for three consecutive days) in a maximum of 30 sessions (average: 20 ± 1.7 sessions; Figure 1B).
To investigate communication in the mPFC-nRE-vCA1 network (Figure 1C) during the task, mice performing stably above threshold were implanted with electrodes bilaterally in the mPFC (prelimbic cortex), the nRe and the vCA1 (near stratum lacunosum-moleculare) for recordings of LFPs (Figures 1C–1E and S1). After recovery, mice were initially recorded in a simple response task, in which they had to touch a single illuminated location upon its first appearance to receive a reward (non-WM: must-touch task). Subsequently they were recorded in the full WM-task. The time line of the entire experimental protocol and individual recording trials in the must-touch and WM-task are illustrated in Figure S2. To probe for WM-related activity across the mPFC-nRE-vCA1 network we first compared power and phase coherence of LFPs in the last 2 s of the choice phase of the WM-task against the last 2 s in the must-touch task. The only difference between these two settings was that in the WM-task the mouse had to choose between two illuminated locations based on WM, whereas in the must-touch task it had to simply touch a single illuminated location without requirement for WM (Figure S2).
Spectral analysis indicated differences in power, coherence and phase relations between WM and non-WM tasks in specific frequency bands (Figure S3). Accordingly, for subsequent analysis we focused on the core frequencies of 7–9 Hz, 19–30 Hz and 40–50 Hz, which lay in the theta, beta and gamma frequency range, respectively, to analyze differences between the tasks.
Theta coherence increases with WM between vCA1 and mPFC but does not involve the nRE
It has been reported that theta oscillations play a prominent role in hippocampal-prefrontal communication during choice phases in T-maze WM tasks.14,15,17,51 We thus analyzed whether this frequency band was also relevant in our touch screen-based paradigm. In accordance with previous reports we found prominent oscillatory activity in the theta range (peak frequency of about 8 Hz) in vCA1 and mPFC14,15,17,51 but also the nRE (Figures 1F and S3A). Since running speed is known to influence the magnitude of hippocampal theta oscillations, we tested whether running affected vCA1 theta power in our touch-screen paradigm by correlating speed and theta power of individual animals in the must touch and DNMTS task on a trial to trial basis. We found no correlation (must touch: r = 0.110, p = 0.258, DNMTS: r = 0.099, p = 0.311, n = 107 trials each, 6 mice), suggesting that running speed had no major effect on theta power in our task. In addition, running speed did not differ in must touch and DNMTS tasks (t(4) = -1.998, p = 0.116). When we compared theta power between non-WM must touch and WM-choice phases, we found that power was lower during WM in all three regions, suggesting that local activity at theta frequency was stronger in the must-touch task (Figure 1F; mPFC t(5) = 6.522, p = 0.001; nRE t(5) = 3.361, p = 0.020; vCA1 t(4) = 2.484, p = 0.068). However, when we analyzed inter-regional coherence as a measure of information flow between regions,52 we found that coherence at theta frequency was specifically enhanced between mPFC and vCA1 during WM choice (mPFC-vCA1 t(3) = 3.249, p = 0.048); nRE- CA1 t(2) = 0.024, p = 0.98; mPFC-nRE t(5) = 0.822, p = 0.45) (Figures 1G and S3B), consistent with previous data from T-maze experiments.14,15,17,18,24,51 In contrast, theta coherence between mPFC and nRE or nRE and vCA1 did not differ significantly between must-touch task and WM choice phase (Figures 1G and S3B). Theta coherence between vCA1 and mPFC correlated significantly with choice accuracy during WM (r(6) = 0.83, p = 0.01; Figure 1I), suggesting that mPFC-vCA1 theta coherence is linked to WM performance not only in T-maze14,15,17 (but see53) but also in our touch screen tasks.
Phase shift analysis suggested that mPFC theta was shifted against vCA1 theta by about 90° (Figures 1H and S3C; see also15,16,24). Similarly, the nRE was shifted against vCA1 (Figures 1H and S3).
Together these data show that theta oscillations are prominent in vCA1, mPFC and nRE with high coherence between the regions. During the choice phase of WM, we find a specific and performance related increase in theta coherence between vCA1 and mPFC. The nRE does not participate in the enhanced theta coherence, suggesting that during choice, theta-related information flow between vCA1 and mPFC does not involve the nRE.
Beta and gamma coherence in the mPFC-nRE-vCA1 network increase during WM choice
Similar to theta oscillations, also power in the beta range was lower in all three brain regions during the choice phase of WM than during the non-WM must-touch-task, although this reduction was significant only for the mPFC (Figures 1F and S3A; mPFC t(5) = 2.952, p = 0.032; nRE t(5) = 2.047, p = 0.096; vCA1 t(4) = 2.717 p = 0.053). Remarkably, coherence between all structures in the network was significantly increased in this frequency range (Figures 1G and S3B; mPFC-vCA1 t(3) = 3.304, p = 0.046; nRE-vCA1 t(2) = 11.980, p = 0.007; nRE-mPFC t(5) = 4.339, p = 0.007). Similar increases in beta coherence have previously been reported for simultaneous recordings from mPFC and mediodorsal thalamus (MD) as well as mPFC and dorsal CA1 during WM.53,54,55 The phase of beta oscillations in mPFC and nRE was shifted against vCA1 and nRE phase was shifted against mPFC with a slight increase in shift during choice compared to must-touch (nRE-mPFC t(10) = 2.253, p = 0.048; unpaired t-test) (Figures 1H and S3C).
In the gamma range, power was not different between non-WM must-touch and WM-choice tasks in any of the three structures (Figures 1F and S3A). However, gamma coherence showed highly significant increases during choice compared to must-touch between mPFC and vCA1 (Figures 1G and S3B; mPFC-vCA1 t(3) = 13.760, p = 0.0008), similar to changes reported from T-maze DNMTS tasks.18,19 In addition we found marginal increases in gamma coherence between nRE and vCA1 (Figure 1G; nRE-vCA1 t(2) = 3.272, p = 0.082). Interestingly, these increases in coherence were accompanied by changes in phase relations with significant reductions in phase difference between mPFC and vCA1 (vCA1 leading, t(6) = 2.579, p = 0.042), and even a change from positive during must-touch to negative during choice between nRE and vCA1(t(4) = 3.206, p = 0.033) (Figures 1H and S3C).
Together, these data show enhanced coherence within the entire mPFC-nRE-vCA1 network during the choice phase of WM at beta frequency, and between mPFC and vCA1 as well as nRE and vCA1 in the gamma range with a change in phase relations between nRE and vCA1 as well as mPFC and vCA1 at this frequency. These findings indicate that the nRE participates in hippocampal-prefrontal communication at beta and gamma frequency during the choice phase of WM.
Phase coherence between nRE and vCA1 during choice increases with task difficulty and correlates with WM performance
To probe whether increased task demands would be reflected by changes in network activity, we extended the delay period from 2 s to 4 and 6 s (D2, D4 and D6). This required the animals to hold in mind the sample position for a prolonged period (maintenance) and thus increased task difficulty.19,56 As expected, choice accuracy decreased significantly with delay duration (Figure 2A; D2: 72.3 ± 0.88%; D4: 63.8 ± 1.48%; D6: 55.7 ± 1.52%; repeated measures Anova (rma): F(1.779, 23) = 50,63, p < 0.0001; post-hoc Dunnett (ph): D2-D4 p = 0.001; D2-D6 p < 0.0001).
With longer delay durations theta power during the choice phase increased significantly in the mPFC (rma: F(2, 24) = 14.63, p < 0.0001; ph: D2-D6 p = 0.004) and nRE (rma: F(2, 24) = 7.048, p < 0.004; ph: D2-D6 p = 0.002), but not the vCA1 (rma: F(2, 20) = 3.263, p = 0.059), suggesting enhanced engagement of mPFC and nRE with enhanced delay (Figure 2B) (see also15,38). However, the degree of phase coherence did not change significantly between regions in this frequency range (Figure 2C).
In the beta range power increased significantly in the mPFC (rma: F(2, 24) = 3.888, p = 0.035; ph: D2-D6 p < 0.0001), showed no significant change in the nRE (rma: F(2,24) = 1.721, p = 0.20) but decreased in vCA1 (rma: F(2, 20) = 4.044, p = 0.034; ph: D2-D4 p = 0.0002, D2-D6 p = 0.0002) (Figure 2B). However, when we tested for interregional coherence in the beta range we found a specific enhancement between the nRE and vCA1 with extended delay periods in the choice phase of the WM task (Figure 2C; rma: interaction F(4, 28) = 5.251, p = 0.003; ph D2-D6 p = 0.009), suggesting that communication between nRE and vCA1 increased during choice with longer delay intervals.
Power in the gamma frequency band was unchanged in the nRE (rma: F(2, 24) = 2.049, p = 0.15), marginally increased in mPFC (rma: F(2, 24) = 2.980, p = 0.07; ph: D2-D6 p = 0.0005) and significantly decreased in vCA1 with prolonged delay intervals (vCA1, rma: interaction F(4, 40) = 4.630, p = 0.004; ph: D2-D4 p < 0.0001, D2-D6 p < 0.0001) (Figure 2B). Remarkably, however, we found significant increases in gamma coherence between both the mPFC and vCA1 (rma: interaction F(4, 28) = 16.02, p < 0.0001; ph: D2-D4 p < 0.0001, D2-D6 p < 0.0001) as well as the nRE and vCA1 (rma: interaction F(4, 28) = 4.166, p = 0.009; ph: D2-D4 p = 0.014, D2-D6 p < 0.0001) (Figure 2C), corroborating our initial findings of enhanced gamma coherence between these structures during WM compared to non-WM tasks. Interestingly, the switch from positive to negative phase shift values in WM vs. non-WM tasks between nRE and vCA1 (Figure S3) was maintained during all delay durations (average phase shift: D2 -45.3° ± 9.5; D4 -40.5° ± 12.3; D6 -25.1° ± 9.2 vs. non-WM task +52.9° ± 20.9).
Since both WM performance as well as interregional coherence changed with the duration of the delay interval, we tested whether changes in interregional coherence correlated with changes in behavioral performance. Interestingly, whereas mPFC-vCA1 coherence was not significantly correlated with performance (r = −0.128, p = 0.55), we found significant negative correlations specifically for nRE-vCA1 coherence in both the beta and gamma frequency range (Figure 2D), relating a high nRE-vCA1 coherence during the choice phase to reduced accuracy. However, since we do not know to which extent this correlation is independent of the delay duration, we cannot conclude a causality.
In summary, these data showed that increased delay intervals induced brain region-specific changes in power with a general trend of enhanced power in mPFC and reduced power in vCA1. In addition, increased WM demands caused a decrease in WM performance and an increase in interregional coherence specifically between mPFC and vCA1 (gamma) as well as nRE and vCA1 (beta and gamma), with the latter showing negative correlation with task performance.
Coherence between mPFC, nRE and vCA1 during maintenance declines over time
Increasing the difficulty of the WM task caused reduced performance and enhanced oscillatory coherence between mPFC, nRE and vCA1 in the choice phase. As task difficulty was solely determined by delay duration, we reasoned that the cause for any changes in performance and network activity during the choice phase must lie in the preceding delay period (memory maintenance; time between sample touch and illumination of the test locations). Accordingly, we compared network activity between the different delay intervals D2, D4 and D6. Basic levels of power and coherence during maintenance were very similar to those during choice for all frequency ranges we analyzed. Also, the increase in power with delay durations were similar to those during choice (Figure S4).
However, when we analyzed interregional phase coherence for different delay durations, we found marked differences between maintenance and retrieval periods. In particular, phase coherence between mPFC and vCA1 was significantly reduced during longer delay intervals in all frequency bands (mPFC-vCA1: theta, rma: interaction F(4, 28) = 8.205, p = 0.0002; ph D2-D4 p = 0.005, D2-D6 p < 0.0001; beta, rma: interaction F(4, 28) = 8.631, p = 0.0001; ph D2-D4 p = 0.0002, D2-D6 p < 0.0001; gamma, ph: D2-D4 p = 0.0008, D2-D6 p = 0.0003) (Figure 3A). However, reduced phase coherence was also observed between nRE and vCA1 in the beta (rma: interaction F(4, 28) = 8.631, p = 0.0001; ph D2-D4 p = 0.0002, D2-D6 p < 0.0001) as well as mPFC and nRE in the theta range (rma: interaction F(4, 44) = 5.676, p = 0.0009; ph D2-D4 p = 0.0003, D2-D6 p < 0.0001) (Figures 3B and 3C, respectively).
As the general procedure of the WM task differed only in the duration of the delay period, we hypothesized that the deterioration in coherence seen in D4 and D6 must occur during the extra time of the delay period. Indeed, we found that theta and beta coherences between mPFC and vCA1 during the first 2 s of D4 and D6 did not differ from D2 (theta: D2 0.19 ± 0.026; 1st bin D4 0.20 ± 0.028; 1st bin D6 0.21 ± 0.027; beta: D2 0.10 ± 0.013; 1st bin D4 0.09 ± 0.011; 1st bin D6 0.10 ± 0.012), but then decreased for D4 and D6 in subsequent 2 s bins (theta D4 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.016; D6 rma: F(1.5, 10.6) = 14.56, p = 0.002; ph 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.021, 1st vs. 3rd bin p = 0.001; beta D4 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.011; D6 rma: F(1.3, 9.2) = 6.8, p = 0.02; ph 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.062, 1st vs. 3rd bin p = 0.032; Figure 4), whereas gamma coherence did not change (p > 0.05 for all bins). Similarly, theta and beta coherence between mPFC-nRE and nRE-vCA1, respectively, was not different during the first 2 s (mPFC-nRE theta: D2 0.70 ± 0.019; 1st bin D4 0.68 ± 0.019; 1st bin D6 0.69 ± 0.017; beta: D2 0.48 ± 0.026; 1st bin D4 0.47 ± 0.020; 1st bin D6 0.49 ± 0.019; nRE-vCA1 theta: D2 0.26 ± 0.023; 1st bin D4 0.27 ± 0.028; 1st bin D6 0.31 ± 0.029; beta: D2 0.09 ± 0.009; 1st bin D4 0.09 ± 0.009; 1st bin D6 0.11 ± 0.012) but then declined for D4 and D6 in subsequent 2 s bins (mPFC-nRE theta D4 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.049; D6 rma: F(1.9, 21.6) = 17.12, p < 0.0001; ph 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.012, 1st vs. 3nd bin p = 0.0001; beta D4 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.67(ns); D6 rma: F(1.96, 21.6) = 13.06, p = 0.0002; ph 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.67(ns), 1st vs. 3rd bin p = 0.0006; nRE-vCA1 theta no significant changes, p > 0.05 for all bins; beta D4 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.010; D6 rma: F(1.6, 11.2) = 5.8, p = 0.02; ph 1st vs. 2nd bin p = 0.076, 1st vs. 3rd bin p = 0.051) (Figure S5). Accordingly, communication within the mPFC-CA1-nRE network at theta and beta frequency declined significantly over prolonged delay periods most prominently between mPFC and vCA1.
Interestingly, coherence between mPFC and vCA1 during the delay period positively correlated with WM performance, with animals showing high coherence in the beta range during maintenance performing better in the subsequent choice phase (Figure 4D).
Communication was thus enhanced between mPFC, nRE and vCA1 not only during retrieval but also during maintenance of WM. This interregional coherence, however, deteriorated significantly over time when the delay phase was extended, which in turn coincided with reduced WM performance in the choice phase. Although the correlation of coherence and performance is indirect, it is tempting to speculate that a decline in coherence during maintenance may underlie the drop in subsequent WM performance.
Discussion
The nRE is a major bidirectional relay for information transfer between PFC and HC. This pathway forms a main route for top-down information flow from mPFC to CA1 and at the same time enables data transmission in the opposite hippocampal-prefrontal direction. As the PFC also receives a direct monosynaptic projection from vCA1 the nRE serves as a connector to complete and complement a partly bidirectional hippocampal-prefrontal-thalamic- loop (see Figure 1C). In accordance with its connectivity, inactivation of the nRE disrupts behaviors dependent on hippocampal-prefrontal communication such as WM.35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44 Here we show that neural dynamics and inter-regional communication within this network change with the WM phase and differentially correlate with behavioral performance.
To probe vCA1-mPFC-nRE network dynamics we have used multi-site LFP recordings in a touchscreen-based WM paradigm, which shares some similarity with visuo-spatial WM tests used in primates.49,50 Compared to traditional T-maze tasks this paradigm was significantly more difficult. Mice required more training, tolerated only short delay periods and achieved lower rates of correct responses. On the other hand, this task was presumably less vulnerable toward confounding motor strategies and thus permitted a good temporal separation of the WM-task into different cognitive processes (encoding, maintenance, retrieval). Although oscillatory coupling as measured by coherence of LFPs provides excellent insights into the dynamics of inter-regional communication, it has limitations.52,57 Without evidence that the firing of nearby neurons is modulated with the LFP, its local origin is not proven.57,58 In addition, the measure of oscillatory coherence between brain regions, which are bidirectionally connected via multiple pathways and receive common input from several un-recorded brain regions23 does not permit conclusions about the directionality of interactions or the role of connected but unobserved brain regions. Bearing these limitations in mind we report that despite differences in task design, our LFP recordings revealed strong similarities to dorsal CA1-mPFC network dynamics14,15,17,55,59 as well as vCA1-mPFC network dynamics17,51,59 reported previously for rodents in DNMTSs in T-mazes or two-lever boxes. Most prominently, we found vCA1-mPFC theta coherence to increase in the WM task compared to a non-WM task. Relating coherence levels to choice accuracy, we found that vCA1-mPFC theta coherence correlated with WM performance13,14,15,17,38 (but see53). In addition, vCA1-mPFC gamma coherence, which has previously been implicated in different phases of T-maze DNMTS tasks,18,19,38,53 was enhanced during choice. As direct anatomical connectivity between vCA1 and mPFC is one-way we estimated directionality of information flow using phase lag analysis. Calculated phase lags were compatible with the prevailing view, that CA1 leads the mPFC at theta and gamma frequency.14,15,16,17,18,24,38 However, due to the complex interconnectivity of vCA1 and mPFC23 such analysis has to be treated with caution.57 Accordingly, the exact type of information transmitted at these frequencies is not known but both theta and gamma have generally been proposed to serve the transfer of spatial information from HC to mPFC.2,18,24
Beyond the established changes in CA1-mPFC theta and gamma coherence, our experiments have exposed significant increases in beta coherence between mPFC, vCA1 and nRE during WM. Beta oscillations have originally been associated with sensorimotor processing. As they are prominent throughout the motor system during holding periods but are attenuated during movements and as they occur in sensory areas before and after stimulus onset but disappear during stimulus presentation, beta oscillations have been suggested to serve the maintenance of the current sensorimotor set, or “status quo”.60,61 However, in analogy to a role in sensorimotor processing beta oscillations have recently been recorded within and between brain regions engaged in WM tasks in primates62,63,64 and mice,53,55 where they may serve the maintenance of a “cognitive set”.60,61 Specifically, beta oscillations have been proposed to serve the top-down control of neural ensembles, by shielding the network from irrelevant information to help the application of task rules.2,61,65 Interestingly, enhanced beta coherence has been suggested to evolve in thalamo-cortical loops2 and has recently been recorded between the mediodorsal thalamus and the mPFC during a T-maze WM task in mice54 as well as between nRE and dorsal CA1 during a long-term odor sequence memory task in rats.66 Inactivation studies strongly argue for a role of the nRE in mediating not only top-down mPFC to HC executive control but also prefrontal-hippocampal coherence at beta frequency.67,68,69,70 Our data suggest that top-down control via the nRE may occur at beta frequency.
Taken together we thus hypothesize that within the mPFC-nRE-vCA1 network investigated here, theta and gamma oscillations may carry task relevant bottom-up spatial information, whereas beta oscillations may rather be related to the executive control over this information and may help to attend to relevant information. Reliable information about the direction of information flow in the different frequency bands would help to better estimate the nature of the information conveyed. However, the dense interconnectivity of the three brain regions and their common input from multiple other brain regions, preclude a meaningful analysis of directionality.57 Similarly, we cannot gauge the influence of common input on changes in coherence within the mPFC-nRE-vCA1 network. Such influence has previously been reported for the supramamillary nucleus as well as the cerebellar lobulus simplex.55,71,72 The question of directionality and the role of common input will thus have to be addressed in future studies possibly by combining LFP and unit recordings with rapid pathway selective interventions.18 Regarding the roles of ventral versus dorsal CA1 in hippocampal-prefrontal synchronization, previous studies have reported similar WM-related changes in oscillatory coherence with the mPFC for both regions.14,15,17,18,51,53,55,59 Since the ventral CA1 sends much stronger projections to the mPFC than the dorsal CA1 and since reciprocal connectivity with the nRE is also much higher for ventral than for dorsal CA1,23 we propose that vCA1 mediates synchronization of mPFC and dorsal CA1 at different frequencies as also suggested by inactivation experiments.15 However, direct connections between dorsal CA1 and mPFC as well as nRE exist and may contribute to interregional coherence.23,73,74
Besides prominent WM-related beta coherence throughout the mPFC-nRE-vCA1 network our study reports another interesting and previously unappreciated finding: Reduced oscillatory coherence during WM maintenance with extended delay periods, which in turn coincided with a reduction in WM performance in the subsequent choice phase. To our knowledge such distributed oscillatory delay activity and its relevance for WM performance has not previously been reported, possibly due to differences in task designs. This delay activity involved communication between pairs of the three structures and may again serve frequency-specific functions: Whereas theta and gamma oscillations may help to maintain or constantly reactivate neuronal spatial representations, beta oscillations maybe involved in the executive control of these representations to meet the specific task demands.2,65,75,76
Considering the above the observed network dynamics could be explained as follows: the prolongation of the delay interval from 2 to 4 and 6 s caused a decline in oscillatory delay coherence between mPFC, nRE, and vCA1 in all three frequencies, which would have impaired the maintenance of task specific spatial representations leading to reduced task performance. In the subsequent choice phase the “loss” of task related representations would have enhanced the exertion of executive power in the futile attempt to make the correct decision (note: enhanced nRE-vCA1 top down attention conveyed by beta coherence would also be compatible with enhanced bottom up gamma coherence to transmit relevant spatial information2,75,77). This hypothesis is supported by the finding that in the beta range reduced coherence between mPFC and vCA1 during the delay correlated significantly with increased coherence between nRE and vCA1 during the subsequent choice phase (r = −0.851, p = 0.015) and both correlated significantly with behavioral performance (Figures 2 and 4).
In summary and irrespective of content or direction of information flow our findings indicate that inter-area oscillatory coherence between mPFC, nRE and vCA1 at theta, gamma and beta frequency correlates with task performance and dynamically changes throughout the different phases of WM.
Limitations of the study
The analysis of LFP coherence provides comprehensive insights into the dynamics of inter-regional communication. However, LFPs may be influenced by volume conducted potentials that originate elsewhere. In addition, the measure of oscillatory coherence between brain regions, which are connected via multiple pathways and receive common input from un-recorded brain regions does not permit conclusions about directionality or the influence of unobserved brain regions.
STAR★Methods
Key resources table
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Antibodies | ||
rabbit anti-NeuN | AbCam | Cat# AB177487; RRID:AB_2532109 |
mouse anti-GFAP-CY3 | AbCam | Cat# AB49874; RRID:AB_880203 |
goat anti-rabbit 488 | Thermo Fischer | Cat#A11008; RRID:AB_143165 |
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
Paraformaldehyde | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# 441244; CAS: 30525-89-4 |
4% Agar | Roth | Cat# 2266.1 |
Triton X-100 | Fisher BioReagents |
Cat# BP151-500; CAS: 9002-93-1 |
Normal Goat Serum NGS | Capricorn Scientific | Cat# GOA-1A, |
Mowiol | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# 81381; CAS: 9002-89-5 |
Dapi | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# 32670; CAS: 28718-90-3 |
Isoflurane | Baxter | Cat# FDG9623 |
Buprenorphine, Temgesic | Indivior Europe Ltd. Dublin | PZN: 00345928 |
Pentobarbital, Narcoren | Boeringer Ingelheim | 6088986.00.00 |
Deposited data | ||
Data and code used for coherogram analysis and running speed analysis | Mendeley Data | https://doi.org/10.17632/v6fz34k2dy.1 |
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
C57Bl/6J mice | Janvier Labs | RRID: MGI:2670020 |
Software and algorithms | ||
Matlab 2018 | The MathWorks Inc, Massachusetts, USA | RRID:SCR_001622 |
Python 3.11 | https://www.python.org/ | RRID:SCR_008394 |
GraphPad Prism version 9 | GraphPad Software, Boston, USA | RRID:SCR_002798 |
Neuroexplorer version 5 | Nex Technologies, Colorado Springs, USA | RRID:SCR_001818 |
G∗Power - Release 3.1.9.7 | https://www.psychologie.hhu.de/arbeitsgruppen/allgemeine-psychologie-und-arbeitspsychologie/gpower | RRID:SCR_013726 |
ABET II touch software | Campden instruments, Loughborough, UK | Model 89505 |
DAS-32 Cheetah Data Acquisition Software | Neuralynx Inc, Tucson, USA | Cheetah 5.0 version 5.7.4 https://neuralynx.com/software/cheetah |
Other | ||
Epifluorescent microscope | Zeiss | Model Axio Imager M2 |
Glass slides | Roth | Cat #0656 |
Vibratome | Leica | Model VT1200S |
Triangular conditioning chambers | Campden instruments, Loughborough, UK | Model 80614 |
Stereotactic frame | David Kopf Instruments, USA | Model 940 |
Dental Cement | Kulzer | Cat# 64707945 |
Digital Lynx SX acquisition system | Neuralynx Inc, Tucson, USA | Digital Lynx 4SX acquisition system https://neuralynx.com/hardware/digital-lynx-sx |
Electrode interface board | Neuralynx Inc, Tucson, USA | EIB-8; https://neuralynx.com/hardware/eib-8 |
Stainless-steel electrodes (Ø=0.075mm) | Advent research materials, Oxford, UK | Cat# FE6215 |
Resource availability
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Peer Wulff (p.wulff@physiologie.uni-kiel.de).
Materials availability
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Experimental model and study participant details
All experiments were performed in accordance with the German law on animal protection and approved by the Animal Care and Ethics Committee of the Christian Albrecht University, Kiel.
C57Bl/6J mice (RRID: MGI:2670020)
A total of 25 adult male (n=15) and female (n=10) C57Bl/6J mice (RRID: MGI:2670020) were used in this study, of which 14 were used for LFP analysis (9 male, 5 female). Animals were housed in a temperature-controlled environment with a 12-hour light/dark cycle. Two weeks before the start of the behavioral training, animals were housed individually and food intake was measured daily for a week. Thereafter, mice received approximately 80% of daily food intake, allowing the animals after initial weight loss to stabilize at 80-85% of the starting body weight. Animals selected for surgery were taken off food restriction for at least 4 days before the surgery. During the 2-week recovery period, food was available ad libitum.
Method details
Behavioral training
Behavioural training and testing was performed in triangular conditioning chambers (Model 80614, Campden instruments, Loughborough, UK) that were placed in sound- and light attenuating Faraday cubicles. Each chamber contained a house light, tone generator, video camera and LCD touch screen with a liquid reward dispenser in the opposing corner (Figure 1). ABET II touch software (Campden instruments, Loughborough, UK) using custom written protocols was used to control the chambers as well as to record behaviour.
The week before training started, mice were handled and food restricted to reach 80% of their starting body weight. During the entire training course, the animals received one training session of 30 minutes per day. Habituation and pre-training followed published protocols.78 Pretraining consisted of several sessions in which the animals were trained to touch an illuminated field of the screen (5 options: 1 illuminated, 4 dark) in order to receive a reward from the dispenser (8μl strawberry milk). Once the mice were doing so, to further improve performance, touching of a non-illuminated field was punished by bright lighting of the chamber, the sounding of a tone for 1 second and a time out period, delaying the start of the next trial by 5 seconds. During DNMTS training, the mouse was placed in the recording chamber and one out of five possible locations was lit. The trial was initiated when the mouse touched the illuminated sample location, which in turn caused the light to tun off. Touching of the sample location was rewarded in 33% of cases by 5μl of strawberry milk from the dispenser to encourage the mice to move away from the touch screen during the delay and thus prevent behavioral/mediating strategies. Upon sample touch, the delay period was initiated, which for initial DNMTS training was set to 2 seconds. After the delay, the sample location plus one new test location was illuminated. Responding to the test (non-match) location by touch was considered a correct choice and resulted in a reward (5μl strawberry milk). An incorrect choice (either the sample light or an unlit field) was punished by bright lighting of the chamber, the sounding of a tone for 1 second and a time out period of 5 seconds. Combinations of locations were randomized during the entire session to make the response location unpredictable. Once animals reached stable threshold performance at the 2 second delay (70% accuracy for 3 consecutive days), the animal proceeded to electrode implantation. After a 2-week post-surgery recovery period a subgroup of 6 animals was first re-habituated to touching the screen while tethered using a task in which each touch of a lit location was rewarded (3 sessions) before proceeding to the 2 second delay task. All other mice were directly retrained on the 2 second delay task until performing above threshold again (4 days above 70% accuracy, usually reached within a week). Consecutively, the delay duration was increased first to 4 seconds (8 to 12 sessions per animal) and finally to 6 seconds (5 to 10 sessions per animal).
Surgery
Mice were implanted with 6 single stainless-steel electrodes bilaterally in the mPFC, the nRe and the vCA1 (directed at stratum lacunosum-moleculare) for recordings of local field potentials (LFPs). Anesthesia was induced using 3% isoflurane in O2 by inhalation and adjusted between 1.5 and 2% isoflurane throughout surgery. The animals were head fixed in a stereotactic frame (David Kopf Instruments, USA) and body temperature was maintained by a heating mat placed underneath the animal. After exposing the skull, small holes were drilled in the skull and electrodes were implanted bilaterally at fixed coordinates (mPFC 2.2 AP/1.2 LM/ −1.8 DV at a 17° angle; nRE −0.82 AP/ 1.55 LM / −4.5 DV at a 17° angle; vCA1 −3.0 AP/ 3.6 LM / −2.8 DV). A stainless-steel screw was placed on the dura of the hind brain for referencing and grounding. Electrodes were attached to an electrode interface board (Neuralynx Inc, Tucson, USA), which was secured on the skull using dental cement. Mice received an intraperitoneal injection with buprenorphine for post-operative analgesia and were allowed to recover in a warm cage until fully awake. After a recovery time of 2 weeks, the animals were re-trained (see above).
Electrophysiology
Local field potentials were recorded using stainless-steel electrodes (Ø=0.075mm; Advent research materials, Oxford, UK) and a Digital Lynx acquisition system (Neuralynx Inc, Tucson, USA) with Cheetah software (Neuralynx Inc, Tucson, USA) at a sample rate of 1.6 kHz, band-pass filtered for 1-200Hz. In order to synchronize the recording to the behavioral performance, the ABETII software was programmed to send TTL signals to the recording system throughout the trials (at trial start, sample location on, correct touch, etc.). After acquisition, data was analyzed using NeuroExplorer (Nex Technologies, Colorado Springs, USA). The power spectral density for individual behavioral epochs was calculated by single-taper Fourier Fast Transform (FFT; 1-80 Hz; 512 frequency values; single taper- Hann; 50% window overlap). Full spectral power (1-80Hz) was transformed logarithmically to dB for spectral examination or by percentage of total power for statistical analysis. Coherence analysis was performed using the “coherence for continuous” function in NeuroExplorer software, in which the FFTs of signals x and y are calculated, followed by their spectral and cross-spectral densities (; ; . Here is a complex conjugate of z. , and values are averaged across the specified behavioral epochs within each session and coherence values are calculated . Coherence phase values are calculated as the phase of . Due to the 1-200 Hz bandpass filter, data recorded at frequencies below 3 Hz were considered unreliable and not taken into analysis. Based on spectral analysis, target peak frequency bands were selected for further analysis (theta peak 7-9 Hz, beta peak 19-30 Hz, gamma peak 40-50 Hz). Subsequent analysis, averaging and statistics were performed using custom written MATLAB scripts.
Histology and imaging
At the end of the experiment small electrolytic lesions were produced for post-mortem identification of electrode positions. To this end animals were deeply anesthetized by intraperitoneal injection of Pentobarbital (50 mg per 30 g body weight) and 0.5 mA pulses were applied for 5 seconds to all electrodes. Immediately afterwards, animals were transcardially perfused with phosphate buffered saline (PBS, pH 7.4) for 1 minute followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS for 12 minutes (approximately 100ml). Brains were removed, post-fixated in 4% PFA for 4 hours and stored in PBS until further processing. For sectioning, brains were embedded in 4% agar and cut into coronal sections on a Leica VT1200S vibratome (thickness 40μm). Free-floating sections were permeabilized in 0.4% Triton X-100 in PBS for 30 min and blocked in PBS containing 4% normal goat serum (NGS) and 0.2% Triton X-100 for 30 min at room temperature. Primary antibodies (rabbit anti-NeuN, 1:1000, AbCam, and mouse anti-GFAP-CY3 conjugated, 1:750, AbCam) were incubated with sections overnight at 4°C in PBS, 2% NGS, 0.1% Triton X-100. Sections were washed three times for 10 min in PBS, 1% NGS at room temperature and incubated with goat anti-rabbit 488 (1:1000, Thermo Fischer) as secondary antibody for 3 hours at room temperature. Sections were then washed once in PBS containing 1% NGS and twice in PBS alone for 10 min. Sections were mounted onto glass slides (Roth) and cover-slipped using Mowiol (Sigma) containing dapi (1:500, Honeywell Fluka). Sections were imaged on a Zeiss Axio Imager M2 epifluorescent microscope with a 10x objective to identify locations of electrolytic lesions.
Quantification and statistical analysis
Behavioral data was analyzed per session per animal using ABETII software (Campden instruments, Loughborough, UK). Performance was averaged per animal over all sessions within a specific delay interval (D2, D4, D6) and consecutively averaged over animals per delay interval. Animals that were unable to perform the task to threshold at a 2 second delay interval were taken out of the analysis.
For electrophysiological data, only data from animals that were able to perform the task to threshold at a 2 second delay interval post-surgery was used. Data from correct trials only was filtered and analyzed per animal per session using Neuroexplorer (Nex Technologies, Colorado Springs, USA). Values were imported into MATLAB for further processing and averaging. Electrodes showing poor quality recordings and electrodes outside the target areas were removed from analysis. Since recordings from both hemispheres showed no obvious differences regarding changes in power and coherence during the task, we averaged power values from bilateral electrodes and coherence values from bilateral electrode pairs (e.g. left mPFC-left vCA1, right mPFC-right vCA1) within animal per session. Data then was averaged per animal over all sessions within a specific delay interval (D2, D4, D6) and consecutively averaged over animals per delay interval.
Final group sizes for LPF recordings in the DNMTS task with increased delay durations were mPFC n=13, nRE n=13, vCA1 n=11 (power analysis) and mPFC-vCA1 n=8, nRE-vCA1 n=8, nRE-mPFC n=12 (coherence analysis). Final group sizes of LFP recordings in the subgroup of animals in the WM choice vs must touch task (Figures 1 and S3) were mPFC n=6, nRE n=6, vCA1 n=5 (power analysis) and mPFC-vCA1 n=4, nRE-vCA1 n=3, nRE-mPFC n=6 (coherence analysis).
For correlation analysis of running speed and vCA1 theta power, running speed was determined for the last two seconds before touch in must touch (n=107 trials) and WM-choice phases (n=107 trials) in 6 animals using DeepLabCut79 and correlated with vCA1 theta power on a trial by trial basis. The number of trials needed for a moderate correlation (r =0.3)80 to reach significance was calculated with G-power81 (effect size p=0.3, alpha=0.05, power= 9). The trials that were included in the analysis of running speed and theta power were randomly drawn from the recorded trials.
For statistical analysis we have used GraphPad Prism version 9. Data was tested for normality using a Shapiro-Wilk test and analyzed for significance using 2-way-ANOVA (repeated measures, rma; factors delay duration and trial epoch) with a post-hoc Dunnett’s test (ph) for multiple comparisons or paired-sample T-test as appropriate. For correlation analysis, Pearson's correlation coefficient was calculated and linear regression was performed. A p-value equal to or less than 0.05 was considered significant. Unless stated otherwise, all data are shown as mean ± SEM. In figures bars represent means, bars with error bars refer to means ± SEMs; solid lines represent means, shaded areas represent SEMs; dots represent individual data points.
Acknowledgments
We thank Kerstin Kronenbitter for excellent technical assistance. Our work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) FOR 2143, RTG 2154 and SFB 1461 (Project-ID 434434223) to P.W.
Author contributions
Conceptualization, P.W. and J.G.M.; Methodology, P.W., J.G.M., T.Si., and T.Sc.; Formal Analysis, J.G.M. and N.R.; Investigation, J.G.M., A.B., and T.Sc.; Writing, P.W., J.G.M., and N.R.; Visualization, P.W., J.G.M., and N.R.; Supervision, Project Administration, and Funding Acquisition, P.W.
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Inclusion and diversity
We support inclusive, diverse, and equitable conduct of research.
Published: August 3, 2023
Footnotes
Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2023.107532.
Contributor Information
Johanne Gertrude de Mooij-van Malsen, Email: a.de.mooij@vu.nl.
Peer Wulff, Email: p.wulff@physiologie.uni-kiel.de.
Supplemental information
Data and code availability
-
•
Local field potential data reported in this paper will be shared by the lead contact upon request.
-
•
Data and code used for the coherogram analysis and the analysis of running speed were deposited on Mendeley at https://doi.org/10.17632/v6fz34k2dy.1.
-
•
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
References
- 1.Baddeley A. Working memory. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:R136–R140. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Miller E.K., Lundqvist M., Bastos A.M. Working Memory 2.0. Neuron. 2018;100:463–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fuster J.M. The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update. Neuron. 2001;30:319–333. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00285-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gabrieli J.D.E., Singh J., Stebbins G.T., Goetz C.G. Reduced working memory span in Parkinson’s disease: Evidence for the role of frontostriatal system in working and strategic memory. Neuropsychology. 1996;10:322–332. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.10.3.321. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Girotti M., Adler S.M., Bulin S.E., Fucich E.A., Paredes D., Morilak D.A. Prefrontal cortex executive processes affected by stress in health and disease. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2018;85:161–179. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Nikolin S., Tan Y.Y., Schwaab A., Moffa A., Loo C.K., Martin D. An investigation of working memory deficits in depression using the n-back task: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 2021;284:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.01.084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Barch D.M., Ceaser A. Cognition in schizophrenia: core psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012;16:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Floresco S.B., Seamans J.K., Phillips A.G. Selective roles for hippocampal, prefrontal cortical, and ventral striatal circuits in radial-arm maze tasks with or without a delay. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:1880–1890. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-05-01880.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kesner R.P., Hunt M.E., Williams J.M., Long J.M. Prefrontal Cortex and Working Memory for Spatial Response, Spatial Location, and Visual Object Information in the Rat. Cereb. Cortex. 1996;6:311–318. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kolb B., Buhrmann K., McDonald R., Sutherland R.J. Dissociation of the Medial Prefrontal, Posterior Parietal, and Posterior Temporal Cortex for Spatial Navigation and Recognition Memory in the Rat. Cereb. Cortex. 1994;4:664–680. doi: 10.1093/cercor/4.6.664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Murray A.J., Sauer J.-F., Riedel G., McClure C., Ansel L., Cheyne L., Bartos M., Wisden W., Wulff P. Parvalbumin-positive CA1 interneurons are required for spatial working but not for reference memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:297–299. doi: 10.1038/nn.2751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Murray A.J., Woloszynowska-Fraser M.U., Ansel-Bollepalli L., Cole K.L.H., Foggetti A., Crouch B., Riedel G., Wulff P. Parvalbumin-positive interneurons of the prefrontal cortex support working memory and cognitive flexibility. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:16778. doi: 10.1038/srep16778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hyman J.M., Zilli E.A., Paley A.M., Hasselmo M.E. Working memory performance correlates with prefrontal-hippocampal theta interactions but not with prefrontal neuron firing rates. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2010;4:2. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.002.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jones M.W., Wilson M.A. Theta Rhythms Coordinate Hippocampal–Prefrontal Interactions in a Spatial Memory Task. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.O’Neill P.-K., Gordon J.A., Sigurdsson T. Theta Oscillations in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Are Modulated by Spatial Working Memory and Synchronize with the Hippocampus through Its Ventral Subregion. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:14211–14224. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2378-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Siapas A.G., Lubenov E.V., Wilson M.A. Prefrontal Phase Locking to Hippocampal Theta Oscillations. Neuron. 2005;46:141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sigurdsson T., Stark K.L., Karayiorgou M., Gogos J.A., Gordon J.A. Impaired hippocampal–prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophrenia. Nature. 2010;464:763–767. doi: 10.1038/nature08855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Spellman T., Rigotti M., Ahmari S.E., Fusi S., Gogos J.A., Gordon J.A. Hippocampal–prefrontal input supports spatial encoding in working memory. Nature. 2015;522:309–314. doi: 10.1038/nature14445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Tamura M., Spellman T.J., Rosen A.M., Gogos J.A., Gordon J.A. Hippocampal-prefrontal theta-gamma coupling during performance of a spatial working memory task. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:2182. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02108-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bähner F., Meyer-Lindenberg A. Hippocampal–prefrontal connectivity as a translational phenotype for schizophrenia. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;27:93–106. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Meyer-Lindenberg A.S., Olsen R.K., Kohn P.D., Brown T., Egan M.F., Weinberger D.R., Berman K.F. Regionally Specific Disturbance of Dorsolateral Prefrontal–Hippocampal Functional Connectivity in Schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2005;62:379–386. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.4.379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Swanson L.W. A direct projection from Ammon’s horn to prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Res. 1981;217:150–154. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90192-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cassel J.-C., Pereira de Vasconcelos A., Loureiro M., Cholvin T., Dalrymple-Alford J.C., Vertes R.P. The reuniens and rhomboid nuclei: Neuroanatomy, electrophysiological characteristics and behavioral implications. Prog. Neurobiol. 2013;111:34–52. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wirt R.A., Hyman J.M. Integrating Spatial Working Memory and Remote Memory: Interactions between the Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Hippocampus. Brain Sci. 2017;7:43. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7040043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cassel J.-C., Ferraris M., Quilichini P., Cholvin T., Boch L., Stephan A., Pereira de Vasconcelos A. The reuniens and rhomboid nuclei of the thalamus: A crossroads for cognition-relevant information processing? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021;126:338–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Dolleman-Van der Weel M.J., Witter M.P. Nucleus reuniens thalami innervates γ aminobutyric acid positive cells in hippocampal field CA1 of the rat. Neurosci. Lett. 2000;278:145–148. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00935-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mathiasen M.L., Amin E., Nelson A.J.D., Dillingham C.M., O’Mara S.M., Aggleton J.P. Separate cortical and hippocampal cell populations target the rat nucleus reuniens and mammillary bodies. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2019;49:1649–1672. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Scheel N., Wulff P., Mooij-van Malsen J.G. Afferent connections of the thalamic nucleus reuniens in the mouse. J. Comp. Neurol. 2020;528:1189–1202. doi: 10.1002/cne.24811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Varela C., Kumar S., Yang J.Y., Wilson M.A. Anatomical substrates for direct interactions between hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the thalamic nucleus reuniens. Brain Struct. Funct. 2014;219:911–929. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0543-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Vertes R.P., Hoover W.B., Szigeti-Buck K., Leranth C. Nucleus reuniens of the midline thalamus: Link between the medial prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus. Brain Res. Bull. 2007;71:601–609. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Vertes R.P., Linley S.B., Hoover W.B. Limbic circuitry of the midline thalamus. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015;54:89–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wouterlood F.G., Saldana E., Witter M.P. Projection from the nucleus reuniens thalami to the hippocampal region: Light and electron microscopic tracing study in the rat with the anterograde tracerPhaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin. J. Comp. Neurol. 1990;296:179–203. doi: 10.1002/cne.902960202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McKenna J.T., Vertes R.P. Afferent projections to nucleus reuniens of the thalamus. J. Comp. Neurol. 2004;480:115–142. doi: 10.1002/cne.20342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Vertes R.P., Hoover W.B., Do Valle A.C., Sherman A., Rodriguez J.J. Efferent projections of reuniens and rhomboid nuclei of the thalamus in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 2006;499:768–796. doi: 10.1002/cne.21135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ali M., Cholvin T., Muller M.A., Cosquer B., Kelche C., Cassel J.-C., Pereira de Vasconcelos A. Environmental enrichment enhances systems-level consolidation of a spatial memory after lesions of the ventral midline thalamus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017;141:108–123. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Barker G.R.I., Warburton E.C. A Critical Role for the Nucleus Reuniens in Long-Term, But Not Short-Term Associative Recognition Memory Formation. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:3208–3217. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1802-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ferraris M., Ghestem A., Vicente A.F., Nallet-Khosrofian L., Bernard C., Quilichini P.P. The Nucleus Reuniens Controls Long-Range Hippocampo–Prefrontal Gamma Synchronization during Slow Oscillations. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:3026–3038. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3058-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hallock H.L., Wang A., Griffin A.L. Ventral Midline Thalamus Is Critical for Hippocampal-Prefrontal Synchrony and Spatial Working Memory. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:8372–8389. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0991-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hembrook J.R., Mair R.G. Lesions of reuniens and rhomboid thalamic nuclei impair radial maze win-shift performance. Hippocampus. 2011;21:815–826. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Jayachandran M., Linley S.B., Schlecht M., Mahler S.V., Vertes R.P., Allen T.A. Prefrontal Pathways Provide Top-Down Control of Memory for Sequences of Events. Cell Rep. 2019;28:640–654.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.06.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Maisson D.J.-N., Gemzik Z.M., Griffin A.L. Optogenetic suppression of the nucleus reuniens selectively impairs encoding during spatial working memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2018;155:78–85. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Roy A., Svensson F.P., Mazeh A., Kocsis B. Prefrontal-hippocampal coupling by theta rhythm and by 2–5 Hz oscillation in the delta band: The role of the nucleus reuniens of the thalamus. Brain Struct. Funct. 2017;222:2819–2830. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1374-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Vetere G., Kenney J.W., Tran L.M., Xia F., Steadman P.E., Parkinson J., Josselyn S.A., Frankland P.W. Chemogenetic Interrogation of a Brain-wide Fear Memory Network in Mice. Neuron. 2017;94:363–374.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Xu W., Südhof T.C. A Neural Circuit for Memory Specificity and Generalization. Science. 2013;339:1290–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.1229534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Chudasama Y. In: Encyclopedia of Psychopharmacology. Stolerman I.P., editor. Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2010. Delayed (Non)Match-to-Sample Task; p. 372. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Dias R., Aggleton J.P. Effects of selective excitotoxic prefrontal lesions on acquisition of nonmatching- and matching-to-place in the T-maze in the rat: differential involvement of the prelimbic-infralimbic and anterior cingulate cortices in providing behavioural flexibility: Differential frontal role in behavioural inhibition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2000;12:4457–4466. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816X.2000.01323.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Talpos J.C., McTighe S.M., Dias R., Saksida L.M., Bussey T.J. Trial-unique, delayed nonmatching-to-location (TUNL): A novel, highly hippocampus-dependent automated touchscreen test of location memory and pattern separation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2010;94:341–352. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Teutsch J., Kätzel D. Operant Assessment of DMTP Spatial Working Memory in Mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2019;13:193. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dexter T.D., Palmer D., Hashad A.M., Saksida L.M., Bussey T.J. Decision Making in Mice During an Optimized Touchscreen Spatial Working Memory Task Sensitive to Medial Prefrontal Cortex Inactivation and NMDA Receptor Hypofunction. Front. Neurosci. 2022;16:905736. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.905736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kim C.H., Romberg C., Hvoslef-Eide M., Oomen C.A., Mar A.C., Heath C.J., Berthiaume A.-A., Bussey T.J., Saksida L.M. Trial-unique, delayed nonmatching-to-location (TUNL) touchscreen testing for mice: sensitivity to dorsal hippocampal dysfunction. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2015;232:3935–3945. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4017-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Benchenane K., Peyrache A., Khamassi M., Tierney P.L., Gioanni Y., Battaglia F.P., Wiener S.I. Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the hippocampal- prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron. 2010;66:921–936. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bygrave A.M., Jahans-Price T., Wolff A.R., Sprengel R., Kullmann D.M., Bannerman D.M., Kätzel D. Hippocampal–prefrontal coherence mediates working memory and selective attention at distinct frequency bands and provides a causal link between schizophrenia and its risk gene GRIA1. Transl. Psychiatry. 2019;9:142. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0471-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Parnaudeau S., O’Neill P.-K., Bolkan S.S., Ward R.D., Abbas A.I., Roth B.L., Balsam P.D., Gordon J.A., Kellendonk C. Inhibition of Mediodorsal Thalamus Disrupts Thalamofrontal Connectivity and Cognition. Neuron. 2013;77:1151–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Liu Y., McAfee S.S., Van Der Heijden M.E., Dhamala M., Sillitoe R.V., Heck D.H. Causal Evidence for a Role of Cerebellar Lobulus Simplex in Prefrontal-Hippocampal Interaction in Spatial Working Memory Decision-Making. Cerebellum. 2022;21:762–775. doi: 10.1007/s12311-022-01383-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Deacon R.M.J., Rawlins J.N.P. T-maze alternation in the rodent. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:7–12. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bastos A.M., Schoffelen J.-M. A Tutorial Review of Functional Connectivity Analysis Methods and Their Interpretational Pitfalls. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2015;9:175. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Buzsáki G., Schomburg E.W. What does gamma coherence tell us about inter-regional neural communication? Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:484–489. doi: 10.1038/nn.3952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Mukai J., Tamura M., Fénelon K., Rosen A.M., Spellman T.J., Kang R., MacDermott A.B., Karayiorgou M., Gordon J.A., Gogos J.A. Molecular Substrates of Altered Axonal Growth and Brain Connectivity in a Mouse Model of Schizophrenia. Neuron. 2015;86:680–695. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Engel A.K., Fries P. Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2010;20:156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Spitzer B., Haegens S. Beyond the Status Quo: A Role for Beta Oscillations in Endogenous Content (Re)Activation. eNeuro. 2017;4 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0170-17.2017. ENEURO.0170-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Axmacher N., Schmitz D.P., Wagner T., Elger C.E., Fell J. Interactions between Medial Temporal Lobe, Prefrontal Cortex, and Inferior Temporal Regions during Visual Working Memory: A Combined Intracranial EEG and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:7304–7312. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1778-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lundqvist M., Rose J., Herman P., Brincat S.L., Buschman T.J., Miller E.K. Gamma and Beta Bursts Underlie Working Memory. Neuron. 2016;90:152–164. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Siegel M., Warden M.R., Miller E.K. Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:21341–21346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908193106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Schmidt R., Herrojo Ruiz M., Kilavik B.E., Lundqvist M., Starr P.A., Aron A.R. Beta Oscillations in Working Memory, Executive Control of Movement and Thought, and Sensorimotor Function. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:8231–8238. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1163-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Jayachandran M., Viena T.D., Garcia A., Veliz A.V., Leyva S., Roldan V., Vertes R.P., Allen T.A. Nucleus reuniens transiently synchronizes memory networks at beta frequencies. Nat Commun. 2023;14:4326. doi: 10.1038/s41467-023-40044-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Cholvin T., Loureiro M., Cassel R., Cosquer B., Geiger K., De Sa Nogueira D., Raingard H., Robelin L., Kelche C., Pereira de Vasconcelos A., Cassel J.C. The Ventral Midline Thalamus Contributes to Strategy Shifting in a Memory Task Requiring Both Prefrontal Cortical and Hippocampal Functions. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:8772–8783. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0771-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kafetzopoulos V., Kokras N., Sotiropoulos I., Oliveira J.F., Leite-Almeida H., Vasalou A., Sardinha V.M., Papadopoulou-Daifoti Z., Almeida O.F.X., Antoniou K., et al. The nucleus reuniens: a key node in the neurocircuitry of stress and depression. Mol. Psychiatry. 2018;23:579–586. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Mathiasen M.L., O’Mara S.M., Aggleton J.P. The anterior thalamic nuclei and nucleus reuniens: So similar but so different. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020;119:268–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Prasad J.A., Macgregor E.M., Chudasama Y. Lesions of the thalamic reuniens cause impulsive but not compulsive responses. Brain Struct. Funct. 2013;218:85–96. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0378-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Ito H.T., Zhang S.-J., Witter M.P., Moser E.I., Moser M.-B. A prefrontal–thalamo–hippocampal circuit for goal-directed spatial navigation. Nature. 2015;522:50–55. doi: 10.1038/nature14396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Heck D.H., Fox M.B., Correia Chapman B., McAfee S.S., Liu Y. Cerebellar control of thalamocortical circuits for cognitive function: A review of pathways and a proposed mechanism. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2023;17:1126508. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2023.1126508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Ye X., Kapeller-Libermann D., Travaglia A., Inda M.C., Alberini C.M. Direct dorsal hippocampal–prelimbic cortex connections strengthen fear memories. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:52–61. doi: 10.1038/nn.4443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Qin C., Bian X.-L., Wu H.-Y., Xian J.-Y., Cai C.-Y., Lin Y.-H., Zhou Y., Kou X.-L., Chang L., Luo C.-X., Zhu D.Y. Dorsal Hippocampus to Infralimbic Cortex Circuit is Essential for the Recall of Extinction Memory. Cereb. Cortex. 2021;31:1707–1718. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Bastos A.M., Vezoli J., Bosman C.A., Schoffelen J.-M., Oostenveld R., Dowdall J.R., De Weerd P., Kennedy H., Fries P. Visual Areas Exert Feedforward and Feedback Influences through Distinct Frequency Channels. Neuron. 2015;85:390–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Tang W., Jadhav S.P. Sharp-wave ripples as a signature of hippocampal-prefrontal reactivation for memory during sleep and waking states. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2019;160:11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Gregoriou G.G., Gotts S.J., Zhou H., Desimone R. High-Frequency, Long-Range Coupling Between Prefrontal and Visual Cortex During Attention. Science. 2009;324:1207–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.1171402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Oomen C.A., Hvoslef-Eide M., Heath C.J., Mar A.C., Horner A.E., Bussey T.J., Saksida L.M. The touchscreen operant platform for testing working memory and pattern separation in rats and mice. Nat. Protoc. 2013;8:2006–2021. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Mathis A., Mamidanna P., Cury K.M., Abe T., Murthy V.N., Mathis M.W., Bethge M. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Cohen J. 2nd ed. L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Faul F., Erdfelder E., Buchner A., Lang A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G∗Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods. 2009;41:1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
-
•
Local field potential data reported in this paper will be shared by the lead contact upon request.
-
•
Data and code used for the coherogram analysis and the analysis of running speed were deposited on Mendeley at https://doi.org/10.17632/v6fz34k2dy.1.
-
•
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.